You are on page 1of 6

Neuroscience Letters 405 (2006) 24–28

Articulatory rehearsal in verbal working memory: A possible


neurocognitive endophenotype that differentiates between
schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder

Oliver Gruber , Eva Gruber, Peter Falkai
Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Saarland University Hospital, Homburg (Saar),
Germany
Received 26 April 2006; received in revised form 8 June 2006; accepted 9 June 2006

Abstract
Recent fMRI studies have identified brain systems underlying different components of working memory in healthy individuals. The aim of
this study was to compare the functional integrity of these neural networks in terms of behavioural performance in patients with schizophrenia,
schizoaffective disorder and healthy controls. In order to detect specific working memory deficits based on dysfunctions of underlying brain circuits
we used the same verbal and visuospatial Sternberg item-recognition tasks as in previous neuroimaging studies. Clinical and performance data
from matched groups consisting of 14 subjects each were statistically analyzed. Schizophrenic patients exhibited pronounced impairments of
both verbal and visuospatial working memory, whereas verbal working memory performance was preserved in schizoaffective patients. The
findings provide first evidence that dysfunction of a brain system subserving articulatory rehearsal could represent a biological marker which
differentiates between schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.
© 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Functional neuroanatomy; Brain imaging; Schizophrenia; Pathophysiology; Neurocognitive endophenotype

There is a continuing debate on whether schizoaffective dis- ies cover a variety of cognitive functions. As a result these
order represents a diagnostic entity that can be biologically tasks are reasonably sensitive to cognitive impairments, but
distinguished from schizophrenia [7,13,16,21,23]. they lack specificity for detecting more selective dysfunctions
Comparative neuropsychological studies of patients with these of neural systems that underlie single cognitive processes. Just
two diagnoses may help to clarify this issue. While there is recently, researchers have begun to apply more refined task
an abundance of neuropsychological investigations in paradigms derived from experimental cognitive psychology in
schizophrenic patients indicating a broad range of cognitive order to determine cognitive dysfunctions in a more specific
impairments with a focus on working memory, attention and way [7]. Furthermore, due to the advent of functional brain
problem-solving [2,12,15], little is known about imaging tech- niques it has become possible to identify brain
neurocognitive deficits in schizoaffective patients. Existing regions involved in the single cognitive processes that are
evidence suggests that schizoaffective patients exhibit a pattern of necessary to perform these experimental tasks. Combining
cognitive impairments very similar to the one observed in functional brain imaging in healthy human subjects and
schizophrenia [2]. experimental neuropsychologi- cal investigation in patients
In particular, schizoaffective patients did not perform may prove helpful in discovering the neuropathophysiological
signif- icantly different from patients with schizophrenia in a mechanisms of different psychi- atric disorders.
number of classical neuropsychological tasks measuring Working memory dysfunctions are a promising candidate
various aspects of memory, executive functions, cognitive endophenotype that may enlighten schizophrenia research
flexibility, reasoning and problem-solving [1,4,7,17,18,22]. field as to the genetic and other etiologic factors [3,6]. Recent
However, this evidence is still very limited as most of the tasks functional neuroimaging studies have shown with high con-
that were used in these stud- sistency that distinct brain systems underlie different working
memory components in humans [8–11]. The aim of the present
study was to compare the functional integrity of these neural

Corresponding author. Tel.: +49 6841 16 24245; fax: +49 6841 16 24270.
E-mail address: oliver.gruber@uniklinikum-saarland.de (O. Gruber).

0304-3940/$ – see front matter © 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights
reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.062
O. Gruber et al. / Neuroscience Letters 405 (2006) 24– 25
28
networks in patients with schizoaffective disorder, in patients Table 1
with schizophrenia as well as in healthy controls. In order to Sample demographic and clinical variables
attribute working memory deficits observed in the patients
Schizophrenia Schizoaffective Control
to specific neural network dysfunctions, the present study Number of 14 14 14
employed the same experimental design as used in one of our subjects Gender 64.3 64.3 64.3
(% female)
previous fMRI studies [11]. Age at evaluation 37.8 (9.2) 38.4 38.9 (8.6)
(8.3)
Fourteen schizoaffective and 31 schizophrenic inpatients Education (y) 13.7 (3.5) 13.7 13.7 (2.7)
(3.3)
were recruited from the Psychiatric Department at Saarland Uni- Age at onset 30.6 (7.6) 26.4 (5.8)

Duration of illness (y) 7.2 (8.5) 11.9 (6.5)

versity Hospital. Inclusion criteria for patients were diagnosis
of schizoaffective disorder or schizophrenia according to ICD- Symptom severity
CGI 3.8 (1.3) 3.8 –
10 and DSM-IV criteria and age range from 18 to 65 years. (1.0)
Due to weak diagnostic reliability, in particular during the ini- PANSS positive 11.1 (5.1) 10.9 –
tial diagnosis of schizoaffective disorder, differences between (6.5)
PANSS negative 12.1 (3.4) 10.6 –
patient groups may obscure [13]. Therefore, we only included Medication type
patients with at least three prior hospital admissions that ful- Neuroleptics
filled the aforementioned criteria based on a consensus of several Typical only 2 0 –
experienced clinicians including the supervision by two senior Atypical only 12 10 –
researchers. Exclusion criteria were acute suicidality, involun- Both types 0 4 –
Approximated daily 316 485 –
tary treatment, current substance abuse, history of brain
CPZ equivalents
trauma, diseases with alterations of cerebral metabolism, Mood stabilizers
uncorrected
visual or auditory disability and mental retardation. Criteria Lithium 0 1 –
for Other 0 10 –
recruitment of 41 healthy comparison subjects were the same Antidepressants 1 6 –
as for patients plus the absence of any past or present Benzodiazepines 5 5 –
psychiatric disorder. All subjects gave written informed Anti-Parkinson agents 2 0 –
consent. None (unmedicated) 0 0 14
Experimental neuropsychological testing was performed
under standardized laboratory conditions. The behavioural
experiment consisted of four tasks, testing for different types 39) = 4.95, p < 0.05) as well as a trend for between-group effects
of working memory functions, i.e. articulatory rehearsal, non- in the two visuospatial task conditions (visuospatial “rehearsal”:
articulatory maintenance of phonological information, visu- F(2, 39) = 2.63, p < 0.1; visuospatial pattern maintenance: F(2,
ospatial “rehearsal” and maintenance of visuospatial patterns. 39) = 2.46, p < 0.1). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc contrasts
The order of the tasks was counterbalanced across subjects. indicated a significantly reduced performance of
For a detailed description of the experimental paradigm, the schizophrenic patients in the articulatory rehearsal task
stim- ulation protocol and the task instructions see Gruber and compared to both healthy controls (p < 0.001; d (effect size
von Cramon [11]. The clinical state of the patients was index) = 0.86) and schizoaffective patients (p < 0.005; d = 0.77;
assessed at the day of the experiment using clinical global see Fig. 1D). On the contrary, schizoaffective patients did not
impression scale (CGI), montgomery asberg depression scale show a reduction of performance in comparison with healthy
(MADRS), positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS). controls (p = 1.0; d = 0.09). In the second verbal working
After obtaining data from 14 patients with schizoaffective memory task test- ing for non-articulatory maintenance of
disorder, out of the groups of 31 schizophrenic patients and 41 phonological informa- tion, schizophrenic patients performed
healthy control subjects subgroups were recruited that significantly worse than healthy controls (p < 0.05; d = 0.61)
matched the schizoaffective patients regarding gender, age whereas no difference was observed between schizoaffective
and educa- tional status. Statistical analyses were carried out patients and healthy controls (p = 1.0; d = 0.13). Direct
using SPSS for Windows. comparison between schizophrenic
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the patient and
control groups are shown in Table 1. As a result of careful match- Table 2
Accuracy on the different working memory tasks (mean percentage of correct
ing, the three groups of 14 subjects each did not differ from
answers and standard deviation)
each other with regard to age (F(2, 39) = 0.06, p = 0.94), gen-
der (p = 1.0) or years of education (p = 1.0). The two patient Schizophrenia Schizoaffective Control
(n = 14) (n = 14) (n =
groups were also highly comparable regarding psychopatholog-
14)
ical symptoms according to the rating scales.
Behavioural performance data as shown in Table 2 were Articulatory rehearsal 81.9 ± 11.6 92.8 ± 4.6 94.0 ± 4.5
nor-
mally distributed except of the percentage of correct responses group Non-articulatory maintenance of phonological
in the visuospatial “rehearsal” task (Kolmogorov–Smirnov information
test: p < 0.05). One-way ANOVA revealed significant between-
26 O. Gruber et al. / Neuroscience Letters 405 (2006) 24–
77.2 ± 11.2 85.1 ± 9.1 87.2 ± 5.6 28
effects in task performance for the two verbal task condi- Visuospatial “rehearsal” 83.4 ± 13.4 88.7 ± 8.6 91.7 ± 5.6
tions (articulatory rehearsal: F(2, 39) = 10.62, p < 0.001; non- Visuospatial pattern 81.4 ± 12.9 80.5 ± 15.8 90.0 ± 7.2
articulatory maintenance of phonological information: F(2, maintenance
Fig. 1. Brain systems underlying specific components of human working memory (A: Articulatory rehearsal; B: Non-articulatory maintenance of phonological
information; C: Visuospatial working memory, i.e. both visuospatial “rehearsal” and visuospatial pattern maintenance) [11] and dysfunctions of these brain
systems indicated by performance deficits in matched groups of patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder (D).

and schizoaffective patients revealed a trend for a minor per- the level of significance (schizophrenic patients versus con-
formance in the former group (p = 0.07; d = 0.48). Furthermore, trols: p = 0.23; d = 0.37; schizoaffective patients versus controls:
in the visuospatial “rehearsal” task there was also a statistical p = 0.15; d = 0.41; schizophrenic versus schizoaffective patients:
trend for reduced performance of schizophrenic patients as com- p = 1.0; d = 0.04).
pared to healthy subjects (p = 0.09; d = 0.46) whereas differences Because the assumption of normality of performance data
between schizoaffective and schizophrenic patients (p = 0.46; was not met for all experimental task conditions (see above), the
d = 0.30) as well as between schizoaffective patients and healthy results of the parametric statistical tests were confirmed by
controls (p = 1.0; d = 0.17) were not significant. Finally, both non- parametric tests. Using the Kruskal–Wallis test we
groups of patients showed reduced performance in the visu- confirmed the significance of between-group effects in verbal
ospatial pattern maintenance task. However, due to a high working mem- ory task performance (articulatory rehearsal: p <
inter- individual variability these performance reductions did not 0.005; non- articulatory maintenance of phonological
reach information: p < 0.05)
whereas between-group effects in the visuospatial working serving articulatory rehearsal in verbal working memory is
memory tasks did not reach the level of significance (visuospatial common in schizophrenia, but not in schizoaffective disorder
rehearsal: p = 0.29; visuospatial pattern maintenance: p = 0.12). patients, whereas both patient groups share deficits of visu-
Mann–Whitney tests confirmed the reductions of verbal work- ospatial working memory. Further investigations in a higher
ing memory performance in schizophrenic patients compared number of subjects as well as in relatives are required in order to
to healthy controls (articulatory rehearsal: Z = 3.02, p = 0.002; explore whether characterization of patients according to these
non-articulatory maintenance of phonological − information:
neurocognitive parameters may facilitate the development of a
Z = 2.58, p = 0.009) as well as to schizoaffective patients (artic- neurobiological classification of psychiatric disorders.
− rehearsal: Z = 2.79, p = 0.004; non-articulatory main-
ulatory
tenance of phonological−information: Z = 2.00, p = 0.04).
Taken together, schizophrenic patients − exhibited marked
References
deficits in both verbal and visuospatial working memory. By
contrast, verbal working memory performance was preserved [1] W.W. Beatty, Z. Jocic, N. Monson, R.D. Staton, Memory and frontal- lobe
in a matched group of schizoaffective patients (Table 2, Fig. dysfunction in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder, J. Nerv. Ment.
1D). A series of functional MRI studies has reliably Dis. 181 (1993) 448–453.
demonstrated that different dissociable brain systems subserve [2] R.W. Buchanan, M. Davis, D. Goff, M.F. Green, R.S.E. Keefe, A.C. Leon,
K.H. Nuechterlein, T. Laughren, R. Levin, E. Stover, W. Fen- ton,
working mem- ory performance in the same tasks as used in
S.R. Marder, A summary of the FDA-NIMH-MATRICS workshop on
the present study [8–11]. Based on these prior findings, the clinical trial design for neurocognitive drugs for schizophrenia, Schizophr.
present study suggests that the functional integrity of a left- Bull. 31 (2005) 5–19.
hemispheric premotor- parietal network underlying the [3] H.M. Conklin, C.E. Curtis, J. Katsanis, W.G. Iacono, Verbal working
articulatory rehearsal mech- anism (Fig. 1A) is disturbed in memory impairment in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree rel-
atives: evidence from the digit span task, Am. J. Psychiatry 157 (2000)
terms of impaired perfor- mance in the majority of patients
275–277.
with schizophrenia, but not in patients with schizoaffective [4] J.D. Evans, R.K. Heaton, J.S. Paulsen, L.A. McAdams, S.C. Heaton,
disorder. This difference can- not be attributed to effects of D.V. Jeste, Schizoaffective disorder: a form of schizophrenia or affective
age, gender, educational status, psychopathological symptoms disorder? J. Clin. Psychiatry 60 (1999) 874–882.
or medication (see Table 1). [5] K. Fleming, T.E. Goldberg, J.M. Gold, D.R. Weinberger, Verbal working-
The findings of this study are in line with prior neu- memory dysfunction in schizophrenia—use of a Brown- Peterson
paradigm, Psychiatry Res. 56 (1995) 155–161.
ropsychological investigations of schizophrenic patients pro- [6] D.C. Glahn, S. Therman, M. Manninen, M. Huttunen, J. Kaprio, J.
viding evidence for impairments of both verbal and visuospa- Lonnqvist, T.D. Cannon, Spatial working memory as an endophenotype
tial working memory [3,5,6,14,19,24]. The outcome regard- for schizophrenia, Biol. Psychiatry 53 (2003) 624–626.
ing schizoaffective patients is not fully consistent with the [7] D.C. Gooding, K.A. Tallent, Spatial working memory performance in
literature as several investigators reported similar cognitive patients with schizoaffective psychosis versus schizophrenia: a tale of two
disorders? Schizophr. Res. 53 (2002) 209–218.
impairments in both schizoaffective and schizophrenic [8] O. Gruber, Effects of domain-specific interference on brain activation
patients [1,4,7,17,18,22]. However, the methodical associated with verbal working memory task performance, Cereb. Cortex
comparability of these studies is very limited, and none of 11 (2001) 1047–1055.
these prior investiga- tions employed tasks derived from [9] O. Gruber, T. Goschke, Executive control emerging from dynamic inter-
experimental psychology and functional neuroimaging that actions between brain systems mediating language, working memory
and attentional processes, Acta Psychol. 115 (2004) 105–121.
specifically assess dysfunctions of the articulatory rehearsal [10] O. Gruber, D.Y. von Cramon, Domain-specific distribution of working
mechanism. While from the present study alone one cannot memory processes along human prefrontal and parietal cortices: a func-
exclude a general verbal cognitive deficit in the schizophrenic tional magnetic resonance imaging study, Neurosci. Lett. 297 (2001) 29–
patients as compared to the schizoaffective patients, this seems 32.
to be unlikely in the light of these prior find- ings. In particular, [11] O. Gruber, D.Y. von Cramon, The functional neuroanatomy of human
working memory revisited—evidence from 3-T fMRl studies using clas-
a previous study which directly addressed general intellectual sical domain-specific interference tasks, Neuroimage 19 (2003) 797–
functions and language measures in both schizophrenic and 809.
schizoaffective patients did not show any evi- dence for [12] A.L. Hoff, W.S. Kremen, Neuropsychology in schizophrenia: an update,
differences between the patient groups [22]. Hence, our Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 16 (2003) 149–155.
finding of a significant difference between schizophrenic and [13] L. Kempf, N. Hussain, J.B. Potash, Mood disorder with psychotic fea-
tures, schizoaffective disorder, and schizophrenia with mood features:
schizoaffective patients in verbal working memory perfor-
trouble at the borders, Int. Rev. Psychiatry 17 (2005) 9–19.
mance indeed suggests a more specific deficit of [14] J.H. Kim, D.C. Glahn, K.H. Nuechterlein, T.D. Cannon, Maintenance
schizophrenic patients in articulatory rehearsal. By contrast, in and manipulation of information in schizophrenia: further evidence for
the visuospa- tial working memory task under conditions of impairment in the central executive component of working memory,
interference we observed reduced performance rates both in Schizophr. Res. 68 (2004) 173–187.
[15] G. Kuperberg, S. Heckers, Schizophrenia and cognitive function, Curr.
schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients which is in
Opin. Neurobiol. 10 (2000) 205–210.
accordance with previous findings [7]. [16] M. Maj, R. Pirozzi, A.M. Formicola, L. Bartoli, P. Bucci, Reliability and
Information based on studies of healthy relatives and twins validity of the DSM-IV diagnostic category of schizoaffective disorder:
of schizophrenic patients indicate that both verbal and visu- Preliminary data, J. Affect. Disord. 57 (2000) 95–98.
ospatial working memory deficits may represent endopheno- [17] T.C. Manschreck, B.A. Maher, S.M. Beaudette, D.A. Redmond, Context
memory in schizoaffective and schizophrenic disorders, Schizophr. Res.
typic markers of schizophrenia [3,6,20]. The present findings
26 (1997) 153–161.
provide first evidence that dysfunction of a brain system sub-
[18] L.S. Miller, T. SwansonGreen, J.A. Moses, W.O. Faustman, Compar-
[22] A. Reichenberg, M. Weiser, J. Rabinowitz, A. Caspi, J. Schmei-
ison of cognitive performance in RDC-diagnosed schizoaffective and
dler, M. Mark, Z. Kaplan, M. Davidson, A population-based cohort
schizophrenic patients with the Luria-Nebraska neuropsychological bat-
study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral function-
tery, J. Psychiatr. Res. 30 (1996) 277–282.
ing in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and
[19] S. Park, P.S. Holzman, Schizophrenics show spatial working memory
nonpsychotic bipolar disorder, Am. J. Psychiatry 159 (2002) 2027–
deficits, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49 (1992) 975–982.
2035.
[20] S. Park, P.S. Holzman, P.S. Goldmanrakic, Spatial working-memory
[23] M.A. Taylor, Are schizophrenia and affective-disorder related—a selec-
deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic-patients, Arch. Gen. Psychi-
tive literature-review, Am. J. Psychiatry 149 (1992) 22–32.
atry 52 (1995) 821–828.
[24] B.E. Wexler, A.A. Stevens, A.A. Bowers, M.J. Sernyak, P.S. Goldman-
[21] H.G. Pope, J.F. Lipinski, B.M. Cohen, D.T. Axelrod, Schizoaffective
Rakic, Word and tone working memory deficits in schizophrenia, Arch.
disorder – an invalid diagnosis – a comparison of schizoaffective disor-
Gen. Psychiatry 55 (1998) 1093–1096.
der, schizophrenia, and affective-disorder, Am. J. Psychiatry 137 (1980)
921–927.

You might also like