You are on page 1of 59

Accepted Manuscript

Full Length Article

Comparing Two Roads to Success: Self-Control Predicts Achievement and


Positive Affect Predicts Relationships

Incheol Choi, Sarah Lim, Rhia Catapano, Jongan Choi

PII: S0092-6566(18)30134-X
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.07.001
Reference: YJRPE 3728

To appear in: Journal of Research in Personality

Received Date: 28 November 2017


Revised Date: 29 June 2018
Accepted Date: 2 July 2018

Please cite this article as: Choi, I., Lim, S., Catapano, R., Choi, J., Comparing Two Roads to Success: Self-Control
Predicts Achievement and Positive Affect Predicts Relationships, Journal of Research in Personality (2018), doi:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.07.001

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers
we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and
review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process
errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 1

Comparing Two Roads to Success:

Self-Control Predicts Achievement and Positive Affect Predicts Relationships.

Incheol Choi

Seoul National University

Sarah Lim

Cornell University

Rhia Catapano

Stanford University

Jongan Choi

Seoul National University

Author Note

Incheol Choi, Department of Psychology, Seoul National University, Seoul, Korea; Sarah

Lim, Department of Marketing, Cornell University, NY, USA; Rhia Catapano, Department of

Marketing, Stanford University, CA, USA; Jongan Choi, Center for Happiness Studies, Seoul

National University, Seoul, Korea

The datasets of all studies are available at on online supplement. All the studies were not

preregistered. I. Choi, S. Lim, and J. Choi developed the study conceptualization. S. Lim and J.

Choi were responsible for the data collection and preparation. I. Choi, S. Lim, and J. Choi

conducted the data analysis. All four authors contributed to the report writing. This research was

funded by a grant from the Center for Happiness Studies Seoul National University.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 2

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jongan Choi, Center for

Happiness Studies, Seoul National University, Gwanak-gu, Gwanak-ro 1, Bldg. 220, Seoul,

08826, South Korea; Phone: +82-2-880-6399; E-mail: therock2@snu.ac.kr


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 3

Abstract

Previous work suggests that two key predictors of success across a variety of domains are self-

control and emotional well-being (EWB). In the current project, we explore the relative

contributions of these two factors to success in two domains: academic and professional

achievement, and interpersonal relationships. Across five studies (N = 1130, 51.06% female), we

find that although both are important to success, self-control is more predictive of success in

achievement-related domains, while EWB is more predictive of success in relationship-related

domains. This finding holds for college students (Studies 1 and 5), middle-school students

(Study 2), East Asian adults (Study 3), and North American adults (Study 4), suggesting that it is

cross-cultural and robust across age and measurement.

Keywords: self-control, positive affect, emotional well-being, success, interpersonal

relationship, academic achievement, professional achievement


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 4

Comparing Two Roads to Success:

Self-Control Predicts Achievement and Positive Affect Predicts Relationships.

Self-control and positive affect are arguably two of the most compelling predictors of

success in life. Numerous studies have repeatedly shown that both lead to positive outcomes

across an impressively wide range of domains (for reviews, see de Ridder, Lensvelt-Mulders,

Finkenauer, Stok, & Baumeister, 2012; Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005). Indeed, there is a

great deal of overlap in the realms where the two traits predict positive outcomes: academic

achievement (for self-control, Duckworth & Seligman, 2005; for positive affect, Gilman &

Huebner, 2006), work (for self-control, Converse, Piccone, & Tocci, 2014; for positive affect,

Boehm & Lyubomirsky, 2008), wealth (for self-control, Moffitt et al., 2011; for positive affect,

Marks & Fleming, 1999), health (for self-control, Tice & Baumeister, 1997; for positive affect,

Cohen & Pressman, 2006), and interpersonal relationships (for self-control, Tangney,

Baumeister, & Boone, 2004; for positive affect, Diener & Seligman, 2002). Hence, there appear

to be two roads to success: self-control and positive affect.

Yet, there seem to exist dilemmas in which exerting self-control and pursuing positive

affect are at odds with each other. Indeed, the various definitions of self-control contain a

possible conflict with pursuing the desire to feel good in the moment. The famous Marshmallow

experiment (Mischel & Ebbesen, 1970) is a good example. Should we eat the marshmallow right

away for instant gratification or delay eating it for a long-term goal? Dilemmas such as this occur

most often in domains such as eating, sleeping, and working, all of which are highly crucial to

happiness (Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster, & Vohs, 2012; Hofmann & Van Dillen, 2012). Eating

can be one of the happiest moments in people’s daily lives, not only in Western cultures, but also

in East Asian cultures (Choi, Catapano, & Choi, 2017; Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). In
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 5

contrast, working can oftentimes constitute the unhappiest moments in both cultures (Choi,

Catapano, & Choi, 2017; Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). Not having enough sleep also

substantially affects positive emotion (Dement & Vaughan, 1999; Gujar, Yoo, Hu, & Walker,

2011; Talbot, McGlinchey, Kaplan, Dahl, & Harvey, 2010). Thus, should we follow the advice

of the self-control literature and resist the temptation to eat, sleep, and pursue leisure? Or should

we follow the advice of positive psychology research and enjoy the emotional benefits of eating,

sleeping, and engaging in leisure?

Despite both academic and practical implications of comparing the relative importance

of self-control and positive affect, few studies have integrated the two lines of research by

examining both traits simultaneously to compare their relative importance in achieving success

(cf. Hofmann, Luhmann, Fisher, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2013; Wiese, et al., 2017). This surprising

lack of integration may be attributable to a long-standing trend to view positive affect as an

outcome, rather than a cause. Previous research has asked, ‘What brings positive affect?’ instead

of ‘What does positive affect bring?’ However, Lyubomirsky et al.’s (2005) systematic review,

along with Fredrickson’s broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2001, 2004), has led to a

critical shift in thought by showing that positive affect is not only a consequence, but also an

antecedent of positive life outcomes. If positive affect is not only a result, but also a cause of

success, it makes sense to compare its causal power across domains with that of self-control,

sometimes championed as one of the most reliable predictors of success in life (Baumeister &

Exline, 1999; Baumeister & Tierney, 2011).

Achievement vs. Interpersonal Relationships

In the current paper, we argue and demonstrate that the extent to which self-control and

positive affect contribute to success may differ across life domains; specifically, self-control may
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 6

be a better predictor of achievement than positive affect, while positive affect may be a better

predictor of interpersonal relationships than self-control. Two lines of research give rise to this

prediction.

First, a recent debate regarding the ‘optimal’ level of things that are considered as

positive (Grant & Schwartz, 2011; Pierce & Aguinis, 2013) has begun to examine how excessive

self-control or positive affect may each have side effects, leading to negative outcomes under

certain circumstances (Ford & Mauss, 2014; Gruber, Mauss, & Tamir, 2011; Mauss et al., 2012).

This body of research reveals that excessive levels of self-control and positive affect lead to less

success in systematically different outcome domains than do moderate levels. On the one hand,

growing evidence indicates that individuals with too much self-control may suffer from

interpersonal costs (Koval, van Dellen, Fitzsimons, & Ranby, 2015; Letzring, Block, & Funder,

2005). For example, people with excessive self-control, compared to those with moderate self-

control, are less willing to sacrifice in interpersonal relationships, and therefore are more likely

to experience difficulty in building and maintaining close relationships (Righetti, Finkenauer, &

Finkel, 2013). On the other hand, Oishi, Diener, and Lucas (2007) found an inverted U-shaped

relationship between happiness (life satisfaction, more specifically) and salary. Although there

remains the possibility of reverse causality (Cheung, Hill, & Jackson, 2017), this result suggests

that too much happiness may not be as beneficial to achievement as a moderate level of

happiness. In addition, other studies have shown that excessively high levels of positive affect

are often unrelated to, or even detrimental to, academic performance in college (Okun, Fairholme,

Karoly, Ruehlman, & Newton, 2006; Okun, Levy, Karoly, & Ruehlman, 2009). As a result, it is

evident that excessive levels of both self-control and positive affect do not mirror each other in

the domains of achievement and relationship, respectively. These findings suggest that the
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 7

relative associations between the two traits and outcomes may vary across different domains.

Specifically, self-control is more strongly associated with outcomes in the domain of

achievement than positive affect, whereas the reverse pattern may emerge in the domain of

interpersonal relationships.

Second, previous meta-analytic studies, which have individually investigated the effect

sizes of self-control and positive affect for various outcomes, provide suggestive evidence that

the beneficial effects of self-control and positive affect may vary across different domains. A

systematic meta-analysis by de Ridder et al. (2012) shows that the average effect size of self-

control for work and school performance (r = .36) was relatively larger than the effect size of

self-control for interpersonal functioning (r = .25). This difference suggests that self-control may

play a more critical role in success for achievement than for interpersonal relationships.

Meanwhile, we find the reverse pattern for positive affect effect sizes. In order to calculate the

effect sizes of positive affect for achievement and relationship outcomes, we conducted random-

effect meta-analyses with subsets of studies in the meta-analysis by Lybomirsky et al. (2005); the

models for achievement and relationship outcomes included 41 and 51 out of 338 studies,

respectively. The results revealed that the average effect size of positive affect for relationship

outcomes (r = .31) was larger than that of positive affect for achievement outcomes (r = .25),

suggesting that positive affect may play a greater role in interpersonal relationships than in

achievement. Although the effect size differences obtained from previous meta-analytic studies

were not very large, they support our speculation that the relative contributions of self-control

and positive affect to positive outcomes may vary as a function of life domains.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 8

Despite such suggestive evidence, it still remains to be empirically tested as to which of

the two is more important, and in which domains. To the best of our knowledge, the current

research is among the first to do so.

Overview

Although previous work has suggested that both positive affect and self-control are

predictive of success in various domains, prior literature sheds little light on the relative strengths

of these effects, and how they varies by domain. The current research is the first to fill this gap

by systematically comparing the relative importance of self-control and positive affect in

predicting success across these domains and measuring the two simultaneously. We predict that

self-control is a stronger predictor of success in the achievement domain than is positive affect,

while positive affect is a stronger predictor of success in the relationship domain than is self-

control. As both Lyubormisky et al. (2005) and Fredrickson (2001, 2004) adopted emotional

well-being (EWB; balance between positive and negative emotions) to demonstrate the critical

role of EWB in predicting positive life outcomes, we used EWB in order to examine the role of

positive affect in these domains.

We conducted five studies to examine our prediction that self-control is a more

important predictor of success in the achievement domain than positive affect, while the reverse

is true in the relationship domain. Study 1 compared the relative contributions of the two traits in

the academic and interpersonal domains with a sample of college students. Study 2 extended

Study 1 in two ways: 1) it examined the results with middle-school students; and 2) it used

informant-reported outcome variables, as well as self-reported ones. Studies 3 and 4 extended

these findings to non-student samples, in both an East Asian culture (Study 3) and a North
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 9

American culture (Study 4). Finally, Study 5 employed a longitudinal design to establish

temporal precedence for our hypothesized relations.

Study 1

Study 1 examined the relative relationships of both self-control and EWB with academic

achievement and interpersonal relationships among college students. Each domain included both

subjective (i.e., academic and relationship satisfaction) and objective (i.e., GPA and number of

close friends) indices of success. Note that since GPA and the number of close friends were

proxies for objective consequences, rather than subjective perceptions or attitudes although they

were self-reported, these proxies were considered as objective indicators.

Method

Participants. One hundred and ten undergraduate students (39 male, 71 female; Mage =

22.59, SDage = 1.64) at a large university in Korea completed the survey in exchange for

monetary compensation (approximately US $30).

Previous meta-analyses examining the effects of self-control (de Ridder et al., 2012) and

those of positive affect (Lyubormisky et al., 2005) on achievement and relationships have shown

medium effect sizes (i.e., the average correlation coefficients range from .25 to .36). On the basis

of these meta-analyses, we estimated that the two traits would exhibit effect sizes of

approximately r = 0.30. Given this estimated effect size, with an alpha level of 0.05, and a power

level of 0.80, the power analysis revealed that a minimum sample size would be 82 participants.

Throughout all of the studies, we recruited more than 100 participants; our samples in Study 1 (N

= 110), Study 2 (N = 358), Study 3 (N = 176), Study 4, (N = 264), and Study 5 (N = 222) were

sufficiently sensitive to detect effect sizes equivalent to r = 0.26, 0.15, 0.21, 0.17, and 0.19,

respectively. It is noteworthy that although we determined the sample size by using a power
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS

10

analysis, the sample size of Study 1 might still be a bit small because the effect sizes obtained

from previous meta-analyses were likely overestimated due to publication bias. However, the

sample sizes of the other studies (Study 2 – 5) were large enough to achieve adequate power with

more conservative effect sizes.

Measures.

Emotional Well-Being (EWB). Emotional well-being was measured with the Positive

and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS is a 20-

item scale designed to measure an individual’s level of positive affect (PA) and negative affect

(NA). This scale includes ten PA items (e.g., “interested”, “excited”, “strong”; α = .88) and ten

NA items (e.g., “distressed”, “upset”, “nervous”; α = .86). Participants rated how frequently they

experienced 20 affective states in the past four weeks on 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(very rarely) to 5 (very often). Emotional well-being was calculated by subtracting the mean of

the NA scores from the mean of the PA scores.

Trait self-control (TSC). The Trait Self-Control Scale (TSCS; Tangney et al., 2004) is a

well-validated scale to assess dispositional self-control. Participants indicated the extent to which

36 items (e.g., “I am good at resisting temptation”, “I have a hard time breaking bad habits”

(reverse coded), “I keep everything neat”; α = .91) described themselves on 5-point Likert scales

(1 = not at all; 5 = very much). The TSCS consists of five factors – self-discipline,

deliberate/nonimpulsive action, healthy habits, work ethic, and reliability. We further examined

the relationship between these multiple factors and outcomes and found no consistent results.

Thus, we used only the overall averages of the TSCS in the following analyses.

Domain satisfaction. As a subjective indicator of success in each domain, participants

rated their satisfaction with their own academic performance (“How much are you satisfied with
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS

11

your academic performance?”) and interpersonal relationships (“How much are you satisfied

with your interpersonal relationships?”) on 7-point Likert scales (1 = completely dissatisfied; 7 =

completely satisfied).

Grade Point Average (GPA). Participants reported their GPA for the last semester.

Number of close friends. Participants reported the number of friends whom they know

very well and trust.

Analytic Strategy.

In order to examine the relative importance of self-control and EWB in success

throughout different domains, we used multiple statistical tools: bivariate correlation, multiple

regression, and dominance analysis. Historically, multiple regression has most commonly been

used to determine the relative importance of various predictors. However, standardized

coefficients from multiple regression models fail to partition variance into different predictors in

the presence of an intercorrelation between the predictors (Darlington, 1968). Moreover, a

number of studies provide empirical evidence that standardized coefficients are not appropriate

statistical indices to represent relative importance (Johnson, 2000; LeBreton, Ployhart, & Ladd,

2004, Tonidandel & LeBreton, 2011). Hence, we used dominance analyses (Budescu, 1993) as a

supplement to our bivariate correlation and multiple regression analyses. Dominance analysis

(DA) is a method for determining relative importance among a set of related predictors. It fully

partitions the total variance explained and allows us to directly examine a predictor’s

contribution by producing estimates of importance, while addressing potential problems caused

by an intercorrelation of predictors. DA is particularly well-suited for investigating the relative

importance of variables in the case that a priori ordering of predictors is not theoretically or

empirically determined (Johnson & LeBreton, 2004), as in the case of self-control and EWB. In
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS

12

the current research, dominance analyses were conducted to compare the relative importance of

self-control and EWB with regard to their ability to account for the variance in outcomes across

life domains. We were interested in the overall importance of the predictors, although dominance

analysis allows us to examine different types of dominance (e.g., complete dominance and

conditional dominance; Azen & Budescu, 2003). In the following analyses, only general

dominance was examined, which represents the variance of a predictor accounted for by itself,

and in combination with another predictor. As such, if the average amount of variance explained

by a predictor is greater than that of the variance explained by another, the predictor is said to be

dominant over the other predictor.

It is noteworthy that bivariate correlation, multiple regression, and DA provide different

types of information that are mutually complementary (LeBreton, Hargis, Griepentrog, Oswald,

& Ployhart, 2007). Accordingly, we can gain a more complete understanding of the roles of self-

control and EWB in predicting outcomes across life domains by leveraging multiple analytic

approaches simultaneously.

Results

A correlation analysis revealed that both self-control and EWB were significantly related

to positive outcomes in each domain, corroborating previous research that both are reliable

predictors of success (see Supplemental Table 1 for descriptive statistics and zero-order

correlations of all variables).

A series of hierarchical regressions were conducted to examine the unique contributions

of self-control and EWB to outcomes in academics and interpersonal relationships, after

controlling for each other. In each regression model, demographic variables (i.e., gender and age)
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS

13

were entered at step 1, then self-control and EWB were simultaneously entered at step 2 (see

Table 1). 1

In the academic domain, self-control was positively associated with both academic

satisfaction (β = .41, p < .001) and GPA (β = .25, p = .026), while EWB was not significantly

associated with these variables (for academic satisfaction, β = .16, p = .108; for GPA, β = .05, p

= .643). Meanwhile, EWB was significantly associated with relationship satisfaction (β = .46, p

< .001) and showed a marginal trend with the number of close friends (β = .21, p = .062). Yet,

the association between self-control and these variables was not significant (for relationship

satisfaction, β = .17, p = .088; for the number of close friends, β = .17, p = .130). In short, self-

control outperformed EWB in predicting academic success, whereas EWB outperformed self-

control in the domain of interpersonal relationships. 2

In our dominance analysis, we estimated the general dominance of self-control and

EWB for outcomes after controlling for demographic variables (i.e., age and gender).

Specifically, the general dominance coefficients were computed by averaging the squared

semipartial correlations across all possible subset regressions; with 4 predictors (self-control,

EWB, age, and gender), there were 4 regressions of only one predictor, 6 regressions of two

different combinations of predictors, 4 regressions of three different combinations of predictors,

and 1 regression of all predictors, for a total of 15 regressions. General dominance coefficients

represent the average incremental variance of self-control and EWB across the regression

models. The relative weight coefficients indicate the proportion of variance explained by self-

control and EWB in relation to the total amount of variance explained. Table 2 presents the

results of the dominance analysis. Results demonstrated that self-control, as compared to EWB,

accounted for more explained variance in academic satisfaction (69% vs. 30%) and GPA (61%
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 14

vs. 18%), whereas EWB accounted for more explained variance in relationship satisfaction (70%

vs. 30%) and the number of close friends (54% vs. 44%) than did self-control.

In summary, both the regression and dominance analyses yielded converging results to

support the notion that self-control (vs. EWB) is relatively more associated with success in the

domain of academic achievement, while EWB (vs. self-control) is relatively more associated

with success in the domain of interpersonal relationships.

Discussion

Study 1 found that academic satisfaction and GPA are more strongly associated with

self-control than with EWB, whereas relationship satisfaction and the number of close friends

were more strongly associated with EWB than with self-control. This pattern provides initial

evidence that the relative contribution of self-control and EWB to success varies as a function of

domains. This pattern holds for both subjective and objective indices of success in each domain.

Although Study 1 provides initial evidence regarding the relative importance of self-

control and EWB in success, Study 1 was conducted only with college undergraduates and with a

limited number of outcome variables. Furthermore, although GPA was treated as an objective

index of achievement, we relied on self-reported GPA scores. Therefore, in Study 2, we

addressed these issues by testing whether the same pattern is applicable to a non-college student

sample and to a wider range of outcome variables.

Study 2

Study 2 was designed to extend Study 1 in two ways. First, Study 2 attempted to

replicate the results of Study 1 in a different population, adolescents. Second, we measured

participants’ school and social success with informant-reported measures, as well as self-reported

and objective measures. Previous research has posited that sometimes informant evaluations
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 15

provide more valid information than self-evaluations (e.g., Martin-Storey, Serbin, Stack,

Ledingham & Schwartzman, 2012). Hence, Study 2 added caregiver-reported measures to

examine the relative importance of self-control and EWB in a more comprehensive way, and

used test scores from the participants’ school, rather than using self-reports.

Method

Participants. Three hundred and fifty-eight middle school students in Korea (175 male,

183 female; Mage = 14.38, SDage = .72) participated in the survey. One hundred and eighty-four of

the students were in their first year of middle school, while the rest were in their second year.

Students’ caregivers (306 female, 51 male, 1 missing; Mage = 44.38, SDage = 5.88) offered

informant reports of the school and the social functioning of the students. The majority of

informants were students’ mothers (83.5%), while the remaining informants were fathers (14%)

or grandmothers (2.5%). Questionnaires were directly sent home and completed separately by

students and their caregivers.

Measures.

Self-control and EWB. The Trait Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al., 2004) was adjusted

to assess the dispositional self-control of adolescents. Some items were excluded because they

were inappropriate for adolescents (e.g., I sometimes drink or use drugs to excess). A total of

twelve items were used in the study (α = .75). As in Study 1, EWB was measured with the

PANAS (for PA, α = .84; for NA, α = .87).

School and social functioning. Subscales of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory 4.0

(PedsQL 4.0; Varni, Seid, & Kurtin, 2001) were adjusted to assess young students’ school and

social functioning. The PedsQL is composed of two identical questionnaires: a student self-

report and a caregiver report. The school functioning subscale consists of five items measuring
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 16

students’ general adjustment in school. In this study, we used two items that were relevant to

academic performance (e.g., “paying attention in class”; for the self-report, α = .78; for the

parent-report, α = .86). To assess participants’ relationships with their peers, five items of the

social functioning subscale were used (e.g., “getting along with other children”; for the self-

report, α = .88; for the parent-report, α = .88). For each item, both the participants and their

caregivers rated the frequency of their problems during the last semester using a 5-point Likert

scale (1 = never a problem; 5 = almost always a problem). In accordance with previous studies

(Upton et al., 2005; Varni, Seid, Knight, Uzark, & Szer, 2002), all items were reverse-coded and

linearly transformed to a zero-to-100 scale (i.e., 1 = 100, 2 = 75, 3 = 50, 4 = 25, 5 = 0), with

higher scores representing better functioning.

Test scores. Unlike Study 1, in which academic achievement (i.e., GPA) was self-

reported, test scores, a proxy for academic achievement, were obtained through their school. For

the first-year students, exam subjects included Korean, mathematics, English, science and social

studies. Second-year students took the same set of exams, excluding social studies.

Number of close friends. Participants reported the number of close friends, as in Study

1.

Results

Consistent with Study 1, self-control and EWB were significantly correlated with both

academic achievement and positive relationship outcomes (see Supplemental Table 2 for

descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations of all variables).

We began by conducting hierarchical regression analyses (see Table 3). Self- and

informant-reported school functioning scores were significantly related to self-control (for the

self-report, β = .30, p < .001; for the informant-report, β = .21, p < .001) and EWB (for the self-
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 17

report, β = .24, p < .001; for the informant-report, β = .14, p = .010). Furthermore, test scores

were significantly associated with self-control (β = .12, p = .030), but not EWB (β = .04, p =

.529). On the other hand, EWB was significantly associated with both self- and informant-

reported social functioning (for the self-report, β = .41, p < .001; for the informant-report, β =

.18, p = .002), while the relationship between self-control and social functioning was not

significant (for the self-report, β = .08, p = .132; for the informant-report, β = .07, p = .230). The

same pattern was observed for the number of close friends: EWB served as a significant

predictor of the number of friends (β = .20, p = .001), whereas self-control did not (β = .003, p =

.963).

We also performed dominance analysis as in Study 1, to compare the relative

importance of self-control and EWB (see Table 4). Results demonstrated that self-control

explained more of the variance in academic achievement, whereas EWB accounted for more of

the variance in predicting positive interpersonal outcomes. Specifically, self-control explained

52% of the variance in self-reported and 48% of the variance in informant-reported school

functioning, whereas EWB explained 41% and 31% of the self- and informant-reported school

functioning, respectively. For test scores, self-control accounted for 12% of the explained

variance, while EWB explained only 4% of the variance. In contrast, EWB (vs. self-control)

played a more pivotal role in explaining the variance in students’ social functioning (for the self-

report, 79% vs. 16%; for the informant-report, 66% vs. 24%) and the number of close friends

(45% vs. 5%).

In short, both the regression and dominance analyses suggest that the school

achievement of middle-school students is better explained by self-control, whereas the quality of

their interpersonal relationships is better explained by emotional well-being.


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 18

Discussion

Consistent with Study 1, Study 2 revealed that the importance of self-control and EWB

vary across domains. Study 2 found that school functioning and test scores were better explained

by self-control, whereas social functioning and the number of friends were better explained by

EWB. Notably, this pattern was consistent across three different types of measures: self-report,

informant-report, and objective outcome measures. In addition, by replicating the same pattern as

in Study 1 with young adolescents, Study 2 supports the notion that this pattern is not limited to

certain ages or groups (i.e., college students).

Study 3

In Studies 1 and 2, we found that in student populations, self-control was a better

predictor of academic achievement than was EWB, while EWB was a better predictor of

interpersonal success than was self-control. In addition, Study 2 revealed that these factors are

not only associated with success as perceived by the actor, but also as perceived by close others.

Study 3 aimed to extend these findings by using an adult sample in order to determine whether

the findings of Studies 1 and 2 can indeed hold among different age groups (i.e., middle-aged

adults), and across additional metrics for success in the relationship and achievement domains.

To this end, Study 3 assessed not only relationship satisfaction in general, but also the quality of

relationships with a participant’s spouse, family members, and friends. Furthermore, we

extended the research to another achievement domain that is more pertinent to adults,

occupational achievement, as indexed by factors such as income.

Method

Participants. Participants were one hundred and seventy-six Korean adults (104 male,

72 female), ranging in age from 30 to 59 years (Mage = 43.65, SDage= 8.29). Participants
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 19

completed a survey online in exchange for monetary compensation (approximately US $10). All

participants had a college degree (76.1%) or post-graduate degree (23.9%). Seventy-one percent

of the participants were employed in business, finance, and administration, 15.9% in education,

law and government service, 5.7% in management, 4% in sales and service, and 3.4% in

manufacturing and utilities.

Measures.

Self-control and EWB. As in Studies 1 and 2, trait self-control was measured with the

TSCS (α = .90), and EWB was assessed with the PANAS (for PA, α = .90; for NA, α = .91).

Burnout. Burnout, or psychological and emotional drain, was measured as a subjective

indicator of successful job functioning. Three items adjusted from the Maslach Burnout

Inventory (MBI; Maslach & Jackson, 1981) were used (e.g., “I feel burned out from my work”; α

= .86). Participants reported how much they agreed with each item on a 5-point Likert scale (1 =

not at all; 5 = very much).

Income. Participants’ monthly income was measured as a proxy for objective

occupational success. Due to its skewness, a log-transformed score of income was used for the

analysis.

Relationship satisfaction scores. Relationship satisfaction (“How satisfied are you with

your relationships with other people in general?”), marital satisfaction (“How satisfied are you

with your marriage?”), and family satisfaction (“How satisfied are you with your family?”) were

measured with one item each on 7-point scales (1 = completely dissatisfied; 7 = completely

satisfied).

Number of close friends. Participants reported the number of close friends whom they

know very well and trust.


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 20

Friendship intimacy. Participants rated the degree of their friendship intimacy (“How

much intimacy do you feel in your relationships with friends?”) on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not

at all; 7 = very much).

Results

A correlation analysis indicated that both self-control and EWB showed significant

relationships with the outcome variables (see Supplemental Table 3 for descriptive statistics and

zero-order correlations of all variables).

A series of hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted to identify the relative

contributions of self-control and EWB to positive outcomes (see Table 5). Although burnout was

significantly associated with both self-control (β = -.32, p < .001) and EWB (β = -.16, p = .05),

the coefficient of self-control was larger in magnitude than that of EWB. Additionally, income

was significantly related to self-control (β = .26, p = .001), but not to EWB (β = .05, p = .489). In

the interpersonal domain, EWB was significantly related to general relationship satisfaction (β =

.40, p < .001), family satisfaction (β = .53, p < .001), and marital satisfaction (β = .48, p < .001).

Self-control was also significantly related to all three variables (for general relationship

satisfaction, β = .20, p = .014; for family satisfaction, β = .18, p = .009; for marital satisfaction, β

= .16, p = .033); however, in general, the coefficients of self-control were smaller than those of

EWB. In addition, we created a composite index of social relationships (α = .82)by averaging

the standardized scores of the three variables and found that EWB (β = .55, p < .001) was more

strongly correlated with success in social relationships than self-control (β = .21, p = .002).

Furthermore, EWB was significantly associated with friendship intimacy (β = .38, p < .001) and

the number of close friends (β = .19, p = .034), whereas self-control was not (for friendship

intimacy, β = .01, p = .923; for the number of close friends, β = .14, p = .153).
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 21

A dominance analysis was also performed to compare the relative contributions of self-

control and EWB (see Table 6). In the work domain, self-control accounted for more of the

explained variance in occupational outcomes than did EWB (for burnout, 56% vs. 32%; for

income, 19% vs. 6%). In the domain of interpersonal relationships, however, EWB (vs. self-

control) accounted for more of the explained variance in relationship satisfaction (62% vs. 34%),

family satisfaction (61% vs. 25%), and marital satisfaction (56% vs. 22%). Likewise, EWB

accounted for more of the explained variance in friendship intimacy (79% vs. 15%) and the

number of close friends (49% vs. 34%) than did self-control. Taken together, these results

suggest that occupational success was better explained by self-control, whereas the quality of

social relationships was better explained by EWB.

Discussion

The results of Study 3 replicated those of Studies 1 and 2 with a different sample (i.e.,

middle-aged adults) and with measures of a different achievement domain (e.g., income). By

doing so, Study 3 provides additional evidence that self-control, as compared to EWB, is more

strongly related to achievement, whereas EWB is more significantly associated with

interpersonal relationships.

This finding builds on Studies 1 and 2 in a number of key ways. First, we find that

burnout is more related to self-control than to EWB. Given that burnout is heavily tied to

emotions, as it reflects the experience of negative emotions and emotional drain, one might

expect EWB to act as a better predictor of burnout. However, we find that self-control accounts

for more of the explained variance of burnout than EWB, which underscores the significant

impact of self-control in the occupational sphere. Second, Study 3 sheds further light on the

scope of EWB’s role in interpersonal relationships; EWB is not only important more generally in
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 22

interpersonal success, but also in specific and diverse relationships, including an individual’s

spouse, family, and friends.

Finally, it is worth noting that the difference in the relative importance of self-control

and EWB was more pronounced in the subjective indicators (e.g., relationship satisfaction) than

in the objective indicators (e.g., the number of friends) in both Studies 1 and 3. For instance, the

variance explained by EWB (vs. self-control) was greater for relationship satisfaction (for Study

1, 70% vs. 30%; for Study 3, 62% vs. 34%) than for the number of friends (for Study 1, 54% vs.

44%; for Study 3, 49% vs. 34%). This pattern suggests that the relative influences of EWB (vs.

self-control) on interpersonal outcomes (vs. achievement) may be more significant in terms of

one’s subjective perceptions or attitudes than for the objective consequences themselves.

Study 4

Study 4 had two major aims. First, it aimed to examine the cultural generality of our

findings. All participants in Studies 1–3 were recruited in Korea; therefore, it is an open question

as to whether this relationship can be generalized to other cultures, particularly Western cultures.

Therefore, in Study 4, we recruited participants from the United States to examine whether our

findings would be replicated among American participants.

In addition, we included another achievement indicator, occupational prestige (Nakao &

Treas, 1994), which represents the shared belief regarding the “social standing” of a profession

for a certain society and time period (Christ et al., 2012; Nakao & Treas, 1992). A high-prestige

job implies that it is not only highly paid, but also engenders admiration and respect within a

society. For example, doctors and teachers score higher in occupational prestige than do taxi

drivers and sales clerks. Previous work has found that occupational prestige scores are highly
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 23

correlated with other measures of socioeconomic status (SES), suggesting that occupational

prestige is an objective indicator of achievement (Nakao & Treas, 1994).

Method

Participants. Two hundred and sixty-four U.S residents (133 males, 131 female),

ranging in age from 20 to 65 (Mage = 34.46, SDage= 10.18) were recruited through Amazon’s

Mechanical Turk. They received monetary compensation (US $1) in exchange for their

participation. Participants included 10 African Americans, 216 Caucasians, 18 Asians, and 20

Others. A total of 69.7% of the participants had a college degree or higher degree, 21.2% had

some college credit without a degree, and 9.1% had a high school diploma. Participants had

occupations in business, finance, and administration (42.6%), education, law, and government

service (8%) management (9.5%), sales and service (18.9%), manufacturing and utilities (9.9%),

transportation and material moving (3.4%), health (2.7%), and natural resource, agriculture and

related production (2.3%); the remaining 2.7 % of participants did not report their occupation.

Measures.

Self-control and EWB. Trait self-control and EWB were assessed with the TSCS (α =

.92) and the PANAS (for PA, α = .92; for NA, α = .93), respectively.

Burn out. As in Study 3, three items from the MBI (α = .81) were used to assess

psychological and physical exhaustion resulting from their work.

Income. Participants self-reported their monthly gross income. Due to its skewness, we

log-transformed income and used the log-transformed score for analysis.

Occupational prestige. Occupational prestige was included as an indicator of

occupational achievement. Participants self-reported their occupation (“Please specify your

occupation”), which was used to generate an occupational prestige score based on Nakao and
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 24

Treas’ (1994) occupational prestige ratings. For example, a salesperson in a retail shop was

assigned an occupational prestige score of 29, whereas a lawyer in a law firm was assigned a

score of 75. The scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores representing higher prestige.

Relationship satisfactions. Using the same items as in Study 3, participants self-reported

their satisfaction with their relationships in general, their marital relationship, and their family

relationships.

Friendship intimacy. Participants reported the extent to which they feel intimacy with

their friends on a 7-point Likert scale.

Number of close friends. Participants self-reported the number of their close friends.

Results

Again, both self-control and EWB were significantly correlated with the outcome

variables, with the exception of the number of close friends, which was not significantly

correlated with self-control (see Supplemental Table 4 for descriptive statistics and zero-order

correlations of all variables).

We conducted a series of hierarchical multiple regressions to examine the relative

predictive power of self-control and EWB on occupational and interpersonal outcomes (see

Table 7). As in Study 3, self-control, compared to EWB, showed a stronger relationship with

outcomes in occupational domains. Specifically, although burnout showed a significant

relationship with both self-control (β = -.35, p < .001) and EWB (β = -.26, p < .001), the

magnitude of self-control’s coefficient was larger than that of EWB. In addition, occupational

prestige was positively related to self-control (β = .19, p = .015), but not to EWB (β = .02, p =

.820). Income was not significantly predicted by self-control (β = 11, p = .177) or EWB (β = .04,

p = .654). Conversely, EWB was more strongly related to outcomes in interpersonal domains
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 25

than was self-control. Both self-control (β = .15, p = .038) and EWB (β = .36, p < .001) were

significantly related to marital satisfaction; however, the coefficients of EWB were more than

twice the size of those related to self-control. General relationship satisfaction and family

satisfaction were associated with EWB (for general relationship satisfaction, β = .56, p < .001;

for family satisfaction, β = .45, p < .001), but not with self-control (for general relationship

satisfaction, β = .04, p = .571; for family satisfaction, β = .05, p = .459). As in Study 3, the index

of social relationships (α = .74) was computed. EWB (β = .56, p < .001) was significantly

predictive of success in relationships, but self-control (β = .10, p = .121) was not. Furthermore,

friendship intimacy had a positive relationship with EWB (β = .57, p < .001), whereas it had a

marginally significant trend in the opposite direction with EWB (β = -.12, p = .08). Neither self-

control nor EWB was predictive of the number of close friends (for self-control, β = -.02, p =

.852; for EWB, β = .13, p = .105).

The dominance analysis (see Table 8) shows that, as in Studies 1-3, self-control, as

compared to EWB, accounted for more of the explained variance in occupational outcomes: for

burnout, 51% vs. 41%; for income, 9% vs. 5%; for occupational prestige, 70% vs. 21%. On the

other hand, EWB explained more of the variance in interpersonal outcomes than did self-control:

for relational satisfaction, 72% vs. 21%; family satisfaction, 73% vs. 24%; for marital

satisfaction, 59% vs. 32%; for friendship intimacy, 81% vs. 12%; for the number of close

friends, 50% vs. 10%.

Discussion

In Study 4, we replicated, in general, our findings with a Western sample. As in Studies

1-3, outcomes in the occupational domain, such as burnout and occupational prestige, were more

strongly related to self-control than to EWB. On the other hand, outcomes in the interpersonal
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 26

domain, such as relationship/family/marital satisfaction and friendship intimacy, were more

strongly associated with EWB than self-control. In Study 4, we also find that self-control is a

better predictor of occupational prestige than is EWB, which suggests that self-control, rather

than positive affect, may be a more important predictor of obtaining socially desirable jobs. It is

worth noting that self-control and positive affect could not significantly predict the number of

friends or income, respectively, although their trends were consistent with the results in Study 3.

These discrepancies might be due to differences in the sampling methods (offline vs. on- line) or

in the sample characteristics, such as cultural background (Eastern Asian vs. Western), age, and

socioeconomic status. Whether this is the case remains to be tested in further work.

Study 5

Results obtained from multiple samples (college students, middle-school students,

Korean/American adults) and measures (self-reports, informant-reports, objective indices)

provided converging evidence that the relative importance of self-control and EWB in success

varies, according to domain. However, due to the cross-sectional design of the previous studies

in which self-control, EWB, and outcomes were measured simultaneously, it did not allow us to

examine the temporal precedence of self-control and EWB over outcomes across the domains.

Moreover, the relationship between the two traits and outcomes, particularly subjective

indicators of success (e.g., satisfaction with relationships and job burnout) might be inflated due

to common method variance (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). To empirically

address these limitations, in Study 5, we conducted a longitudinal study in which self-control and

EWB were assessed at an initial time point as predictor variables. Then, approximately 6 months

later, subsequent outcomes in academic and interpersonal relationship domains were measured.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 27

In Study 5, we examined the prospective associations of the two traits with outcomes in the

achievement and relationship domains, and we compared their magnitudes in each domain.

Method

Participants. Two hundred and eighty-five first-year students at a large university in

Korea completed the first questionnaires. Of these students, 222 completed the second

questionnaire3, and are included in the below analyses (102 male, 120 female; Mage = 19.10,

SDage = .69). They completed two surveys in the exchange of monetary compensation

(approximately US $40).

Procedure. Participants visited a laboratory and completed the first survey packet,

including the TSCS and the PANAS (along with other measures, which were irrelevant to this

project, and are therefore not reported) in April, early in the first semester (Time 1). In October,

during the middle of the second semester, they returned to the laboratory and completed the

second packet containing questionnaires assessing their success in the academic and

interpersonal relationship domains (Time 2). As in the first packet, several surveys were included

in the second packet for multiple research projects.

Measures.

Self-control and EWB. Trait self-control and EWB were measured in the same manner

as in previous studies. Participants completed the TSCS (α = .89) and the PANAS (for PA, α =

.84; for NA, α = .75) at Time 1.

Domain satisfaction. Using the same questions as in Study 1, participants reported their

subjective satisfaction with their academic and interpersonal relationships at Time 2.

Grade Point Average (GPA). Participants reported their GPA for their first semester at

Time 2.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 28

Number of new friends. Participants reported the number of new friends they made in

college at Time 2.

Results

First, we examined the correlation between self-control and EWB and the outcome

variables. Consistent with Studies 1-4, the results revealed that self-control and EWB were

positively correlated with the outcome variables; specifically, self-control at Time 1 was

associated with academic satisfaction, GPA, and interpersonal relationship satisfaction at Time 2,

while EWB was correlated with academic satisfaction, interpersonal relationship satisfaction and

the number of new friends (see Supplemental Table 5 for descriptive statistics and zero-order

correlations of all variables)

As in Studies 1-4, we generated multiple hierarchical multiple regression models to

examine the predictive power of self-control and EWB in academic and interpersonal outcomes,

after controlling for each other (see Table 9). Self-control was more predictive of academic

outcomes, using both subjective and objective indicators, than was EWB. That is, although

academic satisfaction was significantly predicted by self-control (β = .35, p < .001) and EWB (β

= .14, p = .035), the coefficients for self-control were more than twice as large as those of EWB.

Furthermore, self-control predicted GPA (β = .35, p < .001), but EWB did not (β = -.06, p =

.368). In addition, we found the same pattern, even after controlling for participants’ SAT scores,

which were collected at Time 1 (for self-control, β = .38, p < .001; for EWB, β = -.09, p = .752).

Meanwhile, the reverse pattern was observed for interpersonal outcomes. EWB prospectively

predicted relationship satisfaction (β = .39, p < .001) and the number of new friends (β = .15, p =

.033). However, self-control did not predict either interpersonal outcome (for relational

satisfaction, β = .08, p = .192; for the number of new friends, β = .04, p = .599).
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 29

The dominance analysis yielded converging evidence that the relative importance of

self-control varied by the outcome domain (see Table 10). For academic satisfaction and GPA,

self-control explained more of the variance in predicting positive outcomes than did EWB (for

satisfaction, 70 % vs. 20%; for GPA, 77% vs. 2%). In contrast, EWB explained more of the

variance in relationship outcomes, as compared to self-control. Specifically, EWB explained

83% of the variance in relationship satisfaction and 56% of the variance in the number of new

friends, while self-control explained 12% and 11% in relationship satisfaction and the number of

new friends, respectively.

Discussion

In Study 5, we employed a longitudinal design to explore the relationship between initial

self-control and EWB, and subsequent outcomes in the academic and relationship domains, and

we compared their predictive power. Consistent with Studies 1-4, self-control predicted

subsequent academic outcomes, such as academic satisfaction and GPA, to a greater extent than

did EWB. Conversely, EWB was more strongly related to subsequent relationship-related

outcomes, including future relationship satisfaction and the number of new friends than was self-

control.

As in Studies 1-4, we found that self-control was more strongly associated with success

in achievement, while EWB was more strongly associated with success in relationships, even

after minimizing common method variance. Furthermore, Study 5 provides initial evidence

supporting the temporal precedence of self-control and EWB over outcomes in life domains,

although it is difficult to establish casual directionality conclusively without experimental

evidence.

Meta-Analysis
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 30

We conducted further meta-analyses to explore the overall pattern across studies. The R

package metafor (Viechtbauer, 2010) was used to specify four random-effect models with self-

control as the predictor and achievement variables as the outcomes; self-control as the predictor

and relationship variables as the outcomes; EWB as the predictor and achievement variables as

the outcomes; and EWB as the predictor and relationship variables as the outcomes. We used

standardized betas as predictors in the meta-analyses (Kim, 2011; Nieminen, Lehtiniemi,

Vähäkangas, Huusko, & Rautio, 2013; Woolley & Fishbach, 2017). 4 Twelve regressions from

Studies 1-5 were included in the models for the achievement outcomes; 17 regressions were

included in the models for the relationship outcomes.

The results of the tests for heterogeneity revealed that there was significant or marginal

heterogeneity within the effects in the models (for the model of self-control and the achievement

outcomes, Q(11) = 20.88, p = .005; for the model of self-control and the relationship outcomes,

Q(16) = 24.85, p = .072; for the model of EWB and the achievement outcomes, Q(11) = 27.11, p

= .004; for the model of EWB and the relationship outcomes, Q(16) = 120.08, p < .001). This

pattern suggests that moderators likely influence the effect sizes of self-control and EWB on

success. Future research is necessary to understand which individual and contextual factors act as

moderators for these effects.

The effect of self-control on the achievement outcomes was significant after controlling

for EWB (β = .27, SE = .03, 95% CI = [0.21, 0.32], p < .001), as was the effect of EWB on

achievement after controlling for self-control (β = .11, SE = .03, 95% CI = [0.05, 0.16], p < .001).

Although the effect sizes of self-control and EWB on the achievement outcomes were significant,

the magnitude of the effect size for self-control was larger than that of EWB, and their 99% CIs

did not overlap (self-control 99% CI = [0.19, 0.34]; EWB 99% CI = [0.03, 0.18]). This finding
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 31

supports our account that self-control is a stronger predictor of achievement outcomes than EWB

(p < .01). For the relationship outcomes, the effects of both self-control and EWB were

significant after controlling for the other (for self-control, β = .07, SE = .02, 95% CI = [0.03,

0.11], p < .001; for EWB, β = .36, SE = .04, 95% CI = [0.29, 0.43], p < .001). Here we see a

reversal, such that for relationship outcomes, EWB had significantly greater predictive power

than did self-control (p < .01), as their 99% CIs did not overlap (self-control 99% CI = [0.02,

0.12]; EWB 99% CI = [0.27, 0.45]). These meta-analyses provide further evidence that the

relative importance of self-control and EWB in predicting success varies across life domains; our

data suggest that self-control plays a pivotal role in predicting success in the achievement

domain, while EWB plays a critical role in predicting success in the interpersonal relationship

domain.

General Discussion

The current research makes both conceptual and practical contributions regarding what it

means and what it takes to be successful. Achievement and relationships are arguably two of the

most important life domains. According to the current research, achieving success in these two

domains seems to require different combinations of self-control and EWB. Specifically, for

academic and occupational achievement, self-control seems to be more important than EWB,

which indicates that pursuing positive affect and neglecting self-control has the potential to limit

an individual’s ability to succeed in academics and in their occupation. This finding supports a

recent meta-analysis revealing that self-control is most strongly associated with success at work

and school rather than in other domains (de Ridder et al., 2012). As for interpersonal

relationships, however, the opposite was true. Both subjective and objective indices of

interpersonal success were more strongly associated with EWB than with self-control. This
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 32

contrast between self-control and EWB in achievement versus social relationships is in line with

Oishi et al. (2007)’s finding that higher happiness leads to better social relationships, but not

necessarily to better achievement. The current project builds on this work not only by exploring

the role of positive affect, but also by exploring its comparative strength with self-control across

these domains.

As readers might have noted, we did not include the interaction term in our analysis and

instead looked at the main effects of self-control and positive affect. However, one might argue

that self-control moderates the effect of positive affect, or vice versa. To address this issue, we

examined whether the levels of self-control and EWB serve as a moderator to one another across

our five studies. Our analysis suggests that self-control and positive affect do not have an

interactive effect on success; out of 29 models, only 4 reached significance, and there was no

consistent interaction effect across Studies 1-5. The lack of significant interaction effects

between self-control and EWB suggests that self-control and EWB have an additive effect on

success in these domains. In other words, although self-control and EWB have different levels of

contribution to achievement and relationships, individuals with high levels of both self-control

and EWB are more likely to achieve success in these domains than those with only high self-

control or high EWB. That is, success is best predicted by a combination of high self-control and

high EWB.5

It is also worth noting that the correlations between self-control and EWB were found to

be significant in all five studies (r = .52, .45, .54, .64, and .31), suggesting the possibility that

there may exist a positive loop between trait self-control and positive affect. Prior empirical

work also provides support for the positive loop hypothesis. For instance, participants in a

positive mood were less susceptible to ego depletion (Tice, Baumeister, Shmueli & Muraven,
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 33

2007) and temporal discounting (Pyone & Isen, 2011). Additionally, Hofmann et al. (2013)

found that trait self-control predicted greater experiences of positive emotions and fewer

experiences of negative emotions. These studies and the current research, taken together, suggest

the possibility of a virtuous cycle between self-control and positive emotions, such that positive

emotions are likely to boost one’s resources for self-control; in turn, an increased ability to

exercise self-control is likely to evoke positive emotions.

Although the current research expands our understanding of the beneficial roles of self-

control and positive affect by demonstrating that their effects vary across life domains, several

limitations should be considered for future research. First, since the current research is

correlational in nature, we are unable to make strong causal inferences. Although Study 5

adopted a longitudinal design and provided initial evidence in regard to establishing temporal

precedence between the two traits and outcomes while minimizing concerns about common

method variance, our longitudinal analyses are limited because different variables were measured

at each point; thus, we did not apply statistical modeling techniques to directly examine casual

directionality (e.g., the bivariate latent score change model). In addition, the time interval

between two points (6 months) might not be sufficient for psychological change to manifest

itself. Thus, it is important for future research to systematically examine the causal relationship

between self-control and positive affect, and outcomes in various life domains. In particular, a

longitudinal intervention design in which participants regularly engage in practicing activities to

enhance self-control or positive affect can provide experimental evidence for causality.

Next, our findings were repeatedly observed with different ages, ranging from young

adolescents and young adults to middle-aged adults. Nevertheless, it remains to be seen whether

the pattern holds across the lifespan, particularly among older adults. Aging theories, such as
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 34

socioemotional selective theory (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999) and strength and

vulnerability integration theory (Charles, 2010), propose that older adults, compared with

younger adults, are more likely to focus on emotionally meaningful goals and avoid negativity.

Age-related changes in goals and emotional experiences might qualify the positive roles of self-

control and positive affect across the life domains. Further work is necessary to systematically

examine the possible moderating effect of age.

Another important area for future research is the mechanism underlying these

differential effects of self-control and EWB on achievement versus relationship success. One

possible mechanism for this finding is the thinking-mode, which can be influenced by levels of

self-control and positive affect. Previous research suggests that people with high self-control are

likely to engage in deliberative, analytical, and systematic thinking (Fennis, Janssen, & Vohs,

2009; Janssen, Fennis, Pruyn, & Vohs, 2008; Pocheptsova, Amir, Dhar, & Baumeister, 2009),

while those with greater positive affect are more likely to engage in intuitive, holistic and

heuristic thinking (Isen, Johnson, Mertz, & Robinson, 1985; Isen & Means, 1983; Isen,

Niedenthal, & Cantor, 1992). Based on these findings, one possibility is that people with high

self-control may tend to rigidly adhere to rational rules in interacting others, which may appear

to be cold and calculating, and in turn, may be less likely to build positive social relationships

than those with high positive affect. Conversely, people with high levels of positive affect may

be inclined to make intuitive and heuristic decisions, even in academic and occupational contexts

where deliberative and critical thinking may improve performance; thus, they may not achieve as

much as those with high self-control. In future research, it would be interesting to directly test

this possibility.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 35

Our findings offer a clearer picture of the prerequisites for a successful life. While both

self-control and EWB are important to overall success, the two dominate in different domains.

Success is not as simple as strong self-control or high EWB. Rather, success is a nuanced

product of a number of factors, including self-control and positive affect.


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 36

References

Azen, R., & Budescu, D. V. (2003). The dominance analysis approach for comparing predictors

in multiple regression. Psychological Methods, 8, 129–141. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12303

Baumeister, R. F., & Exline, J. J. (1999). Virtue, personality, and social relations: SelfǦcontrol as

the moral muscle. Journal of Personality, 67, 1165-1194. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00086

Baumeister, R. F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength.

New York, NY: Penguin Press.

Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Does happiness promote career success? Journal of

Career Assessment, 16, 101-116. doi: 10.1177/1069072707308140

Bradburn N.M.(1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

Budescu, D. V. (1993). Dominance analysis: A new approach to the problem of relative

importance of predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 542-551.

doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.542

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of

socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165-181. doi: 10.1037/0003-

066X.54.3.165

Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: A model of emotional well-being

across adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1068-1091. doi: 10.1037/a0021232

Cheung, F., Hill, P., & Jackson, J. J. (2017, February 3). Does upward mobility result in greater

well-being? Evidence from a pre-registered study on a large population-based survey.

doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/ZQ49M
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 37

Choi, J., Catapano, R., & Choi, I. (2017). Taking stock of happiness and meaning in everyday

life: An experience sampling approach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8,

641-651. doi: 10.1177/1948550616678455

Christ, S. L., Fleming, L. E., Lee, D. J., Muntaner, C., Muennig, P. A., & CabanǦMartinez, A. J.

(2012). The effects of a psychosocial dimension of socioeconomic position on survival:

Occupational prestige and mortality among US working adults. Sociology of Health &

Illness, 34, 1103-1117. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2012.01456.x

Cohen, S., & Pressman, S. D. (2006). Positive affect and health. Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 15, 122-125. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00420.x

Converse, P. D., Piccone, K. A., & Tocci, M. C. (2014). Childhood self-control, adolescent

behavior, and career success. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 65-70. doi:

10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.007

Darlington, R. B. (1968). Multiple regression in psychological research and practice.

Psychological Bulletin, 69, 161-182. doi: 10.1037/h0025471

Dement, W. C., & Vaughan, C. (1999). The promise of sleep: A pioneer in sleep medicine

explores the vital connection between health, happiness, and a good night's sleep. New

York, NY: Dell.

de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012).

Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide

range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 76-99. doi:

10.1177/1088868311418749

Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13, 81-84.

doi: 10.1111/jopy.12050
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 38

Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic

performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939-944. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

9280.2005.01641.x

Fennis, B. M., Janssen, L., & Vohs, K. D. (2009). Acts of benevolence: A limited resource

account of compliance with charitable requests. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 906–

924. doi:10.1086/593291

Ford, B. Q., & Mauss, I. B. (2014). The paradoxical effects of pursuing positive emotion: When

and why wanting to feel happy backfires. In J. Gruber & J. Moskowitz (Eds.), The light

and dark sides of positive emotion (pp. 363–381). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford

University Press.

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-

and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218-226. doi:

10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical

Transactions-Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 359, 1367-1377.

doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1512

Gilman, R., & Huebner, E. S. (2006). Characteristics of adolescents who report very high life

satisfaction. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 293-301. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-

9036-7

Grant, A. M., & Schwartz, B. (2011). Too much of a good thing: The challenge and opportunity

of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 61-76. doi:

10.1177/1745691610393523
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 39

Gruber, J., Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2011). A dark side of happiness? How, when, and why

happiness is not always good. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 222-233. doi:

10.1177/1745691611406927

Gujar, N., Yoo, S. S., Hu, P., & Walker, M. P. (2011). Sleep deprivation amplifies reactivity of

brain reward networks, biasing the appraisal of positive emotional experiences. Journal

of Neuroscience, 31, 4466-4474. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3220-10.2011

Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An

experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 102, 1318-1335. doi: 10.1037/a0026545

Hofmann, W., Luhmann, M., Fisher, R. R., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2013). Yes, but

are they happy? Effects of trait self-control on affective well-being and life satisfaction.

Journal of Personality. 82, 265-277. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12050

Hofmann, W., & Van Dillen, L. (2012). Desire: The new hot spot in self-control research.

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 317-322. doi:

10.1177/0963721412453587

Isen, A. M., & Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy.

Social Cognition, 2, 18-31. doi: 10.1521/soco.1983.2.1.18

Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive

affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 48, 1413-1426. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1413

Janssen, L., Fennis, B. M., Pruyn, A. T. H., & Vohs, K. D. (2008). The path of least resistance:

Regulatory resource depletion and the effectiveness of social influence techniques.

Journal of Business Research, 61, 1041–1045. doi:10.1016/j .jbusres.2007.09.013


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 40

Johnson, J. W. (2000). A heuristic method for estimating the relative weight of predictor

variables in multiple regression. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 35, 1–19. doi:

10.1207/S15327906MBR3501_1

Johnson, J. W., & LeBreton, J. M. (2004). History and use of relative importance indices in

organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 238–257. doi:

10.1177/1094428104266510

Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science,

330, 932-932. doi: 10.1126/science.1192439

Kim, R. S. (2011). Standardized regression coefficients as indices of effect sizes in meta-analysis

(Paper 3109). Tallahassee: Florida State University

Koval, C. Z., VanDellen, M. R., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Ranby, K. W. (2015). The burden of

responsibility: Interpersonal costs of high self-control. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 108, 750-766. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000015

LeBreton, J. M., Hargis, M. B., Griepentrog, B., Oswald, F. L., & Ployhart, R. E. (2007). A

multidimensional approach for evaluating variables in organizational research and

practice. Personnel Psychology, 60, 475-498. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00080.x

Lebreton, J. M., Ployhart, R. E., & Ladd, R. T. (2004). A Monte Carlo comparison of relative

importance methodologies. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 258-282. doi:

10.1177/1094428104266017

Letzring, T. D., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (2005). Ego-control and ego-resiliency:

Generalization of self-report scales based on personality descriptions from acquaintances,

clinicians, and the self. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 395-422. doi:

10.1016/j.jrp.2004.06.003
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 41

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does

happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803-855. doi: 10.1037/0033-

2909.131.6.803

Marks, G. N., & Fleming, N. (1999). Influences and consequences of well-being among

Australian young people: 1980–1995. Social Indicators Research, 46, 301-323. doi:

10.1023/A:1006928507272

Martin-Storey, A., Serbin, L. A., Stack, D. M., Ledingham, J. E., & Schwartzman, A. E. (2012).

Self and peer perceptions of childhood aggression, social withdrawal and likeability

predict adult personality factors: A prospective longitudinal study. Personality and

Individual Differences, 53, 843-848. doi: 10.1007/BF00912327

Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 2, 99-113. doi: 10.1002/job.4030020205

Mauss, I. B., Savino, N. S., Anderson, C. L., Weisbuch, M., Tamir, M., & Laudenslager, M. L.

(2012). The pursuit of happiness can be lonely. Emotion, 12, 908-912. doi:

10.1037/a0025299

Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329-337. doi: 10.1037/h0029815

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., ... & Caspi,

A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 2693-2698. doi:

10.1073/pnas.1010076108

Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1992). The 1989 socioeconomic index of occupations: Construction from

the 1989 occupational prestige scores. Chicago, IL: National Opinion Research Center.
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 42

Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1994). Updating occupational prestige and socioeconomic scores: How

the new measures measure up. Sociological Methodology, 24, 1–72. doi: 10.2307/270978

Niedenthal, P. M., & Cantor, N. (1992). An influence of positive affect on social categorization.

Motivation and Emotion, 16, 65-78.doi: 10.1007/BF00996487

Nieminen, P., Lehtiniemi, H., Vähäkangas, K., Huusko, A., & Rautio, A. (2013). Standardised

regression coefficient as an effect size index in summarising findings in epidemiological

studies. Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Public Health, 10, doi: 10.2427/8854

Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). The optimum level of well-being: Can people be too

happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 346-360. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-

6916.2007.00048.x

Okun, M. A., Fairholme, C., Karoly, P., Ruehlman, L. S., & Newton, C. (2006). Academic goals,

goal process cognition, and exam performance among college students. Learning and

Individual Differences, 16, 255-265. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2006.04.001

Okun, M. A., Levy, R., Karoly, P., & Ruehlman, L. (2009). Dispositional happiness and college

student GPA: Unpacking a null relation. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 711-715.

doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.010

Pierce, J. R., & Aguinis, H. (2013). The too-much-of-a-good-thing effect in management.

Journal of Management, 39, 313-338. doi: 10.1177/0149206311410060

Pocheptsova, A., Amir, O., Dhar, R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Deciding without resources:

Resource depletion and choice in context. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 344–355.

doi:10.1509/jmkr.46.3.344
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 43

Pyone, J. S., & Isen, A. M. (2011). Positive affect, intertemporal choice, and levels of thinking:

Increasing consumers' willingness to wait. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 532-543.

doi: 10.1509/jmkr.48.3.532

Righetti, F., Finkenauer, C., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Low self-control promotes the willingness to

sacrifice in close relationships. Psychological Science, 24, 1533-1540. doi:

10.1177/0956797613475457

Talbot, L. S., McGlinchey, E. L., Kaplan, K. A., Dahl, R. E., & Harvey, A. G. (2010). Sleep

deprivation in adolescents and adults: Changes in affect. Emotion, 10, 831-841. doi:

10.1037/a0020138

Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High selfǦcontrol predicts good

adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of

Personality, 72, 271-324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x

Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance,

stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8, 454-458.

doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x

Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive

affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental

Social Psychology, 43, 379–384. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.007

Tonidandel, S., & LeBreton, J. M. (2011). Relative importance analysis: A useful supplement to

regression analysis. Journal of Business and Psychology, 26, 1-9. doi: 10.1007/s10869-

010-9204-3

Upton, P., Eiser, C., Cheung, I., Hutchings, H. A., Jenney, M., Maddocks, A., & Williams, J. G.

(2005). Measurement properties of the UK-English version of the Pediatric Quality of


SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 44

Life Inventory™ 4.0 (PedsQL™) Generic Core Scales. Health and Quality of Life

Outcomes, 3, 22-29. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-3-22

Varni, J. W., Seid, M., & Kurtin, P. S. (2001). PedsQL™ 4.0: Reliability and validity of the

Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory™ Version 4.0 Generic Core Scales in healthy and

patient populations. Medical Care, 39, 800-812. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200108000-

00006

Varni, J. W., Seid, M., Knight, T. S., Uzark, K., & Szer, I. S. (2002). The PedsQLTM 4.0

Generic Core Scales: Sensitivity, responsiveness, and impact on clinical decision-making.

Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 175-193. doi:10.1023/A:1014836921812

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of

Statistical Software, 36, 1-48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures

of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

Wiese, C. W., Tay, L., Duckworth, A. L., D'Mello, S., Kuykendall, L., Hofmann, W., ... & Vohs,

K. D. (2017). Too much of a good thing? Exploring the invertedǦU relationship between

selfǦcontrol and happiness. Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12322

Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2017). Immediate rewards predict adherence to long-term goals.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 151-162. doi:

10.1177/0146167216676480
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 45

Footnotes
1
We conducted a series of hierarchical regressions, wherein positive affect and negative

affect were separately and simultaneously entered, along with self-control, as predictors of each

outcome variable. Consistent with our predictions, positive affect and negative affect were more

related to outcomes in the interpersonal domains, relative to those in the achievement domains.

Across five studies, both positive affect and negative affect were significant predictors of

interpersonal relationship outcomes, except that negative affect was not significantly associated

with the number of close friends or new friends (see the Supplemental Material for details).
2
In order to examine the nonlinear effects of self-control and EWB, we conducted

hierarchical polynomial regressions in which self-control and EWB were each entered as a

predictor for each outcome variable, and then the squared values of self-control and EWB were

entered as additional predictors. In 9 out of 29 polynomial models, either self-control squared or

EWB squared significantly predicted the outcome variable. We did not find any consistent

nonlinear effects of self-control or EWB, which is consistent across the five studies.
3
Further analyses revealed no difference between the 63 dropouts and the 222 remaining

participants in terms of age, gender, self-control, and EWB.


4
All standardized betas were computed from models in which age and gender were

included as covariates. No significant difference was found when the standardized betas from the

model without the two covariates were applied.


5
Additionally, we conducted mediation analyses in which either self-control or EWB was

entered as an independent variable, and the other served as a mediator throughout all studies. Out

of 58 mediation models, EWB or self-control emerged as a significant mediator in 31 models.

We found that EWB consistently mediated the relationship between self-control and outcomes in
SELF-CONTROL, POSITIVE AFFECT, AND SUCCESS 46

the domain of relationships (17 out of 19 models). This finding suggests that that emotional

experience could be a potential mechanism underlying the association between self-control and

the positive quality of interpersonal relationships. Further research should be undertaken to

systematically examine this possibility.


47

Tables:

Table 1. Results of Hierarchical Regressions on Academic and Interpersonal Success (Study1)

Academic Relationship Number of


Independent   GPA  
Satisfaction satisfaction close friends
variables
 Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2
-.11 .02 .12 .14 -.01 .02 .11 .21
Gender    
[-.69, .47] [-.48, .53] [-.09, .33] [-.06, .34] [-.56, .54] [-.45, .48] [-1.14, 1.36] [-.99, 1.42]
.02 -.003 .04 .03 .03 .02 -.08 -.08
Age
[-.17, .20] [-.17, .16] [-.02, .10] [-.03, .09] [-.13, .19] [-.11, .16] [-.44, .29] [-.44, .27]
*** * †
1.07 .23 .42 .97
SC
[.54, 1.59] [.03, .44] [-.06, .90] [-.29, 2.22]
*** †
.19 .02 .51 .52
EWB
[-.04, .42] [-.07, .11] [.30, .72] [-.03, 1.07]
2
ΔR .26 .08 .31 .11
2
R  .002 .26  .02 .10  .001 .31  .002 .11

Note. Unstandardized coefficients are reported. 95% confidence intervals are reported in the brackets. SC = self-control; EWB =
emotional well-being; GPA = grade point average.

p < .1. *p < .05. **p < .01. *** p < .001.
48

Table 2. Dominance Analysis: Contributions of Self-Control and EWB in Predicting Academic and Interpersonal Success (Study 1)

Academic Relationship Number of


GPA
Independent satisfaction satisfaction close friends
variable
GDa RWb GD RW GD RW GD RW

SC .18 69% .06 61% .09 30% .05 44%

EWB .08 30% .02 18% .22 70% .06 54%

R2 .26 .10 .31 .11

Note. SC = self-control; EWB = emotional well-being; GPA = grade point average; GD = general dominance; RW = relative weight.
a
General dominance coefficients reflect the average contribution to R2 that a predictor makes across all possible subset regressions (Budescu, 1993).
b
Relative weight coefficients restate this contribution as a percentage of the total R2 explained by predictor variables.
49

Table 3. Results of Hierarchical Regression on Academic and Interpersonal Success (Study 2)

School functioning School functioning Social functioning Social functioning Number of


Independent  (self)

(parents)
 Test scores 
(self)

(parents)

close friends
variables
 Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2
* * ** ** * ** **
-1.82 -.16 5.12 6.31 4.37 4.74 1.93 2.84 1.21 1.67 -5.70 -5.35
Gender      
[-6.40, 2.56] [-4.25, 3.93] [.56, 9.81] [1.89, 10.74] [1.22, 7.51] [1.60, 7.87] [.06, 5.62] [.56, 5.62] [-1.74, 4.16] [-1.23, 4.56] [-8.84, -2.58] [-8.44, -2.27]
* † *** ** * †
-3.72 -2.33 -3.04 -2.12 -6.31 -6.04 -1.45 -.50 -1.08 -.63 -2.38 -1.97
Age
[-6.91, -.54] [-5.17, .52] [-6.25, .18] [-5.20, .97] [-8.50, -4.12] [-8.22, -3.86] [-3.61, .71] [-2.44, 1.44] [-3.13, .97] [-2.65, 1.38] [-4.55, -20] [-4.12, .18]
*** *** *
12.58 9.11 3.67 2.28 1.89 0.08
SC
[8.21, 1695] [4.39, 13.85] [.35, 7.05] [-.69, 5.25] [-1.20, 4.99] [-3.22, 3.37]
*** * *** ** **
6.24 3.76 0.65 7.17 2.95 3.46
EWB
[3.60, 8.89] [.89, 6.62] [-1.38, 2.68] [5.37, 8.97] [1.08, 4.83] [1.47, 5.46]
2
ΔR .21 .10 .02 .19 .04 .04
2
R  .02 .23  .02 .12  .11 .13  .01 .20  .01 .05  .04 .08

Note. Unstandardized coefficients are reported. 95% confidence intervals are reported in the brackets. SC = self-control; EWB =
emotional well-being.

p < .1. *p < .05. **p < .01. *** p < .001.
50

Table 4. Dominance Analysis: Contributions of Self-control and EWB in Predicting Academic and Interpersonal Success (Study 2)

School School Social Social


Number of
functioning functioning Test scores functioning functioning
close friends
(self) (parent) (self) (parent)
Independent
GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW
variable
SC .12 52% .06 48% .02 12% .03 16% .01 24% .00 5%

EWB .09 41% .04 31% .005 4% .16 79% .03 66% .04 45%

R2 .23 .12 .13 .20 .05 .08

Note. SC = self-control; EWB = emotional well-being; GD = general dominance; RW = relative weight.


51

Table 5. Results of Hierarchical Regressions on Occupational and Interpersonal success in Korean sample (Study 3)
Relationship Family Marital Friendship Number of
Independent  Burn Out  Income(log)  satisfaction  satisfaction  satisfaction  intimacy  close friends
variables
 Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2
† *** *** *** ** *** † *
.22 .31 -.18 -.20 .12 .01 -.39* -.54 -.60 -.76 .17 .12  -.46 -.55
Gender       [-1.00,
[-.04, .48] [.08, .55] [-.24, -.13] [-.26, -.15] [-.21, .45] [-.28, .30] [-.77, -01]. [-.82, -.25] [-1.05, -.15] [-1.12, -.39] [-.17, .52] [-.20, .45] [-1.07, -.03]
07]
** * *** *** ***
-.01 .003 .005 .003† -.01 -.02 -.04** -.05 -.05 -.06 .01 .004 -0.003 -0.01
Age
[-.02, .01] [-.01, .02] [.002, 010] [.00, .01] [-.31, .01] [-.04, -.004] [-.06, -.01] [-.07, -.03] [-.07, -.02] [-.08, -.04] [-.01, 03] [-.02, 0.2] [-.03, .03] [-.05, .02]
** * ** *
-.67 .14 .54 .57 .59 .02 0.57
SC
[-1.02, -.31] [.06, .22] [.11, .97] [.14, .99] [.05, 1.13] [-.46, .51] [-.21, 1.34]
† *** *** *** **
-.12 .01 .39 .61 .66 .39 .29
EWB
[-.25, .00] [-.02, .04] [.24, .54] [.46. .76] [.47, .85] [.22, .56] [.02, .56]
2
ΔR .18 .08 .28 .42 .34 .15 .08
2
R  .02 .20  .24 .32  .01 .29  .07 .49  .09 .42  .01 .16  .02 .10

Note. Unstandardized coefficients are reported. 95% confidence intervals are reported in the brackets. SC = self-control; EWB =
emotional well-being.

p < .1. *p < .05. **p < .01. *** p < .001.
52

Table 6. Dominance Analysis: Contributions of Self-Control and EWB in Predicting Occupational and Interpersonal Success in
Korean Sample (Study 3)

Income Relationship Family Marital Friendship Number of


Burnout
(log) satisfaction satisfaction satisfaction intimacy close friends
Independent
variables GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW

SC .11 56% .06 19% .10 34% .12 25% .09 22% .02 15% .03 34%

EWB .06 32% .02 6% .18 62% .30 61% .24 56% .12 79% .05 49%

R2 .20 .32 .29 .49 .42 .16 .10

Note. SC = self-control; EWB = emotional well-being; GD = general dominance; RW = relative weight


53

Table 7. Results of Hierarchical Regressions on Occupational and Interpersonal success in American sample (Study 4)

Occupational Relationship Family Marital Friendship Number of


Independent  Burn Out  Income(log)  prestige  satisfaction  satisfaction  satisfaction  intimacy  close friends
variables
 Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2
** ** * * † *
-.06 .03 -.21 -.22 -1.34 -1.80 .33 .25 .19 .11 .33 .24  .36* .30  .06 -.10
Gender      
[-.31, .19] [-.18, .24] [-.34, .08] [-.35, -.09] [-4.23, 1.48] [-4.61, 1.02] [.06, .60] [.03, .47] [-.14, 53] [-.18, .41] [-.005, .661] [-.06, .54] [.03, .68] [.10, .58] [-.54, .41] [-.57, .38]
* *** *** **
-.02 -.01 .01 .01 .04 .003 .001 -.01 .004 -.01 -.01 -.02 -.002 -.01 .02 .01
Age
[-.03, -.004] [-.02, .003] [.01, .02] [.01, .02] [-.10, .18] [-.14, .14] [-.01, .01] [-.02, .003] [-.01, .02] [-.02, .01] [-.03, .003] [-.04, -.01] [-.02, .01] [-.02, .004] [-.01, .04] [-.01, .04]
*** * *
-.66 .11 4.11 .08 .13 .37 -.30† -.05
SC
[-.91, -.41] [-.05, .27] [.80, 7.42] [-.18, .33] [-.22, .48] [.02, .73] [-.64, .03] [-.64, .53]
*** *** *** *** ***
-.21 .02 .17 .50 .50 .40 .61 .21
EWB
[-.32, -.10] [-.05, .08] [-1.30, 1.64] [.38, .61] [.34, .65] [.24, .55] [.46, .75] [-.04, .47]
2
ΔR .29 .02 .04 .33 .23 .20 .24 .01
2
R  .03 .32  .11 .13  .004 .05  .02 .35  .01 .24  .02 .23  .02 .26  .01 .02

Note. Unstandardized coefficients are reported. 95% confidence intervals are reported in the brackets. SC = self-control; EWB =
emotional well-being.

p < .1. *p < .05. **p < .01. *** p < .001.
54

Table 8. Dominance Analysis: Contributions of Self-Control and EWB in Predicting Occupational and Interpersonal Success in

American sample (Study 4)

Income Occupational Relationship Family Marital Friendship Number of


` Burnout
(log) prestige satisfaction satisfaction satisfaction intimacy close friends

Independent
GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW
Variables

SC .16 51% .01 9% .03 70% .08 21% .06 24% .08 32% .03 12% .003 10%

EWB .13 41% .007 5% .01 21% .26 72% .17 73% .14 59% .21 81% .01 50%

R2 .32 .13 .05 .35 .24 .22 .26 .02

Note. SC = self-control; EWB = emotional well-being; GD = general dominance; RW = relative weight


55

Table 9. Results of Hierarchical Regressions on Academic and Interpersonal Success (Study5)

Academic Relationship Number of


Independent   GPA  
satisfaction satisfaction new friends
variables
 Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2  Step 1 Step 2
* † * †
.23 .35 .14 .18 .07 .16 -.73 -.65
Gender    
[-.14, .61] [.01. .70] [-.01, .30] [.03, .32] [-.28, .42] [-.16, .48] [-1.49, .03] [-1.41, .10]
-.13 -.17 -.07 -.08 -.14 -.16 -.04 -.06
Age
[-.41, .14] [-.41, 08] [-.18, .04] [-.18, .03] [-.39, .11] [-.39, .07] [-.59, .51] [-.60, .48]
*** ***
1.04 .42 .23 .22
SC
[.66, 1.42] [.26, .58] [-.12, .58] [-.60, 1.04]
* *** *
.20 -.04 .52 .44
EWB
[.01, .38] [-.11, .04] [.35, .69] [.04, .84]
2 .17 .11 .18 .03
ΔR
2 .01 .18 .02 .13 .01 .19 .02 .04
R 
Note. Unstandardized coefficients are reported. 95% confidence interval are reported in the brackets. SC = self-control; EWB =
emotional well-being; GPA = grade point average.

p < .1. *p < .05. **p < .01. *** p < .001.
56

Table 10. Dominance Analysis: Contributions of Self-Control and EWB in Predicting Academic and Interpersonal Success (Study

5)

Academic Relationship Number of


GPA
satisfaction satisfaction new friends

Independent variable GD RW GD RW GD RW GD RW

SC .13 70% .10 77% .02 12% .005 11%

EWB .04 20% .003 2% .15 83% .02 56%

R2 .18 .13 .19 .04

Note. SC = self-control; EWB = emotional well-being; GD = general dominance; RW = relative weight; GPA = grade point average
57
58

Highlights:

· Self-control and positive affect are important predictors of success in life


· The relative contributions of self-control and positive affect to positive outcomes vary as
a function of life domains.
· Self-control is more strongly related with success in the achievement domain than
positive affect.
· Positive affect is more strongly related with success in the interpersonal relationship
domain.

You might also like