You are on page 1of 14

field review

Developing &
delivering effective
anti-bias training:
Challenges &
recommendations
Evelyn R. Carter, Ivuoma N. Onyeador, & Neil A. Lewis, Jr.

abstract
Organizations invest nearly $8 billion annually in diversity
training, but questions have arisen about whether training
actually reduces biased attitudes, changes behavior, and
increases diversity. In this article, we review the relevant
evidence, noting that training should be explicitly aimed at
increasing awareness of and concern about bias while at
the same time providing strategies that attendees can use
to change their behavior. After outlining five challenges to
developing and delivering training that meets these goals, we
provide evidence-based recommendations that organizations
and facilitators can use as a blueprint for creating anti-bias
training programs that work. One recommendation is to
couple investment in anti-bias training with other diversity
and inclusion initiatives to help ensure that the billions spent
each year yield meaningful change.

Carter, E. R., Onyeador, I. N., & Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2020). Developing & delivering effective
anti-bias a: Challenges & recommendations. Behavioral Science & Policy, 6(1), 57–70.
W
hat do Starbucks, Delta Airlines, and increasing diversity has produced mixed results,
the Napa Valley Wine Train have in leading many investigators to conclude that it
common? Over the past five years, in does not work.16,17 It seems likely, however, that
response to public outcries over racist incidents the inconsistent results stem from differences in
between employees and customers, all three how the training is carried out. Some programs,
have invested considerable time and money for instance, focus mainly on making people
in diversity training programs. 1–3 They are not aware of their own biases or on providing
alone: it has been estimated that organiza- attendees with a laundry list of things they
w tions invest nearly $8 billion each year in such should not do (often with little explanation as
training.4 to why their behavior should change). Others
Core Findings design situations so that attendees can experi-
The training can take different forms, but all ence firsthand what it is like to be a target of
What is the issue? programs implicitly or explicitly aim to reduce bias. Some training sessions are led by experts
Organizational diversity people’s biased attitudes and behaviors during in the subjects of diversity and inclusion,
initiatives tend to include
everyday activities in organizations, such as whereas others are led by facilitators with little
explicit anti-bias training
in the hope of alleviating when interacting with customers or colleagues expertise or experience in the domain. Finally,
harmful biases among or making hiring and promotion decisions. In whereas some training programs are bolstered
employees in the this article, the term bias refers to differences in by resounding institutional support, others are
workforce. But is this
money well spent? Recent how people are viewed and treated as a result perceived as a specialized interest of a small
research highlights the not of their individual characteristics but of corner of the organization, garnering less
need to redevelop anti-bias group features, such as gender, skin color, or support from leadership and participation by
initiatives to do more than
ethnicity. Bias can be positive for some people employees as a result. These differences could
simply create awareness of
biases or prompt defensive (conferring preference or granting privilege) affect outcomes and result in mixed findings as
reactions. Instead, anti-bias or negative (being discriminatory or otherwise to the conditions that make training succeed or
training should increase
causing disadvantage) and can influence inter- fail. Indeed, only some of the above strategies
awareness of bias and its
lasting impact, plant seeds actions between individuals or be expressed have been empirically shown to reduce bias.
that inspire sustained through policies and practices that affect one
learning, and teach skills group differently from another.5 Thus, we argue that diversity training should go
that enable attendees to
manage their biases and beyond telling people that bias exists or creating
change their behavior. Organizations have varied reasons for pouring uncomfortable experiences that are more likely
money into diversity training. After all, the to prompt defensiveness than learning. Rather,
How can you act? well-documented consequences of bias in the most effective training is anti-bias training
Selected recommendations
include:
companies are far-reaching. Bias can harm the that is designed to increase awareness of bias
1) Including training as mental and physical health of employees who and its lasting impact, plant seeds that inspire
part of a broader diversity experience it,6–8 interfere with their perfor- sustained learning, and teach skills that enable
and inclusion strategy
mance and engagement,9,10 and undermine attendees to manage their biases and change
2) Prioritizing the
learning needs of the their professional development and promo- their behavior. Although a dearth of consistent
primary audience over tion.11,12 Bias also undercuts efforts to increase evidence of success has led many to conclude
positive reviews inclusion and diversity in who gets hired and fills that training does not work, 16,17 empirical
management positions. Conversely, employees research exists that can provide a blueprint for
Who should take
view companies that explicitly commit to how to build a training program that does.
the lead?
Researchers and recognizing and celebrating diversity as more
organizational leaders trustworthy.13 Further, what is often called the In this article, we explain the logic behind our
“business case” for diversity holds that reducing argument, present five challenges to devel-
bias and increasing diversity has the potential oping and delivering effective anti-bias training,
to increase profitability for companies, improve and offer evidence-based recommendations for
their reputation, and limit internal and external how to overcome those challenges (see Table
liability.14,15 1 for a summary). We acknowledge, however,
that it is not easy to develop a single training
The problem is that research on how well diver- program that will reduce the bias that affects
sity training succeeds in reducing bias and all of the myriad groups in an organization.

58 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020


Table 1. Summary of challenges, recommendations, & implementation agents
Challenge Recommendation Implementation Agent

Being realistic about what training can Develop a comprehensive diversity and Organizational executives responsible for
accomplish on its own inclusion strategy with training as one strategic planning initiatives
component. Determine which goals
require alternative strategies.

Selecting the proper goals for the Tailor training to match the desired Organizational executives who choose
programs outcomes for individuals, groups, and the goal; facilitator who tailors training
the organization. Focus on increasing accordingly
awareness of bias and changing behavior.

Deciding how to manage attendee Do not let discomfort dissuade the Training facilitator
discomfort organization from pursuing training;
discomfort is an important part of learning
how to communicate across differences.

Minimizing counterproductive effects of Teach attendees concrete strategies for Training facilitator
discussing bias managing their bias, but do not overdo it:
emphasize just two or three.

Demonstrating impact Develop and execute a plan for evaluating Organizational executives and training
the efficacy of training. facilitator

Because bias and stereotypes can take many of 17 different bias-reduction interventions
forms,18 a training program that helps to reduce found that only eight reduced participants’
bias against one group may not necessarily be implicit preference for White people over
effective to the same degree for another. For Black people. 20 Further, the effects of even the
instance, whereas diversity efforts that high- most effective interventions (such as exposing
light group differences improve conditions for participants to people from another group who
groups present in moderate numbers, such as behave counter to stereotypes and providing
White women, efforts that emphasize equal people with strategies they can use to mitigate
treatment and minimize the salience of group bias) had worn off just 24 hours later. 21 This
differences improve conditions for groups with analysis and other research suggest that without
low representation, such as Black people.19 We consistent reinforcement, trainees’ biases will
also note the need for more research to under- rebound after people return to the environ-
stand whether a training program built using our ments that reinforce those biases. 22
recommendations would reduce bias against
understudied groups, such as people who are For example, after attending an anti-bias training
disabled or obese. session, a hiring manager may be excited about
implementing new bias-management strate-
gies. Yet this manager must balance the goal of
Challenge 1: Being Realistic recruiting and offering jobs to a diverse group
About What Training Can of candidates with the competing need to fill
Accomplish on Its Own open positions swiftly. If the company has not
Because of budgetary and time considerations, invested in targeted recruiting of candidates
diversity training is often offered as a one-time from historically underrepresented groups or
opportunity. Yet a company that relies solely has not reduced the urgency of filling a partic-
on a single training session to combat bias is ular position, the newfound knowledge and
doomed to fail. One major reason is that bias motivation to counteract bias will not be enough
is multifaceted, born of a combination of an to combat the pressing need to fill the position
individual’s exposure to stereotypes about and and stiffen the manager’s resolve to hold out for
direct or indirect experiences with people from a qualified person who also adds to the compa-
different groups. This complexity makes bias ny’s diversity. 23 Indeed, research suggests that
difficult to eradicate. Indeed, an investigation accountability structures produce better results

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 59


“organizations cannot rely on employees’ goodwill alone to
change the culture”
than anti-bias training by itself. 24 (Accountability can undermine an organization’s diversity and
structures consist of programs, staff positions, inclusion efforts. Moreover, organizations that
or groups that have explicit responsibility for use a comprehensive strategy communicate
meeting such goals as increasing the represen- the message that bias is malleable and can
tation of minorities.) In other words, although be changed over time, which can encourage
anti-bias training can be a great way to raise individuals to adopt bias-­ m anagement
awareness of bias and provide people with strat- strategies.32,33
egies to fight it, training alone cannot guarantee
change once attendees leave a session. Organizations with the goal of integrating
training into their overall diversity and inclusion
Recommendation: Include Training as Part strategy could first collect data to understand
of a Broader Diversity & Inclusion Strategy the representation and dispersion of people
Organizations must ensure that training is from different groups throughout the organiza-
accompanied by investments in structural tion, employees’ perceptions of inclusion, and
changes—alterations in organizational policies where diversity-related failures in processes
and ways of operating—that will help sustain might be occurring (such as during the hiring
learning and facilitate behavioral change. 25,26 process or during performance reviews). The
Why are structural changes imperative even data would help to identify which training
within an organization that is full of employees topics would have the greatest impact on the
personally motivated to be egalitarian? One organization’s employees. The organization
reason is that prejudice is facilitated by the daily could then create a plan in which training for
consumption of information that confirms and employees and leaders is one component,
reifies stereotypes. 27,28 Another is that, as is illus- alongside the establishment of a committee
trated by the hiring manager example above, responsible for overseeing and reporting on
many factors compete with an individual’s progress. After training, organizations could
motivation to prioritize diversity, manage bias, provide suggestions for additional reading to
and promote inclusion. Combating prejudice attendees to further their learning and estab-
requires similarly multifaceted efforts: organiza- lish behavioral nudges before key decisions to
tions cannot rely on employees’ goodwill alone remind attendees of the strategies to which
to change the culture. 29,30 they committed. 34 Such nudges could include
reminding managers to avoid giving person-
A meta-analysis of 260 studies of anti-bias ality feedback before performance reviews
training highlights the importance of going are written or asking recruiters to reflect on
beyond having a training program on its own. It key job requirements before they discuss their
found that training was more effective when it assessments of job candidates. In these ways,
was integrated into a broader strategy (g = 0.57; organizations can ensure the training lessons
see note A for more information on the statis- extend beyond the session itself and influence
tics) than when it was provided as a stand-alone employees’ everyday behavior.
program (g = 0.36). 31 An integrated strategy
might introduce anti-bias training along with
systems that hold individuals responsible for Challenge 2: Selecting the
reducing bias, improve processes for responding Proper Goals for the Programs
to bias incidents, and provide networking Researchers—such as Frank Dobbin and his
opportunities for employees from underrepre- colleagues—have analyzed a combination of
sented groups. Training is more effective in this laboratory experiments and organizational data
broader context in part because the integrated and concluded that diversity training is ineffec-
strategy addresses the variety of ways that bias tive. 35 For example, Dobbin and his coauthors

60 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020


examined how six different diversity-related such as where bias comes from and how it
initiatives (training, audits, networking programs, contributes to inequity in society), behavioral
mentoring programs, a task force, and a desig- learning (skill development, as judged by self-­
nated diversity officer) affected manager-level reports, observations of managers or trainers, or
diversity in more than 800 companies. Mento- responses to hypothetical scenarios), and attitu-
ring programs and initiatives that provided dinal/affective learning (self-reported attitudes
organizational oversight for diversity through an toward stigmatized group members and beliefs
individual or a task force increased management in one’s own ability to become less biased). 31
diversity by nearly 40%. In contrast, the impact Overall, training had the largest immediate
of training was generally negligible and for some impact on generating positive attendee reac-
groups was counterproductive, contributing to a tions toward the facilitator and the training itself
5% decrease in representation for Black women (g = 0.61). It also increased, to varying degrees,
and a 5%–10% increase for Black men and awareness of bias (cognitive learning; g = 0.57),
Hispanic women. The authors concluded that skill at behaving in less-biased ways (behav-
training is not effective for increasing manager ioral learning; g = 0.48), and positive feelings
diversity. However, an alternative conclusion is toward groups that were experiencing discrim-
not necessarily that anti-bias training is ineffec- ination (attitudinal/affective learning; g = 0.30).
tive but rather that it is not a panacea. Over time, however, the effects decayed for all
outcomes except awareness of bias (cognitive
Many factors influence employee diversity, learning). Thus, one outcome that anti-bias
promotion rates, and other outcomes. Consider training can reliably affect in the long term is
the goal of increasing manager-level diversity. what attendees learn and retain about bias.
If White employees have more opportunities
to lead high-visibility projects and have greater If awareness of bias is the main outcome that
access to key decisionmakers than employees persists, should organizations even bother trying
of color do, this imbalance will necessarily to change behavior as well? Based on the data,
affect the pipeline of qualified employees who the short answer is yes. In a separate analysis,
can become managers. 36 Training cannot fix Bezrukova and her colleagues asked what works
these disparities. Similarly, anti-bias training better for changing attitudes and behavior:
cannot promise comprehensive change that diversity training that aims to increase attendees’
will eliminate all bias that occurs during perfor- awareness of their biases and cultural assump-
mance reviews37 or all overt or subtle biases in tions (awareness-based training), programs that
other interactions. 38–40 Instead, it is best posi- help attendees learn to monitor and change
tioned to achieve more modest and immediate their behavior (behavior-based training), or
outcomes, such as educating people about a combination of the two?31 They found that
how biases can manifest or motivating people focusing on awareness is useful but should not
to change their behavior. be the only focus of training. Awareness-based
training produced the smallest changes in atti-
Recommendation: Focus Training on Both tudes and behavior overall (gs = 0.22 and 0.35,
Increasing Awareness of Bias & Providing respectively), whereas behavior-based training
Strategies for Changing Behavior was significantly more effective at changing
Some researchers suggest that the closest both attitudes (g = 0.41) and behavior (g = 0.53).
analog of training is teaching and that organiza- The training programs that incorporated both
tions considering diversity training should first awareness-based and behavior-based elements
identify the learning outcomes of interest.41 In were about as effective as behavior-based
the meta-analysis mentioned above, Katerina training at changing attitudes (g = 0.40) and
Bezrukova and colleagues identified four behavior (g = 0.54). In summary, anti-bias
potential kinds of outcomes from diversity training is least effective when it focuses only
training: attendee reactions (self-reported feel- on raising awareness of bias: the best strategy
ings toward the facilitator or training), cognitive is either to focus on teaching attendees strate-
learning (knowledge gained about the topic, gies for changing biased behavior or to do that

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 61


and also incorporate elements that will raise to provide them with foundational knowledge
people’s awareness of their bias and the effects about the prevalence of bias before diving into
it can have.42 We favor the combined approach strategies for mitigating it. Other attendees may
because it can help people to understand why be eager for more complex discussions, looking
they should want to change their behavior. to anti-bias training as an opportunity to validate
their experiences with bias and to discuss mean-
ingful changes their organization can enact.
Challenge 3: Deciding How to These dynamics make it challenging to design a
Manage Attendee Discomfort training program that all attendees will react to
A critical component of anti-bias training is positively. It is reasonable and understandable
discussing disparities between groups. These that organizational leaders want employees to
conversations can be uncomfortable. For have good reactions to training programs, but
example, people from racial minority and this goal should not be pursued at the expense
majority groups both find discussions about of other learning outcomes.
race to be challenging and thus may avoid
intergroup conversations about race to prevent Recommendation: Prioritize the
the accompanying anxiety and discomfort.43–46 Learning Needs of the Primary
Although avoidance certainly eases strain Audience, Not Positive Reviews
between people in the short term, avoidance As part of establishing the desired learning
is not always possible during anti-bias training outcomes for anti-bias training, organizations
and, more generally, when working in a diverse must identify the primary audience for any
organization. During training, some attendees particular anti-bias training session. Potential
$8 billion is spent on
may become defensive and belittle the training targets of bias will undoubtedly be in the audi-
diversity training annually
content or even try to undermine the facilitator ence of any training session, and their needs
in an effort to deflect negative emotions.47,48 may be very different from those of attendees
Facilitators must be prepared to contend who are potential perpetrators of bias. For
with the various reactions that emerge during example, people of color learn about racism and
training. talk about racism at a younger age than White
individuals do.55,56 Thus, a discussion of the exis-
Facilitators must also navigate the tricky tence of bias and the importance of managing
50% of US organizations
are expected to offer dynamics related to differences in how people it may fall flat for people of color, who instead
implicit bias training from various groups perceive the prevalence may gain more from learning about how to
in the future
of bias and in their willingness to engage in cope with the bias they face or from discussing
the training activities. White participants, for organizational procedures for reporting discrim-
instance, may believe that racism is less prev- ination. Although one training session cannot
alent in modern society than Black participants address all these topics effectively, a scaffolded
do.49,50 Also, recognizing one’s own bias is approach to learning—through a series of work-
distressing, particularly for majority-group shops, comprehensive diversity and inclusion
members who do not want to appear preju- efforts (as described in Challenge 1), or both—
diced. 51,52 Some attendees may be reluctant can ensure that everyone within an organization
to acknowledge and accept that they have gets the education and support they need.
biases that affect their behavior, or they may
be actively hostile to the very notion of anti- After the identity of the primary audience has
bias training.53,54 Other attendees may generally been established, the next question to consider
agree that there is a need to improve organiza- is the potential reactions that attendees will
tional inclusion and reduce bias. have to training. Some facilitators will prioritize
designing training programs that attendees will
These divergent perceptions and attitudes can, evaluate positively over training programs that
in turn, produce different expectations for what achieve learning outcomes. Several studies’
anti-bias training should cover. Some attendees findings suggest, however, that this impulse
may prefer to have an introductory conversation is misguided. Certainly, the perception of too

62 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020


“a moderate amount of discomfort is a critical catalyst for the
introspection that can guide a person toward more egalitarian
behavior in the future”
much threat can activate defensiveness that (gs = 0.71 and 0.37, respectively). However, this
stifles learning and growth. 57 Yet research has approach undermines other outcomes that may
shown that a moderate amount of discomfort is be important for long-term change, such as
a critical catalyst for the introspection that can behavioral learning.31 Specifically, when training
guide a person toward more egalitarian behavior is voluntary, behavioral learning is significantly
in the future. 58 This research aligns with other lower compared with when training is manda-
evidence that showing people how bias has tory (gs = 0.42 and 0.63, respectively), perhaps
influenced their decisions can reduce subse- because those who could benefit most from the
quent discrimination59 and increase sensitivity to training avoid attendance.62,63 Moreover, making
subtle forms of discrimination.60 When facilita- training mandatory is a simple way to demon-
tors and organizations make winning audience strate that anti-bias training and, more broadly,
approval a key outcome, they risk neglecting diversity and inclusion efforts are important to
content that may produce long-lasting learning the organization.
(that is, ongoing sensitivity to the occurrence of
bias) and behavioral change.
Challenge 4: Minimizing
Another reason to prioritize attendees’ learning Counterproductive Effects
needs over their immediate reactions is that of Discussing Bias
attendees’ positive or negative perceptions of A major goal of anti-bias training is to increase
training do not necessarily relate to whether awareness of the ways that bias manifests in
they learn the concepts presented during the society, organizations, and individuals. However,
experience.61 For instance, attendees react several scholars have identified unexpected,
more positively to training that includes a often ironic effects that sometimes result from
variety of components (such as interactive attempts to teach about and address bias. For
discussion, video, and lecture-based content; example, people more readily dismiss claims of
g = 0.73) than to training that relies on one workplace discrimination in organizations that
component (g = 0.59).31 Despite this preference, explicitly value diversity compared with claims
training programs with many components, as in organizations that do not.64 Communicating
compared with those with one component, do that the solution to bias is simple and can be
not produce statistically significant differences addressed by just offering a training course can
in behavioral learning (many: g = 0.51; one: decrease empathy for victims of bias.65 Addi-
g = 0.39), cognitive learning (many: g = 0.54; tionally, referring to bias as ubiquitous may,
one: g = 0.67), or attitudinal/affective learning ironically, produce the perception that bias
(many: g = 0.30; one: g = 0.27). Thus, although is acceptable (for instance, “It must be OK if
audiences may prefer training programs with everyone has it”)66 or that bias-reduction efforts
multiple components to training programs with are futile (for instance, “You can’t succeed
one component, the multifaceted approach because bias is too widespread to root out”).
does not yield meaningful change on any
learning outcomes. Beyond these challenges, the findings of
recent research complicate notions of how
Finally, organizations often make training volun- to talk about implicit bias (sometimes called
tary rather than mandatory to generate positive unconscious bias) in particular. An estimated
audience reaction. In some ways, this strategy is 20% of U.S. organizations offer training meant
effective: overall, people respond more favor- to combat implicit bias, and the number is
ably to voluntary versus mandatory training expected to increase to 50% in the near future.4

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 63


Although this training can be effective,67 the two to three recommendations emerged as the
way that implicit bias is framed can significantly ideal number for prompting change (d = 0.27),
affect people’s motivation to challenge the particularly for participants who were less moti-
discrimination that results from it. In a series vated to alter their ways. Throughout a training
of studies, participants read a definition that program, facilitators may suggest a broad
framed racial bias as “implicit and unconscious” range of strategies to manage and reduce bias;
or “explicit and conscious” before reading about however, by the end, facilitators who want to
a discriminatory incident. Those who read about ensure that their attendees change their behav-
implicit racial bias perceived less intent in the iors should encourage them to focus on two or
incident, and they therefore perceived the inci- three behaviors.
dent as less harmful and the perpetrator as less
worthy of blame and punishment.68,69 Because
the very framing of anti-bias training content Challenge 5:
can undermine the critical understanding Demonstrating Impact
of bias necessary for motivating behavioral Clearly, it is critical that facilitators put substan-
change, facilitators must pay close attention to tive thought into the framing and content of
their framing. anti-bias training. However, this effort is for
naught if the impact of training is not evaluated.
Recommendation: Help Attendees Yet barriers can interfere with such evaluation.
Create a Plan for Behavioral Change For instance, although collecting data on the
When building anti-bias training with the goal efficacy of training is a crucial step in assess-
of circumventing the ironic consequences of ment, accessing organizational data, which
talking about bias, facilitators would do well to could show the impact that training has on key
consider classic social psychological research outcomes, can be challenging. In addition, if
on persuasion. Persuasion is often most participation in postworkshop surveys is low,
successful when people are presented with a the paucity of data can undermine the ability to
moderately disturbing outcome and strategies draw statistically informative conclusions, and a
they can use to avoid that outcome.70 Indeed, focus on unrealistic outcomes (as discussed in
anti-bias training is a persuasive endeavor, Challenge 2) can give the misleading impression
designed to present attendees with information that nothing was achieved.
that will motivate them to change their atti-
tudes and behavior. Facilitators must go beyond Unfortunately, a lack of data showing that
relaying information about what bias is and anti-bias training is effective can lead people
propose concrete bias-management strategies to perceive anti-bias training as ineffective.
so that attendees believe they have the ability to Perhaps worse, without data demonstrating a
enact the new behaviors.71–73 return on an organization’s investment in anti-
bias training, organizational leaders will be
Although myriad strategies exist for managing skeptical when they receive future requests to
and reducing bias, facilitators should be careful fund diversity and inclusion initiatives. Without
not to overwhelm attendees with too many of data to point to, facilitators—and the organiza-
them. General research on interventions has tional representatives who hire them—will be
compared the effect of different numbers of ill-equipped to make a strong case for why the
recommendations on behavioral change.74 An training is necessary and worthwhile.
interesting finding was that interventions that
provided one recommendation had a low impact Recommendation: Measure Efficacy
on behavior (d = 0.12), but so did interventions Any plan for delivering training should, from
that provided four or more recommendations the outset, also include a plan for assessing
(d = 0.14). (See note A for a discussion of d how well the program’s goals are met.75 The
statistics.) Giving just one recommendation program goals, of course, should be estab-
suggested the problem was not important, but lished at the beginning of the collaboration
four or more overwhelmed participants. Instead, between the organization and the facilitator to

64 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020


ensure that the objectives of both align and that
the content included in the training serves the
“The field of anti-bias training
selected goals. When deciding on the outcomes
of interest, organizations can also consider what presents a ripe opportunity
their comprehensive diversity and inclusion
strategy should be (as discussed in Challenge 1). for collaboration between
Case studies from a variety of industries provide researchers and practitioners”
insight into what anti-bias training can poten-
tially accomplish,67,76 and empirical evaluations over time. Facilitators and organizations who
conducted once training was complete have want to improve outcomes should follow the
bolstered these findings. For example, The Ohio evidence-based recommendations presented
State University College of Medicine identified a in this article for raising awareness of bias and
goal of increasing the diversity of its incoming sparking a desire to behave in ways that manage
class of medical students. The college devel- it. Nevertheless, anti-bias training cannot and
oped a plan to have admissions committee should not be viewed as a singular opportunity
members complete anti-bias training; the plan to educate and change people. Rather, it must
included provisions for data collection at the be integrated into efforts to meet a broader
end so that the effectiveness of the training commitment to improving diversity and inclu-
could be evaluated. Per the plan, the college’s sion. Finally, every organization implementing
admissions committee members took an these recommendations should measure the
Implicit Association Test and attended a training impact of what it has tried and adjust accord-
session about how implicit bias affects deci- ingly, taking into account the specific context,
sionmaking.77 During the next admission cycle, audience, and desired outcomes.
the college saw a 26% increase in the number
of underrepresented minority students who A Call to Continued Action
matriculated to the institution, compared with for Researchers
the admissions cycle from the previous year. The future success of anti-bias training turns on
This difference was not statistically significant; whether researchers use their skills and expe-
nonetheless, qualitative responses from an eval- rience in program development and evaluation
uation completed by committee members gave to build strong training programs. This call for
some insight into the way the training affected researchers to engage in translational research
them. Specifically, they described being more is not new: over a decade ago, Elizabeth Paluck
cognizant of how bias influenced their reactions cautioned that “by and large, scholars and
to candidates during the interview process. By practitioners have passed up the opportunity
identifying the pretraining goal of increasing for a collaborative project that could harness
the diversity of the incoming class of medical this widespread intervention to improve the
students, the college was able to develop a theory and practice of prejudice reduction and
training approach that positively shaped the social inclusion.”78 The field of anti-bias training
behavior of the admissions committee and that presents a ripe opportunity for collaboration
also included a plan to collect the data needed between researchers and practitioners. We echo
to assess outcomes in the months that followed. Paluck’s recommendations for how to achieve
this collaboration, which should begin by (a)
establishing that anti-bias training can cause
Discussion attitude and behavioral changes, (b) measuring
Social scientists have amassed extensive knowl- efficacy in ways that go beyond self-report,
edge about the features that are important for and (c) conducting research in a variety of
effective anti-bias training. However, the prolif- populations and settings to gain a deeper
eration of training and the lack of standardization understanding of how to make training most
in its delivery undermine the ability to measure effective. Moreover, researchers must continue
the efficacy of training across organizations and to explore the various determinants of bias18,79

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 65


and use these insights to identify bias-reduction rooted in a long-standing hierarchy and main-
tactics that are specific to certain groups80 as tained by present-day beliefs and practices.82,83
well as tactics that have a broader reach.81 These opportunities to reflect on and discuss
bias can influence whether and how members
A Call to Action for Organizations of majority groups notice and make meaning
Seeking Anti-Bias Training of the experiences of members of stigmatized
People who want to bring anti-bias training groups. 49,84–87 Anti-bias-training facilitators
to their organization may be overwhelmed by therefore carry a great responsibility, and those
the task of establishing criteria that will iden- interested in entering this field should take this
tify, from the wealth of facilitators available, the role seriously.
one(s) best suited for their particular group and
situation. With that in mind, we recommend that Merely having a passion for diversity and inclu-
they consider the following factors, regardless sion does not make a person an effective
of whether they plan to hire an outside facili- facilitator. Navigating the challenges described
tator or use internal resources. in this article, especially those related to the
unpredictable and sometimes ironic conse-
The first factor is organizational readiness. It is quences of discussing bias, requires a deep
not worth pursuing anti-bias training if the orga- understanding of the research on bias, diversity,
nization lacks a true commitment to increasing and inclusion as well as experience facilitating
diversity and fostering inclusion. Does your training with a variety of audiences. In an early
organization have a diversity and inclusion version of this article, a reviewer commented
strategy, of which training is one component? that the challenges and recommendations for
Conversely, are there people in the organiza- facilitators had a Goldilocks feel to them: Do
tion who will undermine the efficacy of training, not do too much, do not do too little; present
such as resistant leaders or employees who enough information to motivate people but not
are openly antagonistic to diversity and inclu- so much that you overwhelm them. Walking
sion efforts? If resistance is an issue, company that line is very difficult. And yet it is acceptable,
resources may be better spent by first investing even appropriate, that there be a high bar of
in structural changes to policy and practices entry for facilitating these training programs. No
that can lower the resistance. one expects people to practice medicine or law
after reading a few articles. Similarly, it does and
Leaders should also carefully consider facilitator should require significant time and energy to
experience, including subject matter expertise learn to become an effective anti-bias-training
and experience managing the myriad audience facilitator.
reactions to training (described in Challenge
3). Finally, any person who advocates for anti-
bias training within an organization must have Conclusion
a plan for measuring the impact. Identifying The recommendations in this article should
the desired key outcomes for learning and guide the next decade—and beyond—of anti-
subsequent change will likewise be important bias training. As systemic inequities and biases
when partnering with the facilitator to build become more entrenched, it is not reason-
the training. able to expect a transformation to come from
training alone. However, a well-designed
A Call to Action for Anti-Bias- training program can be a catalyst that produces
Training Facilitators ripple effects within an organization, a commu-
Bias can seem abstract, and anti-bias-training nity, and beyond. Researchers and facilitators
facilitators have the potential to make the should come together to share insights on ways
importance of managing and reducing bias real to make anti-bias training as effective as it can
for attendees. Facilitators have an opportunity be and then use those insights to create a less
to emphasize how biases in our society are biased, more equitable world.

66 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020


endnote author affiliation
A. From the editors to nonscientists: For any given
data set, the statistical test used depends on the Carter: Paradigm Strategy Inc. Onyeador: Yale
number of data points and the type of measure- University and Kellogg School of Management.
ment being used, such as proportions or means. Lewis: Cornell University and Weill Cornell
Hedges’s g is a measure of effect size, with inter- Medical College. Corresponding author’s
pretation as follows: g = 0.20 is considered a small
e-mail: evelyn.carter10@gmail.com.
effect, g = 0.50 is considered a medium effect,
and g = 0.80 is considered a large effect. Cohen’s
d also measures effect size. Typically, d = 0.2 is
author note
small, d = 0.5 is medium, and d = 0.8 is large.
This research was supported by National
Science Foundation Social, Behavioral, and
Economic Sciences Postdoctoral Research
Fellowship 1809370 to Ivuoma N. Onyeador.

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 67


references
1. Bhattacharjee, R. (2015, August 25). 11. Hewlett, S. A. (2014, January 22). 21. Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E.,
‘We were 100 percent in the wrong’: Cracking the code that stalls people Murrar, S., Brauer, M., Devos, T., . . .
Napa Wine Train exec apologizes to of color. Harvard Business Review. Nosek, B. A. (2016). Reducing implicit
Black women thrown off train for Retrieved from https://hbr.org/2014/01/ racial preferences: II. Intervention
laughing loudly. Retrieved from https:// cracking-the-code-that-stalls- effectiveness across time. Journal of
www.nbcbayarea.com/news/local/ multicultural-professionals Experimental Psychology: General, 145,
We-Were-100-in-the-Wrong-Napa- 1001–1016.
12. Zenger, J., & Folkman, J. (2019, June
Wine-Train-Exec-Apologizes-to- 25). Research: Women score higher 22. Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg,
Black-Women-Thrown-Off-Train-for- than men in most leadership skills. K. B. (2017). The bias of crowds: How
Laughing-Loudly-322841941.html Harvard Business Review. Retrieved implicit bias bridges personal and
2. Shen, L. (2017, January 18). Delta from https://hbr.org/2019/06/ systemic prejudice. Psychological
adds diversity training for 23,000 research-women-score-higher-than- Inquiry, 28, 233–248.
crew members. Fortune. Retrieved men-in-most-leadership-skills 23. Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (2011). The
from https://fortune.com/2017/01/18/ 13. Purdie-Vaughns, V., Steele, C. M., person and the situation: Perspectives
delta-diversity-training/ Davies, P. G., Ditlmann, R., & Crosby, J. of social psychology. London, England:
3. Starbucks. (2018). The third place: R. (2008). Social identity contingencies: Pinter & Martin.
Our commitment renewed. Retrieved How diversity cues signal threat 24. Kalev, A., Dobbin, F., & Kelly, E. (2006).
from https://stories.starbucks.com/ or safety for African Americans in Best practices or best guesses?
stories/2018/thethirdplace/ mainstream institutions. Journal of Assessing the efficacy of corporate
Personality and Social Psychology, 94,
4. Lipman, J. (2018, January 25). How affirmative action and diversity policies.
615–630.
diversity training infuriates men and fails American Sociological Review, 71,
women. Time. Retrieved from http:// 14. Bregman, P. (2012, March 12). 589–617.
time.com/5118035/diversity-training- Diversity training doesn’t work. 25. Paluck, E. L., & Green, D. P. (2009).
infuriates-men-fails-women/ Harvard Business Review. Retrieved Prejudice reduction: What works? A
from https://hbr.org/2012/03/
5. Adams, G., Edkins, V., Lacka, D., Pickett, critical look at evidence from the field
diversity-training-doesnt-work
K. M., & Cheryan, S. (2008). Teaching and the laboratory. Annual Review of
about racism: Pernicious implications 15. Eswaran, V. (2019, April 29). The Psychology, 60, 339–367.
of the standard portrayal. Basic and business case for diversity in the 26. Paluck, E. L. (2012). Interventions
Applied Social Psychology, 30, 349–361. workplace is now overwhelming. aimed at the reduction of prejudice
Retrieved from https://www.weforum.
6. Adam, E. K., Heissel, J. A., Zeiders, and conflict. In L. R. Tropp (Ed.), Oxford
org/agenda/2019/04/business-case-
K. H., Richeson, J. A., Ross, E. handbook of intergroup conflict
for-diversity-in-the-workplace/
C., Ehrlich, K. B., . . . Eccles, J. S. (pp. 179–192). New York, NY: Oxford
(2015). Developmental histories of 16. Belluz, J. (2018, May 29). Companies University Press.
perceived racial discrimination and like Starbucks love anti-bias 27. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes
diurnal cortisol profiles in adulthood: training. But it doesn’t work—and and prejudice: Their automatic and
A 20-year prospective study. may backfire. Retrieved from controlled components. Journal of
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, https://www.vox.com/science- Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
279–291. and-health/2018/4/19/17251752/ 5–18.
philadelphia-starbucks-arrest-racial-
7. Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V.
bias-training 28. Weisbuch, M., Pauker, K., & Ambady,
R., & Williams, D. R. (1999). Racism N. (2009, December 18). The subtle
as a stressor for African Americans: 17. Dobbin, F., & Kalev, A. (2018). Why transmission of race bias via televised
A biopsychosocial model. American doesn’t diversity training work? The nonverbal behavior. Science, 326,
Psychologist, 54, 805–816. challenge for industry and academia. 1711–1714.
Anthropology Now, 10(2), 48–55.
8. Jones, K. P., Peddie, C. I., Gilrane, V. L., 29. Chang, E. H., Milkman, K. L., Gromet,
King, E. B., & Gray, A. L. (2016). Not so 18. Zou, L. X., & Cheryan, S. (2017). Two D. M., Rebele, R. W., Massey, C.,
subtle: A meta-analytic investigation axes of subordination: A new model of Duckworth, A. L., & Grant, A. M. (2019).
of the correlates of subtle and overt racial position. Journal of Personality The mixed effects of online diversity
discrimination. Journal of Management, and Social Psychology, 112, 696–717. training. Proceedings of the National
42, 1588–1613. 19. Apfelbaum, E. P., Stephens, N. M., & Academy of Sciences, USA, 116,
9. Levy, D. J., Heissel, J. A., Richeson, J. Reagans, R. E. (2016). Beyond one-size- 7778–7783.
A., & Adam, E. K. (2016). Psychological fits-all: Tailoring diversity approaches 30. Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin,
and biological responses to race-based to the representation of social groups. A. J., & Cox, W. T. (2012). Long-term
social stress as pathways to disparities Journal of Personality and Social reduction in implicit race bias: A
in educational outcomes. American Psychology, 111, 547–568. prejudice habit-breaking intervention.
Psychologist, 71, 455–473. 20. Lai, C. K., Marini, M., Lehr, S. A., Cerruti, Journal of Experimental Social
10. Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, C., Shin, J. L., Joy-Gaba, J. A., . . . Psychology, 48, 1267–1278.
J. (2002). Contending with group Nosek, B. A. (2014). Reducing implicit 31. Bezrukova, K., Spell, C. S., Perry, J. L.,
image: The psychology of stereotype racial preferences: I. A comparative & Jehn, K. A. (2016). A meta-analytical
and social identity threat. In M. P. Zanna investigation of 17 interventions. Journal integration of over 40 years of research
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social of Experimental Psychology: General, on diversity training evaluation.
psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 379–440). San 143, 1765–1785. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 1227–1274.
Diego, CA: Academic Press.

68 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020


32. Carr, P. B., Dweck, C. S., & Pauker, K. Costs, consequences, and possibilities. 55. Brown, T. N., Tanner-Smith, E. E.,
(2012). “Prejudiced” behavior without Current Directions in Psychological Lesane-Brown, C. L., & Ezell, M. E.
prejudice? Beliefs about the malleability Science, 16, 316–320. (2007). Child, parent, and situational
of prejudice affect interracial correlates of familial ethnic/race
44. Schultz, J. R., Gaither, S. E., Urry, H. L., &
interactions. Journal of Personality and socialization. Journal of Marriage and
Maddox, K. B. (2015). Reframing anxiety
Social Psychology, 103, 452–471. Family, 69, 14–25.
to encourage interracial interactions.
33. Neel, R., & Shapiro, J. R. (2012). Is racial Translational Issues in Psychological 56. Hughes, D., Rodriguez, J., Smith, E.
bias malleable? Whites’ lay theories of Science, 1, 392–400. P., Johnson, D. J., Stevenson, H. C.,
racial bias predict divergent strategies & Spicer, P. (2006). Parents’ ethnic-
45. Trawalter, S., & Richeson, J. A. (2006).
for interracial interactions. Journal of racial socialization practices: A review
Regulatory focus and executive function
Personality and Social Psychology, 103, of research and directions for future
after interracial interactions. Journal
101–120. study. Developmental Psychology, 42,
of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,
747–770.
34. re:Work Editors (2017, August 7). Use 406–412.
nudges to make your organization 57. Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna,
46. Trawalter, S., & Richeson, J. A. (2008).
better [Blog post]. Retrieved from M. P. (2003). I love me . . . I love me
Let’s talk about race, baby! When
https://rework.withgoogle.com/ not: Implicit self-esteem, explicit
Whites’ and Blacks’ interracial contact
blog/use-nudges-to-make-your- self-esteem, and defensiveness. In
experiences diverge. Journal of
organization-better/ S. J. Spencer, S. Fein, M. P. Zanna, &
Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
J. M. Olson (Eds.), Motivated social
35. Dobbin, F., Kalev, A., & Kelly, E. (2007). 1214–1217.
perception: The Ontario symposium
Diversity management in corporate 47. Brannon, T. N., Carter, E. R., Murdock- (Vol. 9, pp. 117–145). Mahwah, NJ:
America. Contexts, 6(4), 21–27. Perriera, L. A., & Higginbotham, G. Erlbaum.
36. Wilkins, D. B., & Gulati, G. M. (1996). D. (2018). From backlash to inclusion
58. Monteith, M. J., Lybarger, J. E., &
Why are there so few Black lawyers in for all: Instituting diversity efforts to
Woodcock, A. (2009). Schooling
corporate law firms? An institutional maximize benefits across group lines.
the cognitive monster: The role of
analysis. California Law Review, 84, Social Issues and Policy Review, 12,
motivation in the regulation and control
493–625. 57–90.
of prejudice. Social and Personality
37. Snyder, K. (2014, August 26). The 48. DiAngelo, R. (2011). White fragility. Psychology Compass, 3, 211–226.
abrasiveness trap: High-achieving men The International Journal of Critical
59. Son Hing, L. S., Bobocel, D. R., &
and women are described differently Pedagogy, 3(3), 54–70.
Zanna, M. P. (2002). Meritocracy
in reviews. Fortune. Retrieved from 49. Carter, E. R., & Murphy, M. C. (2015). and opposition to affirmative action:
https://fortune.com/2014/08/26/ Group-based differences in perceptions Making concessions in the face of
performance-review-gender-bias/ of racism: What counts, to whom, and discrimination. Journal of Personality
38. McKinsey & Company. (2018). Women why? Social and Personality Psychology and Social Psychology, 83, 493–509.
in the workplace 2018. Retrieved Compass, 9, 269–280.
60. Perry, S. P., Murphy, M. C., & Dovidio,
from https://wiw-assets-production. 50. Norton, M. I., & Sommers, S. R. (2011). J. F. (2015). Modern prejudice: Subtle,
s3.us-east-1.amazonaws.com/pdfs/ Whites see racism as a zero-sum game but unconscious? The role of bias
women_in_the_workplace_2018_print. that they are now losing. Perspectives awareness in Whites’ perceptions of
pdf on Psychological Science, 6, 215–218. personal and others’ biases. Journal
39. Ragins, B. R., Singh, R., & Cornwell, J. M. of Experimental Social Psychology, 61,
51. Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A
(2007). Making the invisible visible: Fear 64–78.
justification-suppression model of the
and disclosure of sexual orientation at expression and experience of prejudice. 61. Yan, V. X., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A.
work. Journal of Applied Psychology, Psychological Bulletin, 129, 414–446. (2016). On the difficulty of mending
92, 1103–1118.
52. Howell, J. L., Collisson, B., Crysel, L., metacognitive illusions: A priori theories,
40. Riordan, C. M. (2014, June 5). fluency effects, and misattributions
Garrido, C. O., Newell, S. M., Cottrell,
Diversity is useless without inclusivity. of the interleaving benefit. Journal of
C. A., . . . Shepperd, J. (2013). Managing
Harvard Business Review. Retrieved Experimental Psychology: General, 145,
the threat of impending implicit attitude
from https://hbr.org/2014/06/ 918–935.
feedback. Social Psychological and
diversity-is-useless-without-inclusivity Personality Science, 4, 714–720. 62. Albarracín, D., Durantini, M. R., Earl,
41. Lai, C. K. (2019, February). Implicit bias A., Gunnoe, J. B., & Leeper, J. (2008).
53. Galinsky, A. D., Todd, A. R., Homan,
training is teaching. Paper presented Beyond the most willing audiences:
A. C., Phillips, K. W., Apfelbaum, E.
at the meeting of the Society for A meta-intervention to increase
P., Sasaki, S. J., . . . Maddux, W. W.
Personality and Social Psychology, exposure to HIV-prevention programs
(2015). Maximizing the gains and
Portland, OR. Retrieved from https://osf. by vulnerable populations. Health
minimizing the pains of diversity: A
io/khrcz/ Psychology, 27, 638–644.
policy perspective. Perspectives on
42. Forscher, P. S., Mitamura, C., Dix, Psychological Science, 10, 742–748. 63. Earl, A., & Nisson, C. (2015). Applications
E. L., Cox, W. T., & Devine, P. G. of selective exposure and attention to
54. Legault, L., Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M.
(2017). Breaking the prejudice habit: information for understanding health
(2011). Ironic effects of antiprejudice
Mechanisms, timecourse, and longevity. and health disparities. In R. Scott &
messages: How motivational
Journal of Experimental Social S. Kosslyn (Eds.), Emerging trends in
interventions can reduce (but also
Psychology, 72, 133–146. the social and behavioral sciences:
increase) prejudice. Psychological
43. Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Science, 22, 1472–1477. An interdisciplinary, searchable,
Negotiating interracial interactions:

a publication of the behavioral science & policy association 69


and linkable resource. https://doi. of health and illness (pp. 82–106). in academic achievement. Social Issues
org/10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0013 Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell. and Policy Review, 11, 159–194.
64. Kaiser, C. R., Major, B., Jurcevic, I., 73. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation 84. Carter, E. R., & Murphy, M. C. (2017).
Dover, T. L., Brady, L. M., & Shapiro, J. intentions: Strong effects of simple Consensus and consistency: Exposure
R. (2013). Presumed fair: Ironic effects plans. American Psychologist, 54, to multiple discrimination claims shapes
of organizational diversity structures. 493–503. Whites’ intergroup attitudes. Journal
Journal of Personality and Social of Experimental Social Psychology, 73,
74. Wilson, K., Senay, I., Durantini, M.,
Psychology, 104, 504–519. 24–33.
Sánchez, F., Hennessy, M., Spring, B.,
65. Ikizer, E. G., & Blanton, H. (2016). Media & Albarracín, D. (2015). When it comes 85. Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Ditlmann,
coverage of “wise” interventions can to lifestyle recommendations, more R. K., & West, T. V. (2012). Intergroup
reduce concern for the disadvantaged. is sometimes less: A meta-analysis of relations in post-conflict contexts:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: theoretical assumptions underlying the How the past influences the present
Applied, 22, 135–147. effectiveness of interventions promoting (and future). In K. J. Jonas & T. A.
multiple behavior domain change. Morton (Eds.), Restoring civil societies:
66. Duguid, M. M., & Thomas-Hunt, M. C.
Psychological Bulletin, 141, 474–509. The psychology of intervention and
(2015). Condoning stereotyping? How
engagement following crisis (pp.
awareness of stereotyping prevalence 75. Jefferson, H., & Lewis, N., Jr. (2018,
135–155). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
impacts expression of stereotypes. April 23). Starbucks won’t have any idea
Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, whether its diversity training works. 86. Eibach, R. P., & Ehrlinger, J. (2006).
343–359. Washington Post. Retrieved from “Keep your eyes on the prize”: Reference
https://www.washingtonpost.com/ points and racial differences in assessing
67. Emerson, J. (2017, July 17). Don’t give
news/posteverything/wp/2018/04/23/ progress toward equality. Personality
up on unconscious bias training—Make
starbucks-wont-have-any-idea- and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32,
it better. Harvard Business Review.
whether-its-diversity-training-works/ 66–77.
Retrieved from https://hbr.org/2017/04/
dont-give-up-on-unconscious-bias- 76. State v. Gregory, 427 P.3d 621 (Wash. 87. Nelson, J. C., Adams, G., & Salter, P. S.
training-make-it-better 2018). (2013). The Marley hypothesis: Denial
of racism reflects ignorance of history.
68. Onyeador, I. N., Henderson, K. A., 77. Capers, Q., Clinchot, D., McDougle,
Psychological Science, 24, 213–218.
& Shapiro, J. R. (2017, October). L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2017). Implicit
Presumed unintentional: Ironic effects racial bias in medical school admissions.
of implicit bias framing on perceptions Academic Medicine, 92, 365–369.
of discrimination. In I. Jurcevic & 78. Paluck, E. L. (2006). Diversity training
E.M. Caruso (Chairs), “. . . Unless the and intergroup contact: A call to action
mind is guilty”: How perspective and research. Journal of Social Issues, 62,
framing distort judgments of criminal 577–595.
liability. Symposium conducted at
the annual conference of the Society 79. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., &
of Experimental Social Psychology, Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed)
Boston, MA. stereotype content: Competence
and warmth respectively follow from
69. Daumeyer, N. M., Onyeador, I. perceived status and competition.
N., Brown, X., & Richeson, J. R. Journal of Personality and Social
(2019). Consequences of attributing Psychology, 82, 878–902.
discrimination to implicit vs. explicit
bias. Journal of Experimental Social 80. Woodzicka, J. A., Mallett, R. K.,
Psychology, 84, 103812. Hendricks, S., & Pruitt, A. V. (2015). It’s
just a (sexist) joke: Comparing reactions
70. Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back to sexist versus racist communications.
into fear appeals: The extended parallel Humor, 28, 289–309.
process model. Communications
Monographs, 59, 329–349. 81. Parker, L. R., Monteith, M. J., Moss-
Racusin, C. A., & Van Camp, A. R. (2018).
71. Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2000). Promoting concern about gender bias
Challenge and threat appraisals: The with evidence-based confrontation.
role of affective cues. In J. P. Forgas Journal of Experimental Social
(Ed.), Studies in emotion and social Psychology, 74, 8–23.
interaction, second series. Feeling and
thinking: The role of affect in social 82. Lewis, N. A., Jr., & Yates, J. F. (2019).
cognition (pp. 59–82). New York, NY: Preparing disadvantaged students for
Cambridge University Press. success in college: Lessons learned
from the preparation initiative.
72. Fisher, W. A., Fisher, J. D., & Harman, J. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
(2003). The information–motivation– 14, 54–59.
behavioral skills model: A general
social psychological approach to 83. Oyserman, D., & Lewis, N. A. (2017).
understanding and promoting health Seeing the destination and the path:
behavior. In J. Suls & K. A. Wallston Using identity-based motivation to
(Eds.), Social psychological foundations understand and reduce racial disparities

70 behavioral science & policy  |  volume 6 issue 1 2020

You might also like