You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064

The perfectly motivated nurse and the others: workplace and


personal characteristics impact preference of nursing tasks

SVEN H. KOCH P h D , R N 1, RUMYANA PROYNOVA MSc


2
, BARBARA PAECH PhD
3
and
THOMAS WETTER P h D 4,5
1
Postdoctoral Research Associate, Institute of Medical Biometry and Informatics, Unit of Medical Informatics,,
2
Research Associate, 3Professor, Institute of Informatics, 4Professor, Institute of Medical Biometry and
Informatics, Unit of Medical Informatics, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Gemany and 5Department of
Biomedical Informatics and Medical Education, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA

Correspondence KOCH S.H., PROYNOVA R., PAECH B. & WETTER T. (2014) Journal of Nursing Management
Thomas Wetter 22, 1054–1064.
Institute of Medical Biometry and The perfectly motivated nurse and the others: workplace and personal
Informatics characteristics impact preference of nursing tasks
Im Neuenheimer Feld 305
Heidelberg 69120 Aims To identify whether motivation of nurses coincides with personal values,
Germany workplace or personal characteristics.
E-mail: thomas.wetter@urz.uni-hd. Background Shortage of nursing workforce compromises patient care. Motivation
de
and job satisfaction are factors considered to make nurses quit. Little is known
about measurement and variation of nurses’ motivation. Funding for human
resource programmes is limited – effective programmes could focus on nurses in
need of motivational support.
Methods Exploratory study with nurses using questionnaires in an academic
hospital in Germany. Work motivation was approximated through preference of
nursing tasks. Questionnaires measured personal values, preference of generic
nursing tasks, and workplace and personal characteristics.
Results A total of 212 questionnaires were usable. Higher motivation was found
in groups of nurses with the dominant personal value ‘Benevolence’, with high
self-rated expertise, in the middle of their career or working in surgical or general
wards. Motivation was low in nurses with the dominant value ‘Hedonism’, or
nurses in internal medicine or with low to medium self-rated expertise or who
used computers infrequently.
Conclusions Motivation coincided with dominant personal values, workplace and
personal characteristics. The results should be validated in other settings.
Implications for nursing management Human resource programmes could focus
on nurses whose motivation is at risk. Prospectively highly motivated individuals
should be hired with priority.
Keywords: nurses, personal values, population characteristics, task preference, work
motivation

Accepted for publication: 4 February 2013

(Hasselhorn et al. 2003, Zarea et al. 2009, Hill


Introduction
2011). Research suggests that nurses’ motivation
The shortage in nursing staff compromises quali- and job satisfaction is among the contributing fac-
fied patient care in many countries worldwide tors to high turnover of nurses and low enrolment

DOI: 10.1111/jonm.12083
1054 ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
The perfectly motivated nurse and the others

rates in nursing schools (Toode et al. 2010, Lu (Toode et al. 2010), and systematic research into the
et al. 2011). influence of personal values on motivation of nurses
Many approaches have been suggested to deal with seems to be lacking. The framework of ‘Motivation to
the nursing shortage. They include open communica- Care’ for professional nursing work by Moody and
tion, improving technology, nurse empowerment, Pesut (2006) defines motivation as a ‘values-based…
building long-lasting and fulfilling partnerships, and inner urge that activates and guides human behavior’.
efficient workplace organisation (Hussain et al. 2012). Personal values are concepts that guide individuals
However, limited funds are available to improve during their life, such as the concepts of benevolence,
nurses’ motivation, although more targeted spending self-direction and power, and have been systematically
might boost return on investment. Currently, funds researched by Schwartz (1992) and others. Personal
are often spent indiscriminately on the whole nursing values significantly influence preferences and beliefs.
workforce and on unfocused hiring activities. Possibil- In our research, we rely on the validated personal val-
ities for focused spending include (1) investing in ues theory of Schwartz and co-workers (Schwartz
training or workplace improvements for nurses who et al. 2001), which has specific descriptions of each of
are less motivated than the others and (2) focused the 10 personal values (see Table 1). Schwartz (1992)
hiring of prospectively highly motivated nurses. How- showed that all these different values are to some
ever, there is no consensus about which nurses are extent present in individuals of different races, nation-
perfectly motivated, and should be hired with priority, alities and social or cultural backgrounds, and pro-
and which nurses are currently less motivated and vided validated value measurement questionnaires.
would benefit from additional spending to improve However, no research has been found that systemati-
their work situation. cally investigates nurses’ motivation in relation to a
Factors that affect nurses’ work motivation can be fundamental model of personal values.
categorised as (1) workplace characteristics, (2) work-
ing conditions, (3) personal characteristics, (4) individ-
Objectives of the study
ual priorities and (5) internal psychological states
(Toode et al. 2010). A recent review summarized find- The objective of this study was to determine whether a
ings on factors that increased nurses’ motivation connection exists between motivation of nurses and
(Toode et al. 2010). Workplace characteristics (cate- their personal values. Furthermore, we aimed to increase
gory 1) were good team collaboration (Hertting et al.
2004), high autonomy or opportunities to learn (Jans-
Table 1
sen et al. 1999) and high workload (van den Berg
Personal values determined by Schwartz and their characteristics*
et al. 2006). Working conditions (category 2) included
suitable working hours (De Cooman et al. 2008), Personal value Characteristics
rewards and promotions (Hertting et al. 2004, Ozturk Achievement Personal success through demonstrating
et al. 2006, Peters et al. 2010). Personal characteris- competence according to social standards
tics (category 3) demonstrated that higher age in non- Benevolence Preservation and enhancement of the welfare
of people with whom one is in frequent personal
intensive care unit (ICU) nurses (van den Berg et al. contact.
2006) or lower age in general (Koivula et al. 1998) Conformity Restriction of actions, inclinations and impulses
increased motivation, as did higher-level college likely to accept or harm others and violate social
norms or standards.
education (Koivula et al. 1998) but not years of nurs- Hedonism Pleasure and sensuous gratification for oneself
ing experience (Kivimaki et al. 1995). Among individ- Power Social status and prestige, control and dominance
ual priorities (category 4) that increased motivation over people and resources
Security Safety, harmony and stability of society, of
were having control over own time (Hertting et al.
relationship and of self
2004) and the opportunity or personal calling to help Self-direction Independent thought and action-choosing,
others (Raatikainen 1997, Ozturk et al. 2006, De creating, exploring
Cooman et al. 2008). Internal psychological states Stimulation Excitement, novelty and challenge in life
Tradition Respect, commitment and acceptance of the
(category 5) included experiencing meaningfulness of customs and ideas that traditional culture or
the work (Hertting et al. 2004, Ozturk et al. 2006) religion provide the self.
and experiencing responsibility for outcomes (Edgar Universalism Understanding, appreciation, tolerance and
protection for the welfare of all people and for
1999, Ozturk et al. 2006). nature
However, current research about factors affecting
nurses’ work motivation is fragmented and insufficient *Table modified from Schwartz et al. (2001).

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064 1055
S. H. Koch et al.

knowledge on other motivational factors in the work- selected the number of questionnaires they wanted for
place characteristics (hospital department, ward type) their ward. They then explained the study and made
and personal characteristics (age, years of nursing the questionnaires equally available to nurses working
experience, professional expertise, gender, and, because in each shift.
of the focus of our study, computer use and After completing the questionnaires, participants
preference). returned them in sealed envelopes to the study office
where questionnaires were anonymized. Participants
could opt in for a lottery draw and wards with high
Methods
overall return rates received small rewards. For this
Attitudes towards a job (e.g. job dissatisfaction) have purpose, identifying and ward cover information was
been shown to influence behavioural intentions (e.g. recorded before and separately from accessing the
intention to reduce effort) (Fishbein & Ajzen 1975, questionnaire contents. The anonymized question-
Steers & Porter 1983). As behavioural intentions are naires were scanned and answers were automatically
concrete mental outcomes of underlying forces that processed and tabulated using Evasys. A test with a
used to be called motivation, we subsequently worked subset of the questionnaires confirmed the accuracy of
with the assumption that attitudes towards specific automatic scanning. Answers that Evasys could not
tasks can be used as proxies for the level of motiva- scan were processed manually.
tion. An exploratory study using questionnaires was
performed during 55 days in August and September
2011. According to the regulations of the Human Outcome measures
Subjects Review board, no approval was needed. Study outcome measures were (1) the participants’
Rather, the workers’ council was involved and gave dominant personal values, (2) their motivation and (3)
consent. their workplace, and personal characteristics. Out-
come measures were assessed through questionnaires,
Study context and participants which consisted of a total of 137 questions of which
95 were relevant to this paper. An additional section
The study was performed at the tertiary care hospital not covered here measured preference of hypothetical
of the University of Heidelberg, which acts as a regio- software features.
nal referral centre and is one of the largest and most
renowned medical centres in Germany. Specialized Dominant personal values
care is provided to about 700 000 patients yearly. At The dominant personal values of the participants were
the time of the study, the hospital employed 2851 determined through the portraits values questionnaire
nurses. (PVQ) (Schwartz et al. 2001) in the German version
The study was performed with nurses working in all (Hinz et al. 2002). The questionnaire included 40
areas except psychiatry and paediatrics because their questions, asking the participant to indicate his or her
organisational structure differed from the others. All agreement to 40 value-relevant properties on six-point
nurses working in eligible areas were invited to Likert scales. It asked participants to compare them-
participate. selves with a certain person. One example statement
from the English PVQ which measures power is: ‘It is
important to her to get respect from others. She wants
Data collection and measurement
people to do what she says’.
Questionnaires were developed in cooperation with
nurses working at the hospital and tested by five stu- Motivation
dents and nurses. The Evasys survey system (Electric Motivation was measured as the nurses’ attitude
Paper, L€uneburg, Germany) was used for layout, towards generic nursing tasks. A total of 45 questions
printing and later scanning of the 10-page question- about generic nursing tasks were developed based on
naires. literature review and expert consultation. According
Nursing managers of the 63 wards included were to nursing workplace textbooks and own experience
contacted and individually visited by the first author. (first author) these tasks are performed throughout
During the visit, nurse managers were informed about the hospital, although we did not collect data on
the aims, procedures, time-frame, potential incentives whether some tasks occur rarely in some settings. An
and available support during the study, and then example of a question to measure task preference is:

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


1056 Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064
The perfectly motivated nurse and the others

‘How much do you like participating in ward values, or characteristics of their workplace or person.
rounds?’ Nurses indicated their liking on six-point For workplace characteristics we asked for type of
Likert scales ranging from ‘very much’ to ‘not at all’. ward (three groups) and hospital department (five
In addition, participants could elect not to answer for groups). Personal characteristics were gender, years
specific tasks. This gave them the opportunity not to working in nursing (six groups), age (five groups), self-
vote on tasks they did not know well enough. The rated work experience (nine groups) and daily com-
choice not to answer was rarely made. Respective puter use (four groups). To ensure meaningful results,
items were left out and case numbers were reduced only groups of more than 30 participants were used for
accordingly. later analysis. We found 26 groups of that size from a
total of 44 groups, including the 10 personal values.
Workplace and personal characteristics Second, for each group a two sided t-test was per-
The workplace and personal characteristics were cov- formed using the statistical software SPSS version
ered by 10 questions. Workplace characteristics were 19.0.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) which compared the
determined through questions about the hospital group with the rest of the sample and determined how
departments (internal medicine, surgery, orthopaedics, many tasks group members liked significantly more or
womens’ health and ‘Kopfklinik’ – the departments less, with missing answers excluded test by test. The
of otorhinolaryngology, ophthalmology, neurology, relevant variance-dependent t-test statistic was deter-
neurosurgery, radiology and dentistry) and the ward mined through Levene’s test and a 95% confidence
type (general ward, intensive care unit, outpatient). interval was used. Finally, the percentage motivation
Personal characteristics were determined by eight was determined for each group: the number of tasks
questions. Using nine-point Likert scales nurses were with significantly different motivation (number of dis-
asked about their professional expertise (‘novice’ to liked tasks subtracted from number of liked tasks)
‘expert’) and computer preference (‘dislike’ to ‘like was divided by the total number of tasks. This num-
very much’). Computer use was measured in hours ber multiplied by 100 was later used as the percentage
per day (0, 1–2, 3–5, > 5) and in years since having of excess or shortcoming of motivation.
started to work with computers in general (0–1, 2–5, While the analysis above allows one to identify the
> 5). Age was measured in five decades (starting at general level of motivation or lack thereof in groups
16–25) and years working in nursing (0–1, 2–5, within our sample, we sought for patterns of discrimi-
6–10, 11–15, 16–20 and > 20). Gender was also nating tasks that draw a more subtle picture because
recorded. they share the capacity of being strongly liked by
some groups and strongly disliked by others. We were
interested in groups that differed strongly from other
Data analysis
groups in more than one such task and whether
A total of 265 of the originally distributed 1400 ques- respective tasks could be subsumed under some
tionnaires were returned. Among these, 212 were themes. Tasks were identified that satisfied two condi-
usable for the analysis. ‘Usable’ was defined as com- tions: for at least one group the average item value
pleted by nurses (as opposed to other hospital staff), was below the 5% quantile of that task in the whole
at least 50% of the questions answered and the PVQ sample and for at least one other group it was above
value questionnaire revealing one dominant value. the 95% quantile. For such tasks we assigned a D for
To determine a participant’s dominant personal ‘strongly dislike’ to that group (or groups) that scored
value, his or her answers were processed as described below the 5% quantile and an L for ‘strongly like’ to
by Schwartz et al. (2001), which resulted in a list of that group (or groups) that scored above the 95%
personal values for each participant, rank-ordered by quantile. For example, ‘advising relatives’ is one of the
relevance. In the sequel we only used each partici- discriminating tasks collected in Table 2 (below); it
pant’s dominant (most important) value after having scores below 5% of the average item value in the
removed participants where two or more values were whole sample in the group self-rated expertise = 6 and
equally important. As visualization, we determined the above the 95% quantile in the groups self-rated exper-
distribution of the three most frequently occurring tise = 8 and work experience > 21 years. We iterated
dominant personal values in relation to age (Figure 1). through all pairs of groups and identified such dis-
Differences in motivation were identified for groups criminating tasks and then ranked the pairs of groups
of participants through statistical analysis. First, par- according to the number of discriminating tasks they
ticipants were grouped according to their dominant were involved with.

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064 1057
S. H. Koch et al.

60%
50%

MoƟvaƟon
40%
30% Benevolence
20% Hedonism
10% Self-direcƟon
0%
< 25 26–35 36–45 >46
Age

Figure 1
Percentage of nurses with the three most frequent dominant values in relation to their age – a cross-section of prevalent values of the current
nurses’ population. Note that the figure does not show the longitudinal development of personal values of individuals over time.

Table 2
Tasks that discriminate

Results events during the study were that two callers expressed
privacy concerns during the study based on the per-
Workplace and personal characteristics,
sonal value part of the questionnaires and indicated
dominant personal values and other study
that they did not want to participate, and that three
coverage data
questionnaires were submitted in which the personal
Some of the characteristics of our sample have already value part was removed.
been described in the data analysis section above. We The prevalence of dominant personal values in relation
had a higher proportion of men (23%) apparently par- to age is not equally distributed over nurses with different
ticipating. About 18% of the nursing workforce of the ages (see Figure 1). Rather, with increasing age, the prev-
university are male, although we did not check whether alence of ‘hedonism’ decreases, ‘self-direction’ increases,
this proportion also holds for the individual wards and ‘benevolence’ is high through all age groups with a
where the questionnaires had been distributed. Other moderate peak at ages 36–45 years. The figure aggre-
potential sampling biases did not show. Unexpected gates the age groups 46–55 years and 56+ years because

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


1058 Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064
The perfectly motivated nurse and the others

of a low number of participants at ages 56–65 years. A presented for illustrative purposes. In workplace charac-
total of 152 (71%) participants had one of three domi- teristics, nurses working in surgery had the highest moti-
nant personal values. Each of these values occurred in at vation (+29%) among the groups identified, while
least 15% of the participants. The others (n = 60, 29%) nurses in internal medicine ( 9%) were less motivated.
had one of the remaining seven dominant values, which These differences cannot be explained as being caused
occurred less frequently and because of the small group through organisational structure, work atmosphere or
sizes were not further analysed. other characteristics of individual wards, because several
organisational units each (17 surgical and 16 internal
medicine wards) contributed to both percentage motiva-
Work motivation of groups with specific personal
tion values above. Using computers infrequently coin-
values, workplace or personal characteristics
cided with lower motivation ( 9%). For the remaining
An interesting correlation can be seen with respect to measurements (computer preference, computer use in
work experience and age. While only slight deviations years, and gender) and the groups not shown in Figure 2,
from the average motivation exist for work experience no difference exceeded the threshold of 9%.
below 15 years, motivation increases above 16 years For self-rated expertise, an interesting trend can be
(4% for 16–20 years and 16% for 21+ years). How- seen. Figure 3 shows that nurses with a low self-rated
ever, this trend seems to reverse with age: nurses aged expertise (item values 1–3, n = 22) had a slightly lower
36–45 (9%) were found to be motivated clearly above motivation when compared with other nurses with
average while ages 46–55 years had a small excess higher self-rated expertise. For medium self-rated exper-
motivation of 2% with further decrease to 4% in tise, (item value 4, n = 23) motivation rose to an average
ages 56–65 years, although here the sample size n = 8 proficiency, but with higher expertise (item values 5–7,
is far too small to be conclusive. n = 104) decreased again down to 15%. Finally, with
Figure 2 shows the results for groups of nurses with very high self-rated expertise (item values 8 and 9,
specific dominant personal values, or specific workplace n = 35 and n = 28, respectively) motivation sharply
or personal characteristics. To keep the results focused, increased to up to 24%. In addition, when all partici-
a threshold of +9% (for excess) or 9% for reduced pants with expertise above a threshold were compared
motivation is pragmatically suggested to designate a with the participants below the threshold, excess moti-
meaningful deviation from medium motivation. Among vation was 29% for expertise item values  7
personal values, ‘benevolence’ was found to coincide (n = 107) and excess motivation was 49% for expertise
with higher motivation, while ‘hedonism’ ( 9%) coin- item values  8 (n = 63). Although the groups with an
cided with lower motivation. Self-direction did not expertise of 1–5 and 9 had a low number of participants,
exceed the threshold but its lower motivation ( 7%) is they are shown in the figure for illustrative purposes.

35% Dominant Workplace characteristics Personal characteristics


personal value
25%

15%

5%

–5%

–15%

–25%
Benevolence
(n = 77)

General ward
Hedonism
(n = 34)

(n = 106)

Surgery (n = 87)

Internal Medicine
(n = 59)

ExperƟse = 6
(n = 34)

ExperƟse = 8

Work experience
(n = 35)

>21 y (n = 49)

(n = 36)

Use 1..2 hrs (n = 94)


Age 36…45 y

Daily Computer

Figure 2
Motivation for specific dominant personal values, self-rated expertise, work area and other individual traits. Only characteristics above the
threshold of 9% and groups with at least 30 participants are reported. Numbers of distinct organisational units in the workplace characteristics
block where respondents worked were 39 (general wards), 17 (surgical wards) and 16 (internal medicine wards). For a Likert scale of 1 to 9
self-rated expertise = 6 means a medium self-esteem, while 8 means high self esteem. For daily computer use 1–2 hours is the second
lowest possible choice while 0 hours was below the 30-person threshold.

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064 1059
S. H. Koch et al.

criminating theme between nurses in internal medicine


versus surgery.

Discussion
The study could show an empirical connection between
nurses’ motivation in terms of preference for nursing
tasks and their personal values, workplace or personal
characteristics. The perfectly motivated nurse seems to
Figure 3
be one with dominant personal value of ‘benevolence’,
Motivation (% of the overall tasks) in relation to self-rated expertise. or high self-rated expertise, or in the middle of his/her
career or who works in surgical or general wards. By
We now take a closer look at pairs of groups among contrast, lower motivation was found in nurses with
the groups of Figure 3, in search of pairs whose the dominant value of ‘hedonism’ or nurses who
discriminating tasks form an intelligible pattern and worked in internal medicine, had a low to medium
suggest some underlying cause. Overall, we found 26 self-rated expertise or who use computers infrequently.
discriminating tasks (i.e. tasks that were strongly dis- These are cliches, of course, and all variations exist
liked by at least one and strongly disliked by another between these extremes. It should be noted, however,
group of nurses). Four discriminating tasks were sin- that sample broadness was wide enough to claim that
gle: each such task discriminated just one pair of nurse results are not determined through local workplace
groups. Ten discriminating tasks could be grouped as characteristics of individual wards. Each column of
pairs of two: each of such two tasks discriminated Figure 2 represents  30 subjects from several wards
between the same groups of nurse. However, none of each. Therefore, we believe that our findings deliver
these 14 revealed meaningful themes. In contrast, some material for both targeted recruiting and human
Table 2 displays those tasks that were involved in resource programmes directed to those whose motiva-
identifying pairs of groups characterized through at tion may be at risk. Below, we address first how our
least three discriminating tasks. findings relate to those of others.
Table 2 lists nine of those 45 tasks that were most
involved in discriminating. It presents the task’s inter-
Relationship to other studies
nal identifier, the task itself, those groups from
Figure 3 that were discriminated by that task and the Appropriateness of our taking preferences for nursing
indicator D or L. The Table shows that the already tasks as a proxy for motivation may not appear
described largest swing from low to high general moti- straight forward. Few other approaches to concepts
vation with increasing self-rated expertise carries such as motivation have been taken in other investiga-
through to the detailed analysis. Through the tasks T3, tions two of which will now be compared.
T18, T40, T39 and T19 the nurse group self-rated Simpson (2009) worked on the related concept of
expertise = 6 formed five discriminating pairs (D) with work engagement. She refers to an approved approach
group self-rated expertise = 8 (L), and it was four times of measuring work engagement by Schaufeli et al.
D in discrimination from work experience 21+ years (2006), which, according to Simpson, has been
(L), where three tasks (T3, T18 and T40) led to dis- applied to all kinds of professions other than nursing.
criminations in both pairs. Thus, we see a first pattern She presented the respective questionnaire to 167
here of little vs. much knowledge or experience on the nurses in six hospitals and arrived at three major
part of the nurses leading to disliking or liking tasks determinants of work engagement in nurses: vigour,
T3, T18, T40, T39 and T19. A second pattern is seen dedication and absorption. At first glance, mainly ded-
in the discrimination between nurses working in inter- ication relates to our approach as we ask how much
nal medicine (three times D) vs. surgery (three times nurses like certain nursing tasks. However, Simpson’s
L) in tasks T21, T30 and T12. items are more distant and abstract. She asked about
When trying to characterize the tasks of the two attitudes towards work in general rather than attitudes
patterns above, information and communication are towards nursing work. Her investigation allows com-
the common theme of tasks that discriminates parison across professions, but lacks the opportunity
between more and less knowledgeable nurses while of a closer look into the subtleties of nurses’ work.
patient case-related action tasks are the common dis- Furthermore, Simpson investigated the more dramatic

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


1060 Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064
The perfectly motivated nurse and the others

situation of breaking up with one’s job, while we con- Although no research was found that systematically
sider motivation within ongoing professional work. links nurses’ motivation to personal values, other stud-
Finally, the work engagement questionnaires that ies partly second our findings of the influence of per-
Schaufeli et al. (2006) developed for scientific research sonal values on motivation. Two concepts that relate to
settings may be rejected as being too intrusive for rou- ‘benevolence’ were found to increase motivation in
tine personnel screening outside scientific research sit- other studies: experiencing meaningfulness of the work
uations. Schaufeli’s questions share this risk (Schwartz (Hertting et al. 2004, Ozturk et al. 2006) and experi-
et al. 2001) with value questions that we applied and encing responsibility for outcomes (Edgar 1999, Ozturk
that initiated the idea of using other less intrusive et al. 2006). Another concept that relates to ‘self-direc-
questions as proxies. tion’ was found to coincide with increased motivation:
In an investigation of impressive size (8556 of having control over one’s own time (Hertting et al.
approximately 13 000 invited nurses completed their 2004). In contrast, our study found that participants
questionnaires) Tourangeau and Cranley (2006) also with a predominant value of self-direction had slightly
investigated motivational determinants in the context lower motivation (7%). These results are not shown in
of keeping or leaving a job. Among the major factor Figure 2 because they did not meet the threshold inclu-
groups they found personal characteristics and organi- sion criteria for significant influence on motivation.
sational commitment apparently related to factors we This may mean that the concepts actually measured by
tried to cover. However, in their research, personal Hertting et al. (2004) were more superficially practical
characteristics primarily means age or level of educa- than the deeply rooted ‘self-direction’.
tion and, as it appears, values. However, values are not Our findings of workplace characteristics that
meant in the theoretically and experimentally approved increased motivation, such as working at general
Schwartz sense (Schwartz 1992, Schwartz et al. 2001), wards vs. ICU or outpatient departments might relate
but in a common sense ad hoc understanding of ‘loy- to research about greater work satisfaction in interme-
alty’, ‘seniority’, ‘job security’ and ‘career security’. In diate care nurses compared with ICU nurses (Goetz
Tourangeau and Cranley’s (2006) work organisational et al. 2012), although another study did not find moti-
commitment is even more immediate and ad hoc: it is vational differences in ICU/non-ICU nurses (Tummers
number of years with the current hospital. We do not et al. 2002). In our investigation, internal medicine
address organisational commitment explicitly but it and surgery nurses differed in their general level of
may be found in some of our items (organise processes motivation and specifically in their motivation for
at the ward, control pharmacy stock) and we would patient case-related action tasks.
have added more if it had been our core interest. Among personal characteristics, our findings about
To summarize, to the best of our knowledge, there increased motivation in nurses aged 36–45 years are
are no investigations that come really close to captur- inconclusive with other studies which, on one hand,
ing motivation as such, detached from the question of found higher motivation with greater age in non-ICU
whether nurses will stay or leave. This does not mean nurses (van den Berg et al. 2006) but, on the other
that we perfectly map motivation through the prefer- hand, with lower age in nurses in general (Koivula
ences for nursing tasks, but at present it seems to be et al. 1998). Our finding of higher motivation corre-
the best approximation, especially as the low intru- lated with high self-rated expertise or lower motiva-
siveness of our common nursing work task-related tion correlated with low to medium self-rated
questions suggests appropriateness outside of the con- expertise could potentially relate to a study that found
text of scientific research. higher motivation in nurses with higher-level college
Our findings about the influence of personal values education (Koivula et al. 1998). This consideration is
on motivation coincide with individual traits, which supported by the pattern of information and knowl-
were found to increase motivation in other studies. The edge tasks that strongly contrast the medium self-rated
concept of ‘benevolence’, which we found to coincide expertise with the high self-rated expertise group. For
with higher motivation, is described by Schwartz et al. nursing experience our results of higher motivation
(2001) as the ‘Preservation and enhancement of the wel- with higher experience seem to be contrary to a study
fare of people with whom one is in frequent personal that did not find a correlation with nursing experience
contact’. This concept seems to relate to the opportu- (Kivimaki et al. 1995), but seem to correlate with
nity or personal calling to help others that previous results in experimental social psychology in a non-
research found to increase motivation (Raatikainen nursing population, where task attractiveness (the way
1997, Ozturk et al. 2006, De Cooman et al. 2008). in which we measured motivation) correlated with

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064 1061
S. H. Koch et al.

expertise (Trope 1980). This relates to our second by dominant value. In contrast, Glazer and Beehr’s
strongest contrast between medium self-rated expertise (2002) research determined the average personal values
vs. long work experience. No study could be identified of the whole population. However, their globally domi-
with results resembling our findings of lower motiva- nant values were similar to ours: ‘benevolence’, ‘self-
tion in nurses who use computers infrequently. This direction’, as in our sample, followed by ‘universality’,
lack of evidence may be because spending hours and which was fourth in our sample but was not followed
hours at the computer is a relatively new phenome- up because of our smaller group size. They determined
non. Investigations may be underway but results have ‘conformity’ instead of ‘hedonism’ to be among the
not yet reached the journals. four most important values. A potential explanation
Work motivation theories (Latham & Pinder 2005) might be cultural difference or a difference in the value
might suggest explanations for the discrepancies determination methods. Another possible explanation
between our findings and other studies for the differ- is that the ‘hedonistic’ nurse has entered this stage after
ent workplace characteristics – for example our the end of their investigation (in our sample it predomi-
findings of higher motivation on surgical or lower nantly is the youngest generation, which was still in
motivation on internal medicine wards, as well as dif- high school when Glazer & Beehr collected their data)
ferences in ICU vs. non ICU nurses. Job design charac- whereas the conformant nurse may have left this stage
teristics were found to influence an individual’s in our investigation and would have become visible if
motivation (Locke & Latham 2002), offering a poten- we had interviewed retired nurses.
tial reason for the differences we found in surgical vs. Regarding such ‘generations’ arguments, our
internal medicine nurses. Such job design characteris- research is in accord with Tourangeau and Cranley
tics might include a difference in job autonomy, work- (2006) who identified a silent generation born before
load, rewards policy, work organisation, decision 1945 and Generation X born between 1965 and 1980.
authority and team collaboration, which were found In their work, silence means loyalty and acceptance of
to influence motivation in other studies (Janssen et al. seniority and is a way of conforming with one’s work
1999, Tummers et al. 2002, 2006, Hertting et al. environment. We did not find this ‘conformity’ fre-
2004, van den Berg et al. 2006, Ozturk et al. 2006, quently as a dominant value in our sample of nurses,
De Cooman et al. 2008). who were, however, mostly born after 1956. Touran-
Our finding of different percentages of dominant val- geau and Cranley’s (2006) ‘Xers’ overlap with our age
ues for nurses with different ages seems to contradict groups 26–35 years and 36–45 years. Our age groups
the research of Schwartz (1992) who found that per- are characterized by a strong ‘self-direction’ value. In
sonal values do not change over time. However, differ- our categorization of values according to Schwartz
ent percentages of dominant values in our research (1992) ‘self-direction’ is central in Schwartz’ quadrant
were not measured longitudinally over time in one ‘openness for change’. This is precisely what Touran-
cohort of nurses. Our measurement is a cross-section, geau and Cranley (2006) report about their Xers.
which might show that the nursing profession has
attracted persons with a different set of dominant val- Future research
ues in past decades. For example, during the 1960s
Future research should replicate our study in other
‘benevolence’ and ‘self-direction’ clearly dominated,
populations and other settings and with a larger popu-
whereas during the 2000s ‘hedonism’ almost reached
lation to determine motivational influences for other
the level of ‘benevolence’, reflecting that the perception
less prevalent personal values. In addition, future
of nursing tasks at the time when the decision is made
research might use other personal factors (e.g. paren-
to become a nurse changes over time. Another potential
tal leave). In addition, propensity to perform nursing
explanation could be that nurses with certain dominant
tasks has been used as proxy for motivation
values have a higher probability of remaining within
throughout this investigation. From our findings it
the nursing profession.
can, at best, be a proxy for professional motivation
Our findings of three dominant values (‘benevolence’,
and other methods to measure motivation in general
‘self-direction’, ‘hedonism’) is partly seconded by other
should be also used for comparison.
research in nurses in Hungary, Italy, the UK and the
USA (Glazer & Beehr 2002). Our approach used differ-
Limitations
ent methods to determine personal values: we deter-
mined the dominant, single most important value of The low return rate of 212 usable questionnaires of
each individual and then formed subsets of the sample 1400 distributed to eligible clinical units is a clear lim-

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


1062 Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064
The perfectly motivated nurse and the others

itation of the investigation. Nurses may have refrained ence of personal values on nurses’ work motivation
from filling in the questionnaire because they deemed could be identified.
it only minimally compensated for additional effort or
What this paper adds:
they may have perceived it as an intrusion into their
● An empirical connection exists between nurses’
privacy. The latter especially bears the risk of a selec-
tion bias as to the personality-related objectives of the work motivation and their dominant personal val-
investigation. ues: groups of nurses with the dominant personal
The generalizability of our study is limited because value of ‘benevolence’ are more motivated, while
our sample was recruited at a single hospital. individuals with ‘hedonism’ are less motivated.
● Higher motivation was found in nurses with high self-
Although this was a large university hospital, findings
at other hospitals or in other countries might differ. rated expertise, nurses in the middle of their career
Furthermore, our findings could potentially be biased and nurses working on surgical or general wards.
● Lower motivation was found in nurses in internal
because we offered an incentive: potentially more
nurses might have participated with dominant values medicine, or who had a low to medium self-rated
which are keen on incentives (e.g. ‘hedonism’). The expertise or used computers infrequently.
generalizability is also possibly limited because of dif-
ferent hospital settings: although generic task descrip- Implications for nursing management
tions were used, and many wards of each major type
(surgical, internal medicine, ICU, non-ICU, etc.) con- The implications of our findings that nurses’ motiva-
tributed completed questionnaires, it cannot be safely tion correlates with their workplace and personal
excluded that task preferences were influenced by the characteristics can be used by hospital management to
way in which tasks are implemented in organisational focus efforts on increasing motivation. Training or
structures of specific clinics. workplace improvements can be increased for nurses
working in internal medicine, ICUs, and functions,
and for nurses with the dominant value of ‘hedonism’
Conclusion and to a lesser extent ‘self-direction’. Highly moti-
vated nurses coming from surgery or general wards,
The tentative criteria presented to inform human and with the dominant values of ‘benevolence’ can be
resource management about staff motivation have been hired with priority. Such an approach should, how-
found and tested in an academic teaching hospital. The ever, have in mind that the measurement of personal
approach fosters an indirect assessment of motivational values, as it was done in this investigation by using
factors, which helps to differentiate between motiva- the 40-item PVQ value questionnaire, is somewhat
tion-related needs of subpopulations of the nursing intrusive and might raise privacy concerns when done
work force. Programmes to increase motivation of routinely as part of the hiring procedure. However,
nurses currently working could be more focused and asking job applicants about the amount of time they
potentially more efficient, and hiring activities could spend with computers or about their preferences for
better aim at attracting individuals with higher a priori certain nursing tasks, does not have any stigma
motivation. However, further research is required in attached and might be a feasible question for job
order to transfer this information to other care settings applicants. Therefore, a selection of the questions we
and to reduce the intrusiveness of personal values asked our subjects about their work and task related
questionnaires. preferences can take the place of the potentially too-
What is already known about the topic: intrusive value-related questions.
● Motivation is influenced by factors ranging from
the individual, the workplace, working conditions, Acknowledgement
individual priorities and internal psychological
The authors thank all nurses of the Heidelberg Univer-
states. However, this knowledge is still fragmented
sity Hospital who served as voluntary subjects.
and insufficient.
● Personal values are concepts that guide individuals
Sources of funding
during their life (e.g. the concepts of ‘benevolence’,
‘self-direction’ and ‘power’). Although some indi- The research has been financially supported by DFG
vidual properties have been previously linked to (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) under grant
motivation, no systematic research about the influ- WE2467_7–1.

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064 1063
S. H. Koch et al.

Ethical approval Latham G.P. & Pinder C.C. (2005) Work motivation theory
and research at the dawn of the twenty-first century. Annual
According to German legislation permission was given Review of Psychology 56, 485–516.
through the Workers’ Council of Heidelberg Univer- Locke E.A. & Latham G.P. (2002) Building a practically useful
sity Hospital. theory of goal setting and task motivation – a 35-year odyssey.
American Psychologist 57 (9), 705–717.
Lu H., Barriball K.L., Zhang X. & While A.E. (2011) Job satis-
References faction among hospital nurses revisited, a systematic review.
International Journal of Nursing Studies 49 (8), 1017–1038.
van den Berg T.I., Landeweerd J.A., Tummers G.E. & van Moody R.C. & Pesut D.J. (2006) The motivation to care, appli-
Merode G.G. (2006) A comparative study of organisational cation and extension of motivation theory to professional
characteristics, work characteristics and nurses’ psychological nursing work. Journal of Health Organization and Manage-
work reactions in a hospital and nursing home setting. Inter- ment 20 (1), 15–48.
national Journal of Nursing Studies 43 (4), 491–505. Ozturk H., Bahcecik N. & Baumann S.L. (2006) Nursing satis-
De Cooman R., De Gieter S., Pepermans R., Du Bois C., Caers R. faction and job enrichment in Turkey. Nursing Science Quar-
& Jegers M. (2008) Freshmen in nursing, job motives and terly 19 (4), 360–365.
work values of a new generation. Journal of Nursing Manage- Peters D.H., Chakraborty S., Mahapatra P. & Steinhardt L.
ment 16 (1), 56–64. (2010) Job satisfaction and motivation of health workers in
Edgar L. (1999) Nurses’ motivation and its relationship to the public and private sectors, cross-sectional analysis from two
characteristics of nursing care delivery systems, a test of the Indian states. Human Resources for Health 8, 27.
job characteristics model. Canadian Journal of Nursing Lead- Raatikainen R. (1997) Nursing care as a calling. Journal of
ership 12 (1), 14–22. Advanced Nursing 25 (6), 1111–1115.
Fishbein M. & Ajzen I. (1975) Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Schaufeli W.B., Bakker A.B. & Salanova M. (2006) The mea-
Behavior, An Introduction to Theory and Research. Addison- surement of work engagement with a short questionnaire, a
Wesley, Reading, MA. cross-national study. Educational and Psychological Measure-
Glazer S. & Beehr T.A. (2002) Similarities and differences in ment 66 (4), 701–716.
human values between nurses in four countries. Interna- Schwartz S.H. ed. (1992) Universals in the Content and Struc-
tional Journal of Cross Cultural Management 2 (2), 185– ture of Values, Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in
202. 20 Countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.
Goetz K., Beutel S., Mueller G., Trierweiler-Hauke B. & Mahler Academic Press, San Diego, CA.
C. (2012) Work-related behaviour and experience patterns of Schwartz S.H., Melech G., Lehmann A., Burgess S., Harris S. &
nurses. International Nursing Review 59 (1), 88–93. Owens V. (2001) Extending the cross-cultural validity of the the-
Hasselhorn H.M., Tackenberg P. & M€ uller B.H. (2003) Prema- ory of basic human values with a different method of measure-
ture departure from nursing in Germany as a growing prob- ment. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 32 (5), 519–542.
lem for the health care system – a review. Gesundheitswesen Simpson M. (2009) Predictors of work engagement among med-
65 (1), 40–46 (in German). ical–surgical registered nurses. Western Journal of Nursing
Hertting A., Nilsson K., Theorell T. & Larsson U.S. (2004) Research 31 (1), 44–65.
Downsizing and reorganization, demands, challenges and Steers R.M. & Porter L.W. (1983) Motivation and Work
ambiguity for registered nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing Behavior. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
45 (2), 145–154. Toode K., Routasalo P. & Suominen T. (2010) Work motiva-
Hill K.S. (2011) Nursing and the aging workforce, myths and tion of nurses, a literature review. International Journal of
reality, what do we really know? Nursing Clinics of North Nursing Studies 48 (2), 246–257.
America 46 (1), 1–9. Tourangeau A. & Cranley L.A. (2006) Nurse intention to
Hinz A., Albani C., Gießler A. & Br€ ahler E. (2002) Welche remain employed, understanding and strengthening determi-
Werte sind den Deutschen etwas wert? Ergebnisse einer nants. Journal of Advanced Nursing 55 (4), 497–509.
repr€asentativen Umfrage Psychosozial 25 (2), 21–30. Trope Y. (1980) Self-assessment, self-enhancement, and task
Hussain A., Rivers P.A., Glover S.H. & Fottler M.D. (2012) preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 16 (2),
Strategies for dealing with future shortages in the nursing 116–129.
workforce, a review. Health Services Management Research Tummers G.E., van Merode G.G. & Landeweerd J.A. (2002)
25 (1), 41–47. The diversity of work, differences, similarities and relation-
Janssen P.P., Jonge J.D. & Bakker A.B. (1999) Specific determi- ships concerning characteristics of the organisation, the work
nants of intrinsic work motivation, burnout and turnover and psychological work reactions in intensive care and non-
intentions, a study among nurses. Journal of Advanced Nurs- intensive care nursing. International Journal of Nursing Stud-
ing 29 (6), 1360–1369. ies 39 (8), 841–855.
Kivimaki M., Voutilainen P. & Koskinen P. (1995) Job enrich- Tummers G.E., van Merode G.G. & Landeweerd J.A. (2006)
ment, work motivation, and job satisfaction in hospital Organizational characteristics as predictors of nurses’ psycho-
wards, testing the job characteristics model. Journal of Nurs- logical work reactions. Organization Studies 27 (4), 559–584.
ing Management 3 (2), 87–91. Zarea K., Negarandeh R., Dehghan-Nayeri N. & Rezaei-
Koivula M., Paunonen M. & Laippala P. (1998) Prerequisites Adaryani M. (2009) Nursing staff shortages and job satisfac-
for quality improvement in nursing. Journal of Nursing Man- tion in Iran, issues and challenges. Nursing and Health
agement 6 (6), 333–342. Sciences 11 (3), 326–331.

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


1064 Journal of Nursing Management, 2014, 22, 1054–1064

You might also like