You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 631–635

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The Maximization Paradox: The costs of seeking alternatives q


Ilan Dar-Nimrod a,*, Catherine D. Rawn a,*, Darrin R. Lehman a, Barry Schwartz b
a
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada V6T 1Z4
b
Department of Psychology, Swarthmore College, 500 College Avenue, Swarthmore, PA 19081-1397, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Contrary to the common belief that more options lead to better decisions, recent research has demon-
Received 28 August 2008 strated that choosing from a large number of options can have detrimental psychological effects. We
Received in revised form 17 December 2008 investigated whether people were willing to sacrifice resources for more options, and whether choice-
Accepted 7 January 2009
making orientation moderated such willingness. As predicted, people who were motivated to make the
Available online 6 February 2009
best choice possible—‘‘maximizers”—were more willing to sacrifice resources such as time to attain a lar-
ger choice array than were people who tend to search for a satisfactory choice (i.e., ‘‘satisficers”). Addi-
Keywords:
tionally, maximizers who sacrificed to attain more options were ultimately less satisfied with their
Maximizing tendencies
Satisficers
choice relative to maximizers who chose from a small assortment, and to satisficers (Studies 2 and 3).
Choice-making strategies We term the pattern in which maximizers tend to sacrifice resources to attain more options that ulti-
Customer satisfaction mately reduce their satisfaction, the ‘‘Maximization Paradox”.
Sacrifice Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Assortment size

1. Introduction with the ‘‘Paradox of Choice”. Although people tend be attracted


to larger assortments, only some (i.e., maximizers) may be willing
Imagine it is Friday evening and you are going to the movies, to make sacrifices such as time, energy, or money, in order to glean
although you are not sure which movies are currently playing. those larger assortments—sacrifices that ultimately lead to dissat-
You debate going to one of two cinemas, equal in price and ambi- isfaction with the chosen alternative. We propose the term ‘‘Max-
ance. SuperCity Theatre offers 12 movie screens; whereas Town imization Paradox” to reflect this moderated ‘‘Paradox of Choice.”
Cinema offers only four screens. Assuming the same types of mov- Choice-making orientation (Schwartz et al., 2002) is an individ-
ies usually play at both cinemas, which would you choose? ual difference variable that differentiates people based on their ap-
All else being equal, the larger theatre is the rational alternative. proach to making decisions. At one extreme, maximizing describes
Theoretically, increasing assortment size should increase the like- the tendency to approach choices with the goal of finding the
lihood that a person’s preference can be satisfied (Schwartz, ‘‘best” possible alternative. At the other extreme, satisficing de-
2004). Indeed, much research has demonstrated that people tend scribes the tendency to approach choices with the goal of finding
to prefer more rather than fewer options. For example, people an option that is ‘‘good enough” according to their threshold of
are more attracted to arrays of 30 jams than to arrays of six jams acceptability.1 Maximizers may be particularly likely to value larger
(Iyengar & Lepper, 2000), and to arrays of 50 ice cream flavors than assortments more than smaller assortments in their quest for the
to arrays of 10 ice cream flavors (Rozin, Fischler, Shields, & Masson, best, and this may seem to be a logical approach: the chances of find-
2006). Yet people are less satisfied with outcomes chosen from lar- ing an ideal alternative may seem greater when one has more op-
ger assortments rather than smaller assortments (Chernev, 2003; tions rather than fewer from which to choose. Past research has
Iyengar & Lepper, 2000; Lee & Lee, 2004; Schwartz, 2004, 2000). shown that maximizers remember experiencing more dissatisfaction
Schwartz (2004) identified people’s attraction to, yet dissatisfac- with their past choices than do satisficers (Schwartz et al., 2002). We
tion with larger assortments rather than smaller assortments the sought to extend these earlier findings and predicted the Maximiza-
‘‘Paradox of Choice.” In this paper, we propose that individual dif- tion Paradox. Because of their desire to obtain the best option, max-
ferences in choice-making orientation moderate costs associated imizers will be willing to sacrifice resources in order to attain larger
assortments, yet they will experience dissatisfaction with their
selection from this larger assortment.
q
The first two authors contributed equally to this manuscript and are listed
alphabetically. We thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable suggestions.
1
* Corresponding authors. Tel.: +1 604 822 2442; fax: +1 604 822 6923. Although we refer to ‘‘maximizers” and ‘‘satisficers” for ease of discussion, this
E-mail addresses: ilan@psych.ubc.ca (I. Dar-Nimrod), cdrawn@psych.ubc.ca (C.D. individual difference variable is considered a unidimensional continuum (see
Rawn). Schwartz et al. (2002)).

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.007
632 I. Dar-Nimrod et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 631–635

The nascent literature investigating choice-making orienta- 1.1.2. Procedure and materials
tion provides some support for both components of the Maxi- Participants completed a questionnaire packet as part of a larger
mization Paradox. When asked to recall past purchases, both study. These questionnaires were administered in the following or-
large (in the hundreds of dollars) and small (less than ten dol- der, separated by filler items.
lars), the tendency to maximize positively predicted the num-
ber of products considered as well as the length of time 1.1.2.1. Maximization Scale. The 13 item Maximization Scale (Sch-
spent deciding, and negatively predicted satisfaction with the wartz et al., 2002), was used to assess maximizing tendencies
choice outcome (Schwartz et al., 2002). In more recent work, (e.g., ‘‘Renting videos is really difficult. I’m always struggling to
maximizers obtained objectively better post-graduate jobs than pick the best one”). Ratings were made on a 7-point scale (1 = com-
did satisficers: Maximizers negotiated starting salaries 20% pletely disagree, 7 = completely agree). Higher scores indicated a
higher than satisficers (Iyengar, Wells, & Schwartz, 2006). De- stronger tendency to seek the best option when making a choice
spite making such superior negotiations, maximizers reported (i.e., ‘‘maximizing”). The items were summed to create a composite
less satisfaction with their outcome than did satisficers. Taken measure of choice-making orientation (Cronbach a = .572).
together, past research shows that across decisions that can be
evaluated subjectively and objectively, maximizers are dissatis- 1.1.2.2. Choices Questionnaire. Participants read the following
fied with their choices. vignette:
The job hunt study (Iyengar et al., 2006) provides some indica- Your cleaning supplies (laundry detergent, rags, carpet cleaner,
tion that maximizers may have sacrificed more time and effort to dish soap, toilet paper, glass cleaner, etc.) are running low. You
glean their superior job outcome, but the evidence is suggestive have the option of going to the nearest grocery store (5 min away),
at best. Although maximizing tendencies positively predicted plans which offers four alternatives for each of the items you need or
to apply for more jobs, this effect held only among people at lower- drive to the grand cleaning superstore (25 min away), which offers
ranked universities, and no data are reported about whether max- about 25 different alternatives for each of the items (for approxi-
imizers actually applied for more jobs than did satisficers. Despite mately the same price). Which store would you go to?
some indications that maximizers and satisficers were candidates The vignette described a trade-off between options. One option
of similar quality, maximizers did not receive more interviews or contained a larger assortment but came at a cost of increased time
job offers than did satisficers. Maximizers did self-report actively (and gas) investment. The other option offered a more limited
seeking more information related to the job hunt (i.e., seeking ad- assortment but also reduced time (and gas) investment. We antic-
vice from experts, friends and family) compared with satisficers, ipated that maximizers would be more willing than satisficers to
but this effort did not translate to an increased number of inter- sacrifice resources in order to attain the larger assortment.
views or job offers. Therefore there is no evidence that maximizers
actively increased the number of job alternatives from which they 1.2. Results and discussion
chose.
Based on recall of past consumer purchases and self-reported We conducted a logistic regression analysis predicting store
data, it appears that maximizers may be more likely than satis- choice from maximization score. The analysis revealed a significant
ficers to make sacrifices to gain more options. Yet no research effect of choice-making orientation, v2 (1) = 4.86, p = .03. For every
has examined whether maximizers will explicitly and deliber- one unit increase in maximization score, the odds of choosing the
ately make sacrifices to gain extra alternatives when making a superstore increased by 6% (.95CI: .6%, 11.9%), Wald = 4.5, p = .03,
choice. We examine this hypothesis in three methodologically OR = 1.06. As predicted, higher scores on the Maximization Scale
varied studies, including a hypothetical scenario, a controlled predicted a greater likelihood of choosing the superstore (i.e., lar-
lab study, and a field study. All studies require trading-off be- ger selection but increased travel time) than the neighborhood
tween choice set size and resources (i.e., time and effort). store with less selection. Thus, Study 1 provided initial support
We anticipate that maximizers will be particularly dissatisfied for the idea that maximizers are more likely than satisficers to bear
with their choice when choosing from a larger rather than smaller increased costs (i.e., time and gas) in exchange for more options. In
set of alternatives. All else equal, the chances of identifying the the following two studies, we examined reactions to actual choices
best option from a given selection decreases as the number of in the lab (Study 2) and in the field (Study 3).
alternatives increases. When faced with a large assortment, maxi-
mizers, who are motivated by the desire to attain the best possible 2. Study 2: chocolate study
option, are less likely to find the best option, leading to dissatisfac-
tion. In three studies, we examined whether the Maximization Par- Study 2 investigated the Maximization Paradox in the con-
adox would occur among everyday choices. We predicted that trolled lab environment. We anticipated that maximizers, in their
maximizers would be more likely than satisficers to sacrifice to quest for the best, would be more willing than satisficers to sacri-
gain more options from which to choose, but maximizers who do fice time in exchange for more options. We also anticipated that
so will be less satisfied with their selections. maximizers who had sacrificed for the larger set would be less sat-
isfied than those who chose from the smaller number of options.

1. Study 1: vignettes study 2.1. Method

1.1. Method 2.1.1. Participants and procedure


Ninety-six participants (18 males, 74 females, and four unspec-
1.1.1. Participants ified) completed personality questionnaires including the Maximi-
One hundred and one students from the psychology subject zation Scale (Schwartz et al., 2002). They were then told that they
pool participated in this study for course credit (28 males, 69 fe-
males, four unspecified; age range: 17–42, M = 20.3, SD = 3.19).
Gender and ethnicity did not correlate significantly with any of 2
This low reliability coefficient falls within the range of previously reported values
the key variables in any of the three studies, and were not included (see Schwartz et al., 2002 for rationale). Attenuating reliability increases the effect
in reported analyses. size found in this study.
I. Dar-Nimrod et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 631–635 633

would get to consume one chocolate. Participants were instructed t = 2.27, p = .03. Among participants who chose from the smaller
to take their upcoming chocolate taste-test seriously, and were gi- assortment, choice-making orientation did not significantly predict
ven a ‘‘Chocolate Tasting Instructions Sheet” with tips on how to satisfaction, b = .19, t = 1.48, p > .10.
taste chocolate to experience it fully. The experimenter offered To compare the satisfaction ratings among maximizers faced
them a choice between two assortments, from which they would with different assortments we divided the sample via median split
eventually choose the chocolate to eat. One assortment contained on maximization score (Mdn = 56.5; theoretical midpoint = 52,
six chocolates and the other contained 30 chocolates (including range = 13–91). An ANOVA (choice-making orientation x assort-
the six chocolates in the smaller assortment). For the opportunity ment size) revealed a marginally significant main effect of
to choose from the larger assortment, participants had to agree choice-making orientation on satisfaction, F(1, 92) = 3.03, p = .09,
to complete an additional questionnaire at the end of the experi- qualified by an interaction, F(1, 92) = 5.85, p = .02, g2p = .06 (see
ment that was for the chocolatier and of no interest to the exper- Fig. 1). A planned comparison indicated that maximizers who sac-
imenter (i.e., make a larger time investment). Participants then rificed the time to choose from a large assortment were less satis-
told the Experimenter which assortment they preferred. Choco- fied with their chocolate than were maximizers who chose from
lates were presented in narrow boxes that held six chocolates the small assortment, t(47) = 2.23, p = .03, d = .65. The satisfaction
per box, side by side. Participants saw all six or all 30 chocolates ratings of satisficers were not affected by assortment size
at once, depending on which assortment they selected. Each choc- t(45) = 1.25, p = .22. See Table 1 for means.
olate was accompanied by a note identifying its name (e.g., Almond The results of Study 2 support our hypothesis that maximizers
Burst, Orange Cream) and the chocolate mix (e.g., dark or milk). are more willing than satisficers to sacrifice time to gain more op-
After indicating they had chosen a chocolate, the Experimenter left tions. Consistent with predictions, maximization negatively pre-
to retrieve the chocolate, and participants completed an additional dicted satisfaction with one’s choice among those who sacrificed
questionnaire. Upon the Experimenter’s return, participants were to gain the larger assortment. Choice-making orientation did not
given the chocolate and were left to eat it alone and rate their sat- significantly predict satisfaction among those choosing from the
isfaction. Participants provided basic demographic information, smaller assortment. Thus the results support the Maximization
were debriefed, paid $10, and thanked for their participation. Paradox: maximizers who sacrificed to choose from the larger
Demographics were not related to the variables of interest and will assortment enjoyed the chocolate less than their counterparts
not be discussed. who chose from the smaller assortment. Study 3 sought greater
ecological validity by testing the Maximization Paradox in the field.
2.1.2. Materials
2.1.2.1. Maximization Scale. Choice-making orientation was as- 3. Study 3: ice cream study
sessed by the Maximization Scale (Cronbach a = .75).
In Study 3 we investigated the Maximization Paradox among
2.1.2.2. Satisfaction with choice questionnaire. The satisfaction ques- customers in two ice cream parlors, one of which offers approxi-
tions were adapted from Iyengar and Lepper (2000). These three mately 200 flavors and the other offers approximately 20 flavors.
questions asked participants to report their satisfaction with the We anticipated that maximizers would be more likely than satis-
chocolate they had eaten (e.g., ‘‘How much did you enjoy the sam- ficers to seek out the larger ice cream parlor, and paradoxically that
ple you tasted?”). The items were rated on a 7-point Likert scale maximizers who chose from the larger ice cream parlor would be
(1 = not at all, 7 = extremely). Items were aggregated into a single less satisfied than those who chose from the smaller ice cream
measure of satisfaction with one’s chosen chocolate (Cronbach parlor.
a = .95).
3.1. Method
2.2. Results and discussion
3.1.1. Participants
2.2.1. Willingness to sacrifice for larger assortment size: part 1 of the One hundred and twenty-four participants (55 males, 66 fe-
Maximization Paradox males, three unspecified; age range = 13–71, M = 31.24,
A logistic regression analysis predicting choice of assortment SD = 12.06) were recruited outside two ice cream parlors in Van-
size from maximization score revealed a significant effect of max- couver, Canada, immediately after purchasing ice cream. Sev-
imization, v2(1) = 7.09, p < .01. For every one unit increase in max- enty-three participants were recruited from a parlor that offers
imization, the odds of sacrificing time to choose from the larger approximately 200 flavors of ice cream and 48 participants were
assortment increased by 5.5% (.95CI: 1.2%, 10%), Wald = 6.36, recruited from a parlor that offers approximately 20 flavors. The
p = .01, OR = 1.06.

2.2.2. Satisfaction with chocolate: part 2 of the Maximization Paradox


20
We anticipated that maximizers who had sacrificed time to
choose from a larger assortment of chocolates would be less satis- 18
fied with their choice than would satisficers who had just made the
Satisfaction

same sacrifice. To test this hypothesis, we regressed the aggregated 16


satisfaction measure on assortment size, choice-making orienta-
tion, and their interaction. No main effect for assortment size, 14
b = .05, |t| < 1, or choice-making orientation, b = .10, |t| < 1,
emerged, but the expected choice-making orientation by assort- 12

ment size interaction did, b = .28, t = 2.71, p < .01. To investigate


10
the nature of the interaction we conducted simple slope analyses 6 Alternatives 30 Alternatives
predicting satisfaction from maximization score separately for
Satisficers Maximizers
each assortment (Aiken & West, 1991). Among participants who
chose from the larger selection, a stronger tendency to maximize Fig. 1. Satisfaction with chocolate as a product of choice-making orientation and
predicted decreased satisfaction with the choice, b = .39, number of alternatives.
634 I. Dar-Nimrod et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 631–635

Table 1 7
Mean choice satisfaction scores as a function of choice set size and choice-orientation.

Large assortment Small assortment 6.6

Satisfaction
M SD n M SD n 6.2
Study 2: chocolate
Maximizers* 4.73 1.86 19 5.64 0.98 30 5.8
Satisficers 6.00 0.85 12 5.44 1.47 35
Study 3: ice cream 5.4
Maximizers* 5.81 1.16 43 6.56 0.70 18
Satisficers 6.50 0.73 30 6.37 1.03 30
5
*
Note: row means significantly differ, p < .05. Small Assortment Large Assortment
Satisficers Maximizers

neighborhoods in which the two ice cream parlors were located Fig. 2. Satisfaction with ice cream as a product of choice-making orientation and
differed in terms of residential density. Whereas the parlor with number of alternatives.
the larger assortment was in an industrial area of the city, located
away from commercial or residential units, the parlor with the action we conducted simple slope analyses predicting satisfaction
smaller assortment was located in an area with high residential from maximization score separately for each assortment size.
and commercial density. Moreover, all the patrons who were re- Among patrons at the large parlor, higher maximization scores
cruited at the smaller assortment parlor arrived on foot whereas negatively predicted patrons’ satisfaction with their ice cream,
all the patrons who were recruited from the larger assortment par- b = .42, t = 3.55, p = .001. Among patrons at the small parlor,
lor arrived by car and left immediately following the study/ice no relation between choice-making orientation and satisfaction
cream consumption. Therefore we inferred that patrons of the lar- emerged, b = .03, |t| < 1.
ger parlor were sacrificing more time and effort to specifically seek To compare the satisfaction ratings among maximizers faced
out a larger assortment than were those in the smaller parlor3. with different assortments we divided the sample via median split
on maximization score (Mdn = 58.5). An ANOVA revealed an inter-
3.1.2. Procedure and materials action between choice-making orientation and parlor on satisfac-
Research assistants approached customers leaving the ice tion, F(1, 117) = 5.53, p = .02, g2p = .05 (see Fig. 2). A planned
cream parlors with purchase in hand, offering them $5 for their comparison indicated that maximizers at the large parlor were less
participation in a short study about making choices. The question- satisfied with their selection than were maximizers at the small
naire asked participants to rate how satisfied they were with their parlor, t(59) = 2.52, p = .02, d = .66. Satisfaction ratings of satisfic-
ice cream selection (1 = not at all, 7 = very satisfied). Participants ers were not affected by assortment size |t| (58) < 1. See Table 1 for
were also asked how frequently they visited the parlor; controlling means.
for this did not influence results. The Maximization Scale (a = .72) Results of Study 3 provide additional evidence for the Maximi-
and demographic questions followed. zation Paradox in a field setting. Maximizers tended to seek out a
larger ice cream assortment than did satisficers despite the fact
3.2. Results that it cost them in terms of satisfaction.

3.2.1. Preference for larger assortment size: part 1 of Maximization 4. General discussion
Paradox
To investigate the relation between choice-making orientation Across three studies, we demonstrated the Maximization Para-
and choice of ice cream parlor we conducted a logistic regression dox: choice-making orientation positively predicts willingness to
analysis predicting location from maximization score. The analysis sacrifice time and effort to gain additional options from which to
revealed a marginally significant effect of choice-making orienta- choose (Studies 1 through 3), yet such sacrifices ultimately de-
tion, v2(1) = 3.37, p = .07. For every one unit increase in maximiza- crease satisfaction with the chosen option (Studies 2 and 3). Study
tion score, the odds of being at the larger assortment parlor 1 offers the first evidence that people will deliberately sacrifice
increased by 3% (.90CI: .3%, 6.1%), Wald = 3.25, p = .07, OR = 1.03. their time and effort in search of more options. As predicted, this
Notably, 71% (15/21) of the extreme maximizers (i.e., those with finding is moderated by choice-making orientation. Study 1 relied
scores greater than 1 SD above the sample mean) visited the larger on participants to accurately report how they would respond to a
assortment parlor, whereas only 40.9% (9/21) of the extreme satis- hypothetical situation; Studies 2 and 3 improve this methodology
ficers (i.e., those with scores lower than 1 SD below the sample by investigating actual choices. Study 2 replicates Study 1’s result
mean) did. in an actual rather than hypothetical choice, and extends it by
investigating satisfaction with the selected alternative. Results re-
3.2.2. Satisfaction with ice cream choice: part 2 of the Maximization vealed that maximizers were more likely than satisficers to explic-
Paradox itly and deliberately sacrifice their time to choose from a large
We next conducted a regression analysis predicting satisfaction rather than a small chocolate assortment, yet were ultimately less
from choice-making orientation and assortment size. Choice-mak- satisfied with their selection when they did so. Study 3 replicated
ing orientation was a significant predictor of satisfaction, b = .23, Study 2’s findings in a natural setting. Maximizers were more likely
t = 2.58, p = .01, but this relation was qualified by a significant to be found at an ice cream parlor that offers myriad alternatives
choice-making orientation by assortment size (location) interac- located in an isolated industrial area of the city, rather than at a
tion, b = .19, t = 2.24, p = .03. To investigate the nature of the inter- neighborhood ice cream parlor offering few alternatives. Moreover,
maximizers choosing from the large assortment were less satisfied
3
with their chosen ice cream than were maximizers choosing from
The large ice cream parlor’s owner did not want us to highlight its distant
location, so we were unable to explicitly ask people whether they sacrificed time and
the small assortment, and to satisficers in general.
effort to get to the parlor. However, this fact in itself supports our inference that Previous research has revealed the detrimental psychological
people who went to the larger parlor were sacrificing to get there. effects of choosing from large rather than small choice set sizes
I. Dar-Nimrod et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 631–635 635

(e.g., Chernev, 2003; Iyengar & Lepper, 2000; Schwartz, 2000, ment. Regarding the latter, maximizers are sensitive to regret (Sch-
2004). The present studies empirically demonstrate some people’s wartz et al., 2002). Choices made after sacrificing for more
willingness to sacrifice resources to attain additional alternatives. options—as in our studies—may be especially disappointing for
In addition, they make a novel contribution to the growing litera- maximizers because of regretting lost time and effort in the search
ture highlighting how individual differences in choice-making ori- process in addition to regretting unselected alternatives. It is also
entation moderate the psychological impact of choice set size. We possible that sacrificing to gain more alternatives leads maximizers
extend earlier work that relies on memory biases (Schwartz et al., to increase expectations for what they consider to be the ‘‘best”
2002) and inconclusive self-reported assessments of effort exerted alternative, thereby making it more difficult to actually attain the
in the choice process (Iyengar et al., 2006) to show for the first time best alternative. Future research that determines the processes at
that maximizers will consciously sacrifice to glean more options work may help maximizers avoid the cycle of sacrifice and dissat-
from which to choose. Simultaneously, we offer more evidence isfaction antithetical to their personal well-being.
that maximizers are dissatisfied with their choices, in this case Would maximizers consciously sacrifice for more alternatives if
when they choose from a large number of alternatives. they were aware of the impending satisfaction reduction that
Notably, satisficers did not experience the commonly found ad- accompanies vast searches? Generally, people have difficulty antic-
verse effects of choosing from many alternatives (e.g., Iyengar & ipating future physical and emotional states (Gilbert & Wilson,
Lepper, 2000). Satisficers tended not to sacrifice time and energy 2007; Giordano et al., 2002; Loewenstein, 2005). Maximizers
to attain larger choice sets. Past research has claimed a widespread may be especially poor at predicting their future states, to the ex-
‘‘lure of choice” (Bown, Read, & Summers, 2003) yet the current re- tent that they are captivated by pursuing the best option at the
sults suggest that large choice set sizes may not be as alluring to moment of choice. Making information about their future (dis)sat-
satisficers as they are to maximizers. Contrasting results among isfaction salient for maximizers while they are making decisions
maximizers, the few satisficers who opted to sacrifice for the larger may help maximizers curtail the choice process in time to experi-
set were as satisfied with their choice outcome as if they had cho- ence some satisfaction with the choice. In Studies 2 and 3, maxi-
sen from the smaller selection. Such outcome satisfaction dovetails mizers who did not sacrifice for a larger alternative were just as
nicely with work showing that satisficers have better health (e.g., happy with their selections as were satisficers, suggesting that
reduced stress and depression) than do maximizers (Schwartz some intervention to help maximizers opt out of sacrificing for
et al., 2002), despite sometimes choosing objectively poorer out- more options may improve their outcome satisfaction.
comes than maximizers (Iyengar et al., 2006). More research is
needed to determine the precise mechanism that enables satisfic- 5. Conclusion
ers to manage choices in a way that supports rather than hinders
their well-being. Doing so may reveal strategies to assist maximiz- Choice is highly valued in society. More options from which to
ers to cope productively with choices. choose are perceived as better than are fewer options because, log-
A key strength of our studies is that participants naturally se- ically, the larger set is more likely to yield a desirable option (Iyen-
lected whether they would sacrifice for more alternatives. This de- gar and Lepper, 2000; Schwartz, 2004). Yet some people,
sign lends critical external validity to our findings while enabling a particularly maximizers, suffer adverse consequences from the
true test of whether people will choose to sacrifice for more options promise offered by the larger set of alternatives. Identifying this
or not. In today’s marketplace there is a plethora of choice, includ- paradox is an important step toward helping people deal with
ing the choice to seek out large stores with vast arrays of options or the vast arrays of options they can choose to face or avoid on a dai-
small stores with limited selection. Based on our results maximiz- ly basis.
ers should be more attracted than satisficers to large stores with
great selection despite remote locations, rather than to small local
References
shops. Yet maximizers will likely experience lower satisfaction with
purchases made from larger rather than smaller stores. Regularly Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting,
sacrificing resources for more alternatives, and feeling dissatisfied interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
with the ultimate choice seems like a particularly unrewarding pat- Bown, N. J., Read, D., & Summers, B. (2003). The lure of choice. Journal of Behavioral
Decision Making, 16, 297–308.
tern that may contribute to broader emotional discontent found in Chernev, A. (2003). When more is less and less is more: The impact of ideal point
previous research4 (Schwartz et al., 2002). This pattern may be espe- availability and assortment in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research,
cially problematic should future research show that the Maximiza- 30, 170–183.
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science,
tion Paradox occurs for countless everyday choices like the ones we
317, 1351–1354.
studied as well as important life decisions. Giordano, L. A., Bickel, W. K., Loewenstein, G., Jacobs, E. A., Marsch, L., & Badger, G. J.
(2002). Mild opiod deprivation increases the degree that opiod-dependent
outpatients discount delayed heroin and money. Psychopharmacology, 163,
4.1. Future directions
174–182.
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can one desire
This paper sets the stage for much research exploring the im- too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
pact of choice-making orientation on decision processes and out- 995–1006.
Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R. E., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better but feeling worse:
comes. The next step toward fully understanding choice-making Looking for the ‘‘best” job undermines satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17,
orientation is to determine the precise mechanisms through which 143–150.
maximizers decide to sacrifice for more alternatives, and then how Lee, B-K, & Lee, W-N (2004). The effect of information overload on consumer choice
quality in an on-line environment. Psychology and Marketing, 21, 159–183.
they come to be dissatisfied after choosing from that larger assort- Loewenstein, G. (2005). Hot–cold empathy gaps and medical decision making.
Health Psychology, 24, S49–S56.
Rozin, P., Fischler, C., Shields, C., & Masson, E. (2006). Attitudes towards large
4
numbers of choices in the food domain: A cross-cultural study of five countries
The correlates of maximizing choice orientation, specifically general life satisfac- in Europe and the USA. Appetite, 46, 304–308.
tion, may have affected outcome satisfaction results. If general life satisfaction was Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice. New York, NY: HarperCollins.
affecting satisfaction ratings, we would expect maximizers to be less satisfied than Schwartz, B. (2000). Self-determination: The tyranny of freedom. American
satisficers regardless of the choice set size, which is a result that we do not find. Psychologist, 55, 79–88.
Instead, we find no significant differences in satisfaction between maximizers and Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. R.
satisficers choosing from a small assortment, and a significant difference between (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of
maximizers choosing from large vs. small assortments. Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1178–1197.

You might also like