You are on page 1of 9

International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology (2005) 69, 771—779

www.elsevier.com/locate/ijporl

Speech recognition abilities in noise for


children with severe-to-profound unilateral
hearing impairment
Melissa N. Ruscetta a,*, Ellis M. Arjmand b, Sheila R. Pratt a

a
University of Pittsburgh, 4033 Forbes Tower, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA
b
Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, 3333 Burnet Avenue, Cincinnati, OH 45229, USA

Received 29 June 2004; received in revised form 3 January 2005; accepted 11 January 2005

KEYWORDS Summary
Speech recognition
abilities; Objective: The purpose of this research was to compare the signal-to-noise ratio
Unilateral hearing required for equal performance for children (aged 6—14 years) with normal hearing
impairment; (N = 17) versus those with severe-to-profound unilateral hearing impairment (N = 20)
Binaural listening who can be at a disadvantage, particularly when sounds are presented to the impaired
ear. The listening environment is designed to approximate that which is encountered
in a typical classroom.
Methods: All signals (Hearing in Noise Test-Children and Nonsense Syllable Test) were
presented in the sound field from various azimuths with continuous noise presented
from all quadrants. The children were required to repeat twenty items, from each
test, in each listening condition. The intensity of the presentations was varied based
on the accuracy of previous items. Average signal-to-noise ratios are compared
between and within groups.
Results: (1) In most listening conditions, significantly greater signal-to-noise ratios
were needed by those with unilateral hearing impairment than those with normal
hearing, on both speech tests. (2) In every listening condition, both groups required
significantly greater signal-to-noise ratios to perform equally well on the Nonsense
Syllable Test as on the Hearing-In-Noise Test-Children. (3) For the Hearing-In-Noise
Test-Children, children with normal hearing required significantly greater signal-to-
noise ratios when facing the signal than when the signal was presented to a normally
hearing ear (monaural direct). (4) On both tests, the children with unilateral hearing
impairment required significantly greater signal-to-noise ratios when facing the signal
than in the monaural direct condition. (5) On both tests, the children with unilateral

* Corresponding author. Present address: Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh, ENT-ADM 1st floor — MT, 3705 Fifth Avenue, Pittsburgh,
PA 15213, USA. Tel.: +1 412 692 8032/383 6540; fax: +1 412 692 6074.
E-mail addresses: melissa.ruscetta@chp.edu (M.N. Ruscetta), ellis.arjmand@cchmc.org (E.M. Arjmand), spratt@pitt.edu
(S.R. Pratt).

0165-5876/$ — see front matter # 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2005.01.010
772 M.N. Ruscetta et al.

hearing impairment required significantly greater signal-to-noise ratios when the


signal was presented to the impaired ear (monaural indirect) than when facing the
signal.
Conclusions: (1) The children with unilateral hearing impairment require a more
advantageous listening condition to perform equally as well as their normally hearing
counterparts. (2) All of the children benefited from signals delivered in a monaural
direct condition. (3) The children with unilateral hearing impairment performed best
in the monaural direct condition or when facing the signal at zero degrees. (4)
Significantly greater signal-to-noise ratios were needed for both groups when
restricted contextual cues were available versus when sentential cues were available.
# 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction ous difficult listening situations that could possibly


compromise the normal development of language
There is ample empirical evidence available to and auditory perceptual skills [3,4,7,8,47]. Studies
demonstrate that binaural listening is superior have reported that children with UHI exhibit delays
to monaural listening [5,8,20,35,41,44]. Those in educational achievement [3,4,7,8,12,46], com-
binaural factors that contribute to a listening advan- munication problems [7,8], and behavioral difficul-
tage include; binaural summation [8,28,40,45], the ties [8,47].
head shadow effect [8,20,38,44,48], localization Boyd (1974) examined the effects of hearing
[26,27,29,35,37,41,49], and binaural release from impairment on educational performance and noted
masking [8,13,21,23,29]. Research conducted pri- that many children with UHI demonstrated lags in
marily with adults suggests that people with only academic achievement including reading problems,
one normally hearing ear experience a variety of spelling problems, and problems with arithmetic.
listening difficulties, including difficulty in under- Sarff [43] indicated that many school-age children
standing speech under quiet and noisy conditions with high frequency UHI (>2000 Hz) demonstrated
regardless of the location of the sound source deficits in educational performance. Similarly, an
[8,19,20,44]. Giolas and Wark (1967) also noted that unpublished report by Simon (1977) showed that
the participants in their study reported feelings of 32.6% of a sample of children whose impaired ear
embarrassment, annoyance, confusion, and help- average was greater than 80 dB, lagged behind
lessness. expected performance by as much as 1—3.5 years.
Although adults are affected by unilateral hear- The difficulties were in reading comprehension
ing loss, the potential impact on the development of or reading vocabulary and auditory memory
auditory and auditory-dependant skills in infants problems.
and children is substantial. Infants acquire compe- The affect on academic skills also is reflected by
tency in their central auditory processing skills an increased risk of academic failure. In a survey of
and language and speech recognition abilities over 60 children with UHI, Bess (1986) found that 35%
time. Investigations have demonstrated systematic failed at least one grade in school, and an addi-
improvements in performance as children mature to tional 13% required resource assistance. Further-
adolescence [24]. Anatomical studies demonstrate more, the children with UHI, who failed a grade
that the human auditory cortex continues to also, exhibited significantly lower verbal IQs than
develop up until adolescence [33]. These findings those children with UHI who did not fail a grade.
may partially explain age-related improvement in This study also showed that children with severe-
speech recognition. to-profound UHI exhibited significantly lower full
Given the listening problems encountered by scale IQs than those with more mild losses. Tea-
adults with unilateral hearing impairment (UHI), it chers consistently rated children with UHI as having
is not unreasonable to suspect that children with UHI greater difficulty in peer relations, less social con-
also can encounter similar problems, particularly in fidence, a greater likelihood of acting out or exhi-
school where a myriad of competing sounds are pre- biting withdrawn behavior in the classroom,
sent. Such listening problems may interfere with or greater frustration and an increased dependence
preclude the normal development of communicative on the teacher, and more frequent distractions.
skills that are essential to the child’s learning. Oyler et al. [39] produced similar results. Of 38
Although there is little clinical or experimental children with UHI, they found that 23.5% had
data on the communicative skills of children with repeated at least one grade and, of 54 children
UHI, there appears to be sufficient evidence to with UHI, 40.7% had received some sort of special
suggest that these children are subjected to numer- services.
Speech-in-noise for children with severe-to-profound UHI 773

Little research regarding rehabilitation/treat- investigation. One group included seventeen children
ment of UHI has been completed, however, a hand- who had normal hearing in both ears and who func-
ful of investigations on the performance of children tioned as a control group. The second group included
with UHI using CROS (contralateral routing of signal) twenty children with UHI (defined here as a pure tone
hearing aids are available [32,34,44]. In general, the average threshold (PTA) of 70 dB HL) in one ear, and
findings of these reports suggest that users of the NH (defined here as thresholds of 20 dB HL at all
CROS technology show improvement in the ability of frequencies tested) in the opposite ear. Standard
children with UHI to hear and understand lower audiometric procedures were used to obtain air con-
intensity sounds and speech. A popular form of duction thresholds from 250 to 8000 Hz and bone
rehabilitation used today is the FM system, which conduction between 500 and 4000 Hz [1].
affords a better signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and Children with a previous diagnosis of develop-
allows the child to better focus on the primary mental delay who exhibited substantive problems
signal. However, no empirical studies of perfor- with receptive vocabulary or single word recogni-
mance with this technology among children with tion were excluded from participation. The Peabody
UHI are available. Picture Vocabulary Test-III (PPVT-III) [14] was used
The specific aim of the present investigation is to to assess receptive vocabulary. The PPVT-III allows
compare the SNRs for equal speech recognition the tester to determine the equivalent age range at
performance for children with normal hearing which the child is performing. Age appropriate
(NH) and for those with severe-to-profound UHI. receptive vocabulary was required for participation
The speech materials, Hearing in Noise Test-Chil- (i.e., chronological age must fall within perfor-
dren (HINT-C) [18], and the Nonsense Syllable Test mance age range). Word recognition scores were
(NST) [15] were chosen because they allowed for assessed with the Northwestern University-Chil-
examination of performance with and without con- dren’s Perception of Speech Test (NU-CHIPS) [16].
textual cues. Equal performance at lower SNRs was A score of 95% or better was required for participa-
expected on the sentence recognition task (HINT-C) tion. Finally, normal middle ear function as indi-
than on the NST because of the increased linguistic cated by tympanometry [2], was demonstrated at
and speech acoustic contextual cues available to the each visit.
listener. Moreover, the listening environment also The age range of 6—14 years was selected for
was arranged in order to simulate listening from a several reasons. Historically, children with UHI have
variety of angles of incidence to the signal. not been identified until the early elementary
For people with normal bilateral hearing, turning school years. This is likely due to compensation
one ear towards the signal (monaural direct [MD] techniques that are employed by this group using
condition) allows easier listening and better speech the normally hearing ear to their benefit. A recent
discrimination in noise than when listening while examination of 50 children with UHI, not acquired
facing the signal at zero degrees orientation [8]. This through trauma or infection from the Hearing Cen-
finding was expected in the present investigation ter at Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh, indicated an
with children. Also, it was anticipated that children average age of diagnosis of 5 years, 2 months with a
with severe-to-profound UHI would perform better median age of 5 years. In the future, universal
when in the MD condition than when they listened newborn hearing screening will allow the average
while facing the signal directly. Finally, the present age of identification to continue to be lowered.
investigation allowed examination of the impact of Therefore, it was believed that children as young
having subjects with severe-to-profound UHI listen in as 6 years of age could be identified for participation
a monaural indirect (MI) condition (where the in the present investigation. Also, children as young
impaired ear of the individual faces the signal). as 6 years can reliably perform the required tasks
Performance in this condition was expected to be proposed herein.
poorer than when the subject listened while facing The upper age limit was decided upon because;
the signal directly at zero degrees (zero degrees some speech recognition abilities in noisy listening
condition) or when in the MD condition. conditions appear to be adult-like between 13 and
15 years of age [25]. It is likely that participants
aged 14 years and younger, would have similarly
2. Methods immature speech recognition abilities and may be
more susceptible to the affects of hearing loss and
2.1. Subjects noise and therefore make up the population of
interest for this investigation. Interesting future
Two groups of both male and female school-aged research might aim at replicating this experiment
children (6—14 years) participated in the current with children with UHI, over the age of 14 years,
774 M.N. Ruscetta et al.

but because of speech recognition maturation previous sentence. If a sentence is repeated exactly,
issues, this older group must be examined sepa- then the signal is decreased by 2 dB; if repeated
rately. incorrectly, the signal is increased by 2 dB. After the
The number of subjects selected for the experi- complete list is administered, an average SNR is
ment resulted in an estimated power of .79 or obtained for the last 7 items (10 item list) or the
greater when calculated with an alpha of .05 and last 17 items (20 item list) and can be charted and
a moderate effect size of .25. This effect size was compared to normal performance. This procedure
reasonable given the variability and the size of the (using two, 10 sentence-lists from the available 13)
effects observed in previous studies with adults and was followed in this investigation, however, the
children. average SNRs were compared between groups, con-
ditions, and tests rather than to performance
2.2. Protocol norms. For the NST, ordinarily a list of 25 nonsense
syllables is delivered at some suprathreshold level, a
All members of each group received the same correct or incorrect response is recorded for each
experimental testing. The test environment was item, and a percent correct is obtained. For this
constructed so that all target sounds were pre- study, the first 20 items from one of the six available
sented through an audiometer (GSI-16) and pre- lists on form A were delivered in the same fashion as
sented in the sound field. Multi-talker babble for the HINT-C, and an average SNR was obtained for
(Auditec of St. Louis) was continuously presented each condition.
at 65 dB (peak SPL) as measured at the location of The order of presentation of the test materials
the ear with a sound level meter. The noise was (sentences [HINT-C] versus nonsense syllables
routed through a Crown D-75A amplifier with loud- [NST]) was randomized, and the order of the three
speakers positioned at 458, 1358, 2258, and 3158 test conditions was randomized for each type of
azimuths. The tasks (described below) were com- stimuli.
pleted in each of the following listening conditions
relative to the signal loudspeaker: 2.4. Data analyses

1. The child faced the signal loudspeaker at zero Following, are results of t-test comparisons of (1)
degrees orientation with the signal presented in the average SNR of the NH subjects versus the
noise. subjects with UHI with each speech material in
2. The child’s right ear faced the signal loudspeaker each condition; (2) The average SNR needed for
and the signal was presented in noise. the NST versus the HINT-C for each group, in each
3. The child’s left ear faced the signal loudspeaker condition; and (3) the average SNR required in
and the signal was presented in noise. different conditions for both groups, using both
speech materials.
For each child, the head was not stabilized phy-
sically but was monitored visually to ensure that
head orientation did not shift during testing. The 3. Results
task involved repeating the target sentences and
nonsense syllables. A comparison of the data obtained from the right
and left ears of the control subjects revealed no
2.3. Speech materials significant differences. Therefore, only the right-
ear data for the control subjects was used for
The speech materials included the Hearing in Noise analyses.
Test-Children (HINT-C) [18], and the Nonsense Syl-
lable Test (NST) [15]. Both tests are appropriate for 3.1. Children with severe-to-profound UHI
the target age group. A study using the NST revealed compared to the control group
that children between the ages of 6—13 years
peformed similarly to an adult population [6]. The Significant differences are noted in the SNR needed
HINT-C was developed to be used with children as to achieve equal performance comparing children
young as 6 years [18]. These tests were not used in with NH and those with UHI in almost all test
their traditional sense. Ordinarily, a complete list of conditions, on both the NST and the HINT-C. The
ten or twenty sentences from the HINT-C is pre- children with UHI required more advantageous lis-
sented with the noise presented continuously at tening conditions (or greater SNR) to perform as
65 dB SPL. The level at which each sentence is well as children with NH. Fig. 1 is a graphic repre-
presented is dependant upon performance on the sentation of the average amount of SNR advantage
Speech-in-noise for children with severe-to-profound UHI 775

Fig. 1 Average SNR advantage needed for the group with severe-to-profound UHI group to perform equally as well as the
control group.

needed by the group with UHI to perform equally as 3.2. Comparison of the HINT-C versus
well as the control group. These numbers are col- the NST
lapsed across tests. Table 1 shows the average SNRs
needed for 50% speech recognition performance by In every listening condition, children with NH and
both groups on both tests in each listening condi- those with UHI needed significantly greater SNRs to
tion. The differences were significant ( p  .05) in perform equally as well on the NST when compared
every listening condition on both tests except when to the HINT-C. Fig. 2 is a graphic representation of
the children completed the NST while sitting in the the average amount of SNR advantage needed.
MD condition. These numbers are collapsed across all listening

Table 1 Average SNRs needed for equal performance in each listening condition
NST HINT-C
Zero Monaural Monaural Zero Monaural Monaural
degrees direct indirect degrees direct indirect
condition condition condition condition condition condition
(dB SNR) (dB SNR) (dB SNR) (dB SNR)
Control group 2.06 .89 N/A 1.84 4.23 N/A
(used .89 dB SNR) (used 4.23 dB SNR)
Severe-to-profound 6.18 3.42 9.84 dB SNR .39 2.02 3.44 dB SNR
UHI group
Difference score 4.12 2.53 8.95 dB SNR 2.23 2.21 7.67 dB SNR
(amount of
advantage needed
by S/P UHI group)
p-Value .011* .084 .000* .007* .021* .000*
*
Indicates a significant difference where p  0.5.
776 M.N. Ruscetta et al.

Fig. 3 Average SNR advantage needed for control group


Fig. 2 Average SNR advantage needed to perform to perform equally as well in the zero degrees condition as
equally well on NST as on HINT-C. in the MD condition.

conditions. The required advantages for the control pared to in the MD condition. The needed advantage
group ranged from 3.90 dB SNR in the MD condition ranged from 2.41 dB SNR on the HINT-C ( p = .000) to
to 5.12 dB SNR in the zero degrees condition. The 2.76 dB SNR on the NST ( p = .023). This group also
members of the severe-to-profound UHI group required significantly more advantageous listening
required average advantages ranging from 5.44 dB conditions to perform equally well in the MI condi-
SNR in the MD condition to 6.40 dB SNR in the tion as compared to the zero degrees condition. The
MI condition. These differences were significant magnitude of the needed advantage ranged from
( p  .05) in all listening conditions for both groups. 3.05 dB SNR on the HINT-C ( p = .000) to 3.66 dB SNR
on the NST ( p = .021). Fig. 4 is a graphic representa-
3.3. Children with NH seated at different tion of the average amount of SNR advantage
orientations to the signal needed collapsed across tests.

For children with NH, significantly greater SNRs were


needed to perform equally well on the HINT-C in the
zero degrees condition than in the MD condition, with 4. Discussion
an average difference of 2.39 dB SNR ( p = .014).
However, while this pattern held true for the NST, The superiority of binaural over monaural hearing in
the average difference between SNR needed was not adverse listening conditions is well documented for
significant at the p  .05 level ( p = .287). Fig. 3 is a individuals with normal hearing [5,8,20,35,41,44].
graphic representation of the average amount of SNR Factors potentially contributing to a binaural advan-
advantage needed collapsed across tests. tage are (1) binaural summation, (2) the head
shadow effect, (3) localization abilities, and (4)
3.4. Children with severe-to-profound UHI binaural release from masking. Research conducted
seated at different orientations to the signal with adults shows that people with only one nor-
mally hearing ear experience a variety of listening
For children with UHI, significantly more advanta- difficulties. Other studies have reported that
geous listening conditions were needed to perform children with unilateral sensorineural hearing loss
equally well in the zero degrees condition as com- exhibit delays in educational achievement [3,4,7,
Speech-in-noise for children with severe-to-profound UHI 777

that for those with normal hearing (91%). This find-


ing was supported by Boney and Bess (1984) who also
noted that as the listening condition worsened, the
word recognition abilities of both those with mini-
mal hearing impairment and those with normal
hearing worsened. Hart [22] noted that children
with UHI exhibit a significant deterioration in speech
understanding under actual classroom noise and
reverberation conditions. Also, McCartney [30]
reported that subjects with UHI experienced little
difficulty understanding in quiet but they had trou-
ble when speech was presented in the MI condition.
Design and procedural problems with the two later
investigations included small sample sizes, inability
to control the test environment, and failure to
specify calibration procedures.
Subsequent studies attempted to address these
design problems. Bess et al. [9] found that the
children with UHI performed more poorly than their
normally hearing counterparts in both localization
and speech recognition of nonsense syllables. These
children also had considerable difficulty under-
standing in a background of noise. The results of a
study by Bovo et al. [11] produced similar results,
showing that children with profound or complete
Fig. 4 Average SNR advantage needed for severe-to- UHI had difficulty in speech discrimination in
profound UHI group to perform equally as well in more adverse listening conditions as well as difficulty with
detrimental listening conditions as in more advantageous sound localization. In the current study, children
listening conditions. with UHI required greater SNR than children with
normal hearing to perform equally as well on tests of
speech recognition-in-noise. This was true even in
8,47], communication problems [7,8], and beha- the most advantageous listening condition. This
vioral difficulties [8,41]. general finding was not unexpected and is consistent
A detrimental SNR is a substantial hurdle facing with previous research. Some of the tests situations
children with UHI. Several studies have illustrated (i.e., MI condition) were such that the children with
this problem. Nober and Nober (1975) reported that UHI were at a disadvantage because of their inability
the average noise intensity in four elementary class- to benefit from a positive head shadow effect.
rooms was 65 dBA. McCroskey and Devens [31] It is interesting to note that, children with UHI did
examined 78 occupied classrooms and found an not require significantly greater SNR to perform as
average noise level between 40 and 50 dBA. Finally, well as children with normal hearing in the MD
Bess et al. (1984) examined 19 classrooms and found condition on the NST. Also, greater SNRs were not
a median noise level of 41 dBA in unoccupied class- needed for children with normal hearing to perform
rooms and 56 dBA in those that were occupied. equally well on the NST in the zero degrees condi-
Additionally, classrooms have been found to be far tion versus in the MD condition. One explanation for
too reverberant with the usual range equaling 0.5— this might be that the restricted contextual cues
1.2 s with the typical classroom having an SNR of available on the NST made the task so difficult that
+6 dB and a reverberation time of 0.8 s [8]. the children were unable to use top down processing
Several sources have demonstrated the debilitat- or take advantage of environmental cues (i.e., NH
ing effects of noise and reverberation on the speech ear facing the signal) that might normally have
understanding of both children with normal hearing aided performance.
and those with UHI [8,10,17,18,36]. In work by Ross The advantage provided by context was consis-
and Giolas [42], it was found that the mean word tent with the differences found between the NST
recognition scores, obtained under usual classroom and the HINT-C. Significantly greater SNRs were
conditions, were significantly lower for listeners needed when restricted speech acoustic and pho-
with minimal hearing impairment (32%) and those nologic cues were available to the listener than
with hearing impairment who were aided (20%) than when sentential cues also were available. This
778 M.N. Ruscetta et al.

was true for both groups of children when seated in such cases, education and counseling may be
all orientations to the signal. required.
When comparing results, within subjects, as a Research into the SNR required for equal perfor-
function of orientation to the signal, all of the mance on the NST and HINT-C by children with
children performed best in the MD condition. This varying degrees and configurations of UHI, in various
was expected because, as stated previously, it is noise conditions, is ongoing. Future research should
well known that turning one normally hearing ear include an assessment of performance on speech-in-
towards the signal allows easier listening and better noise tasks with personal and/or classroom assistive
speech discrimination in noise than when listening listening devices.
while facing the signal directly [8]. It also was
anticipated and confirmed in the present investiga-
tion that children with severe-to-profound UHI
Acknowledgements
would perform more poorly when they listened in
the MI condition than when they listened in the zero
degrees or the MD conditions. This study was supported in part through funding
received from the Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh,
Eberly Family Endowed Chair in Pediatric Otolary-
5. Conclusions/future research nogology at the Hearing Center at Children’s Hospi-
tal of Pittsburgh.
This study revealed that children with UHI require Special thanks to Diane Sabo and Mona El-Kady for
more advantageous listening conditions to perform their efforts in the development of this project.
equally as well as their normally hearing counter- Special thanks to Audra Webber and Jennifer Horn
parts on tests of speech recognition performance- for their efforts collecting and organizing the data.
in-noise. Both children with normal hearing and Special thanks to Elaine Rubenstein for her efforts in
children with UHI benefit from signals being deliv- statistical analyses.
ered directly to a normal hearing ear when listen-
ing in noise. Furthermore, children with UHI
perform best when signals are delivered either References
directly to a normally hearing ear or from zero
degrees azimuth. [1] Acoustics–—audiometric test methods, Part 1, Basic Pure
These results suggest that, for classrooms con- Tone Air and Bone Conduction Threshold Audiometry,
taining children with UHI, placement in a preferred American National Standards Institute (ANSI), New York,
seat may provide some benefit if the positioning 1989, p. S3.6.
[2] American Speech-Language-Hearing Association guidelines
of the teacher and child are relatively static. It for screening infants and children for outer and middle ear
may reduce the distance between the child and disorders, birth through 18 years, in: Guidelines for Audio-
the teacher and allow a better SNR in some class- logic Screening, Rockville, MD, 1997, pp. 15—22.
rooms. However, as evidenced by the data herein, [3] F.H. Bess, Children with UHI, J. Acad. Rehabil. Audiol. 15
(1982) 206—216.
even in the best listening conditions, children
[4] F.H. Bess, The minimally hearing-impaired child, Ear Hear. 6
with UHI require better signal-to-noise ratio than (1985) 43—47.
do children with normal hearing to perform [5] F.H. Bess, The unilaterally hearing-impaired child: a final
equally well, suggesting that preferential seating comment, Ear Hear. 7 (1986) 52—54.
alone may not provide enough benefit for these [6] F.H. Bess, A.M. Gibler, Syllable recognition skills of unilat-
children. erally hearing-impaired children, Paper presented at the
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Conven-
Sound field amplification systems may be a bet- tion, Los Angeles, California, 1981.
ter option and would likely benefit all children in [7] F.H. Bess, A. Tharpe, Unilateral hearing impairment in
classrooms. Children with normal hearing would children, Pediatrics 74 (1984) 206—216.
benefit if loudspeakers were placed facing either [8] F.H. Bess, A.M. Tharpe, An introduction to unilateral sensor-
ineural hearing loss in children, Ear Hear. 7 (1986) 3—13.
of their ears. Children with UHI would benefit if
[9] F.H. Bess, A. Tharpe, A. Gibler, Auditory performance of
seated such that their normally hearing ear were children with unilateral sensorineural hearing loss, Ear Hear.
facing a loudspeaker. This environment also would 7 (1986) 20—26.
improve the SNR for most individual listeners. Per- [10] S. Boney, F.H. Bess, Noise and reverberation effects on
sonal assistive listening devices, such as FM sys- speech recognition in children with minimal hearing loss,
tems, also would improve the SNR for the individual Presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Asso-
ciation, San Francisco, California, 1984.
listener although it is recognized that some chil- [11] R. Bovo, A. Martini, M. Agnoletto, A. Beghi, D. Carmignoto,
dren are resistant to wearing them, while some M. Milani, A. Zangaglia, Auditory and Academic Performance
teachers are reluctant to wear the microphones. In of Children with UHI, Scand. Audiol. Suppl. 30 (1988) 71—74.
Speech-in-noise for children with severe-to-profound UHI 779

[12] S.F. Boyd, Hearing loss: its educationally measurable effects imposed monaural hearing loss and normally hearing,
on achievement, Master’s degree research requirement, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Washing-
Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Illinois, 1974. ton, Seattle, Washington, 1974.
[13] R. Carhart, T.W. Tillman, P.J. Dallos, Unmasking for pure [31] F.L. McCroskey, J.S. Devens, Acoustic characteristics of
tones and spondees: interaural phase and time disparities, J. public school classrooms constructed between 1890 and
Speech Hear. Res. 11 (1968) 722—734. 1960, in: Proceedings of Technical Program, NOISEXPO,
[14] L. Dunn, L. Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Manual, Atlanta, Georgia, 1975.
American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN, 1997. [32] A.L. Miller, Body type hearing aids for unilateral hearing
[15] B. Edgerton, J.L. Danhauer, Clinical Implication of Speech losses, J. Speech Hear. Disord. 32 (1967) 268—269.
Discrimination Testing Using Nonsense Stimuli, University [33] B.C. Moore, D.A. Vickers, B.R. Glasburg, T. Baer, Comparison
Park Press, Baltimore, MD, 1979. of real and simulated hearing impairment in subjects with
[16] E.L. Elliot, D.R. Katz, Northern University Children’s Percep- unilateral and bilateral cochlear hearing loss, Br. J. Audiol.
tion of Speech Test (NU_CHIPS), AUDITEC of St. Louis, St. 31 (1997) 227—245.
Louis, MO, 1980. [34] M.R. Navarro, D.O. Vogelson, An objective assessment of a
[17] T. Finitzo-Hieber, T. Tillman, Room acoustics effects on CROS hearing aid, Arch. Otolaryngol. 100 (1974) 58—59.
monosyllable word discrimination ability for normal and [35] V.E. Newton, Sound localization in children with a severe
hearing-impaired children, J. Speech Hear. Res. 21 (1978) unilateral hearing loss, Audiology 22 (1983) 189—198.
440—458. [36] L.W. Nober, E.H. Nober, Auditory discrimination of learning
[18] D. Gelnett, A. Sumida, M. Nilsson, S. Soli, Development of disabled children in quiet and classroom noise, J. Learn.
the hearing in noise test for children (HINT-C), Presented at Disabil. 8 (1975) 656—773.
the American Academy of Audiology Annual Convention, [37] B. Nordlund, Directional audiometry, Acta Otolaryngol. 51
Dallas, Texas, April 1995. (1964) 1—18.
[19] T.G. Giolas, D.J. Wark, Communication problems associated [38] W.O. Olsen, The effect of head movement on speech recep-
with UHI, J. Speech Hear. Disord. 32 (1967) 336—343. tion under monaural and binaural listening conditions, ASHA
[20] E. Harford, J. Barry, A rehabilitative approach to the pro- 7 (1965) 405.
blem of unilateral hearing impairment: the contralateral [39] R.F. Oyler, A.L. Oyler, N.D. Matkin, Warning: a unilateral
routing of signals (CROS), J. Speech Hear. Disord. 30 (1965) hearing loss may be detrimental to a child’s academic
121—138. career, Hear. J. 18 (1987) 18—22.
[21] J.D. Harris, Monaural and binarual speech intelligibility and [40] I. Pollack, Monaural and binaural threshold sensitivity for
the stereophonic effect on temporal cues, Laryngoscope 75 tones and white noise, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 20 (1948) 52—58.
(1965) 428—446. [41] U. Rosenhall, The influence of hearing loss on directional
[22] S. Hart, An investigation into some speech discrimination hearing, Scand. Audiol. 14 (1985) 187—189.
problems of children with UHI, Unpublished doctoral dis- [42] M. Ross, T.G. Giolas, Effect of three classroom listening
sertation, Manchester University, Manchester, England, conditions on speech intelligibility, Am. Ann. Deaf 116
1968. (1971) 580—584.
[23] I.J. Hirsh, The influence of interaural phase on interaural [43] L.S. Sarff, An innovative use of free field amplification in
summation and inhibition, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 20 (1948) regular classrooms, in: R. Roeser, M. Downs (Eds.), Auditory
536—544. Disorders in School Children, Thieme-Stratton Inc., New
[24] T. Hnath-Chisolm, E. Laipply, A. Boothroyd, Age-related York, 1981.
changes on a children’s test of sensory-level speech percep- [44] I. Shapiro, Children’s use of CROS hearing aids, Arch. Oto-
tion capacity, J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 41 (1998) 94—106. laryngol. 103 (1977) 712—716.
[25] C. Johnson, Children’s phoneme identification in reverbera- [45] W.A. Shaw, E.B. Newman, I.J. Hirsh, The difference between
tion and noise, J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43 (2000) 144—157. monaural and binaural thresholds, J. Exceptional Psychol.
[26] L. Jongkees, R. van der Veer, Directional hearing capacity in 37 (1947) 229—242.
hearing disorders, Acta Otolaryngol. 48 (1957) 465—474. [46] B. Simon, Unilateral hearing impairment and the question of
[27] L. Jongkees, R. van der Veer, On directional sound localiza- learning disability, Unpublished manuscript, Paper pre-
tion in unilateral deafness and its explanation, Acta Otolar- sented at the Illinois Speech and Hearing Association, Chi-
yngol. 49 (1958) 119—131. cago, Illinois, 1977.
[28] J.W. Keys, Binarual versus monaural hearing, J. Acoust. Soc. [47] D. Stein, Psychosocial characteristics of school age children
Am. 19 (1947) 629—631. with UHI, J. Acad. Rehabil. Audiol. 16 (1983) 12—22.
[29] A. Markides, Advantages of binaural over monaural hearing, [48] T.W. Tillman, R.N. Kastner, J.S. Horner, Effect of head
in: Binaural Hearing Aids, Academic Press, London, 1977. shadow on reception of speech, ASHA 5 (1963) 778—779.
[30] J. McCartney, A comparison of speech discrimination scales [49] F. Tonning, Auditory localization and its clinical applications,
among children with true monaural hearing loss, artificially- Audiology 14 (1975) 368.

You might also like