You are on page 1of 10

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

174 (3rd series)


Neurocognitive Development: Disorders and Disabilities
A. Gallagher, C. Bulteau, D. Cohen and J.L. Michaud, Editors
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64148-9.00010-7
Copyright © 2020 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 10

Autism spectrum disorder


LAURENT MOTTRON*

Department of Psychiatry and Addictology, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada

Abstract
Autism is a frequent, precocious behavioral constellation of social and communicative atypicalities asso-
ciated with apparently restricted interests and repetitive behavior and paired with an uneven ability profile.
Its definition has constantly broadened in the past 75 years, introducing phenotypes increasingly distant
from its initial description, heterogeneous in intelligence and speech level, and associated conditions.
When it is unassociated with other conditions, its origin is mostly genetic, transmissible, and favored
by frequent polymorphisms with small effects present in the general population. Identified de novo rare
mutations with large deleterious effects produce phenotypes only loosely related to nonsyndromic autism.
Autism is associated with brain reorganization at multiple levels, and with a variant of typical information
processing, i.e., the way humans perceive, memorize, manipulate, and attribute emotional value to avail-
able information. Its phenotype evolves over the span of life, with an overall reduction of autistic signs, but
it still requires some level of support. There is no treatment for this condition; however, it is compatible
with high levels of integration into society.

described and then elaborated on in Kanner’s follow-


DEFINITION
up papers. Although the autism category is historically
Autism is a precocious behavioral constellation of centered on an easily recognizable clinical presentation,
social and communicative atypicalities associated with its extension, following accepted definitions, now
apparently restricted interests and repetitive behavior encompasses a spectrum of presentations that vary in
(RIRB) and paired with an uneven ability profile. It is adaptive level, neurogenetic alterations, and similarity
still considered a behavioral syndrome 75 years after with the initial clinical image described by both Kanner
its initial recognition by Kanner (1943) and under and Asperger in the middle of the 20th century. We still
a different form by Asperger (1944). Kanner’s and ignore if the current autism category represents one, sev-
Asperger’s views on autism were relatively clear-cut; eral, or an indefinite number of “natural categories”
their initial definition summarized the particularities (Hollin, 2017).
presented in 11 children (4 for Asperger) who were The definition of autism is grounded on clusters of
clearly distinguishable from their peers. None of the positive and negative clinical traits related to the social
children described by Kanner and Asperger had a recog- character and variety of overt behavior (symptoms), a
nizable neurologic genetic syndrome in addition to situation that requires agreement among experts. In the
autism. All had strong speech atypicalities and/or delay. DSM-5 (2013) categorical definition of “autism spec-
None of these children were considered to be intellectu- trum disorder,” sociocommunicative symptoms should
ally disabled, but their intelligence manifested itself in include quantitative or qualitative alteration in social
an atypical way. Autism was stable over time, though communication and social interaction across multiple
some developmental transformations were originally contexts, as manifested by deficits in social–emotional

*Correspondence to: Laurent Mottron, M.D., Ph.D., MACSS/FCAHS, Department of Psychiatry and Addictology, University
of Montreal, H^opital en sante mentale Rivière-des-Prairies 7070, boul. Perras, 3rd floor, Montreal, QC, Canada H1E 1A4.
Tel: +1-514-972-4841, E-mail: laurent.mottron@gmail.com
128 L. MOTTRON
reciprocity and nonverbal communicative behaviors identifiable psychiatric conditions that extend from call-
used for social interaction, as well as developing, main- ing the same condition another name (for instance, most
taining, and understanding relationships. Furthermore, autistic people being positive for the current criteria of
two symptoms of restricted, repetitive patterns of behav- avoidant personality disorder or speech disorder) to an
ior, interests, or activities should be present, among a actual, independent condition such as anxiety or mood
list of four: stereotypical or repetitive motor movements, disorder. Last the severity specifier indicates that, besides
use of objects, or speech; insistence on sameness, inflex- the mandatory presence of a significant impairment, the
ible adherence to routines, or ritualized patterns or verbal level of adaptation may vary dramatically but must be at
nonverbal behavior; highly restricted, fixated interests least “clinically significant.”
that are abnormal in intensity or focus; and hyper- or
hyporeactivity to sensory input or unusual interests in
sensory aspects of the environment. It encompasses the
PREVALENCE
previous DSM-IV (1994) definition of autistic disorder,
Asperger syndrome, and pervasive developmental disor- Autism was thought to be rare: the first attempt at
ders not otherwise specified. Symptoms must be present epidemiology revealed a prevalence of only 4/10,000.
in the early developmental period and cause clinically It is now common and composed of a spectrum of clinical
significant impairment in present functioning. Intellec- presentations, with considerable variability. Clinicians
tual disability is the only differential diagnosis when it and researchers now must conciliate this variability with
can by itself explain the major part of the clinical pre- demands for categorical diagnosis required by the devel-
sentation. The heterogeneity of the autism spectrum is opment of research and the pressure of social recognition
intrinsic in its current definition, which reciprocally con- and service allocation. Variation in prevalence of autism
tributes to maintaining it (Mottron & Bzdock, 2020). has not been dramatic per se (reported as 2/1000 in
While no autism subtypes are identified in DSM-5 numerous studies published starting in the mid-1990s),
(as Asperger syndrome was in DSM-IV, a form of autism given deliberate increases in public awareness, planned
without speech delay or impairment and excluding increases in availability of services (including diagnostic
measured intellectual disability), four clinical specifiers services), and a purposeful improvement in case finding
are proposed: level of cognitive functioning, associated (Gernsbacher et al., 2005). Within the same study sam-
comorbid neurogenetic or psychiatric conditions, lan- ple, using the same standardized diagnostic instruments
guage level, and severity, identified as the level of assis- (SDIs), the same diagnosing clinicians, and so on, differ-
tance required for functioning. Each of these specifiers ences in diagnostic definition, some of them very subtle,
can be considered a quasidimensional variable, which can be responsible for a fourfold difference in prevalence
results in extreme phenotypic variations, theoretically estimates (Baird et al., 2006).
not interfering with the categorical diagnosis. Intelli- Several characteristics of the autistic phenotype
gence can vary from the lowest levels, where a clinically relevant for epidemiological studies reveal by their var-
significant distinction between autism and intellectual iability the extent to which the current delineation of
disability can be made, conventionally around 18 months the autistic spectrum phenotype is malleable. Sex ratio
of mental age, to the highest levels, where clinical ranges from a quasibalance between boys and girls in
distinction between atypicality resulting from giftedness the case of children with secondary autism who are con-
and autism may be challenging. Language level is a sidered intellectually disabled, to as many as 10 boys for
major factor of heterogeneity and characterized two one girl in the case of children with Asperger syndrome.
subgroups in DSM-IV, with (autism) and without As a sex ratio in favor of males is accepted as an impor-
(Asperger) structural and/or developmental language tant component of the autistic phenotype, this suggests
atypicalities (Mottron, 2020). Autism is probably the that primary and secondary autism are of a different
only neurodevelopmental condition where speech can nature, or that syndromic autism is a phenocopy of
vary from being inexistent to being perfect. The comor- primary autism. However, this sex ratio is measured on
bidity specifier comprises conditions associated with a population detected using diagnostic criteria identical
neurogenetics and psychiatry. The recognition of an for both sexes, assuming that autism is expressed
inventory of diagnosable conditions associated with an identically in males and females. Questioning this
autistic-like phenotype has resulted in the addition of assumption has resulted in the diminishing of the
another 10%–15% of individuals now described as male dominance in the sex ratio to two to three males
showing syndromic, etiologic, or secondary autism, in for one female (Loomes et al., 2017) but does not
contrast with idiopathic or primary autism where there completely suppress it. It also resulted in the emergence
are no identifiable syndromes apart from autism, or of a large number of self-diagnosed women, based on
only mutations that cannot be causally related to the notion of invisible autism, which is questionable
autism. Autistic people, particularly adults, also present (Halladay et al., 2015). The proportion of autistics
AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER 129
regarded as intellectually disabled has fluctuated greatly and motor assessment, observation in natural settings,
through the decades, depending on how autism has and neurogenetic and neuropediatric investigations when
been defined and intelligence measured. The recognition required. SDIs consist of a parent interview autism diag-
of the partial overlap between Asperger syndrome and nostic interview (ADI) and observation scale autism
Kanner’s autism had the effect of adding to the initial diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS) providing stan-
autism category a group with nondelayed speech dardized and fine-tuned symptom definition, assessment
development but showing signs in the social and RIRB procedures, and scoring, which correspond to DSM-IV
areas similar to those encountered in autism. or ICD 10 diagnostic criteria. The use of SDIs should
The current reported prevalence of epilepsy ranges be limited to autism specialized clinics, as they require
from 5% to 40% in autistic individuals. Like sex a high level of expertise in autism and their adminis-
ratio and intellectual disability, it differs considerably tration to avoid misinterpretations. Referrals to neurolo-
according to whether autism is etiologic or idiopathic. gists, geneticist, or third-line pediatricians are limited
Currently, there are no studies on the prevalence of to complex or unclear clinical situations, including
epilepsy in samples with measured intelligence in the epilepsy, as well as medical and genetic conditions with
normal range and from which secondary autism has a neurologic expression and a greater-than-chance asso-
been excluded. Clinical evidence is in the direction ciation with autism or a partial clinical overlap with
of a prevalence of epilepsy in this population that is its criteria. While a combined expertise is required for
not much higher than in the nonautistic population. the entire range of level of functioning, clinical presen-
The often-repeated figure of 20%–30% is clearly an tations, and child and adult differential diagnoses,
overestimate, and would, if anything, characterize clinical teams for adults and children should be relatively
only secondary autism, where epilepsy is considerably autonomous. Adult assessment is more difficult and less
more frequent, as well as microcephaly and intellectual reliable, due to developmental transformations, and
disability (Amiet et al., 2008). This aggregation, along alteration of the original presentation due to the social
with the aforementioned gender balance, points again positive or negative value of differential diagnoses.
toward a separate status for primary and secondary The use of the cutoff provided by checklist diagnostic
autism. In sum, uncertainty about the prevalence of instruments (DSM or SDIs) encompasses more individ-
autism and its major neurologic and demographic corre- uals than the recognition of a prototypical autistic
lations is a consequence of current standards of clinical pattern by clinical experts. Their sensitivity is good to
phenotype delineation, the absence of exclusion criteria very good, but their specificity is low, since they
toward other neurodevelopmental conditions, and the overinclude most child psychiatric and neurodevelop-
arbitrary nature of any categorical delimitation in the mental conditions. Accordingly the specificity of SDIs
distribution of continuous behavioral phenotypic traits. has been demonstrated toward specific groups of
The heterogeneity of individuals currently encompassed idiopathic intellectual disabilities or language/learning
under the DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder (ASD) cri- disorders, but not toward other neurodevelopmental
teria should not be considered as a fact informative on conditions, for which the autism score may range
the neurobiologic mechanisms producing autism, but from marginal to strongly positive based on these
as the result of a checklist identification strategy based criteria. These instruments also miss some very
on consensual criteria, rather than on the recognition of intelligent individuals (especially after puberty, and
one or several prototypes. women), who nevertheless test positive for autism cri-
teria according to expert judgment. In addition, diagnos-
tic cutoffs on the SDIs have the damaging effect of
likening autism to a behaviorally unidimensional cate-
POSITIVE DIAGNOSIS
gory, despite genetic independence of some of its overt
Autism tends to be suspected first by parents or some- behavioral traits. A diagnostic threshold can be reached
times by others such as daycare staff. General physicians, by an enormous number of different combinations
pediatricians, child psychiatrists, and increasingly of phenotypic dimensions, some of them extremely dis-
specialized autism clinics confirm the diagnosis. Any tant from prototypical autism. “Dimensional” scales
reference channel that results in half of the referred as Social Responsiveness Scale have been developed,
individual being diagnosed at the end of the process but they are largely contaminated by variations in
is reliable, if the people not diagnosed are properly psychologic states. However, SDIs are indispensable
oriented, and screening should not try to combine spec- to pinpoint a prototypical category in which both clini-
ificity and sensitivity. The diagnosis and assessment of cal and research purposes are grounded. In particular,
ASDs requires about 2 days of multidisciplinary investi- SDIs have contributed decisively to ascertaining the
gations. It involves the combined use of standardized distinction between autism with or without intellectual
instruments, clinical expertise, neurocognitive, speech, disability and intellectual disability without autism.
130 L. MOTTRON
DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS for Tourette syndrome. However, this second diagnosis
will neither provide information relevant to intervention
Differential diagnoses of autism with typical speech nor contribute to a better understanding of their adaptive
level and neurodevelopmental conditions with limited difficulties, which will still have to be addressed in a
or no impact on measured intelligence, such as Tourette Tourette syndrome framework. A similar position is
syndrome or some presentations of ADHD, are specially recommended in the presence of major ADHD or attach-
challenging as these conditions frequently meet SDI ment disorders.
cutoffs, especially during preschool years. Meeting Conversely, some conditions are so frequent in autism
ADOS or ADI cut-off scores are inversely but quasili- that they should lead to a systematic examination of
nearly dependent on measured intelligence, with system- autism when they are the entry point for neurologic
atic reaching of cutoffs for children whose measured examination. Whereas “pure” developmental dysphasia
mental age is under 15–18 months. Therefore, the most (or specific language impairment), despite definitional
difficult diagnostic issue consists in differentiating the limitations in communicative abilities, does not have
apparent decrease in sociocompetence inherent to any an impact on nonverbal social interaction, this syndrome
neurodevelopmental diagnosis, as well as the restriction is inherent in a large fraction of autistic individuals
in the variety of interests resulting from any develop- (Boucher, 2012). One-third of autistic individuals may
mental delay, from their autism equivalent. A full psychi- fall under this additional diagnosis, which nevertheless
atric investigation of differential diagnosis is justified needs to be distinguished from transient delayed echo-
in the presence of major autistic traits, whether or not a lalia and stereotyped speech intrinsic to the autism
neuropsychiatric co-occurring condition is suspected. diagnosis. Therefore in the presence of other autism
The main neurodevelopmental conditions of psychiatric diagnostic criteria in the two major behavioral areas,
relevance whose phenotype overlaps with the autistic developmental dysphasia should not exclude the search
spectrum include Tourette syndrome, ADHD, and devel- for autism. In addition, positive scoring for ADHD
opmental dysphasia in their strongest forms, attachment criteria is the rule in most children with Asperger syn-
disorders, childhood depression and social phobia, drome, and, consequently, it should not be considered
especially in its selective mutism form. Early-onset an exclusion criterion for this diagnosis. Most persistent
schizophrenia in its prototypical form is not a diagnostic forms of childhood epilepsy, especially precocious
issue, but schizoid personality is currently one of the ones and regardless of their cause, produce complex
differential diagnoses of Asperger syndrome. ADHD is behavioral regressions overlapping with autism criteria,
not an exclusion criterion anymore, but cautious assess- but with disastrous effects on nonverbal intelligence.
ment should distinguish hyperexploratory behaviors, Regression with epilepsy is more dramatic than the loss
frequently in search of perceptually relevant material, of first words and counting of numbers less than 10,
sometimes associated with autism, from distributed which signal autism in one-quarter of autistic children.
motor and ideational activity resulting from pure ADHD. It also occurs at an earlier age (West syndrome) or later
Last the newly defined pragmatic communication disor- age (Landau–Kleffner syndrome) than is the case with
der in DSM-5 is supposed to encompass individuals with apparent loss of speech in autism. Partial seizures, which
similar sociocommunicative alterations to autism, but are difficult to detect clinically in an autistic child, still
without restricted interest and repetitive behavior. Poorly have to be investigated in the presence of dramatic
defined, it is practically not studied and of limited clinical changes or discontinuity in cognitive performance.
utility as most neurodevelopmental conditions could The diagnosis can be challenging as each DSM crite-
satisfy its criteria. rion may have a considerably different expression across
In the presence of incontestable psychiatric diagnosis development, from toddlers to adolescents. Narrowly
anterior or alternative to autism, we suggest applying defined autism is usually not reliably discernible before
an economy principle, i.e., answering the following the middle of the second year of life. In secondary autism,
question: Can the sociocommunicative atypicalities or manifestations of the first diagnosis and especially
limitation in the variety of activities be explained in a waking age delays often precede or mask autistic signs.
more straightforward manner by autism or an alternative A decrease in peer-oriented initiatives and reactions,
diagnosis? For example, an individual presenting with including joint attention, orientation toward a parent’s
strong Tourette syndrome will display poor social adap- voice as well as smile, babbling, and gestures coupling
tation due to attention deficit, irascibility, and side effects during interactions with parents and peers, is generally
of their temper, with the result of limiting the variety of the first observed manifestation. Atypically prolonged
their activities according to what they successfully pro- or unusually angled visual fixation on objects may be
cess. This will lead them to score positively for autism present at that time but is less obvious or salient to
criteria in DSM-5 in addition to satisfying the criteria observers. A period of quasi absence of oral verbal
AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER 131
expression typically follows until 3–5 years of age, when Leber amaurosis. Rare reported medical conditions are
echolalia and then stereotyped speech appear. In less mitochondrial syndrome and prenatal exposure to
than 10% of autistic children, speech will not develop, valproic acid, but routine metabolic testing is not
but in the majority of cases, speech will flourish, still recommended.
varying considerably in terms of functional value Investigating the familial history of an autistic person
(Wodka et al., 2013). The maximum number of recogniz- for neurodevelopmental conditions, learning disabilities,
able signs is observed around the ages of 3 and 6–7, due and psychiatric syndromes is mandatory, leading to stan-
to the presence by this age of language signs that are dard genetic explorations (CGH) in the case of minor
characteristic, if not definitive. Retrospective assessment morphological dimorphism, any sign of a neurodeve-
with SDIs therefore take the 4–5 years of age period as lopmental syndrome, recognizable or not, and learning
the time frame during which targeted signs have to be or intellectual disability. The list of genetic conditions
present, whatever developmental transformation occurs whose phenotypes may present some overlap with
afterwards. The variability of outcomes seems to be autism is open, but the most frequently cited, with a
directly dependent on how early the diagnosis was made reported prevalence rate of less than 5% in autism
(the later the diagnosis, the more stable it is) and how cohorts, are tuberous sclerosis and Fragile X syndrome
close to prototypical autism the child was at this age (FrX). Tuberous sclerosis (TS) is found in 8%–14% of
(the greater the distance from the prototype, the less individuals displaying autism with epilepsy (but excep-
stable the diagnosis). The developmental trend of strictly tional in those without) and is thereby 100 times more
defined autism with speech onset delay contrasts with frequent among autistic people than in the general
that of Asperger syndrome, which may involve preco- population. TS is also one of the most important causes
cious and spectacular speech abilities (Pickles et al., of West syndrome, which is associated with dramatic
2014). The adaptive prognosis as adults is overall poor behavioral and cognitive regression. This results in a
in terms of autonomy and intimate and familial relation- clinical image scoring positive on autism criteria but
ships, with less than one-fourth of diagnosed autistic also with a measured age under the threshold from
adults living independently and/or on their own income. where distinctions between autism and intellectual
With few exceptions, they will benefit from some kind disability becomes possible. FrX is associated with an
of lifelong support as their life remains hampered by autistic phenotype in around one-fifth of cases and
multiple practical and psychologic issues, including should be searched for only in the presence of lowered
mood and anxiety disorders. The notion that autism is nonverbal IQ and physical characteristics. Other com-
a lifelong condition must be tempered by the absence monly mentioned conditions, with a prevalence rate
of predictors of adaptive outcome at toddler age. of less than 1%, include the following syndromes: Down
Critically the outcome cannot be predicted (in both direc- (3x21), Turner (X0), XYY, Klinefelter (XXY), Rett,
tions) for the apparent severity or nonseverity of the Angelman, and Prader-Willi (15q11–q13), DiGeorge
presentation in childhood (Baghdadli et al., 2018). (22q11), Cornelia de Lange, Lesch–Nyhan, Smith–
Lemli–Opitz, San Philippo, Williams (7q11.23), Cohen,
Ito, Joubert, FG, ARX, PKU, 1q21.1, 3q29, 16p11.2,
ASSESSMENT
neurofibromatosis, Moebius and Smith Magenis
Rather than etiologically oriented and related to autism, (17p11.2), and an indefinite number of copy number
clinical investigations should be conducted according to variations that can also exist without an autism or
medical or neurogenetic symptoms presented in a child. autism-like presentation (Richards et al., 2015).
Individuals should be examined for medical signs. Neuropsychologic language/communication and
Medical assessments should investigate the effect on motor characterization are required for diagnosis as well
physical health of being autistic: food selectivity is the as education and support at all ages and developmental
rule, with consequences ranging from specific metabolic levels. For a nonverbal child or a child who has not yet
deprivation to obesity. Gastrointestinal symptoms should reached the age of language development, psychologic
be investigated for themselves and not as potentially testing may be impossible due to lack of compliance or
causative. Sleep disorders are not constant, but have comprehension of task instructions. This may mean
major consequences on familial well-being, and can adapting test administration procedures, the modeling
be alleviated by medical and psychoeducational care. of answers, or modifying the way information is col-
Health issues are relevant as autistics do not or cannot lected. For instance, activities could take place in natural
complain, the major effect being a shorter life expec- settings and information gathering could be limited to
tancy. The most frequent medical condition that mimics an in-depth interview with caregivers. When passing for-
an autistic profile in childhood is sensorial impairment, mal tests is impossible, estimating the complexity and
specifically congenital or early blindness and congenital rapidity of the tasks realized spontaneously by the child
132 L. MOTTRON
may represent the best proxy for nonverbal intelligence. well as clinical applications restricted to a small subgroup
The three major questions to be answered should be: is of the phenotype. Differences in concordance between
the average level of cognitive performance at the normal homozygotic twins (60% and above) vs dizygotic twins
or the intellectually disabled level? Is the cognitive and (around 5%) is considerable, although phenotypic differ-
communication profile consistent with an autism sub- ences between twins may be observed. Concordance
type? What is the communication channel allowing the between siblings ranges from 15% to 40% according to
exchange of information? the gender of the child, with larger concordance when
Neuropsychologic and communication assessment the first child is a girl and the second a boy, and for fam-
should search for and reveal successful channels of ilies who have more than one autistic child (Ozonoff et al.,
communication and specific areas of ability and transmit 2011). There is a substantial drop in aggregation between
this information to caregivers. The interpretation of intel- first- and second-degree relatives. Familial aggregation
ligence level in autism is challenging since some tests extends beyond narrow autism: concordance for autism
underestimate cognitive levels and display an isolated, increases considerably when including the “broad autism
low level of performance, lower than that associated with phenotype” ensemble of cognitive and behavioral traits
the majority of tasks representing the child’s baseline parent to autism but subthreshold for an autism diagnosis.
level (Courchesne et al., 2015). In contrast, other tasks Familial aggregation is greater for probands with higher
are performed at a level markedly superior to this base- measured intelligence, which can be explained by the
line. In the first years, language-mediated tasks requiring inability to differentiate autism from intellectual disability
the understanding of verbal instructions are typically the (whose heritability is overall lower than that of autism) in
most difficult to perform, especially those measuring the very low mental-age equivalents.
ability to face a new social situation presented by means The repeated finding that genetic factors cannot account
of a verbal answer. However, vocabulary tasks requiring for the entire prevalence of autism, and mostly, of its
the child to point to answers or to decode words may be supposed exponential increase, leaves some space for
performed at a typical or even superior level. The highest environmental factors. The most studied (food allergies,
performance is found in the “block design” subtest and in vaccines) have been thoroughly discarded and were ini-
nonverbal fluid intelligence tasks (Raven’s progressive tially associated with one of the most important scientific
matrices) (Nader et al., 2015). Whereas low scores in frauds of the 20th century (Fombonne and Cook Jr, 2003).
an individual’s profile may predict an overall in adapta- Other environmental factors, such as prematurity, pre- and
tion to daily demands without accommodation, high perinatal events, SSRIs, and an undefined list of others
scores predict abilities and intelligence level in adapted causes, may be associated with a minimal portion of autism
conditions. This pattern of performance diverges mark- prevalence, which plausibly coincides with the partial
edly from that observed in autistic people without speech phonotypical overlap between any neurodevelopmental
onset delay (the previous Asperger subgroup), where condition and the current definition of autism.
some verbal tasks such as similarities and information The understanding of the mechanisms involved in an
often present a level of performance superior to what autistic clinical presentation is hampered by the uncer-
is expected at the child’s chronological age. In contrast, tainty regarding the extension of the autistic phenotype.
coding and digit span subtests are usually associated with The decision of DSM-5 to consider any identified neuro-
the lowest levels of performance due to attention diffi- developmental condition as compatible with an autism
culties. Asperger children do not display visual–spatial diagnosis validates interpreting these associations as
peaks. They also present motor clumsiness, not found potentially causative and therefore building “autism
in autism, shown by major difficulties in writing, cycling, animal models” for these conditions. However, the level
and catching balls. of similarity between syndromic and nonsyndromic
autism is low and sometimes trivial, which casts doubt
on the external validity of syndromic autism for nonsyn-
ETIOLOGY AND MECHANISMS
dromic autism (Bishop et al., 2017). Moreover, most
Autism is, in its prototypical form, a genetic condition identified associated conditions and pathogenic copy
(Gaugler et al., 2014). All medical hypotheses about number variations are de novo, whereas some evidence
large-scale causes of autism can be firmly discarded. of familial clustering is dominant for nonsyndromic
The putative role of vaccines, exposure to lead or mer- autism. Therefore there are antagonistic views of the dis-
cury, gluten or diary allergies as widespread causes of tinction between primary and secondary autism. One is
autism is no longer a scientific issue. Knowledge about that if an additional condition is present in only a small
genetic influence on autism combines strong scientific percentage of the autistic spectrum, then this condition
evidence of high heritability with limited actual determi- has in fact produced a phenocopy of primary autism,
nation of genes and modes of transmission involved, as that is, it mimics a phenotype characterizing a certain
AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER 133
condition via a completely different mechanism. In auditory information (Palmer et al., 2017). Enhanced
contrast, autism may represent the final common path- perceptual abilities, or peaks of ability, are solidly
way to an indefinite number of mechanisms (some demonstrated in prototypical autism, but their preva-
known in syndromic autism and unknown in primary lence in the entire AS group depends on how specific
autism), thereby making syndromic autism an oppor- the diagnosis is. They extend from enhanced discrimi-
tunity to understand idiopathic autism and in effect nation of low-level psychophysical auditory and visual
creating a “human model” of autism. In our opinion information to pattern detection (e.g., visual search) and
the latter position neglects the large phenotypical differ- manipulation (e.g., mental rotation, visuospatial con-
ences between “autism” and “autism with …” Accord- struction). Apparently, positive emotions might be less
ingly, there is also the possibility of little or no related to social information, although not impaired
informative similarity between primary autism and sec- per se, than with strong, focused interests and the shar-
ondary autism phenocopies (Harris, 2016). In terms of ing of these. In contrast, once the effect of measured
neurobiologic mechanisms, autism is associated with intelligence has been removed, no cognitive deficit
multiple but heterogeneous genetic, cognitive, func- has unequivocally been found in cases of autism.
tional, and structural variations whose established links Understanding of human intention in autism, an overt
with an autistic behavioral and cognitive phenotype are indicator of social cognition, has been shown in multi-
minimal yet highly dependent on a definition of uncer- ple studies to be at least at the typical level. Numerous
tain boundaries (Rane et al., 2015; O’Reilly et al., findings related to complex purposive actions, problem-
2017; Pereira et al., 2018). Consequently, the search solving, and executive functions yield apparent advan-
for a unique and common neurocognitive mechanism tages and disadvantages, depending on experimental
for all individuals sharing autism criteria should not design and task demands, but no overall image of a
be a research objective. Informative similarity between deficit. Long-term memory performance follows mea-
two autistic phenotypes ranges from maximal (e.g., sured intelligence, but it is also less influenced by
between two idiopathic autistic children sharing the same semantic or emotional properties of the memorized
cognitive profile) to minimal (e.g., between two individ- material than is the case with nonautistics.
uals with secondary autism differing in their related neu- The presence of a significant, and probably dominant,
rodevelopmental condition). Therefore we will limit our fraction of idiopathic autism with a typical level of
presentation of current models to idiopathic autism, as measured intelligence indicates that intellectual disability
each type of secondary autism requires a specific model is not a mandatory component of autism (Charman et al.,
of the relationship between the two diagnoses involved. 2011). Another percentage of apparently intellectually
Autism is now considered a variant of typical disabled individuals actually displays a normal nonverbal
information processing, i.e., the way humans perceive, IQ level, indicating that difficulties with speech or
memorize, manipulate, and attribute emotional value language, whatever their cause, play a strong role in
to available information. Compared with nonautistic how the abilities of some autistic people are judged
people, this variation consists in the less automatic (Courchesne et al., 2015). Yet another fraction of autistic
or mandatory, higher-order processing of incoming individuals with low measured intelligence probably cor-
information (categorization, purposive integration, and responds to the long-term effects of impoverished input
attribution of emotional value). Autistic information pro- due to unavailability of material that they would be able
cessing differences are domain general, encompassing to process well. This mechanism can be inferred from
information regarded as social (e.g., facial expression analogous situations involving deaf adults who were
and language pragmatics) and nonsocial (e.g., visuospa- exposed neither to oral nor to sign language, as autistic
tial processing) (Mottron et al., 2006). A more optional, people seem more dependent than typical individuals
but not in any way absent, relation among cognitive on access to specific kinds, amounts, and arrangements
processes allows for a greater than typical role of of information that may not be easily available or
perception, including in domains not considered to be completely unavailable. Last, in many cases, intellectual
perceptual in nonautistics. For typical individuals, a disability in autism coincides with secondary autism and
major way to attribute value to incoming information follows the same explanations as those used for intellec-
consists in automatically indexing it with a social value tual disability per se (e.g., reduced dendritic spines).
(personal benefit, social adequacy, consistency with Second-generation brain-based models now deem-
collective interests). While nonautistic cognition is phasize the putative action of single cognitive deficits
dominated by an early, automatic, and exclusive bias with cascading effects, such as amygdala or cerebellum
toward social information, the predominance of per- deficits, in favor of distributed reorganization among
ception in autistic cognition allows for enhanced brain functions and levels (Nunes et al., 2018). Most
processing of environmental regularities in visual and structural studies find smaller cells and atypical
134 L. MOTTRON
minicolumnar organization in gray matter, but no evident level of the didactic or play material offered to the child
neurochemical modifications. Functional MRI studies should be adapted to his nonverbal apparent intelligence,
demonstrate diminished activity in frontal and temporal as estimated through its most complex spontaneous
regions but enhanced activity in the visual cortex accomplishments. Printed material, tablets, cell phones,
(Samson et al., 2012). Therefore some reallocation of and screens in general are a profitable entry channel for
regional brain functions must take place. For example, speech and nonverbal abilities, and their access should
typical or superior performance in working memory possible, but reasonably regulated.
or problem-solving tasks is obtained in the presence Behaviors that are intrinsically linked to autism, such
of an inverted balance of frontal-occipital activation. as unarming repetitive movements and focused interests,
Decreased inter- and intrahemispheric connectivity has should not be targeted, since they do not represent a
been supported by most diffusion tensor imaging studies. burden to the autistic child himself. One should not try
Such reduced functional crosstalk between brain regions to suppress in an autistic child what is tolerated in a
is also consistent with the diminished section of the typical child, for example, sporadic tantrums. In most
corpus callosum, a situation that is consistently found cases of primary autism, self-injurious behavior can be
in autism subgroups. An increase in brain size consis- considered a typical autistic expression of extreme con-
tently spares frontal lobes but affects gray and white fusion and negative emotions. Since most self-injurious
matter to a similar extent. Autistic macrocephaly coin- behaviors is sporadic and without damaging physical
cides with the first behavioral autistic signs occurring consequences, it should be addressed as a crisis
around the end of the first year of age. Some, but not situation and dealt with as such. On the other hand,
all, studies report its normalization at an adult age, while life-threatening self-injuring behavior is part of the
others found macrocephaly in 30% of adult autistic indi- phenotype of a number of genetic conditions asso-
viduals compared with 2% of controls. Other cerebral ciated with secondary autism (e.g., Smith–Lemli–Opitz
structures present inconsistent variations that cannot be syndrome).
interpreted even in a research context. However, some co-occurring features of the autistic
spectrum may be successfully targeted. Developmental
dysphasia may be addressed with speech therapy, or
TREATMENT AND SUPPORT
alternative communication channels (e.g., keyboards),
There is no medical treatment for autism: a distributed but the majority of efforts should concentrate on manag-
difference in neuronal organization and functional ing communication with the child in family and class-
allocation of brain resources is yet out of reach of phar- room situations. Moreover, usual targets in speech
maceutical agents (Lord et al., 2018). Parental guidance intervention should be adapted to the autistic child’s
and adaptive measures at home, school, and workplaces, speech development. For instance, it is of poor use to
combined with cognitive-behavioral therapy for asso- attempt to train speech during the “mute” and the echo-
ciated conditions taking communication difficulties lalic periods of autistic speech development. Support for
into account, remain the logical way to address most sensory-motor difficulties, including desensitization but
challenges (Green and Garg, 2018). Early intensive inter- also training of underdeveloped motor sectors, is usually
ventions based on applied behavior analysis, involving provided by an occupational therapist. Adapted psycho-
one-on-one training for at least 20, and up to 40 or more, logic support, possibly combined with pharmacotherapy,
hours per week, are not supported by randomized is required for associated psychiatric conditions. How-
controlled trials. Their cost is unbearable by any health ever, the use of medication with autistic people should
system, their short- and long-term benefits and harms always follow an in-depth, but unsuccessful investiga-
have not been sufficiently established, and the associated tion of the role of contextual cues in producing a crisis
ethical issues remain unaddressed. In contrast, enriching situation, as well as rational attempts to address the crisis
synchrony in parent–child interaction through noninten- in that way (NICE, 2013). For example, most apparent
sive parental guidance has a small but measurable benefit temper tantrums in autistic preschoolers can be alleviated
on the entire range of autistic features up to 6 years later by teaching parents how to decode the communicative
(Pickles et al., 2016). attempts of their child, how to provide him with informa-
Parents should be informed about the cognitive and tion and materials that he can process well, and how
behavioral peculiarities of their child, autism-based gen- to address him in a way that he can understand. Similarly,
eral knowledge, combined with the specific profile of depressive mood states in school-age autistic children are
signs and competences and interests detected at the time often related to bullying, in which case it is the behavior
of diagnosis. Early intervention targets should be timely, of bullies that needs to be addressed. Rare co-occurring
feasible, and ethically acceptable. The developmental obsessive-compulsive disorder or Tourette syndrome,
AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER 135
once distinguished from the repertoire of repetitive Fombonne E, Cook Jr EH (2003). MMR and autistic
autistic movements, can be medicated, if necessary. This enterocolitis: consistent epidemiological failure to find
also applies to attention deficit, damaging levels of activ- an association. Mol Psychiatry 8: 133–134.
ity (especially verbal), anxiety disorder, and depressive Gaugler T, Klei L, Sanders SJ et al. (2014). Most genetic risk
for autism resides with common variation. Nat Genet 46:
mood in verbal autistic adults. Heavy behavioral prob-
881–885.
lems, such as aggressive behavior or tantrums, intracta-
Gernsbacher MA, Dawson M, Goldsmith HH (2005). Three
ble wandering or the socially damaging pursuit of reasons not to believe in an autism epidemic. Curr Dir
restricted interests, are possible but rare. They justify a Psychol Sci 14: 55–58.
combined systemic, cognitive-behavioral and pharmaco- Green J, Garg S (2018). Annual research review: the state of
logic approach. Existing studies have shown that the ben- autism intervention science: progress, target psychological
efit of atypical antipsychotics in this case is very limited and biological mechanisms and future prospects. J Child
and may be even lesser than that associated with a pla- Psychol Psychiatry 59: 424–443.
cebo effect. In contrast, melatonin has a demonstrated Halladay AK, Bishop S, Constantino JN et al. (2015). Sex
effect on sleep delay and length. The access to medical and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: sum-
and dental care for general health issues is a priority as marizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas
of priority. Mol Autism 6: 36.
most health providers are uninformed or unwilling to
Harris JC (2016). The origin and natural history of autism
physically examine an autistic person. Discouraging
spectrum disorders. Nat Neurosci 19: 1390–1391.
the use of alternative treatment that is costly, damaging, Hollin G (2017). Autistic heterogeneity: linking uncertainties
or simply useless is a necessary step in every situation. and indeterminacies. Sci Cult (Lond) 26: 209–231.
Kanner L (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact.
Nervous Child 2: 217–250.
REFERENCES
Loomes R, Hull L, Mandy WPL (2017). What is the male-to-
Amiet C, Gourfinkel-An I, Bouzamondo A et al. (2008). female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic
Epilepsy in autism is associated with intellectual disability review and meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
and gender: evidence from a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry Psychiatry 56: 466–474.
64: 577–582. Lord C, Elsabbagh M, Baird G et al. (2018). Autism spectrum
Asperger H (1944). Die ‘Autistischen Psychopathen’ im disorder. Lancet 392: 508–520.
Kindesalter. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 117: 76–136. Mottron L (2020). Asperger syndrome. In: L Cohen (Ed.), The
Baghdadli A, Michelon C, Pernon E et al. (2018). Adaptive Wiley encyclopedia of health psychology set. John Wiley
trajectories and early risk factors in the autism spectrum: and Sons Ltd, Hoboken, USA. in press.
a 15-year prospective study. Autism Res 11: 1455–1467. Mottron L, Bzdok D (2020). Autism spectrum hetero-
Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A et al. (2006). Prevalence geneity: fact or artifact? Mol Psychiatry 10: https://doi.
of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort org/10.1038/s41380-020-0748-y.
of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulieres I et al. (2006). Enhanced
Project (SNAP). Lancet 368: 210–215. perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight
Bishop SL, Farmer C, Bal V et al. (2017). Identification of principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord
developmental and behavioral markers associated with 36: 27–43.
genetic abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder. Am Nader AM, Jelenic P, Soulières I (2015). Discrepancy between
J Psychiatry 174: 576–585. WISC-III and WISC-IV cognitive profile in autism spec-
Boucher J (2012). Research review: structural language in trum: what does it reveal about autistic cognition? PLoS
autistic spectrum disorder—characteristics and causes. One 10: e0144645.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53: 219–233. NICE (2013). Autism spectrum disorder in under 19s: support
Charman T, Pickles A, Simonoff E et al. (2011). IQ in children and management, clinical guideline [CG170], NICE—
with autism spectrum disorders: data from the Special National Institute for Health and Care Excellence.
Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Psychol Med J Res Nunes AS, Peatfield N, Vakorin V et al. (2018). Idiosyncratic
Psychiatry Allied Sci 41: 619–627. organization of cortical networks in autism spectrum
Courchesne V, Meilleur AA, Poulin-Lord MP et al. (2015). disorder. NeuroImage 190: 182–190.
Autistic children at risk of being underestimated: school- O’Reilly C, Lewis JD, Elsabbagh M (2017). Is functional brain
based pilot study of a strength-informed assessment. Mol connectivity atypical in autism? A systematic review of
Autism 6: 12. EEG and MEG studies. PLoS One 12: e0175870.
DSM-5 (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A et al. (2011). Recurrence risk
disorders (DSM-5®), fifth edn, American Psychiatric for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research
Publishing, Arlington, VA. Consortium study. Pediatrics 128: e488–e495.
DSM-IV (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of Palmer CJ, Lawson RP, Hohwy J (2017). Bayesian approaches
mental disorders, American Psychiatric Association, to autism: towards volatility, action, and behavior. Psychol
Washington, DC. Bull 143: 521–542.
136 L. MOTTRON
Pereira AM, Campos BM, Coan AC et al. (2018). Rane P, Cochran D, Hodge SM et al. (2015). Connectivity in
Differences in cortical structure and functional autism: a review of MRI connectivity studies. Harv Rev
MRI connectivity in high functioning autism. Front Psychiatry 23: 223–244.
Neurol 9: 539. Richards C, Jones C, Groves L et al. (2015). Prevalence of autism
Pickles A, Anderson DK, Lord C (2014). Heterogeneity and spectrum disorder phenomenology in genetic disorders: a
plasticity in the development of language: a 17-year systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2:
follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. 909–916.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 55: 1354–1362. Samson F, Mottron L, Soulières I et al. (2012). Enhanced
Pickles A, Le Couteur A, Leadbitter K et al. (2016). visual functioning in autism: an ALE meta-analysis.
Parent-mediated social communication therapy for Hum Brain Mapp 33: 1553–1581.
young children with autism (PACT): long-term follow- Wodka EL, Mathy P, Kalb L (2013). Predictors of phrase
up of a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 388: and fluent speech in children with autism and severe
2501–2509. language delay. Pediatrics 131: e1128–e1134.

You might also like