You are on page 1of 11

R.L.

Zweigenhaft:
Journalof IndividualD
© 2008
ifferences A Do
Hogrefe
2008; Re 29(1):45–55
& Vol. Mi
Huber En core
Publishers

A Do Re Mi Encore
A Closer Look at the Personality
Correlates of Music Preferences
Richard L. Zweigenhaft
Guilford College, Greensboro, NC, USA

Abstract. Through the development of the Short Test of Musical Preferences (STOMP) and a larger theory of music preferences,
Rentfrow and Gosling (2003, 2006) have helped guide the way toward understanding the role of music in people’s lives, and the
relationship between music preferences and personality. The four music dimensions they established in their 2003 study provide a
broad-brush look at some of the relationships between music preferences and personality. This study of 83 undergraduates at Guilford
College in Greensboro, NC, used the NEO-PI, rather than the Big Five Inventory, which allowed us to examine the six facets that make
up each of the Big Five traits as well as those traits themselves, and it looked separately at the music genres that make up the four music
dimensions identified by Rentfrow and Gosling (2003). The findings provide general support for Rentfrow and Gosling’s work, but
they also demonstrate that the personality patterns for the specific music genres differ considerably from one another, even those that
fall within the same broad music dimensions. The Openness trait was by far the most robust of the Big Five traits assessed by the
NEO-PI, and preferences for some music genres (e.g., folk, international music, and rap/hip-hop) were far more revealing of personality
than others (e.g., classical, rock, and electronic).

Keywords: music preferences, music and personality, music genres

For many years, psychologists have been interested in the More recently, in an effort to expand the focus from spe-
personalities of highly creative people, including, of course, cific groups of especially devoted fans, like deadheads and
musicians (e.g., Barron, 1968, 1969). Researchers have stud- metalheads, to a broader look at a wide range of music
ied the personalities of music majors (e.g., Bourke & Francis, preferences, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) developed the
2000, Shuter, 2000), those who play certain instruments (e.g., Short Test of Music Preferences (STOMP). The 2003 ver-
Ciuffardi and Noemi, 2000), professional women musicians sion of the STOMP asked respondents to rate 14 different
(e.g., Stremikis, 2002), and Australian rock and pop musi- music genres. A factor analysis indicated that these 14 gen-
cians (Gillespie & Myors, 2000). res clustered into four general dimensions that they called
Social scientists also have written about devoted fans of reflective and complex (including preferences for blues,
various music genres and music groups. For example, var- jazz, classical, and folk music), Intense and rebellious
ious sociologists (e.g., Weinstein, 1991) and psychologists (rock, alternative, and heavy metal), upbeat and conven-
(e.g., Arnett, 1996) have studied metalheads, fans of heavy tional (country, sound track, religious, and pop music), and
metal music. In a series of studies that drew on question- energetic and rhythmic (rap/hip-hop, soul/funk, and elec-
naires, interviews, as well as observations of concerts, Ar- tronica/dance music). In a series of studies, they found that
nett (1996) found that metalheads scored higher on sensa- those with preferences for each of these four dimensions
tion-seeking than a control group, they were more likely shared similar personality characteristics. For example,
than other Americans of their age to be from divorced and those who preferred reflective and complex music tended
dysfunctional families, and they were very much alienated to score high on Emotional Stability and Openness.
from mainstream culture. Similarly, sociologist Rebecca Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) assessed personality using
Adams and her students followed the Grateful Dead on one the Big Five Inventory, a 44-item measure that provides
of their tours, gathering data about deadheads through in- scores for each of the five broad personality domains gen-
terviews, surveys, and participant observation (Adams & erally referred to as the Big Five (Neuroticism, Extraver-
Sardiello, 2000). McGown and Dulaney (1999), using a sion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness).
measure of the Grateful Dead experience (GDE) and an Unlike the 240-item NEO-PI itself, however, on which the
assessment of personality traits, found that those with the Big Five Inventory is based, this shorter measure does not
Grateful Dead experience were significantly more likely yield data on the 30 facets that make up the Big Five traits.
than most people to be outgoing, open to new and different For example, each participant in Rentfrow and Gosling’s
experiences, and agreeable. (2003) studies received a score on Neuroticism (on the Big

© 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55
DOI 10.1027/1614-0001.29.1.45
46 R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore

Five Inventory it is called Emotional Stability), but not on students used their NEO-PI scores as part of the self-anal-
the six facets that constitute Neuroticism – anxiety, angry yses they wrote for an Introduction to Personality course.
hostility, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, All agreed to allow the instructor to use the NEO-PI data
and vulnerability. The studies that have been done of met- for this project on music preferences.
alheads and deadheads do not lead one to assume that either As of the fall of 2004, when this study of music prefer-
group is more or less neurotic (or emotionally stable) than ences and personality was designed, 49 students had taken
other people, but they do suggest that metalheads would the NEO-PI; some were still students at the college, but
score higher and deadheads would score lower on angry others had graduated or transferred. Those who were still
hostility, and that both metalheads and deadheads might Guilford students, and those for whom addresses could be
score higher on impulsiveness. As Rentfrow and Gosling obtained, were contacted and asked to fill in the STOMP;
concluded, “future research that includes narrower facets 21 did so. Between the spring of 2004 and the spring of
of personality is needed to provide a finer grained picture 2007, another 62 students took both the NEO-PI and the
of the effects of personality on music preferences” (2003, STOMP (as of the spring of 2004, all students took a re-
p. 1251). vised version of the STOMP that included an additional
Similarly, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) demonstrated seven musical genres: bluegrass, funk, gospel, internation-
the broad personality patterns that correlated with prefer- al, oldies, opera, and punk; an even more recent version of
ences for each of the four music dimensions, but they did the STOMP includes reggae and new age). Thus, this study
not include information on personality patterns for those included NEO-PI scores and STOMP scores for 83 current
who prefer the various genres that fall within each dimen- or former students; for 64 students, the expanded 21-genre
sion. They reported, for example, the correlations between version of the STOMP was used.
the Big Five trait scores and scores on reflective and com- The students fell into three groups: 51 (or 61.4%) were
plex, but they did not report separate correlations between traditional-aged undergraduates; 14 (or 16.9%) were part
these traits (or their facets) and preferences for the four of the college’s adult education program, which means they
music genres that factored onto reflective and complex were Greensboro residents and older than 23; and 18 (or
(blues, classical, folk, and jazz). Even though preferences 21.7%) were part of an early college program that consists
for classical and folk music both loaded on the same factor of gifted high-school students who have been accepted into
and, thus, intercorrelated, it is possible – indeed, it is likely an on-campus program that allows them to enroll in college
– that those who prefer classical music differ in personality courses even though they are still in high school. The av-
from those who prefer folk music. Moreover, Rentfrow and erage age for the early college students was 16.6; for the
Gosling have since added at least nine new music genres traditional aged students, it was 19.4; and for the adult stu-
to their initial 14, indicating that the original 14 did not dents, it was 36.6. As is generally true of the psychology
cover certain genres important to music fans, and under- majors at the college, most were female (66, or 80%) and
scoring the need to consider the specific genres separately. most were white (69, or 83%).
The primary purpose of this study was to extend the
work of Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) by exploring the re-
lationship between music preferences and personality us-
ing finer gradations for each of the two. By using the longer Results
and more complex NEO-PI instead of the shorter and sim-
pler Big Five Inventory, we were able to look at scores for Gender, Race, and Age Differences
the Big Five traits and for the thirty facets that make up
those traits. In addition, we looked not only at Rentfrow After checking to make sure there were no outliers (there
and Gosling’s (2003) four music dimensions, but also at the were none), we looked to see if there were meaningful dif-
individual music genres. ferences between the men and women, the white students
and the students of color, and the traditional and nontradi-
tional age college students in our sample.

Methods
Gender
Between the spring of 2001 and the spring of 2007, students
taking certain psychology courses at Guilford College, a As noted, 66 of the students in our sample were women,
small liberal arts college in Greensboro, North Carolina, and 17 were men. When we compared these two subgroups
were given the option of taking the NEO-PI. About one in terms of music preferences, using independent sample
fourth of the students in these classes chose to do so. This t-tests, we found significant differences on four of the 21
entailed taking the test on their own (this generally took music genres. The men and women differed significantly
between 45 min and 1 h), scoring it (about 15 to 20 min), on only one of the original 14 music genres: The women
and then going over the meaning of the scores with the gave higher ratings to pop, Mf = 4.89, SD = 1.58, than did
instructor (this usually took between 30 and 45 min). Most the men, Mm = 3.82, SD = 1.67, t = 2.46, df = 81, p < .01.

Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55 © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore 47

Table 1. Mean scores and ANOVAs, music preferences for early college students, traditional aged students, and adult
students
Early college Traditional aged Adult students F df p<
n = 18 n = 51 n = 14
Blues 4.71ab 5.45a 4.57b 7.81 2 .001
b b
Country 3.00 3.71 4.79 3.87 2 .03
Folk 3.44a 4.76a 4.07 4.38 2 .02
Heavy metal 2.89 3.45c 2.00c 3.72 2 .03
c c
Rap/hip-hop 3.75 4.71 2.79 5.55 2 .01
Religious 3.89 2.91c 5.36c 10.27. 2 .001
Soulfunk 4.03b 4.50 5.43b 3.12 2 .05
Funk 3.43a 4.53a 3.86 3.29 2 .04
a c b,c
Punk 4.43 4.40 2.50 6.86 2 .002
Gospel 3.71 3.58c 5.14c 3.83 2 .03
a
difference between early college and traditional aged students, p < .05, using Tukey; bdifference between early college and adult students, p <
.05, using Tukey; cdifference between traditional aged students and adult students, p < .05, using Tukey.

The women also gave higher ratings than the men on three students). It appears that these three student subgroups have
of the subsequently added music genres: international (Mf notably different tastes in music, and we will need to keep
= 4.74, SD = 1.57, Mm = 3.54, SD = 1.76, t = 2.39, df = 62, this in mind as we examine the relationship between music
p < .02), gospel (Mf = 4.22, SD = 1.78, Mm = 2.92, SD = preferences and personality.
2.10, df = 62, t = 2.25, p < .03), and punk (Mf = 4.23, SD
= 1.86, Mm = 3.08, SD = 1.61, t = 2.04, df = 62, p < .05).
The Four Music Dimensions and the Big Five
Race Traits

Similarly, when we compared the 64 white students with the First, we looked to see if the four categories of music pref-
13 students of color, we found that these two groups differed erence that were identified in Rentfrow and Gosling’s
in their ratings of three of the 21 music genres: The students (2003) factor analysis demonstrated the same personality
of color gave higher ratings to religious music (Msofc = 4.57, patterns for this sample, based as it was on the NEO-PI
SD = 2.17, Mwh = 3.33, SD = 1.94, t = 2.15, df = 81, p < .04), itself (rather than the much shorter Big Five Inventory).
to soulfunk (Msofc = 5.32, SD = 1.64, Mwh = 4.40, SD = 1.60, The results can be seen in Table 2, which includes the cor-
t = 1.96, df = 81, p < .05)-and to gospel (Msofc = 5.33, SD = relations Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) obtained in two
1.30, Mwh = 3.63, SD = 1.89, t = 2.95, df = 62, p < .005). studies, both based on data from undergraduates at the Uni-
versity of Texas at Austin (their first sample included 1,704
students taking psychology courses in the fall of 2001, and
Age their second sample included 1,383 students taking psy-
chology courses in the spring of 2002), and the 83 students
As noted, in addition to the 51 traditional age students, our in this current study.
sample also included 14 adult education students (over 23 As can be seen, for the first domain of music preference,
years of age) and 18 high school students in an early college reflective and complex, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found
program. When we ran one-way analyses of variance com- only one correlation with a Big Five trait that was statistically
paring these three groups, we found significant differences significant in both their samples: Those high in this domain
in the ratings on 10 of the 21 music genres. These findings, scored higher on Openness (r = .44 and r = .41). The data are
which can be seen in Table 1, indicate that the traditional similar in the current sample (r = .34).
age students gave significantly higher ratings than at least Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) also found significant pos-
one of the other two groups on blues, folk, heavy metal, itive correlations between scores on the second domain of
rap/hip-hop, and funk; the adult students gave significantly music preference, Intense and rebellious, and Openness (r
higher ratings than at least one of the other two groups on = .18, r = .15), and so did we in the current study (r = .15).
country, religious, soulfunk, and gospel; the early college For the third domain of music preference, upbeat and con-
students’ ratings fell between the other two groups on all ventional, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found significant
genres except for punk, which they rated slightly but not correlations on four of the Big Five traits (Extraversion, r
significantly higher than the traditional age students (both = .24 and r = .15; Agreeableness, r = .23 and r = .24; Open-
groups rated punk significantly higher than did the adult ness, r = –.14 and r = –.08; and Conscientiousness, r = .15

© 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55
48 R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore

Table 2. Correlates of the four music preference dimensions with the Big Five personality traits
Reflective & Complex Intense & Rebellious Upbeat & Conventional Energetic & Rhythmic
R&G1 R&G2 Z R&G1 R&G2 Z R&G1 R&G2 Z R& 1 R&G2
N= N= N = 83 N = 1704 N = N = 83 N = N= N = 83 N = N= N = 83
Big Five 1,704 1,383 1,383 1,704 1,383 1,704 1,383
Neuroticism –.08* –.04 –.18 .01 .01 –.11 .07 .04 .09 –.01 .01 .02
Extraversion .01 –.02 .00 0 .08* –.04 .24* .15* .09 .22 .19* .22*
Openness .44* .41* .35* .18* .15* .15 –.14* –.08* –.36*ab .03 .04 .32*ab
Agreeableness .01 .03 .09 –.04 .01 –.03 .23* .24* .13 .08* .09* –.07
Conscientiousness –.02 –.06 –.01 –.04 –.03 –.10 .15* .18* .23 0 –.03 –.12
* p < .05; *p < .05, two-tailed. aZ score comparisons significantly different from R&G1; bZ score comparisons significantly different from
R&G2.

Table 3. Correlations between the four music preferences, the Big Five, and the 30 facets
Reflective & Complex Intense & Rebellious Upbeat & Conventional Energetic & Rhythmic
N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 N = 83
Neuroticism –.18 –.11 .09 .02
Anxiety –.15 –.02 .18 .02
Angry hostility –.28* –.21 .03 –.02
Depression –.11 –.08 .12 –.04
Self-consciousness –.08 .01 .05 –.10
Impulsiveness –.06 –.08 –.13 .13
Vulnerability –.23* –.12 .07 .05
Extraversion .00 –.04 .09 .22
Warmth .05 –.03 .10 .17
Gregariousness –.13 –.04 .07 .19
Assertiveness .20 –.11 .11 .04
Activity –.04 –.18 .11 .00
Excitement seeking –.17 .09 .01 .28*
Positive emotions .08 .10 –.03 .23*
Openness .35* .15 –.36* .32*
Fantasy .23* .02 –.22* .24*
Esthetics .32* .06 –.23* .32*
Feelings .11 .08 –.19 .23*
Actions .26* .10 –.02 .22*
Ideas .28* .09 –.26* .09
Values .15 .25* –.44* .17
Agreeableness .09 –.03 .13 –.07
Trust .22* .03 .01 –.07
Straightforwardness –.02 –.08 .07 –.29*
Altruism .14 .07 .20 .19
Compliance –.11 –.08 .04 –.05
Modesty .09 –.03 .17 –.10
Tendermindedness .12 –.06 .04 .11
Conscientiousness –.01 –.10 .23* –.12
Competence .09 .03 .12 .01
Order –.15 –.16 .17 –.08
Dutifulness .12 –.15 .24* –.08
Achievement striving –.05 –.16 .24* –.15
Self-discipline .01 .01 .19 –.18
Deliberation –.03 .00 .08 –.05
*p < .05, two-tailed.

Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55 © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore 49

and r = .18). In our sample, we found the same patterns for and electronic (the first two of these were on their reflective
all four: Agreeableness (r = .13), Openness (r = –.36), Con- and complex factor, and the third was on what they called
scientiousness (r = .23), and Extraversion (r = .09). Com- energetic and rhythmic). Finally, the fifth factor included
parisons between the correlations in this study and the cor- rap/hip-hop and soul/funk (both of which fell on their en-
relations in their studies, based on conversions to z scores, ergetic and rhythmic factor). Given how much smaller our
revealed that the correlation in this study between this do- sample was than theirs, this factor analysis appears gener-
main and Openness was significantly more negative than ally to confirm their work, but it also suggests that for dif-
the two correlations they obtained (z = 1.86, N = 1,787, p ferent populations (theirs was in Texas, ours in North Car-
< .04 and z = 2.09, N = 1,466, p < .02). olina), and at different times (theirs was done in 2001 and
Finally, on the fourth dimension of music preferences, en- 2002, ours between 2004 and 2007), factor analyses on the
ergetic and rhythmic, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found sig- ratings of the 14 music genres might lead to slightly differ-
nificant patterns that held for both of their samples on two of ent clusters. This, too, demonstrates the need to look sepa-
the Big Five traits: Those who scored high on this dimension rately at the different genres rather than relying solely on
scored high on Extraversion (r = .22, r = .19) and Agreeable- the factors that emerged in their research
ness (r = .08, r = .09). The students in our study demonstrated
a similar pattern for Extraversion (r = .22), but the opposite
pattern for Agreeableness (r = –.07); comparisons based on The Four Music Dimensions and the 30
conversions to z scores approached but did not reach conven-
tional levels of significance (z = 1.27, N = 1,787, p < .11, and Facets Within the Big Five Traits
z = 1.19, N = 1,466, p < .12). In addition, although Rentfrow
and Gosling (2003) found no meaningful relationship between As we have indicated, the NEO-PI includes six facets for
preference for energetic and rhythmic music and Openness (r each of the Big Five traits. When we ran reliability analyses
= .03 and r = .04), for the current sample we found a significant using Cronbach’s αs for the scores on these five sets of
positive correlation (r = .32); conversions to standard scores facets, they were as follows: Neuroticism, .83; Extraver-
revealed that these correlations differed significantly (z = 2.48, sion, .80; Openness, .72; Agreeableness, .71; and Consci-
N = 1,787, p < .01, and z = .09, N = 1,466, p < .02). entiousness, .87. Although these facets intercorrelate with
The current data, therefore, show considerable but not one another, they tap distinctly different dimensions of per-
total agreement with the patterns reported in Rentfrow and sonality. For example, a person who scores high on Neu-
Gosling’s (2003) study. The correlations found in the cur- roticism because of a high score on angry hostility is likely
rent study correlate with those of the first Rentfrow and to be quite different from someone who scores equally high
Gosling sample at r = .78, and with those of the second on Neuroticism but who has a low score on angry hostility.
Rentfrow and Gosling sample at r = .76. The weakest cor- Similarly, a person who scores high on Neuroticism who
relations were on the energetic and rhythmic dimension (r has a high score on self-consciousness differs from a person
= .40 and r = .47), as was evidenced by the fact that they with an equally high Neuroticism score but a low score on
found no significant relationships with Openness in their self-consciousness. Table 3 includes the correlations be-
two samples but we found a significant positive relation- tween scores on the four musical preference dimensions
ship, and they found a positive relationship with Agree- and the 30 facets assessed by the NEO-PI.
ableness, but we found a negative relationship. Similarly,
when we ran reliability analyses for the genres that made
up each of Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003) four music do- Reflective and Complex
mains, we found the following Cronbach’s αs: reflective
and complex, .98; intense and rebellious, .94; upbeat and As can be seen in Table 3, in our study two of the six facets,
conventional, .92; and energetic and rhythmic, .64. angry hostility (r = –.28) and vulnerability (r = –.23) cor-
When we performed a factor analysis similar to the one related significantly; high ratings on this dimension of mu-
employed by Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) – that is, a prin- sic were significantly related to lower scores on angry hos-
cipal components factor analysis using a varimax rotation tility and vulnerability.
– on the ratings of the 83 students in this study on these 14 Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) did not find meaningful
music genres, we obtained five, not four factors, but they relationships between this music dimension and the trait of
were quite similar to those that Rentfrow and Gosling Extraversion, nor did we. There were no significant rela-
found. Two of the factors consisted of the very same gen- tionships with any of the six Extraversion facets (though
res: the first factor included pop, religious, soundtracks, one, assertiveness, approached significance, r = .20, p <
and country (the factor they called upbeat and convention- .07). Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found strong positive
al), and the second factor included heavy metal, rock, and correlations between this music domain and Openness (r =
alternate (what they called intense and rebellious). The .44 and r = .41), as did we (r = .35). Preferences for this
third factor in this factor analysis included jazz and classi- domain correlated positively with all six of the Openness
cal (two of the four genres that loaded on their reflective facets, but only four of the six reached conventional levels
and complex factor). The fourth factor included folk, blues, of significance: openness to fantasy (r = .34), openness to

© 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55
50 R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore

esthetics (r = .40), openness to actions (r = .26), and open- the other two facets, openness to feelings, approached con-
ness to ideas (r = .28). ventional levels of significance (r = .19, p < .08).
The data in the current study suggest a stronger relation- Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found positive correla-
ship between preference for reflective and complex music tions between a preference for this music dimension and
and the fourth of the Big Five traits, Agreeableness, than Agreeableness (r = .23 and r = .24), as did we (r = .13).
was apparent in Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003) earlier The correlations for the six Agreeableness facets indicate
work. Although they found virtually no correlation be- that the stronger relationships were for modesty (r = .17)
tween these two (r = .01 and r = .03), we found a slightly, and altruism (r = .20), although neither reached conven-
though not significantly, stronger correlation of r = .09. In tional levels of significance. Similarly, in all three studies
our study, there was a significant correlation between pref- there were positive correlations between preference scores
erences for this domain and ratings for the trust facet (r = for this music dimension and Conscientiousness (r = .15, r
.22). = .18, r = .23). The data for the six facets indicate that the
Like Rentfrow and Gosling (2003), we found no mean- relationship is based largely on dutifulness (r = .24) and
ingful patterns between this music preference dimension achievement striving (r = .24). .
and the trait of Conscientiousness, nor did preference for
this music dimension correlate significantly with any of the
six facets that constitute this personality trait. Energetic and Rhythmic

Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found that those who pre-


Intense and Rebellious ferred energetic and rhythmic music scored significantly
higher on Extraversion (r = .22 and r = .19) and Agreeable-
ness (r = .08 and r = .09). We, too, found a positive corre-
Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found no significant pattern
lation for Extraversion (r = .22), but, as noted above, we
between scores on preferences for intense and rebellious
found a negative rather than a positive correlation with
music and Neuroticism, nor did we. We did, however, find
Agreeableness (r = –.07). A closer look at the correlations
one near-significant relationship: those who prefer this
for the six facets within Extraversion reveals that two, ex-
kind of music scored lower on angry hostility (r = –.21, p
citement seeking (r = .28) and positive emotions (r = .23),
< .06). Preference for intense and rebellious music did not
were significant. As for the facets that make up Agreeable-
correlate with any of the other traits, and with only one of
ness, one of the six was significant: Those who preferred
the other 24 facets; students who preferred this domain also
this music domain scored significantly lower on straight-
scored high on the openness to values facet of the Openness
forwardness (r = –.29).
trait (r = .25).
As noted above, whereas Rentfrow and Gosling (2003)
found no relationship between preference for this music
dimension and Openness, we found a positive relationship
Upbeat and Conventional (r = .32) that was statistically significant in our study and
significantly stronger than the correlations found in Rent-
Neither Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) nor we found signif- frow and Gosling’s (2003) study. A more careful look at
icant correlations between ratings on upbeat and conven- the correlations for the six facets indicates that four were
tional music and scores on Neuroticism (the correlations in significant: openness to fantasy (r = .24), openness to es-
their samples were r = .07 and r = .04; in this study it was thetics (r = .32), openness to feelings (r = .23), and open-
r = .09). There was a positive correlation between ratings ness to actions (r = .22).
on this music dimension and scores on one of the six facets,
but it did not reach statistical significance: Those who pre-
ferred upbeat and conventional music were more likely to The Specific 21 Musical Genres
score high on Anxiety (r = .18, p < .11).
Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found positive correla- As noted, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) determined that the
tions between ratings of upbeat and conventional music and original 14 musical genres fell into the four music dimen-
Extraversion, as did we, although the pattern was not sig- sions we have examined thus far in this paper; they subse-
nificant in our data for the trait or for the facets that make quently have identified another nine music genres (Gos-
up the trait. As noted, they found a negative relationship ling, 2006, personal communication), seven of which we
between this music domain and Openness (r = –.14 and r included in this study. We looked to see if there were dis-
= –.08), and we found a significantly stronger negative re- tinctive patterns for each of the music genres. As can be
lationship (r = –.36). Our more detailed look at the six fac- seen in Tables 4–7, the patterns for the various music pref-
ets revealed negative correlations with all six facets, four erence genres within the four dimensions are sometimes
of which were statistically significant: openness to fantasy similar, but not the same; in some cases the differences are
(r = –.22), openness to esthetics (r = –.23), openness to dramatic. These tables include the correlations with each
ideas (r = –.26), and openness to values (r = –.44); one of of the Big Five traits (whether they were statistically sig-

Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55 © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore 51

Table 4. Correlations between reflective and complex gen- Table 5. Correlations between Intense and Rebellious gen-
res, traits and facets res, traits, and facets
Blues Classical Folk Jazz Alternate Heavy metal Rock
N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 N = 83
Neuroticism –.12 –.16 –.08 –.17 Neuroticism .05 –.19 –.08
Angry Hostility –.25* Angry hostility –.23*
Vulnerability –.22* Extraversion –.03 –.08 .03
Extraversion .02 –.05 –.02 .07 Openness .08 .19 .04
Assertiveness .32* Values .26* .24*
Openness .30* .04 .40* .27* Agreeableness –.01 –.09 .04
Fantasy .32* Conscientiousness –.01 –.18 .01
Esthetics .23* .29* .32* Order –.22*
Feelings .20 Achievement striving –.22*
Actions .26* .22* *p < .05, two-tailed.
Ideas .29* .22*
Values .25* .27* tive, r = –.28). The ratings for heavy metal correlated sig-
Agreeableness .07 .10 .03 .07 nificantly, and negatively, with one of the Neuroticism fac-
Trust .23*
ets, angry hostility (r = –.23). Ratings for heavy metal also
correlated negatively with Conscientiousness (r = –.18),
Modesty .23*
and significantly so on two of the Conscientiousness facets,
Conscientiousness .02 .03 –.16 .09 order (r = –.22) and achievement striving (r = –.22). There-
Dutifulness fore, despite their reputation as angry and rebellious, those
*p < .05, two-tailed. who preferred heavy metal music scored low, not high, on
angry hostility. Furthermore, although Arnett (1996) re-
nificant or not) and, for the 30 facets that make up those ported that the metalheads in his studies were prone to sen-
five traits, only those correlations that were significant at sation seeking and risk taking, we found only a moderate
the p < .05 level. and nonsignificant relationship for the excitement seeking
Table 4 includes the data for the four genres in the re- facet of the NEO-PI (r = .12, p < .26). Similarly, although
flective and complex domain (blues, classical, folk, and there was a significant negative correlation between pref-
jazz). As can be seen, there were positive and significant erences for heavy metal music and achievement striving,
correlations between the ratings for blues, folk, and jazz these data do not reveal the extreme alienation that Arnett
and scores on the Openness trait, and on some or most of found in his interviewees.
the facets that make up Openness. This was not the case, Four music preference genres factored onto the third
however, for ratings for classical music, the fourth genre in music dimension, upbeat and conventional: country, pop,
this dimension (r = .04). Ratings for folk music correlated religious, and soundtracks. As can be seen in Table 6, there
negatively with angry hostility (r = –.25), and ratings for was one consistent pattern: High scores on these four gen-
jazz correlated negatively with vulnerability (r = –.22), res correlated negatively with Openness. All four of the
positively with assertiveness (r = .32) and positively with correlations with this trait were negative, and there was a
modesty (r = .23). These patterns reveal little information significant negative correlation between each of these mu-
about the personalities of those who like classical music, sic genres and at least one of the six Openness facets. In
more information about those who like the other three gen- addition, ratings for pop correlated positively with Consci-
res in this domain, and the most information about those entious (r = .22), and ratings for religious music correlated
who like jazz (they are not only open, as are those who positively with both the trait of Conscientious (r = .21, p <
prefer the blues and folk music, but they manage to be both .06) and the dutifulness facet (r = .27). There was a positive
assertive and modest). correlation between ratings for soundtracks and both the
The second music dimension, intense and rebellious, in- Agreeableness trait (r = .26) and the modesty facet (r = .28).
cludes alternative, heavy metal, and rock. As Table 5 re- Finally, the fourth music dimension, energetic and rhyth-
veals, only one personality dimension correlated with pref- mic, included three music genres, which Rentfrow and
erences for alternative music (those who rated it high Gosling (2003) called electronic, rap/hip-hop, and
scored high on openness to values) and none correlated soul/funk. As Table 7 indicates, a strong personality profile
with preferences for rock music. The personality correlates emerged for rap/hip-hop, but not for the other two. Ratings
for heavy metal are more revealing, and are intriguing. Rat- for rap/hip-hop correlated significantly with impulsiveness
ings for heavy metal correlated negatively with Neuroti- (r = .22), Extraversion (r = .28), gregariousness (r = .32),
cism (r = –.19 – only one other correlation, between ratings excitement seeking (r = .42), positive emotions ((r = .28),
for international music and Neuroticism, was more nega- Openness ((r = .38), openness to esthetics (r = .33), open-

© 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55
52 R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore

Table 6. Correlations between Upbeat and Conventional Table 7. Correlations between Energetic and Rhythmic
genres, traits, and facets genres, traits, and facets
Country Pop Religious Sound- Electronic Rap/hip-hop Soul/funk
tracks N = 83 N = 83 N = 83
N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 N = 83 Neuroticism –.10 .06 .07
Neuroticism .08 .06 .02 .11 Impulsiveness .22*
Impulsiveness –.23* Extraversion .01 .28* .14
Extraversion .09 .17 .00 –.01 Warmth .22*
Openness –.11 –.31* –.36* –.26* Gregariousness .32*
Esthetics –.28* –.23* Excitement-seeking .42*
Feelings –.27* Positive emotions .28*
Ideas –.30* Openness .04 .38* .22*
Values –.22* –.25* –.54* –.24* Esthetics .33* .22*
Agreeableness .02 .01 .11 .26* Feelings .32*
Modesty .28* Actions .22*
Conscientiousness .07 .22 .21 .17 Values .34*
Dutifulness .27* Agreeableness –.16 –.03 .06
*p < .05, two-tailed.
Straightforwardness –.26*
Altruism .22*
Conscientiousness –.09 –.19 .07
Self-discipline –.24*
*p < .05, two-tailed.

Table 8. Correlations between newly added genres, traits, and facets


Bluegrass International Oldies Opera Punk Gospel Funk
n = 64 n = 64 n = 64 n = 64 n = 64 n = 64 n = 64
Neuroticism –.11 –.28* .05 –.14 –.03 –.02 –.11
Anxiety –.25*
Angry hostility –.28*
Depression –.26*
Vulnerability –.25*
Extraversion –.02 .08 .02 .16 .06 .15 –.01
Assertiveness .26*
Openness .37* .43* .22 .37* .34* .08 .38*
Fantasy .35* .38* .32* .27* .26* .34*
Esthetics .28* .34* .42*
Feelings .32* .25* .25*
Actions .34* .33*
Values .29* .36* .28* .42*
Agreeableness .05 .02 –.02 .05 .16 .26* .01
Compliance –.29*
Conscientiousness –.07 .00 –.18 –.09 –.14 .18 –.38*
Competence –.25*
Order –.25* –.31*
Dutifulness .26* –.32*
Achievement striving –.34*
Self-discipline –.32*
Deliberation –.27*
*p < .05, two-tailed.

Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55 © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore 53

ness to feelings (r = .32), openness to values (r = .34), and The strongest positive correlations for Agreeableness were
correlated negatively with straightforwardness (r = –.26) with ratings for gospel (r = .26) and soundtracks (r = .26),
and self-discipline (r = –.24). Ratings for electronic music and the strongest negative correlation was with ratings for
did not correlate significantly with any of the five traits or electronic music (r = –.16). Finally, the strongest positive
30 facets, and ratings for soul/funk correlated with one of correlation for Conscientiousness was with ratings for pop
the Extraversion facets (warmth, r = .22), one of the Agree- (r = .22) and the strongest negative correlation was with
ableness facets (altruism, r = .22), with Openness (r = .22) ratings for funk (r = –.38)
and with two of the openness facets (openness to esthetics,
r = .22, and openness to actions, r = .22).
What about the seven music preference categories Rent- Grades
frow and Gosling added, which we were able to include for
the last 64 of the 83 students who participated in this study? Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) asked participants to rate
Table 8 includes the correlations between preference scores themselves on a variety of dimensions, one of which was
for each of these genres and scores on the Big Five traits “intelligent.” For a subsample of their participants, they
and the 30 facets. Four of these seven factor onto the re- administered the 50-item Wonderlic IQ test to assess verbal
flective and complex dimension: bluegrass, opera, interna- and analytic reasoning ability. They found that those who
tional, and oldies. As was the case for those who preferred preferred reflective and complex music, and those who pre-
three of the original four genres in this music domain ferred intense and rebellious music, saw themselves as
(blues, folk, and jazz, but not classical), we found that those more intelligent, and they scored higher on the verbal sec-
who preferred these four music genres scored high on tion of the Wonderlic (but not the analytical section). In
Openness (and at least one of the openness facets). Most contrast, those who preferred upbeat and conventional mu-
striking in Table 8 are the data for international music. Rat- sic saw themselves as less intelligent in one of their two
ings for this genre correlated negatively with Neuroticism studies (r = –.05, p < .05) but not in the other study (r =
(r = –.28), and with three of the Neuroticism facets (angry –.02, ns), and they scored lower on the verbal section of
hostility, r = –.28; depression, r = –.26; and vulnerability, the Wonderlic (r = –.18) but not the analytical section (r =
r = –.25). They also correlated positively with assertiveness .02).
(r = 26). Also noteworthy in Table 8 is that ratings for opera We included no self-assessment of intelligence, nor did
correlated negatively with order (r = –.27) and ratings for we administer a test of cognitive abilities, but we did look
oldies correlated negatively with compliance (r = –.29). at the students’ final grades in the course in which they
One of the added genres, punk, factored onto the second were enrolled when they participated in this research.
dimension, intense and rebellious. Ratings for punk corre- When we looked at the correlations between scores on mu-
lated positively with Openness (r = .34) and two of the sical preferences and their grades (one student withdrew
Openness facets (openness to fantasy, r = .26, and openness from the class and received no grade), we found no corre-
to values, r = .28). lations that reached statistical significance, but some sug-
Preferences for gospel music factored onto the upbeat gestive patterns: ratings for rock correlated with high
and conventional dimension. Ratings for gospel music cor- grades (r = .19, n = 82, p < .09) as did ratings for punk (r
related positively with the trait of Agreeableness (r = .26) = .19, n = 63, p < .13), and ratings for country correlated
and the Conscientiousness facet of dutifulness (r = .26). negatively with grades (r = –.15, n = 82, p < .17), as did
Finally, ratings for funk, which factored onto the ener- ratings for rap/hip-hop (r = –.14, n = 82, p < .20).
getic and rhythmic dimension, correlated positively with
Openness (r = .38) and with all of the Openness facets ex-
cept for openness to ideas, and negatively with Conscien-
tiousness (r = –.38) and five of the six Conscientiousness Discussion and Conclusion
facets.
When we step back from all of these findings and ask These findings support Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003)
which students with which music preferences are likely to general contention that music preferences are related to
be the least and most Neurotic, we see that the most nega- personality patterns. They suggest, however, that a more
tive correlations with Neuroticism were with ratings for nuanced look at both the facets that make up the Big Five
international music (r = –28 and (surprisingly) heavy metal traits, and the music genres that make up the four music
(r = –.19). The most positive correlations with Neuroticism preference dimensions identified by Rentfrow and Gosling
were with ratings for country (r = .08) and soundtracks (r (2003) enhance understanding of those personality pat-
= .11). The highest positive correlation for Extraversion terns.
was with ratings for rap/hip-hop (r = .28) and most negative Both their findings and these more current findings in-
correlation was with ratings for heavy metal (r = –.08). The dicate that some music domains, and some music genres,
most positive correlation with Openness was with ratings are much more likely to reveal personality than others. Ta-
for international music (r = .43), and the most negative cor- ble 4 shows, for example, that how the respondents felt
relations were with ratings for religious music (r = –.36). about folk music, one of the reflective and complex genres,

© 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55
54 R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore

correlated significantly with nine traits and facets, but how cant correlations, between ratings of on these genres and
they felt about classical music, also in the reflexive and scores on impulsiveness, self-discipline, or dutifulness.
complex domain, did not correlate with any of the Big Five Moreover, as we have noted, ratings for rock and punk cor-
traits or 30 facets. Similarly, as can be seen in Table 7, how related positively with grades, not exactly indicating rebel-
they felt about rap/hip-hop, which falls in the energetic and lious academic behavior. It is likely that with the passage
rhythmic domain, correlated with 11 traits or facets, but of time, rock and punk have become less rebellious and
how they felt about electronic music, which is in the same more mainstream. In fact, rock music was the highest rated
domain, did not correlate with any. of all 21 genres in this study, with a mean of 5.65, well
Along the same lines, some traits and facets are more above the average rating for the genres, which was 4.20.
likely to be revealed, especially Openness and the six facets The label for intense and rebellious may need to be
subsumed by it. More than 60% of the significant correla- changed.
tions between ratings of music genres and scores on traits As we have noted, the trait of Openness is the most ro-
and facets were for the trait of Openness and the six open- bust of the five traits, and ratings for religious music cor-
ness facets (57 of 93, or 61%); Conscientiousness was a related negatively with Openness (r = –.36); the strongest
distant second (14 of 93, or 15%), followed by Neuroticism correlation was with the facet that assesses openness to val-
(10 of 93, or 11%), Extraversion (7 of 93, or 8%), and, ues (r = –.54, the strongest of all the correlations reported
finally, Agreeableness (5 of 93, or 5%). in Tables 4–8). This facet is defined in the manual in the
Our focused look at the 21 music genres raises some following way: “Openness to Values means the readiness
intriguing issues. For example, as has been noted, in his to reexamine social, political, and religious values” (Costa
research on metalheads, Arnett (1996), using a version of and McCrae, 1992, p. 17). North Carolina is in what is often
Zuckerman, Eysenck, and Eysenck’s (1978) scale, found called “the Bible belt,” and those who are religious in North
that his participants were especially high sensation seekers. Carolina – at least, those in this study who indicate they
Not only did they score higher on this paper and pencil like religious music – appear not only to be sure of their
scale than a control group of nonmetalheads, but in his in- faith, but not prone to question their views or seek out al-
terviews with metalheads Arnett found that they were es- ternative perspectives. Interestingly, they are also unlikely
pecially likely to have engaged in a variety of risky behav- to be open to their feelings (r = –.27), defined in the manual
iors in the previous few months (for example, to have driv- as “receptivity to one’s own inner feelings and emotions
en over 80 mph, to have engaged in unprotected sex, and and the evaluation of emotion as an important part of life”
to have used illegal drugs). In this study, however, we found (Costa & McCrae, 1992, p. 17). When we performed a re-
only a moderate, and nonsignificant, correlation with the gression analysis using preferences for religious music as
NEO-PI scale that measures the facet of excitement seeking the dependent variable, and age, gender, and race as the
(r = .12, p < .26). This suggests that there may be a mean- independent variables, the regression was highly signifi-
ingful distinction between those who prefer certain kinds cant (F = 6.39, df = 3, p < .001), with age as the key pre-
of music, as assessed by the STOMP, and those who iden- dictive variable (r = .41, p < .001) and race as the second
tify strongly with that kind of music, as perhaps is revealed most predictive variable (r = .19, p < .06); gender did not
by someone calling himself or herself a metalhead or a predict at all (r = .02, ns). The older respondents and the
deadhead. Arnett notes that there is an important distinction students of color in our sample were more likely to prefer
between what he calls a “taste culture” and a “subculture” religious music than the younger respondents and the white
(1996, p. 179), and it appears that the STOMP assesses the respondents. Religious music was also one of the two low-
former but not the latter. est ranked genres (M = 3.54); only heavy metal had a lower
So, too, was it surprising that those who indicated pref- average rating by the students in this study (M = 3.08).
erences for the other three intense and rebellious music Interestingly, the same pattern did not hold for gospel mu-
genres – rock and alternative from the original 14 genres, sic, which falls on the same upbeat and conventional di-
and punk from the subsequently added genres – did not mension (the correlation between preferences for gospel
come across as either intense or rebellious. They tended to music and the Openness trait was positive, not negative, r
be open (ratings for alternate and punk correlated positively = .08, and gospel music had a higher average rating, M =
with at least one of the Openness facets), but none, for ex- 3.95).
ample, showed any tendency toward angry hostility (in In their development of the STOMP and a larger theory
fact, the correlations were negative: for rock, r = –.19, p < of music preferences, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003, 2006)
.08, for alternative, r = –.05, and for punk, r = –.20, p < have helped guide the way toward understanding the role
.11) and very little toward excitement seeking (here the cor- of music in people’s lives, and the relationship between
relations were positive: r = .07, r = .14, r = .01), two per- music preferences and personality. The four music dimen-
sonality characteristics one might associate with intensity sions they established in their 2003 study provide a broad
or rebelliousness. In addition, although one might expect look at some of the relationships between music preferenc-
ratings on these genres to correlate negatively with scores es and personality, but the findings in this study indicate
on the traits of Agreeableness and Conscientiousness, they that it is important to examine specific music genres as well
did not. There were no consistent patterns, and no signifi- as broad music dimensions. These findings also suggest

Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55 © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
R.L. Zweigenhaft: A Do Re Mi Encore 55

that the relationship between music preferences and per- Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). NEO-PI-R professional man-
sonality may vary for different groups based on such fac- ual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
tors as geography (those who prefer country music in Texas Gillespie, W., & Myors, B. (2000). Personality of rock musicians.
may differ from those who prefer country music in North Psychology of Music, 28, 154–165.
Carolina) and age. Moreover, the data in this study raise McCown, W., & Dulaney, W. (1999). The Grateful Dead experi-
ence: A factor analytic study of the personalities of people who
some intriguing questions for future research. Why, for ex-
identify with the Grateful Dead. In Robert G. Weiner (Ed.),
ample, was there a negative relationship between the rat-
Perspectives on the Grateful Dead (pp. 113–118). Westport,
ings for opera and the Conscientiousness facet that assesses CT: Greenwood Press.
order? Why was there not the expected relationship be- Rentfrow, P.J., & Gosling, S.D. (2003). The do re mi’s of everyday
tween ratings for rock music (a genre that falls on the ques- life: The structure and personality correlates of music prefer-
tionably labeled intense and rebellious dimension) and an- ences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
gry hostility, and why was there a positive correlation be- 1236–1256.
tween ratings on rock and grades? Perhaps future research Rentfrow, P.J., & Gosling, S.D. (2006). Message in a ballad: The
that combines the qualitative methods used in the earlier role of music preferences in interpersonal perception. Psycho-
research on metalheads (e.g., Arnett, 1996) and deadheads logical Science, 17, 236–242.
(e.g., Adams and Sardiello, 2000) with the quantitative Shuter, D. (2000). Profiling music students: Personality and reli-
benefits provided by the STOMP can answer such ques- giosity. Psychology of Music, 28, 190–196.
tions. Stremikis, B.A. (2002). The personal characteristics and environ-
mental circumstances of successful women musicians. Cre-
ativity Research Journal, 14, 85–92.
Weinstein, D. (1991). Heavy metal: A cultural sociology. New
York: Lexington.
References Zuckerman, M., Eysenck, S. & Eysenck, H.J. (1978). Sensation-
seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural, age, and sex
Adams, R.G. and Sardiello, R. (Eds.). (2000). Deadhead social comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
science: “You ain’t gonna learn what you don’t want to know.” 46, 139–149.
Walnut Creek, CA: Alta Mira Press.
Arnett, J.J. (1996). Metalheads: Heavy metal music and adoles-
cent alienation. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Barron, F. (1968). Creativity and personal freedom. Princeton, Richard Zweigenhaft
NJ: Van Nostrand.
Barron, F. (1969). Creative person and creative process. New Guilford College
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. 5800 W. Friendly Ave.
Bourke, R., & Francis, L.J. (2000). Personality and religion Greensboro, NC 27410
among music students. Pastoral Psychology, 48, 437–444. USA
Ciuffardi, L., & Noemi, V. (2000). Personality features associated Tel. +1 336 316-2269
with the performance of certain musical instruments. Interdis- Fax +1 336 316-2467
ciplinaria, 17, 1–20. E-mail rzweigen@guilford.edu

© 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Individual Differences 2008; Vol. 29(1):45–55

You might also like