You are on page 1of 10

The Role of Attitude Accessibility in the Attitude-to-Behavior Process

Author(s): Russell H. Fazio, Martha C. Powell, Carol J. Williams


Source: The Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 16, No. 3, (Dec., 1989), pp. 280-288
Published by: The University of Chicago Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2489509
Accessed: 12/06/2008 04:51

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ucpress.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We enable the
scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that
promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

http://www.jstor.org
The Role of Attitude Accessibility in the
Process
Attitude-to-Behavior

RUSSELL H. FAZIO
MARTHAC. POWELL
CAROL J. WILLIAMS*
Attitudes toward a number of products and the accessibility of those attitudes as
indicated by the latency of response to an attitudinal inquirywere assessed. Sub-
jects with highly accessible attitudes toward a given product displayed greater atti-
tude-behavior correspondence than did those with relatively less accessible atti-
tudes. Furthermore, subjects with less accessible attitudes displayed more sensi-
tivity to the salience afforded a product by its position in the front row, as opposed
to the back row, than did subjects Withmore accessible attitudes. The implications
of these data for a model of the process by which attitudes guide behavior are
discussed.

T he topic of attitudes clearly occupies a central tion of the individual's immediate perceptions of the
position in researchon consumer behavior (e.g., attitude object in the context of the situation in which
Engel and Blackwell 1982; Kassarjianand Kassarjian the object is encountered. "Perception" refers to the
1979). In part, the attention given to the attitude con- individual's current feelings about, or appraisal of,
struct stems from the fact that much advertising can the object as experienced in the immediate situation.
be describedas social influence attempts aimed at cre- According to the model, attitudes guide such apprais-
ating positive attitudes toward the product. Indeed, als of the object, but only if they have been activated
considerable research has been concerned with un- from memory upon observation of the object. Hence,
derstandingthis persuasion process (e.g., Boyd, Ray, the accessibility of the attitude from memory is postu-
and Strong 1972; Lutz 1975; Petty, Cacioppo, and lated to act as a critical determinant of whether the
Schumann 1983). The assumption underlying such attitude-to-behaviorprocess is initiated. Various as-
persuasive attempts is that the development of posi- pects of the model have received empirical support
tive attitudes will produce a correspondingchange in from both correlational and experimental investiga-
behavior. As a result, consumer research also has tions in such contexts as voting behavior (Fazio and
been concerned with understanding the relation be- Williams 1986), the evaluation of evidence related to
tween attitudes and subsequent behavior (e.g., Day a social policy issue (Houston and Fazio 1989), and
and Deutscher 1982; Ryan and Bonfield 1975; Smith "free-play"behavior with intellectual puzzles (Fazio
and Swinyard 1983). It is this attitude-behaviorrela- et al. 1982; see Fazio 1989 for a review). The major
tion that constitutes the focus of the present article. goal of the present researchwas to assess the general-
Fazio and his colleagues proposed a model of the ity of the model to a consumer behavior domain. The
process by which attitudes guide behavior (Fazio research focuses upon the relation between attitudes
1986; Fazio, Powell, and Herr 1983). In brief, the toward specific products and product-selection be-
model views behaviorin any given situation as a func- havior.
The hypothesis here is that the status of individuals'
attitudes along an attitude/non-attitude continuum
*RussellH. Fazio is Professorof Psychology,and MarthaC. Pow-
ell and CarolJ. Williamsare ResearchAssistants,all in the Depart- moderates the attitude-behavior relation. Fazio and
ment of Psychology,Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405. his colleagues (Fazio 1989; Fazio et al. 1986) have de-
This article was preparedwhile the first author was supportedby fined this continuum in terms of the strength of the
the National Institute of Mental Health ResearchScientist Devel- association in memory between the object and the in-
opment AwardMH00452. The researchwas supportedby a grant dividual's evaluation of the object. At one end of the
from the Ogilvy Center for Research and Development. The au-
thors thank ClarkLeavitt and AlexanderBiel of the Ogilvy Center continuum is the non-attitude; no a priori evaluation
for theirhelpfulconsultationwhile the researchwas being designed of the object is available in memory. If asked to ex-
and David Sanbonmatsufor his assistancein data collection. Por- press an attitude, the individual needs to construct
tions of these data werepresentedat the 1986 Association for Con- one on the spot. Such constructions presumablywill
sumerResearchAnnual Conferencein Toronto.
involve some assessment of attributes of the object
280
? JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH * Vol. 16 0 December 1989
ATTITUDE ACCESSIBILITY 281

that are recalledfrom memory and/or apparentin the inquiry. Hence, it is important to review briefly what
external environment. However, at all other points is known about the validity of this measureas an indi-
along the continuum, an evaluation is available in cation of the accessibility of attitudes and their posi-
memory. Moving along the continuum, the strength tion along the attitude/non-attitude continuum.
of the association between the evaluation and the ob- First, the latency measure has been found to reflect
ject-and hence the accessibility of the attitude-in- what has been postulated to be the conceptual vari-
creases. At the upper end of the continuum is a well- able that determinesthe chronic accessibility of an at-
learned, strong association-sufficiently strong that titude-namely, the strength of the association be-
the evaluation is capable of automatic activation tween the object and the evaluation. A number of ex-
from memory upon mere observation or mention of periments have manipulated the strength of this
the object. Attitude-behaviorconsistency is expected object-evaluation association by having subjects ex-
to vary as a function of position along this attitude/ press their attitudes repeatedly. This researchhas in-
non-attitude continuum. Individuals who possess dicated that such repeated expression enhances the
highly accessible attitudes toward a given product are speed with which individuals respond to later inqui-
expected to be more attitudinally consistent in their ries concerning their attitudes (Fazio et al. 1982; Pow-
product-selection behavior than are individuals ell and Fazio 1984).
whose attitudes are relatively less accessible from Second, and more important, the latency measure
memory. provides a good approximation of the likelihood that
As mentioned earlier, evidence consistent with this the attitude will be activated from memory automati-
hypothesizedmoderatingrole of attitude accessibility cally upon mere observation of the object. It has been
has been obtained in previous research.Most relevant demonstratedthat attitude objects preselected on the
is a correlational field study by Fazio and Williams basis of an individual's having responded quickly to
(1986) in which it was found that the relation between an attitudinal inquiry are more likely to activate the
attitudes toward Reagan and self-reportedvoting be- attitude automatically upon subsequent presentation
havior in the 1984 presidential election varied as a of the object than are attitudes characterizedby rela-
function of the accessibility of the attitude. Attitude tively slow latencies of response to an attitudinal in-
accessibility was measured via latency of response to quiry (Fazio et al. 1986). Sanbonmatsu and Fazio
the attitudinal inquiry. Individuals who were able to ( 1986) observedthe same result in a study specifically
indicate their attitude relativelyquickly (the high atti- concerned with the automatic activation of attitudes
tude accessibility group) displayed greater consis- toward products. Thus, the latency with which one
tency between those attitudes and subsequent voting responds to an attitudinal inquiry is sensitive to the
behavior than did individuals who responded rela- strengthof the object-evaluation association and pro-
tively slowly (the low attitude accessibility group). vides an indication of the likelihood that the attitude
In addition to examining the applicability of the will be activated spontaneously upon one's encoun-
model to product-selection behavior, the present re- tering the object.
searchwas intended to addressan unavoidable short- Subjectsin the present study responded to attitudi-
coming of the voting behavior study-the self-report nal inquiries concerning a large number of products.
nature of the behavior measure. As in any investiga- A subset of these products servedas the targetattitude
tion of choices made within the confines of a voting objects and subsequently were made available as be-
booth, Fazio and Williams (1986) were forced to rely havioral alternatives. According to the process
upon participants'reportsof how they had voted. The model, these behavioral selections should be a func-
respondents were telephoned within a day or two of tion of the individual's perceptions of the object in
the election and asked to reveal whether they had the immediate situation. Is the individual's appraisal
voted and, if so, for whom. Although we do not dis- of the object at that particular moment favorable or
cern any plausible explanation for how such self-re- unfavorable? An attitude that is highly accessible
ports might have been biased by both the attitude ex- from memory and, hence, likely to be activated auto-
pressed months earlier and the accessibility of the at- matically upon the individual's observationof the ob-
titude, the shortcoming of this method of observing ject is apt to result in immediate perceptions that are
the attitude-to-behavior relation cannot be denied. congruent with the attitude. In contrast, when the at-
The present investigation concerned actual overt be- titude is not activated from memory, immediate per-
havior. Subjects selected items from a set of products ceptions are less likely to be influenced by a pre-
arrangedon a table. These selections, which subjects viously constructed evaluation that is available in
took home as "a token of our appreciation" for their memory. Instead, these immediate appraisals are
having participated in the study, constituted the be- likely to be based upon momentarily salient and po-
havioralmeasure. tentially unrepresentative features (memory-based
The presentinvestigation involves, as did the Fazio and/or stimulus-based)of the object that are not nec-
and Williams (1986) study, measurement of attitude essarily evaluated in a manner congruent with the at-
accessibility via latency of response to an attitudinal titude. As a result, greater attitude-behavior consis-
282 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

tency is expected when attitudes are highly accessible the items as a "token of our appreciation"for having
than when attitudes are relatively less accessible from participated. The experimenter unobtrusively re-
memory. corded the products selected and the order in which
they were selected. The selected items were then
METHOD placed in a paper bag for the subject to take home.

Subjects RESULTS
One hundred one individuals participated in the The mean latency of response across objects and
experiment. Sixty-one participated in partial fulfill- subjects was 1. 14 seconds. When a subject's dichoto-
ment of an introductory psychology course require- mous response during the computerized task was in-
ment. The remaining subjects responded to an ad consistent with the subsequent scalar rating, it was
placed in the local newspaperand participatedin re- considered an error. An error was defined as a sub-
turn for monetary payment. ject's responding "like" during the computerized la-
tency task but rating the product more 'negatively
Procedure than the neutral point on the questionnaire scale, or
responding "dislike" during the computer task but
Subjects were informed that the experiment con- assigninga ratingabove the neutralpoint on the scale.
cerned a new way of measuringattitudes via a micro- Such errorswere infrequent. On the average,subjects
computer. Their initial task involved responding to responded inconsistently on 7.3 percent of the 100
each of 100 attitude objects, all of which were familiar trials. The particulardatum from any trial on which
and commonly available products. The subjects were an erroroccurredwas omitted from the analysis.
instructedto pressone of two keys, labeled "like" and Previous research on attitude accessibility has ob-
"dislike," to indicate their feelings about each object. served a relation between attitude accessibility, as
The subjects were told to "respond as quickly and as measured by latency of response to an attitudinal in-
accurately as possible." The presentation was con- quiry, and attitude extremity, as indexed by deviation
trolled by an Apple II+ computer. The orderin which of a scalar value from the neutral point of the scale
the objects were presented was randomized for each (Fazio and Williams 1986; Powell and Fazio 1984).
subject. A three-second interval separatedeach trial. The same relation was evident in the present data set.
The subject's response was recorded, along with the Faster response times were associated with more ex-
latency of the response (from stimulus onset to re- treme ratings. The within-subject correlation be-
sponse) to the nearest millisecond. To familiarize tween response latency and attitude extremity
subjects with the procedure, subjects performed a se- reached a level of significance for 46 percent of the
ries of practicetrialsinvolving differentproductsthan subjects. The mean correlation across subjects was
those used in the actual experimental list. -0.18 (t(100) = I 1.47, p < 0.00). As a result of this
Following the computerized task, subjects com- relation, it was necessarythat all tests of the hypothe-
pleted a number of questionnaires, only the first of sis regardingthe moderating role of attitude accessi-
which was relevant to the present purposes. The sub- bility be conducted in a manner that did not con-
jects provideda scalarattitudinal ratingof each of the found attitude accessibility and attitude extremity.
100 objects. These ratings were made on a 1 (ex- Both within-subject and between-subject analyses
tremely bad) to 7 (extremely good) scale. were conducted. In both cases, the analysis that was
performed examined the impact of accessibility as
Behavior Measure measured by response latency while considering
equivalent attitude scores.
As their final task, subjectswere given the opportu-
nity to select five products from a set of 10 alterna- Within-Subjects Analysis
tives. The 10 products, which had been roughly
equated for perceived value throughinformal pretest- To overcome the naturally existing relation be-
ing, were a Snickers candy bar, a small bag of Fritos tween attitude accessibility and extremity, the within-
corn chips, two boxes of Sun-Maidraisins, a small can subjectsanalysis focused upon any two or more target
of Star-Kist tuna, a can of Dr. Pepper, a box of products for which a given subject had assigned the
CrackerJacks, a bag of Planters peanuts, two cans of same attitude scalarrating. We restrictedourselves to
V-8 juice, two 5-stick packs of Dentyne gum, and a these attitudinal "ties" to ensure that the role of atti-
Mounds candy bar. The products were arranged in tude accessibility was examined in a manner that was
two rows of five on a table and were covered by a ta- independent of attitude scores. For each subject, the
blecloth. proportion of ties for which the subject's behavior
At the appropriatetime, the experimenterremoved concurred with a prediction based upon the subject's
the tablecloth and told the subject to choose five of response latencies was determined. According to the
ATTITUDE ACCESSIBILITY 283

hypothesis, subjects with highly accessible attitudes latency in the present case would have led to the con-
should behave more consistently than those with less sistent classification of individuals with'tendenciesto
accessible attitudes. Thus, for any ties at positions respond quickly to any inquiry as high accessibility
more positive than the neutral point, we predicted subjects.
that the subjectwould either select the productassoci- In one of the analyses that was conducted, behavior
ated with the faster response latency before selecting was coded on a 0 to 5 scale, with products that were
the other product or select the formerproduct and not not selected by a given subject being assigned a score
the latter. The prediction was reversed for any ties at of 0 and the product that was selected firsta score of 5.
attitudinal positions more negative than the neutral The remaining products were assigned intermediate
point. Any ties at the neutral point were not included scores indicative of the order of selection. For each
in the analysis. Averagedacross the 100 subjects with product, the correlation between attitude and behav-
analyzable ties, the proportion of instances confirm- ior was computed within the high, moderate, and low
ing the predictionswas 0.59, significantlygreaterthan accessibility groups. These correlations were ana-
the value of 0.50 expected by chance alone, (t(99) lyzed, following a Fisher's r-to-z transformation, via
= 3.09, p < 0.0025). an a priori polynomial contrast examining whether
they displayed a significant linear trend as a function
Between-SubjectsAnalyses of level of attitude accessibility. This prediction was
confirmed (F( 1,9) = 8.01, p < 0.02).2 Averaged across
The between-subjectsanalyses involved classifying the 10 products, the mean correlations (following a
subjects into groups of high, moderate, and low atti- retransformation of the average zs back to rs) were
tude accessibility for each product. As in the Fazio 0.62, 0.54, and 0.50 for the high, moderate, and low
and Williams (1986) study, this group assignmentwas attitude accessibility groups, respectively.
performedat each and every level of the attitude scale The analysis involved the scoring of behavior as a
to avoid confounding attitude scores with attitude ac- function of order of selection and, thus, assumes that
cessibility. For example, the subsample of subjects subjects chose first their most preferredalternative,
who had assigned a rating of 7 (extremely good) to a then their second most preferreditem, and so on. The
given target product were trichotomized on the basis data also were analyzed with behavior coded dichoto-
of their accessibility scores into high, moderate, and mously as a function of whether the product was or
low groups. The subsamples who had rated the prod- was not selected. This analysis also confirmedthe pre-
uct 6, 5, and so on were similarly trichotomized. This diction. TJhemean correlations were 0.61, 0.59, and
classificationprocedurewas performedanew for each 0.51 in\the>high,moderate, and low attitude accessi-
of the 10 targetproducts. In this way, the attitude dis- bility groups, respectively, and, as in the previous
tributions for any given product were equivalent in analysis, revealed a linear trend (F( 1,9) = 5.09,
the high, moderate, and low attitude accessibility p = 0.051).
groups. Thus, regardlessof how the data are examined, the
Unlike the Fazio and Williams (1986) investiga- findingsconverge upon the notion that attitude acces-
tion, however, attitude accessibility was not indexed sibility moderatedthe attitude-behaviorrelation. The
simply by raw response latency. Instead, the accessi- more accessible a subject's attitude was toward a
bility measure employed for classification purposes given product, the more likely it was that product se-
was the z-score of a given subject's response latency lection behavior was consistent with that attitude.
for a given target product relative to that subject's
mean and standarddeviation of the latencies for the
90 filler products. This within-subject z-score serves Position Effects
to place a given subject's latency of response to a
given object within his or her distribution of latencies As was mentioned earlier, the attitude-to-behavior
for the fillerproducts(see Fazio forthcoming for a dis- process model views behavior as a function of the in-
cussion of indices of baseline speed of responding).
This change was necessitated by the fact that inter- diotaped statement, the end of which contained the electronic
item correlations among the. 10 response latencies markerthat initiated the timing. In contrast, the present study in-
were substantial (average r = 0.42), whereas inter- volved displayingthe name of an object on a computerscreen, the
item correlationsin the Fazio and Williams investiga- onset of which initiated the timing. The time necessaryto readthe
visual display may have enhanced the amount of covariation ob-
tion were quite minimal (average r = 0.19; see Foot- served.Readingtime may be a fairlyconstant individualdifference
note 1 of Fazio and Williams 1986).1 The use of raw that formeda component of each responselatency.
2Giventhe small numberof units of analysisinvolved in this sta-
tistical test, i.e., the 10 products,it seemed desirableto also exam-
IWhy the two studiesdifferedin this regardis not clear. However, ine the data nonparametrically.The data were analyzed via the
one differencein the attitudeaccessibilitymeasurementprocedures Page test for orderedalternatives(Page 1963; Seigel and Castellan
employed in the two studies is worth noting. The Fazio and Wil- 1988), which did reveal a significant linear trend. The same was
liams (1986) investigation involved subjects respondingto an au- true of all additionallineartrendsreportedhere.
284 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

dividual's perceptions of the object in the immediate Thus, the lower the attitude accessibility, the more
situation. A highly accessible attitude is likely to be selection behavior was influenced by the relative sa-
activated from memory automatically and is pre- lience afforded a product by its positioning. In con-
sumed to determine this immediate perception of the trast, the linear trends presented earlier with respect
object. In contrast, an attitude that is relatively low in to attitude-behavior correlations suggest that the
accessibility is less likely to be activated upon obser- greater the attitude accessibility, the more selection
vation of the object. As a result of low accessibility, behavior was influenced by the attitude.
the immediate perception is likely to be influenced by
momentarily salient features of the object.
This hypothesized greater influence of a momen- DISCUSSION
tarily salient dimension for objects associated with a The Role of Attitude Accessibility
relativelyinaccessible attitude than for objects associ-
ated with a highly accessible attitude was apparentin The present findings are consistent with the hy-
a rather surprisingfashion in subjects' selection be- pothesis that attitude accessibility exerts a moderat-
havior. The 10 products were arrangedin two rows of ing role upon the attitude-behaviorrelation, just as is
five for the subjects' viewing and selection.3 On the postulated by the process model. The more accessible
assumption that the products positioned in the front the attitude, the more predictive it was of subsequent
row were more salient than those in the back row, we behavior. Furthermore,this evidence was obtained in
suggest that the selection of a given product will be a situation involving actual behavior as opposed to
more influenced by row status if the attitude is low in self-reports of behavior. Subjects chose, took home,
accessibility than if its accessibility is high. The data and presumably consumed the products. Thus, the
revealed precisely this pattern. Among the products data are very consistent with the attitude-to-behavior
in the front row (Snickers, Mounds, peanuts, Den- process model.
tyne, and tuna), the lower the accessibility of their at- When interpretedwithin the context of the process
titudes, the greater the likelihood that subjects se- model, the findings suggest that subjects scanned the
lected the product. Indeed, across these products, the set of available alternatives and arrivedat immediate
averageproportion of subjectswho selected the prod- perceptions of each (or at least some subset) of the
uct was 0.55, 0.59, and 0.59 in the high, moderate, products. In the case of objects for which a strong
and low accessibility groups. For products in the back evaluative associate existed in memory, this evalua-
row, the reverse was true. The lower the accessibility tion was activated automatically from memory upon
of the attitude, the less likely subjects were to select the subject's observation of the object. That is, obser-
the product (average proportions of 0.50, 0.46, and vation of the object automatically activated the atti-
0.40 for the high, moderate, and low groups, respec- tude from memory if the chronic accessibility of the
tively). The row effects-the difference between the attitude, as estimated by a relatively fast latency of
mean proportion of subjects who selected a product response to an earlier attitudinal inquiry, was high.
positioned in the front row and the mean proportion Such activated attitudes strongly determined the sub-
who selected an alternative that had been positioned ject's feelings towardthe object in the immediate situ-
in the back row-were 0.05, 0.13, and 0.19 for the ation. If the object-evaluation association for a given
high, moderate, and low attitude accessibility groups, product was weak and, hence, the chronic accessibil-
respectively. Analysis of these data indicated that the ity of the attitude was relativelylow, then the immedi-
row effects constituted a significant linear trend ate perception was likely to be influenced by momen-
(F(1,9) = 5.96, p < 0.05).4 tarily salient thoughts or features of the object. The
data suggest that the perception of such objects was
affectedby the greatersalience affordedan object po-
sitioned in the front row as opposed to the back row.
3Productswerearrangedrandomlyat the beginningof the experi-
ment and then fixed across subjects.As a result, an overall prefer- Other momentarily salient factors that might have
ence for front-rowproductsover back-rowproducts(analogousto operatedinclude product attributesthat were recalled
a main effect) may reflect either the influence of row status and/
or the productsthemselves. Obviously, no clear inferences can be
drawn from data reflectingan overall preferencefor the products accessibilitydecreased,which, as reported,they did (mean propor-
in one row over the productsin the other row. However, this con- tions of 0.52, 0.56, and 0.60 forthe high, moderate,and low accessi-
foundingdoes not affectour ability to examine the degreeto which bility groups).For ease of understanding,the data are presentedin
the exhibited preferencefor front-rowproductsrelates to the level the text in terms of row effects,i.e., the differencein the proportion
of attitude accessibility(conceptually analogous to an interaction of subjectswho selected front-rowversusback-rowproducts.
effect). Additionalanalyseson the frequencywith which a given product
4The actual analysis was performedon scores for each product was selected revealed that level of attitude accessibility was inde-
that representedthe proportionof subjectswho selected the prod- pendent of the specificproductsthat were positioned in each row.
uct if it had been in the front row and the proportionwho did not The data did not displayany appreciabledeviationsfrom the over-
select it if the producthad been in the back row. A greaterinfluence all linear trend that was apparentfor the front-row(X2(8) = 4.15, p
of row statuswould be apparentif these scoresincreasedlinearlyas > 0.25) or back-rowproducts(X2(8) = 1.34, p > 0.25).
ATTITUDE ACCESSIBILITY 285

from memory, how thirsty or hungry the subject was, a related discussion of the likelihood of product de-
how recently the subject had eaten a particular food tection). Any productswithin this subset that are neg-
(e.g., a candy bar), what snack foods the subject re- atively valued will be quickly rejected from inclusion
called as having stored at home, and the like. In any in the consideration set. In other words, the percep-
case, such factors appear to exert a greater influence tion or appraisalof the product in the immediate situ-
when the individual's attitude toward a given object ation, determined as it is by the negative attitude acti-
is unlikely to have been activated automatically upon vated from memory, will lead to its rejection. In con-
observation of the object than when the attitude is trast, those products that are both positively valued
highly accessible from memory and capable of auto- and characterized by high attitude accessibility are
matic activation. likely to be included in individual A's considera-
tion set.
Product Positioning and the Construction of the consideration set may proceed
Consideration Set quite differently for our hypothetical individual B.
Lacking highly accessible attitudes, this individual's
The differential influence of row status as a func- attention is unlikely to be attitudinally guided. Those
tion of attitude accessibility is clearly evident from products enjoying the relative salience affordedthem
the data. The precise mechanisms through which this by their position in the front row are more likely to
influence occurred is less clear. One possibility con- be included in the consideration set than are those po-
cerns the potential influence of salient positioning on sitioned in the back row. Thus, the probabilities that
the likelihood that a given alternative will receive individual B will select a front-row product over a
much consideration in the selection process. Con- back-row product may be enhanced. Obviously, the
sumer behavior researchers have distinguished the role of attitude accessibility and product salience in
"evoked" or "consideration" set-those alternatives defining the consideration set merits further investi-
that are actually considered for potential selection- gation.
from the pool of alternatives that are available (e.g.,
Bakeret al. 1986; Howardand Sheth 1969). Although The Attitude Accessibility Measure
the present investigation did not involve the collec-
tion of any data relevant to the question of which Our interpretation of the attitude-behaviorconsis-
items constituted an individual's consideration set, tency data rests on the validity of response latency to
the observed influence of both attitude accessibility an attitudinal inquiry as a measure of the chronic ac-
and position may have operated at the level of inclu- cessibility of the attitude. As indicated earlier, evi-
sion or exclusion from the consideration set. dence regardingsuch validity is provided by previous
For ease of discussion, imagine two individuals, research (Fazio et al. 1986) demonstrating that this
one of whom (A) holds highly accessible attitudes to- measure relates to the likelihood of automatic activa-
ward some subset of the 10 alternatives and one of tion of the attitude upon exposure to the object.
whom (B) does not have highly accessible attitudes Those objects for which an individual could respond
toward any of the alternatives. When scanning the relatively quickly to a direct attitudinal query also
items, individual A may especially notice, and attend were likely to activate the attitude from memory
to, those products toward which s/he has a highly ac- upon their mere presentation. In contrast, objects for
cessible attitude because the affective associate to the which response latencies to an inquiry were slow
object will be activated automatically from memory showed little evidence of producing automatic attitu-
upon observation of the object. Indeed, some very re- dinal evaluation upon their presentation.
cent researchindicates that objects toward which an Although we can be confident that the latency mea-
individual holds highly accessible attitudes, be they sure reflects the chronic accessibility of the attitude,
positively or negatively valenced, are more likely to it must be recognized that the present investigation
be noticed when presented in the visual field than are is correlational in nature. Attitude accessibility was
objects toward which the individual holds less acces- measured and not manipulated. Thus, whatever di-
sible attitudes. Roskos-Ewoldsen and Fazio (1989) mensions are naturallyassociated with attitude acces-
found support for this hypothesis in two investiga- sibility may have contributed to the differences that
tions, one of which involved the measurement of atti- were observed with respect to attitude-behaviorcon-
tude accessibility via response latency (as in the pres- sistency. For example, familiarity may moderate the
ent study) and one of which involved the experimen- attitude-behaviorrelation. Because the present corre-
tal manipulation of attitude accessibility. lational investigation involved the measurement of
Thus, these data suggestthat our hypothetical indi- attitude accessibility and because objects with which
vidual A will quickly and easily notice those products individuals are familiar are likely to be ones toward
concerningwhich accessible attitudes are held even if which they have accessible attitudes, the distinct
those items are positioned in the relatively disadvan- moderatinginfluences of attitude accessibility and fa-
tageous back row (see Alba and Hutchinson 1987 for miliarity cannot be ascertainedfrom the present data.
286 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

However, two counterpoints should be noted. First, Affect Referral


experimentalwork in which attitude accessibility was
manipulated directly, by varyingthe number of times The attitude-behavior process model bears some
that subjects expressed their attitudes, has indicated similarity to Wright's (1975) theoretical statements
a causal impact of attitude accessibility upon both at- regardingwhat he terms affect referral.Affect referral
titude-behaviorconsistency (Fazio et al. 1982, Exper- is a decision-making strategyin which the individual
iment 4) and attitude-judgment consistency (Hous- avoids reviewing any specific attribute information
ton and Fazio 1989). Although further experimental concerning the alternatives, but instead relies upon
research is undoubtedly needed to isolate the causal previously formed, global affective judgments of the
influence of attitude accessibility, this finding does alternatives. Although Wright did not detail any
bolster our confidence that the present results are not mechanisms or process by which such affect referral
due solely to the common dependence of attitude ac- occurs, the affect referralstrategythat he mentions is
cessibility and attitude-behaviorconsistency on some one that our model obviously endorses. Indeed, the
third variable,such as familiarity. model can be viewed as specifying the mechanism un-
Second, we should note, as did Fazio and Williams derlying, and the conditions necessaryfor, such affect
(1986), what we view as the primary advantage of fo- referral. Position along the attitude/non-attitude
cusing upon attitude accessibility as opposed to some continuum dictates the extent to which an affect re-
other strength-relateddimension of attitude, such as ferral strategyis possible. A strong object-evaluation
confidence in one's attitude (e.g., Fazio and Zanna association in memory and, hence, a highly accessible
1978), the manner of attitude formation (e.g., Fazio attitude is a prerequisite for such affect referral. In
and Zanna 1981), affective-cognitive consistency such a case, the attitude will be activated from mem-
(e.g., Norman 1975), or amount of information about ory and influence, if not completely determine, the
the attitude object (e.g., Davidson et al. 1985). Unlike individual's appraisalof the object in the immediate
other indicants of attitude strength, the construct of situation. The sparserthe immediate environment is
attitude accessibility operates at an information-pro- with respect to the availability of new information
cessing level of analysis. As a result, it is much more about the attitude object, the less critical the selective
clearly relevant to the issue of the process by which processingcomponent of our model becomes. In a sit-
attitudes guide behavior. Although other indicants of uation in which no new information is presented (as
the strengthof an attitude may be associated with atti- in the present investigation), the selective processing
tude accessibility and, hence, with our classification amounts to an effortless application of the attitude
of subjects in the present study, the construct of atti- stored in memory as one's appraisal of the object in
tude accessibility has clear implications with respect the immediate situation-much as Wrightappearsto
mean by his affect referralstrategy. With a highly ac-
to the basic mechanisms involved in the production cessible attitude, the immediate appraisal is more
of attitude-consistentbehavior. likely to be congruent with the attitude stored in
Indeed, it has been postulated that various identi- memory than it is when a relatively inaccessible atti-
fied moderatorsof the attitude-behaviorrelation may tude is involved. Active construction of an immediate
exert their impact because they reflect the strength of appraisalis not necessary.
the object-evaluation association and, hence, the ac- In contrast, the selective processing component as-
cessibility of the attitude (Fazio 1986). This has been sumes more importance in an environment that is in-
demonstrated with respect to one such moderator- formation rich. When the situation makes new infor-
the manner of attitude formation. Attitudes based mation about the object available (as in the Fazio and
upon direct behavioral experience with the object Williams voting behavior study or in an ongoing so-
have been found to be both more predictive of later cial interaction), interpretations of this information
behavior (Fazio and Zanna 1981) and more accessi- will be colored by an activated attitude. As a conse-
ble from memory (Fazio et al. 1982, 1983) than atti- quence of what is potentially extensive selective pro-
tudes based upon indirect experience. Similar evi- cessing in this case, immediate appraisals are more
dence has been obtained with respect to the moderat- likely to be congruent with attitudes among individu-
ing variables of personal importance of the attitude als whose attitudes are highly accessible than among
issue (Krosnick forthcoming) and of individual individuals whose attitudes are not. Thus, the extent
differences in self-monitoring tendencies (Kardes et of selective processing depends upon the degree to
al. 1986). The same may hold true for other variables which the situation provides new information. In ei-
that have been identified as moderators of the atti- ther case, the attitude-behaviorprocess model points
tude-behaviorrelation. If so, the construct of attitude to the importance of correspondencebetween percep-
accessibility and the process model may provide a tions of the object in the immediate situation and atti-
conceptual frameworkfor integratingthis set of mod- tudes towardthe object. Such correspondenceis more
erators and for understandinghow and why they in- likely when the attitude involves a strong object-eval-
fluence attitude-behaviorconsistency. uation association and is capable of automatic activa-
ATTITUDE ACCESSIBILITY 287

tion from memory upon mere observation of the ob- (1989), "On the Power and Functionality of Atti-
ject. tudes: The Role of Attitude Accessibility," in Attitude
Structureand Function, eds. Anthony R. Pratkaniset
al., Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaumAssociates, 153-
Final Implications 179.
(forthcoming),"A PracticalGuide to the Use of Re-
The findingsfrom the present investigation, as well sponse Latency in Social Psychological Research," in
as our discussion of them, attest to the relevance of Review of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 11,
the attitude-behaviorprocess model to the consumer eds. Clyde Hendrick and MargaretS. Clark, Beverly
behavior domain. The accessibility of an attitude Hills, CA: Sage.
from memory does appear to moderate the extent to , Jeaw-MeiChen, ElizabethC. McDonel, and Steven
J. Sherman (1982), "Attitude Accessibility, Attitude-
which that attitude guides product selection behav- Behavior Consistency, and the Strengthof the Object-
ior. A major implication of this finding concerns the Evaluation Association," Journal of ExperimentalSo-
role of advertisingas a social influence agent intended cialPsychology, 18 (July), 339-357.
to promote the desired consumer behavior. Appar- Martha C. Powell, and Paul M. Herr (1983), "To-
ently, inducing a positive attitude towardthe brandis ward a Process Model of the Attitude-BehaviorRela-
not in and of itself sufficient to have much influence tion: Accessing One's Attitude Upon Mere Observa-
upon consumer behavior. The general suggestion tion of the Attitude Object,"JournalofPersonality and
offered by the model and the present findings is that Social Psychology,44 (April), 723-735.
the desired behavior is most likely to occur if the atti- David M. Sanbonmatsu, Martha C. Powell, and
tude is highly accessible from memory. Thus, if the Frank R. Kardes (1986), "On the Automatic Activa-
tion of Attitudes," Journal of Personality and Social
goal of advertisingis to influence purchase behavior, Psychology, 50 (February),229-238.
then one needs to be concerned not only with the va- and Carol J. Williams (1986), "Attitude Accessibil-
lence of the resultingattitude but also with its accessi- ity as a Moderatorof the Attitude-Perceptionand Atti-
bility from memory (see Herr and Fazio 1988 for a tude-BehaviorRelations: An Investigation of the 1984
more general discussion of this issue). How social in- Presidential Election," Journal of Personality and So-
fluence attempts can be designed to accomplish this cial Psychology, 51 (September),505-514.
end constitutes an important challenge for the future. and Mark P. Zanna (1978), "Attitudinal Qualities
Relatingto the Strengthof the Attitude-BehaviorRela-
tionship," Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology,
[Received August 1988. Revised May 1989.] 14 (4), 398-408.
and MarkP. Zanna (1981), "Direct Experienceand
Attitude-Behavior Consistency," in Advances in Ex-
REFERENCES perimental Social Psychology, Vol. 14, ed. Leonard
Alba, Joseph W. and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1987), "Di- Berkowitz,New York: Academic Press, 161-202.
mensions of Consumer Expertise," Journal of Con- Herr, Paul M. and Russell H. Fazio (forthcoming), "The
sumerResearch, 13 (March),411-454. Attitude-to-Behavior Process: Implications for Con-
Baker,William, J. Wesley Hutchinson, Danny Moore, and sumer Behavior," in Ad Exposure, Memory, and
Prakash Nedungadi (1986), "Brand Familiarity and Choice, ed. Andrew A. Mitchell, Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
Advertising:Effectson the Evoked Set and BrandPref- rence ErlbaumAssociates.
erence," in Advances in ConsumerResearch, Vol. 13, Houston, David A. and Russell H. Fazio (1989), "Biased
ed. Richard J. Lutz, Provo, UT: Association for Con- Processing as a Function of Attitude Accessibility:
sumer Research,637-642. Making Objective Judgments Subjectively," Social
Boyd, HarperW., Michael L. Ray, and EdwardC. Strong Cognition,7 (Spring),51-66.
(1972), "An Attitudinal Framework for Advertising Howard, John A. and JagdishN. Sheth (1969), The Theory
Strategy,"Journal of Marketing,36 (April), 27-33. ofBuyer Behavior,New York:John Wiley.
Davidson, AndrewR., Steven Yantis, MarelNorwood, and Kardes, Frank R., David M. Sanbonmatsu, Richard T.
Daniel E. Montano (1985), "Amount of Information Voss, and Russell H. Fazio (1986), "Self-Monitoring
About the Attitude Objectand Attitude-BehaviorCon- and Attitude Accessibility," Personality and Social
sistency," Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- PsychologyBulletin, 12 (December), 468-474.
ogy, 49 (November), 1184-1198. Kassarjian,Harold H. and WaltraudM. Kassarjian(1979),
Day, George S. and Terry Deutscher (1982), "Attitudinal "Attitudes Under Low Commitment Conditions," in
Predictions of Choices of Major Appliance Brands," Attitude Research Plays for High Stakes, eds. John C.
Journalof MarketingResearch, 19 (May), 192-198. Maloney and BernardSilverman, Chicago: American
Engel, James F. and Roger D. Blackwell (1982), Consumer MarketingAssociation, 3-15.
Behavior,Hinsdale, IL:Dryden. Krosnick,Jon A. (forthcoming),"AttitudeImportanceand
Fazio, Russell H. (1986), "How Do Attitudes Guide Behav- Attitude Accessibility," Personality and Social Psy-
ior?" in The Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: chologyBulletin.
Foundations of Social Behavior, eds. Richard M. Sor- Lutz, Richard J. (1975), "Changing Brand Attitudes
rentino and E. Tory Higgins, New York: Guilford, Through Modification of Cognitive Structure,"Jour-
204-243. nal of ConsumerResearch, 1 (March),49-59.
288 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Norman, Ross (1975), "Affective-CognitiveConsistency, Ryan, Michael J. and E.H. Bonfield (1975), "The Fishbein
Attitudes, Conformity, and Behavior,"Journal of Per- Extended Model and Consumer Behavior,"Journal of
sonality and Social Psychology, 32 (July), 83-91. ConsumerResearch, 2 (September), 118-136.
Page, Ellis B. (1963), "Ordered Hypotheses for Multiple Sanbonmatsu,David M. and Russell H. Fazio (1986), "The
Treatments: A Significance Test for Linear Ranks," Automatic Activation of Attitudes Toward Products,"
Journal of the American Statistical Association, 58 paper presented at the annual meeting of the Associa-
(March),216-230. tion for Consumer Research, Toronto, Ontario, Can-
Petty, RichardE., John T. Cacioppo, and David Schumann ada.
(1983), "Centraland PeripheralRoutes to Advertising
Effectiveness:The ModeratingRole of Involvement," Seigel, Sidney and N. John Castellan (1988), Nonparamet-
Journal of ConsumerResearch, 10 (September), 135- ric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, New York:
146. McGraw-Hill.
Powell, MarthaC. and Russell H. Fazio (1984), "Attitude Smith, Robert E. and William R. Swinyard(1983), "Atti-
Accessibilityas a Function of RepeatedAttitudinal Ex- tude-Behavior Consistency: The Impact of Product
pression,"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Trial Versus Advertising," Journal of Marketing Re-
10 (March), 139-148. search, 20 (August), 257-267.
Roskos-Ewoldsen,David R. and Russell H. Fazio (1989),
"Do Affectively-LadenObjectsAttractAttention?"pa- Wright, Peter (1975), "Consumer Choice Strategies:Sim-
per presentedat the annual meeting of the Midwestern plifying Vs. Optimizing," Journal of Marketing Re-
PsychologicalAssociation, Chicago. search, 12 (February),60-67.

You might also like