You are on page 1of 9

Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Expert Systems with Applications


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/eswa

Brain processing of vocal sounds in advertising: A functional magnetic


resonance imaging (fMRI) study
Mohamed M. Mostafa
Gulf University for Science and Technology, College of Business, West Mishref, Kuwait

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Keywords: Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), this study aimed at investigating the neural mech-
Advertising anisms associated with human and non-human sounds’ perception in advertising. The study employed a
Consumer neuropsychology block design paradigm in which participants heard human versus non-human sounds in different sets of
fMRI advertisements. The results showed that, compared to nonhuman sounds, human sounds elicited greater
Human auditory cortex
activation in several areas in or around the primary auditory cortex (t > 5.16, p < 0.001). This result sug-
Speech perception
gests that different types of sounds are processed in different functional brain pathways. The existence of
voice-selective areas in the brain lends strong support to the face perception neurocognitive model which
proposes that visual, affective and linguistic information are processed in different cortical regions in the
brain.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction stronger activation in specific areas of the brain in a way quite


similar to the visual system’s activation by certain stimuli such
Our brain’s ability to recognize different categories of sounds is as color or motion.
present from birth (Benson et al., 2001). For example, Purhonen, In this study we aimed to (1) identify the cortical regions that
Kilpelainen-Lees, Valkonen-Korhonen, Karhu, and Lehtonen selectively response to human versus non-human sounds in adver-
(2004) suggest that infants’ brains normally allocate more atten- tising; and (2) test whether the face perception neurocognitive
tion to their own mothers’ voices compared to unfamiliar voices. model is supported within advertising context. Based on previous
Voice recognition plays also an important role in adults’ social research conducted so far, we hypothesize significantly greater
interactions. For instance, humans are able to use voice cues to at- activation in the STG and the STS areas when attending to human
tend to a target speech in a noisy environment (the cocktail-party compared to non-human sounds.
effect). This implies that humans often employ voice recognition
tactics to process different types of information (Litong-Palima,
Violanda, & Saloma, 2004). In fact self-voice recognition constitutes 2. Literature review
an important part of self-recognition (e.g. Rosa, Lassonde, Pinard,
Keenan, & Belin, 2008). In recent years, there has been a marked increase in the appli-
Recent advances in fMRI techniques have revolutionized our cations of neuroimaging methods to marketing (e.g. Butler,
understanding of the brain processing of human and non-human 2008). In fact the marriage between cognitive neuroscience and
sounds perception (Belin, Zatorre, & Ahad, 2002). Several neuroim- marketing has resulted in creating a new marketing sub-discipline
aging and neuropsychological studies have used fMRI techniques known in the literature as neuromarketing or consumer neurosci-
to study certain brain regions’ activation while attending to various ence (Fugate, 2007). Neuromarketing has been defined as ‘‘the
auditory stimuli (e.g. Binder et al., 2000; Degerman, Rinne, Salmi, application of neuroscientific methods to analyze and understand
Salonen, & Alho, 2006). fMRI evidence suggests that the superior human behavior in relation to markets and marketing exchanges’’
temporal gyrus (STG) and the superior temporal sulcus (STS) are (Lee, Broderick, & Chamberlain, 2007, p. 200). The major aim of this
greatly activated when processing human versus non-human new field is to apply neurological findings to consumer behavior
sounds such as animal, machine, and rain sounds (e.g. Belin, using neuroscientific methods such as fMRI (Stoll, Baecke, &
Zatorre, Lafaille, Ahad, & Pike, 2000; Demonet et al., 1992). In these Kenning, 2008). It has been argued that using fMRI to investigate
studies, human sounds have been found to elicit significantly which areas in a consumer’s brain are activated in response to a
specific marketing stimulus can provide a much more honest indi-
cator of their cognition compared to traditional marketing research
E-mail address: moustafa.m@gust.edu.kw tools such as focus groups and questionnaires (Senior, Smyth,

0957-4174/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eswa.2012.04.003
M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122 12115

Cooke, Shaw, & Peel, 2007). This is because unlike conventional subjects’ brains such as the medial prefrontal cortex and the rostral
marketing research methods, neuroimaging data are much less anterior cingulated cortex when they were led to believe that they
susceptible to social desirability and ‘‘interviewer’s’’ effect (Mast were drinking more expensive wines, even though all wines were
& Zaltman, 2005). the same in reality. The authors concluded that a product’s utility
Although the use of fMRI techniques in marketing is still in its does not only depend on intrinsic factors such as composition or
infancy, it seems that results are very promising. For example, in thirst, but also on extrinsic factors that can be manipulated by mar-
a classical fMRI study examining how brand information affects keters. In a similar vein, Knutson, Rick, Wimmer, Prelec, and Loe-
taste perception of sensorily similar products, McClure et al. wenstein (2007) used fMRI to study brain activation related to
(2004) found that the majority of respondents selected Coke over negative price effects. Participants in this study were presented
Pepsi in a blind tasting test. However, when the brand name was with a product while solving an exercise. Later on the correspond-
revealed, around 75% of the respondents preferred Coke. This latter ing price information of the product was presented and participants
choice also correlated with strong brain activation in areas such as were asked whether they would like to buy the product. The
the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex-areas responsible for authors found that high prices caused increased activation in brain
memory and higher cognition functions. Thus, this fMRI experi- areas associated with loss anticipation such as the insula, while low
ment shows that taste as a functional preference can be overridden prices activated areas in the brain responsible for gain processing
by brand preferences retrieved from consumers’ long term mem- such as the nucleus accumbens. This result corroborates neuroscie-
ory at the choice moment. Other brand studies involving fMRI tific studies examining examining neural correlates of gain (e.g.
examined neural correlates of brand choice (Yoon, Gutchess, Fein- Knutson & Peterson, 2005) and loss anticipation (e.g. Sanfy, Rilling,
berg, & Polk, 2006), the influence of culture on the neural ‘‘favorite Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2003).
brand-effect’’ (Schaefer & Rotte, 2010), and the neural mechanism Several neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies have at-
behind brand loyalty (Deppe et al., 2005). tempted to investigate how advertising influence consumers’ brain
A series of fMRI studies have recently attempted to test activity (Kenning, Plassmann, & Ahlert, 2007; Plassmann, Ambler,
(Damasio, 1994, 1996) somatic marker hypothesis within market- Braeutigam, & Kenning, 2007). For example, Ambler, Ioannides,
ing context. The somatic marker hypothesis proposes that plea- and Rose (2000) used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to investi-
surable or aversive somatic markers associated with internal gate consumers’ recall of affective (i.e. drama, suspense and hu-
and/or external stimuli can function to guide behavior by biasing mor) versus cognitive (infomercial) TV commercials. The authors
selections. The somatic marker hypothesis challenges the eco- found that affect-related stimuli in TV ads activated consumers’
nomic rationality concept by assuming that emotions and uncon- anterior and posterior cingulate cortex. The affective ads also elic-
scious feelings may be critical in preference judgments since the ited greater activation in the ventromedial frontal lobes (VMFL)
somatic markers might be crucial even in the absence of an and increased the recall rate three weeks after viewing. In a similar
advantage or a disadvantage associated with the response alter- experimental design using MEG, Ioannides et al. (2000) found that
natives (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997). Schaefer, cognitive-related commercial stimuli greatly activated posterior
Barens, Heinze and Rotte (2006) and Schaefer and Rotte (2007a) parietal and superior prefrontal cortices while affect-related com-
hypothesized that since brands may function as external stimuli, mercial stimuli greatly activates brain emotion regions such as the
neural substrates of brands are involved in generating somatic amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortices.
markers. Using fMRI blocked experimental design in which both Similar recent consumer neuroscience studies have focused on
familiar and unfamiliar car logos were shown to participants, linking emotional response elicited by TV commercials to certain
the authors found that culturally familiar brands generates in- brain regions. For example, it has been reported that familiar brand
creased activation in the medial prefrontal cortex. This result sup- preferences and brand logos trigger greater activation in emotions
ports the somatic marker hypothesis which claims, based on and self-reward regions of the brain (Deppe et al., 2005; Erk et al.,
lesion studies, that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex might play 2002; McClure et al., 2004; Schaefer & Rotte, 2007b). Using EEG
an important role in generating somatic markers since this brain and galvanic skin measures, Ohme, Reykowska, Wiener, and Chor-
region is known to store information related to past rewards and omanska (2009) studied emotional and arousal levels in ‘‘gesture’’
punishments (e.g. Knutson, Fong, Bennett, & Adams, 2003; Paulus versus ‘‘no-gesture’’ ads. The authors found that gesture ads elic-
& Frank, 2003). ited greater activity in the left and right frontal cortex of the brain.
The first fMRI study to investigate neural correlates related to Another stream of research has found that emotional response to
product attractiveness was conducted by Erk, Spitzer, Wunderlich, TV commercials is multidimensional (pleasure-arousal-domi-
Galley, and Walter (2002). The authors found that reward-related nance). Using fMRI analysis, this line of research has found that
brain areas such as the left anterior cingulate, the left orbitofrontal, the pleasure dimension is associated with greater activity in areas
and bilateral prefrontal cortex, as well as in the right ventral stria- such as the inferior frontal gyri while the arousal dimension is
tum are activated by products symbolizing wealth and social status. associated with activation in regions such as the right superior
In a similar vein, Stoll et al. (2008) used fMRI to investigate neural temporal gyrus (Morris et al., 2009).
correlates of attractive packages. The authors found that attractive Using electroencephalography (EEG), Rossiter, Silberstein, Har-
packages resulted in more activation in reward and attention-rele- ris, and Nield (2001) found that TV ads that generated fast left fron-
vant brain regions, whereas unattractive packages activating brain tal hemisphere activity were better recalled. Based on this result,
regions associated with aversion. This result lends strong support to the authors concluded that, contrary to previous research, the left
the recent neuroscience findings which claim that different visual hemisphere of the brain is responsible for transferring visual infor-
stimuli trigger varying levels of attention (e.g. Aharon et al., 2001) mation from the short-term memory to the long-term memory.
and such visual attention has a significant influence on preferences This implies that TV ads may be selected based on the left hemi-
(Shimojo, Simion, Shimojo, & Scheier, 2003). sphere’s electrical sensitivity. Several authors have also used high
Few fMRI studies have investigated neural correlates of pricing. resolution EEG to investigate brain neural response to visual scenes
For example, Plassmann, O’Doherty, Shiv and Rangel (2008) exam- in TV commercials (Astolfi et al., 2008, 2009; Fallani et al., 2009).
ined the influence of pricing on the neural activity and perceived re- Results suggest that TV commercials remembered elicited high
ward. In this study, participants rated the pleasantness of samples cortical activity in the frontal and parietal areas of the brain com-
of wines while presented with their corresponding prices. The pared to TV commercials that were forgotten. The results also sug-
authors found increased activation in reward-related areas in the gest that parietal areas (BA 5, 7 and 40) are central to receiving
12116 M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122

were right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness


Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) with no neurological or audiological
conditions. Because of the nature of the experiment, participants
with former training in phonetics or audiology were excluded.
Claustrophobia, hypertension, diabetes or any psychiatric disease
also represented exclusion criteria. Following Dong et al. (2005),
informed written consent was obtained from each participant after
a full 15 min pre-scan training session. Although the sample size in
this study is atypical in behavioral studies, simulation studies have
shown that twelve participants in fMRI studies are enough to
achieve 80% statistical power (Desmond & Glover, 2002; Murphy
& Garavan, 2004). Our sample is also representative of similar par-
adigm fMRI studies (e.g. Yoon et al., 2006).

3.2. Task and stimuli

Experimental task was conducted in a quiet room where partic-


ipants were placed supine in the MRI scanner. The Presentation
software package (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA) was used
to deliver the auditory digitized stimuli at clearly audible levels.
Sound files included samples of radio commercials arranged in
Fig. 1. Auditory data realignment. 30-s block durations, corresponding to one of two experimental
conditions (human vs. non-human sounds). Each active block rep-
incoming flow of information from cortical networks in the brain
resenting one of the two listening conditions was interleaved with
during the memory encoding process related to TV commercials.
a 15-s resting period. The auditory localizer scan presented a fixed
Similarly, several neuropsychological studies have recently
pseudorandom order of a total of eight blocks of human vocaliza-
investigated other issues related to the brain processing of adver-
tion ads and a total of eight blocks of non-human sounds ads. Scan-
tising stimuli. For example, Stallen, Smidts, Rijpkema, Smit, and
ner noise during the resting period was included as a control or
Klucharev (2010) investigated celebrity effect on brain activation,
baseline condition in the experimental design (Fecteau, Armony,
Langleben et al. (2009) studied brain processing of ‘‘sensation va-
Joanette, & Belin, 2004).
lue’’ ads, Vance and Virtue (2011) examined the unique contribu-
Human sounds ads were obtained in a manner to represent
tion of each cerebral hemisphere during metaphoric ad
different individuals of both sexes and of all ages. To avoid the
comprehension and Treleaven-Hassard et al. (2010) investigated
recency effect, ads presented were all broadcasted more than
the automatic attention to interactive TV commercials.
10 years ago (Morris et al., 2009). Non-human sounds ads were
From this brief literature review we find that although several
selected to represent a wide range of environmental sounds such
studies have dealt with the neural correlates of the brain reaction
as animals, machines, rain, streams, cars, musical instruments,
to advertising stimuli, no previous studies, to the best of our
etc. (e.g. Belin et al., 2002; Bergerbest, Ghahremani, & Gabrieli,
knowledge, have dealt with the brain processing of vocal sounds
2004). Since several researchers have shown that calling partici-
in advertising. In this study we fill this research gap by using fMRI
pants’ attention during auditory tasks may generate increased acti-
analysis to investigate the neural correlates of human vs. non-hu-
vation in brain areas responsible for processing auditory stimuli
man sounds in advertising.
(e.g. Jancke, Mirzazade, & Shah, 1999), participants were scanned
while listening passively to the experimental stimuli. Meyer, Zys-
3. Method set, von Cramon, and Alter (2005) used a similar strategy to ‘‘avoid
a confound between stimulus perception and attentional
3.1. Participants demands’’ (p. 295). However, it should be noted that another
stream of research has found that unattended sounds activates
Participants were fifteen monolingual English speakers (age the human auditory cortex in a similar way to attended sounds
range 21–45 years, mean 31.27 years; SD = 5.87). All participants (e.g. Petkov et al., 2004).

Fig. 2. Auditory data mutual information co-registration.


M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122 12117

functional images using echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR


3000 ms, TE 33 ms, 39 slices, voxel size 3  3  3 mm, 64  64 ma-
trix, flip angle 90°). Finally, a block design containing 2 fMRI runs
was employed to acquire functional images. For each participant
a complete fMRI session took approximately 10 min.

3.4. fMRI data preprocessing

fMRI Data preprocessing and analysis were conducted using the


SPM8 software package (Welcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging,
University College London) running under MATLAB Version 7.7
(Mathworks, Natick, MA). High resolution statistical activation
maps showing brain’s regions of interest were displayed using
the Multi-Image Analysis GUI (MANGO) image processing software
package (e.g. Lancaster et al., 2010; http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/).
Following Lee and Rudebeck (2010), a number of steps were imple-
mented: (1) the acquired time series datasets were realigned to the
first image in each session to correct for head motion artifacts and/
or inter-scan movement. This spatial transformation routine rea-
ligns a single-subject time series of images to the central volume
using a rigid body least square approach (Friston, Frith, Frac-
kowiak, & Turner, 1995). As shown in Fig. 1 the amount of absolute
translational or rotational head movement did not exceed the rec-
ommended threshold of 3.0 mm for any participant in any direc-
tion (Dommes et al., 2009). By convention, translational effects
comprise movements from left-to-right (x-axis), forward-to-back
(y-axis), and top-to-bottom in the slice acquisition direction
Fig. 3. Auditory data design matrix. (z-axis). Rotational effects comprise turns around the x-axis or
(pitch), around the y-axis (roll), and around the z-axis (yaw)
(Huettel, Song, & McCarthy, 2009); (2) a co-registration procedure
3.3. Data acquisition between anatomical and functional data acquired was imple-
mented. The aim of this procedure is to maximize the mutual infor-
Scanning was performed on a 3-Tesla Siemens Trio scanner mation obtained (Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady, & Smith, 2002).
(Siemens Magnetom Vision, Erlangen, Germany) with an eight- Fig. 2 shows that the procedure successfully corrected for image
channel array head coil optimized for whole brain echo planar distortions due to inhomogeneities in the magnetic field; (3) a
imaging. As a standard procedure, a strip-fixation of the forehead mesh deformation approach was used to segment brain matter
to the coil and a vacuum pad were used to minimize head motion into gray and white matter; (4) a 4.0 mm isotropic Gaussian kernel
artifacts. For anatomical localization, high resolution T1-weighted of full width at half maximum (FWHM) was applied to smooth
anatomical scan images were first acquired (TR 1900 ms, TE functional images. An FWHM smoothing of 4 mm or lower have
5.57 ms, flip angle 11°). This was followed by one series of 111 been reported to produce high resolution images compared to

Fig. 4. Contrast and statistical parametric maps showing bilateral activation of auditory cortex for the auditory vs. rest conditions.
12118 M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122

Fig. 5. Statistical activation maps showing regions of significantly greater activation for auditory compared to rest conditions using a threshold of p = .05.

the SPM8 default of FWHM = 8 mm (de Beeck, 2010); (5) a high- average activity in the fMRI session. As proposed by Belin et al.
pass filter was applied to each voxel in the fMRI time series data (2000), statistical parametric maps generated to determine regions
to remove low frequency noise resulting from cardiac and respira- of significant brain activation were corrected for multiple compar-
tory noise; and finally (6) each fMRI data set was normalized to the isons across the brain using a family-wise error (FWE) with a
standard stereotaxic space atlas (Talairach & Tourneau, 1988) threshold of p = 0.05.
based on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template.
However, it should be noted that although the Talairach and the
4. Results
MNI templates are quite similar, they are not identical (Kenning
& Plassmann, 2005).
4.1. Auditory vs. rest (Experiment 1)

3.5. fMRI data statistical analysis Fig. 4 depicts the SPM8 statistical results when comparing the
auditory stimulus vs. rest conditions. It is clear from the figure that
A fixed-effects general linear model based on least square esti- the BOLD-activated regions in the brain correspond to the known
mation was fit for each subject. Following Lattner, Meyer, and areas of the primary auditory cortex. The statistical activation
Friederici (2005), a hemodynamic response function (HRF) design maps depicted in Fig. 5 shows that compared to the baseline/scan-
matrix with a response delay of 6 s was generated. This delay is ner noise, auditory stimuli activated a large portion of the superior
routinely used in fMRI studies since it is known that there is usu- temporal gyrus (STG) bilaterally (F = 16.44, p < 0.05 corrected).
ally a delay of 3–6 s in cerebral blood flow (CBF) response to brain Table 1 panel (a) shows the major areas activated for the auditory
activation (Friston et al., 1995). Fig. 3 shows the design matrix cor- condition collapsed across the two stimulus types vs. rest condi-
responding to the experimental design used in this study. The ma- tion. This result is in line with recently reported fMRI studies. For
trix included six regressors and the last two columns model the example, when comparing participants’ response during silent

Table 1
Activation observed in brain regions involved in ads’ auditory processing.

Anatomical location Brain side x y z Exp. 1 F-value Exp. 2 t-value


Panel (a)
Auditory activation > baseline
STG R 60 20 10 243.7
52 4 6 197.9
52 16 2 187.9
STG L 60 28 12 223.2
45 22 5 161.3
48 4 0 142.0
Panel (b)
Human sounds > nonhuman sounds
STS R 66 32 4 7.4
50 36 8 7.0
62 8 2 7.1
STS L 56 0 6 6.8
58 16 6 5.0
56 14 4 6.7

Auditory activation > baseline statistics.


Height threshold: F = 16.44, p = 0.000 (0.05); Degrees of freedom (2, 206); Expected voxel per cluster (K) = 1.005; Expected number of clusters (G) = 0.005; FWHM = 7.4, 7.6,
7.1; x, y, z: 3.7, 3.8, 3.6 (voxels).
Human sounds > nonhuman sounds statistics.
Height threshold: T = 5.16, p = 0.000 (0.05); Degrees of freedom (1, 206); Expected voxel per cluster (K) = 1.248; Expected number of clusters (G) = 0.005; FWHM = 7.4, 7.6,
7.1; x, y, z: 3.7, 3.8, 3.6 (voxels).
M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122 12119

Fig. 6. Contrast and statistical parametric maps showing bilateral activation of auditory cortex for the human vs. non-human speech conditions.

periods against listening to Shepard’s tones, Shimizu et al. (2007) human auditory stimuli activated a large portion of the STG and
reported greater bilateral activation in the primary auditory cortex. STS bilaterally (t = 5.16, p < 0.05 corrected). The 90% CI shown in
In another study comparing speech and non-speech perception in Fig. 8 indicates a statistically significant difference between the
the brain, Benson et al. (2001) reported several regions with high two conditions. Table 1 panel (b) shows major areas activated by
activation, including the left inferior frontal gyrus and the posterior human sounds as compared to non-human sounds while Fig. 9
superior temporal gyrus (pSTG). The activation in our study seems shows the grand averaged and spatially normalized statistical
to be quasi-symmetrical across the right and left hemispheres parametric maps for all brain areas activated by human vs. non-hu-
which is in line with several neuroimaging studies comparing man sounds. This result is in line with recently reported fMRI stud-
speech sounds to a silent baseline condition (e.g. Binder et al., ies. For example, when comparing participants’ response to human
2000; Dehaene et al., 1997; Mazoyer et al., 1993). versus non-human sounds, Faceteau et al. (2004) reported bilateral
greater sensitivity in the human superior temporal sulcus (STS)
4.2. Human vs. non-human sounds (Experiment 2) when participants listened to human sounds compared to environ-
mental sounds. In another study comparing speech and non-
Fig. 6 depicts the SPM8 statistical results when comparing the speech perception in the brain, Belin et al. (2000) reported a great-
human against non-human sounds stimuli. It is clear from the fig- er activation along the upper bank of the STS when participants lis-
ure that human sounds in ads resulted in greater bilateral activa- tened to human speech versus non-vocal sounds such as natural
tion in STG and STS regions. The statistical activation maps sounds or animal cries. From Fig. 7 we note greater activation in
depicted in Fig. 7 shows that compared to non-human sounds, the left hemisphere compared to the right hemisphere. Apart from

Fig. 7. Statistical activation maps showing regions of significantly greater activation for human speech compared to non-human speech conditions using a threshold of
p = .05.
12120 M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122

Fig. 10. Fitted responses for the auditory vs. rest condition.
Fig. 8. Contrast estimates and 90% confidence interval.

Belin et al. (2002) who reported a great activation to human vocal 5. Discussion and future research
sounds in the right side of the brain, our finding is remarkably in
line with other research findings despite stimuli differences among The objectives of this research were to investigate brain regions
studies such as the use of reversed speech, scrambled speech, syl- responsible for processing human and non-human sounds and to
lables, pseudowords, etc. (e.g. Binder et al., 1997; Demonet et al., test the existence of the face perception model within an advertis-
1992; Zatorre, Evans, Meyer, & Gjedde, 1992). ing context. We employed a block design fMRI paradigm to study
This result is important because it suggests that different types cerebral activities associated with the different types of sounds.
of sounds are processed in different functional brain pathways. In this design participants heard human versus non-human sounds
Similar results were reported by Meyer at al. (2005) when compar- in different sets of advertisements. Statistical analysis based on
ing neural response to speech, laughter and environmental sounds. general linear model showed that human sounds elicited greater
The authors found that posterior STG and the left superior tempo- activation in several areas in or around the primary auditory
ral region are involved in speech processing while areas in the pari- cortex. This result suggests that different types of sounds are
etal auditory fields are involved in processing laughter. On the processed in different functional brain pathways, which implies
other hand environmental sounds elicit greater activation in areas that such pathways are highly selective to human sounds.
such as the posterior supratem-poral plane. Taken together, it Several neuroimaging studies have recently compared brain
seems that there are ‘‘voice-selective’’ areas in the brain which processing of human versus non-human sounds using a wide range
lends strong support to the face perception neurocognitive model. of stimuli (e.g. Belin et al., 2000; Zatorre et al., 1992). However, this
This model proposes that visual, affective and linguistic informa- study is novel in that it is, to our knowledge, the first to contrast
tion are processed in different cortical regions in the brain. This different types of sounds within advertising context. Isolating the
model argues that the presence of human sounds activates partic- sound presented in commercial ads helps explain how consumers’
ular regions in the brain in a way similar to the activation of certain brain process language in advertising. This is an important under-
visual areas by specific stimuli such as color or motion. In fact re- taking because it might be difficult to determine what specific
cent fMRI studies have shown that silent lip-reading activates hu- components of the ad account for brain processing of advertising
man auditory cortex and related auditory cortices in a similar way stimuli. By doing so the study adds depth to the knowledge base
to listening to human sounds (e.g. Calvert et al., 1997; Rosenblum, on neural correlates of auditory stimuli in advertising. Di Salle
2008). et al. (2003) have argued that ‘‘the auditory system has progressed
In order to check the stability of our results we plotted the fitted at a considerably slower pace compared to other functional
curves of peak activation with errors added in Fig. 10. The tight systems’’ (p. 1213). By using fMRI to study how the brain processes
matching between errors and the reference Box-Car function indi- auditor information, this study adds breadth to the debate over the
cates high signals’ reliability. brain regions activated by human versus non-human sounds. By

Fig. 9. Grand averaged and spatially normalized SPMs for activated brain regions (human sound > non-human sound).
M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122 12121

focusing solely on advertising, rather than on other fields, this Damasio, A. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of
the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B (Biological
study enriches the knowledge base of this under-represented area.
Sciences), 351, 1413–1420.
Although this fMRI study has demonstrated the usefulness of de Beeck, H. (2010). Against hyperacuity in brain reading: Spatial smoothing does
this method in confirming the distinctiveness of brain regions not hurt multivariate fMRI analyses? Neuroimage, 49, 1943–1948.
responsible for processing human versus non-human sounds, we Degerman, A., Rinne, T., Salmi, J., Salonen, O., & Alho, K. (2006). Selective attention to
sound location or pitch studied with fMRI. Brain Research, 1077, 123–134.
did not test whether this result holds in the case of different atten- Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., Mehler, J., Cohen, L., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., et al. (1997).
tional states. Future research may test whether the activation of Anatomical variability in the cortical representation of first and second
auditory cortical areas varies with attentional state. We did not also language. Neuroreport, 8, 3809–3815.
Demonet, J., Chollet, F., Ramsay, S., Cardebat, D., Nespoulous, J., Wise, R., et al.
test whether auditory cortex activation holds when presenting par- (1992). The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal
ticipants with subliminal stimuli. Future research should demon- subjects. Brain, 115, 1753–1768.
strate or refute this by demonstrating that auditory cortex Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Kramer, J., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H., Kenning, P., et al.
(2005). Bias specific activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during
activation is independent or just synonymous to conscious speech credibility judgments. Brain Research Bulletin, 67, 413–421.
perception. Future research using fMRI methods should test Desmond, J., & Glover, G. (2002). Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI)
whether the activation in primary and secondary auditory cortex neuroimaging studies: statistical power analysis. Journal of Neuroscience
Methods, 118, 115–128.
occurs is bilateral or hemisphere dependent. Since several research- Di Salle, F., Esposito, F., Scarabino, T., Formisano, E., Marciano, E., Salino, C., et al.
ers have found that human sounds share some acoustic features (2003). FMRI of the auditory system: Understanding the neural basis of auditory
with animal sounds (e.g. Poremba et al., 2004; Rendall, 2003), gestalt. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 21, 1213–1224.
Dommes, E., Bauknecht, H., Scholz, G., Rothemund, Y., Hensel, J., & Klingebiel, R.
future fMRI research should investigate human brain areas acti-
(2009). Auditory cortex stimulation by low-frequency tones: An fMRI study.
vated by animal sounds versus other environmental sounds. Finally, Brain Research, 1304, 129–137.
since the major goal of neuromarketing is to obtain objective infor- Dong, Y., Nakamura, K., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Fukuyama, H., Mazziotta, J., et al.
mation about consumers’ neural correlates (Murphy, Illes, & Reiner, (2005). Neural mechanisms underlying the processing of Chinese words: An
fMRI study. Neuroscience Research, 52, 139–145.
2008), it is expected that neuromarketing studies will provide qual- Erk, S., Spitzer, M., Wunderlich, A., Galley, L., & Walter, H. (2002). Cultural objects
itatively different and superior information compared to traditional modulate reward circuitry. Neuroreport, 13, 2499–2503.
marketing research methods. In fact, initial studies integrating neu- Fallani, F., Astolfi, L., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., Marciani, M., Gao, S., et al. (2009).
Structure of the cortical networks during successful memory encoding in TV
roscientific methods into marketing have led to valid and reliable commercials. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 2231–2237.
findings for marketing theory and practice. While classic neuromar- Fecteau, S., Armony, J., Joanette, Y., & Belin, P. (2004). Is voice processing species-
keting studies have attempted to transfer neuroscientific findings specific in human auditory cortex? Neuroimage, 23, 840–848.
Friston, K., Frith, G., Frackowiak, R., & Turner, R. (1995). Characterizing dynamic
to a marketing-relevant context, future studies might focus on test- brain response with fMRI: A multivariate approach. Neuroimage, 2, 166–172.
ing a range of theoretically-driven marketing research concepts Fugate, D. (2007). Neuromarketing: A layman’s look at neuroscience and its
such as signaling theory of the brand or surrealist imagery in adver- potential application to marketing practice. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 24,
385–394.
tising. Without testing theoretically driven marketing concepts, Huettel, S., Song, A., & McCarthy, G. (2009). Functional magnetic resonance imaging
there is a chance of falling ‘‘unwittingly prey to exactly the kind (2nd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
of blobological approach that has received criticism in social and Ioannides, A., Liu, L., Theofilou, D., Dammers, J., Burne, T., Ambler, T., et al. (2000).
Real time processing of affective and cognitive stimuli in the human brain
general neuroscientific circles’’ (Lee & Chamberlain, 2007, p. 23).
extracted from MEG signals. Brain Topography, 13, 11–19.
Jancke, L., Mirzazade, S., & Shah, N. (1999). Attention modulates activity in the
primary and the secondary auditory cortex: A functional magnetic resonance
References imaging study in human subjects. Neuroscience Letters, 266, 125–128.
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for
the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. (2001).
images. Neuroimage, 17, 825–841.
Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence.
Kenning, P., & Plassmann, H. (2005). Neuroeconomics: An overview from an
Neuron, 32, 537–551.
economic perspective. Brain Research Bulletin, 67, 343–354.
Ambler, T., Ioannides, A., & Rose, S. (2000). Brands on the brain: Neuro-images of
Kenning, P., Plassmann, H., & Ahlert, D. (2007). Applications of functional magnetic
advertising. Business Strategy Review, 11, 17–30.
resonance imaging for market research. Qualitative Market Research, 10,
Astolfi, L., Fallani, F., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., Bianchi, L., Marciani, M., et al. (2008).
135–152.
Neural basis for brain responses to TV commercials: A high-resolution EEG
Knutson, B., & Peterson, R. (2005). Neurally reconstructing expected utility. Games
study. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, 16,
and Economic Behavior, 52, 305–315.
522–531.
Knutson, B., Fong, G., Bennett, S., & Adams, C. (2003). A region of mesial prefrontal
Astolfi, L., Fallani, F., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., Bianchi, L., Marciani, M., et al. (2009).
cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: Characterization with rapid
Brain activity during the memorization of visual scenes from TV commercials:
event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 18, 263–272.
An application of high resolution EEG and steady state somatosensory evoked
Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G., Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Neural
potentials technology. Journal of Physiology–Paris, 103, 333–341.
predictors of purchase. Neuron, 53, 147–156.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. (1997). Deciding advantageously
Lancaster, J., Cykowski, M., McKay, D., Kochunov, P., Fox, P., Rogers, W., et al. (2010).
before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295.
Anatoical global spatial normalization. Neuroinform, 8, 171–182.
Belin, P., Zatorre, R., Lafaille, P., Ahad, P., & Pike, B. (2000). Voice-selective areas in
Langleben, D., Loughead, J., Ruparel, K., Hakun, J., Bush-Winkor, S., Holloway, M.,
human auditory cortex. Nature, 403, 309–312.
et al. (2009). Reduced prefrontal and temporal processing and recall of high
Belin, P., Zatorre, R., & Ahad, P. (2002). Human temporal-lobe response to vocal
‘‘sensation value’’ ads. Neuroimage, 46, 219–225.
sounds. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 17–26.
Lattner, S., Meyer, M., & Friederici, A. (2005). Voice perception: Sex, pitch, and the
Benson, R., Whalen, D., Richardson, M., Swainson, B., Clark, V., Lai, C., et al. (2001).
right hemisphere. Human Brain Mapping, 24, 11–20.
Parametrically dissociating speech and nonspeech perception in the brain using
Lee, A., & Rudebeck, S. (2010). Investigating the interaction between spatial
fMRI. Brain and Language, 78, 364–396.
perception and working memory in the human medical temporal lobe. Journal
Bergerbest, D., Ghahremani, D., & Gabrieli, J. (2004). Neural correlates of auditory
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2823–2835.
reception priming: Reduced fMRI activation in the auditory cortex. Journal of
Lee, N., & Chamberlain, A. (2007). Neuroimaging and psychophysiological
Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 966–977.
measurement in organizational research: An agenda for organizational
Binder, J., Frost, J., Hammeke, T., Cox, R., Rao, S., & Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain
cognitive neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118, 18–43.
language areas identified by functional MRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 17,
Lee, N., Broderick, A., & Chamberlain, L. (2007). What is ‘‘neuromarketing’’? A
353–362.
discussion and agenda for future research. International Journal of
Binder, J., Frost, J., Hammeke, T., Bellgowan, P., Springer, J., Kaufman, J., et al. (2000).
Psychophysiology, 63, 199–204.
Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds. Cerebral
Litong-Palima, J., Violanda, R., & Saloma, C. (2004). Voice tracking and spoken word
Cortex, 10, 512–528.
recognition in the presence of other voices. Physica D, 199, 400–406.
Butler, M. (2008). Neuromarketing and the perception of knowledge. Journal of
Mast, F., & Zaltman, G. (2005). A behavioral window on the mind of the market: An
Consumer Behavior, 7, 415–419.
application of the response time paradigm. Brain Research bulletin, 67, 422–427.
Calvert, G., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., Campbell, R., Iverson, S., Woodruff, P., et al.
Mazoyer, B., Tzourio, N., Syrota, A., Murayama, M., Levrier, O., Salamon, G., et al.
(1997). Silent lipreading activates the auditory cortex. Science, 276, 593–596.
(1993). The cortical representation of speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New
5, 467–479.
York: Putnam.
12122 M.M. Mostafa / Expert Systems with Applications 39 (2012) 12114–12122

McClure, S., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K., Montague, L., & Montague, P. (2004). Neural Rossiter, J., Silberstein, R., Harris, P., & Nield, G. (2001). Brain-imaging detection of
correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44, visual scene encoding in long-term memory for TV commercials. Journal of
379–387. Advertising Research, 41, 13–21.
Meyer, M., Zysset, S., von Cramon, D., & Alter, K. (2005). Distinct fMRI response to Sanfy, A., Rilling, J., Aronson, J., Nystrom, L., & Cohen, J. (2003). The neural basis of
laughter, speech, and sounds along the human per-sylvian cortex. Cognitive economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300, 1755–1758.
Brain Research, 24, 291–306. Schaefer, M., & Rotte, M. (2007a). Thinking on luxury or pragmatic brand products:
Morris, J., Klahr, N., Shen, F., Villegas, J., Wright, P., He, G., et al. (2009). Mapping a Brain response to different categories of culturally based brands. Brain Research,
multidimensional emotion in response to television commercials. Human Brain 1165, 98–104.
Mapping, 30, 789–796. Schaefer, M., & Rotte, M. (2007b). Favorite brands as cultural objects modulate
Murphy, E., Illes, J., & Reiner, P. (2008). Neuroethics of neuromarketing. Journal of reward circuit. Neuroreport, 18, 141–145.
Consumer Behavior, 7, 293–302. Schaefer, M., & Rotte, M. (2010). Combining a semantic differential with fMRI to
Murphy, K., & Garavan, H. (2004). An empirical investigation into the number of investigate brands as cultural symbols. Social, Cognitive, and Affective
subjects required for an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 22, 879–885. Neuroscience, 5(2-3), 274–281.
Ohme, R., Reykowska, D., Wiener, D., & Choromanska, A. (2009). Analysis of Schaefer, M., Berens, H., Heinze, H.-J., & Rotte, M. (2006). Neural correlates of
neurophysiological reactions to advertising stimuli by means of EEG and culturally familiar brands of car manufacturers. Neuroimage, 31, 861–865.
galvanic skin response measures. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Senior, C., Smyth, H., Cooke, R., Shaw, R., & Peel, E. (2007). Mapping the mind for the
Economics, 2, 21–31. modern market researcher. Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal,
Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh 10, 153–167.
inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. Shimizu, Y., Umeda, M., Mano, H., Aoki, I., Higuchi, T., & Tanaka, C. (2007). Neural
Petkov, C., Kang, X., Alho, K., Bertrand, O., Yund, E., & Woods, D. (2004). Attentional response to Shepard’s tones: An auditory fMRI study using multifractal analysis.
modulation of human auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 658–663. Brain Research, 1186, 113–123.
Paulus, P., & Frank, L. (2003). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is crucial for Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., & Scheier, C. (2003). Gaze bias both reflects and
preference judgments. Neuroreport, 10, 1311–1315. influences preference. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1317–1322.
Plassmann, H., Ambler, T., Braeutigam, S., & Kenning, P. (2007). What can Stallen, M., Smidts, A., Rijpkema, M., Smit, G., & Klucharev, V. (2010). Celebrities and
advertisers learn from neuroscience? International Journal of Advertising, 26, shoes on the femal brain: The neural correlates of product evaluation in the
151–175. context of fame. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31, 802–811.
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can Stoll, M., Baecke, S., & Kenning, P. (2008). What they see is what they get? An fMRI
modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. PNAS, 105(3), study on neural correlates of attractive packaging. Journal of Consumer Behavior,
1050–1054. 7, 342–359.
Poremba, A., Malloy, M., Saunders, R., Carson, R., Herscovitch, P., & Mishkin, M. Talairach, P., & Tourneau, J. (1988). Coplanar stereotaxi atlas of the human brain: 3-
(2004). Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey’s Dimensional proportional system thieme medical publishers. New York.
temporal poles. Nature, 427, 448–451. Treleaven-Hassard, S., Gold, J., Bellman, S., Schweda, A., Ciorciari, J., Critchey, C.,
Purhonen, L., Kilpelainen-Lees, M., Valkonen-Korhonen, K., Karhu & Lehtonen, K. et al. (2010). Using the P3a to guage automatic attention to interactive
(2004). Cerebral processing of mother’s voice compared to unfamiliar voice in television advertising. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31, 777–784.
4-month-old infants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 52, 257–266. Vance, K., & Virtue, S. (2011). Metaphoric advertisement comprehension: The role of
Rendall, D. (2003). Acoustic correlates of caller identity and affect intensity in the the cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 10, 41–50.
vowel-like grunt vocalizations of baboons. Journal of the Acoustic Society of Yoon, C., Gutchess, A., Feinberg, F., & Polk, T. (2006). A functional magnetic
America, 113, 3390–3402. resonance imaging study of neural dissociation between brand and person
Rosa, J., Lassonde, P., Pinard, P., Keenan, L., & Belin, P. (2008). Investigation of judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 31–40.
hemispheric specialization in self-voice recognition. Brain and Language, 68, Zatorre, R., Evans, A., Meyer, E., & Gjedde, A. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and
204–214. pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science, 256, 846–849.
Rosenblum, L. (2008). Speech perception as a multimodal phenomenon. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 405–409.

You might also like