You are on page 1of 13

Journal ~ Voice

Vol. 12, No. 3, pp. 315-327


© 1998 Singular Publishing Group, Inc.

Laryngeal Adduction in Resonant Voice

*Katherine Verdolini, tSDavid G. Druker, ?Phyllis M. Palmer, and **Hani Samawi


*Division of Otology and Laryngology, Harvard Medical School;
*Voice and Speech Laborator3; Massachusetts Eye and Ear hTfinnar).,,
*Communications Sciences and Disorders, MGH Institute of Health Professions;
*Voice/Speech/Swallowing Division, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Brigham
and Women's Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts; f'Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology;
**Department of Preventive Medicine and Environmental Health, Biostatistics Division, The University of lowa;
"~National Center for Voice and Speech, Iowa Ci~, Iowa;
"P'POfficeof hTformation Resources, UniversiO, of Utah Hospitals and Clinics, Salt Lake Cit); Utah, U.S.A.

Summary: The primary question in this study was whether subjects with nod-
ules and subjects with healthy larynges would produce "resonant voice" with a
similar laryngeal configuration. A second question regarded whether the elec-
troglottographic closed quotient (EGG CQ) could be used to noninvasively dis-
tinguish resonant from other voice types. Twelve adult singers and actors served
as subjects, including 6 persons with healthy larynges and 6 persons with nod-
ules. Performers were used as an attempt to maximize token validity and stabil-
ity. Subjects produced repeated tokens of resonant, pressed, normal, and breathy
voice during sustained vowels. Laryngeal adduction was directly estimated us-
ing blinded, ordinal, visual-perceptual ratings based on videoscopic views of the
larynx. EGG CQs were further calculated based on separate trials. The percep-
tual ratings indicated that subjects in both groups produced resonant voice with
a barely adducted or barely abducted laryngeal configuration that was distinct
from configurations for pressed and breathy (but not normal) voice. Previous lit-
erature suggests that this configuration may be relevant in many cases of voice
therapy ( 1). Average CQs distinguished resonant from pressed voice, but incon-
sistently distinguished resonant from breathy voice. Further CQs were reliably
different across healthy subjects and subjects with nodules. Thus, the utility of
this measure to noninvasively estimate resonant voice may be limited, particu-
larly without ongoing subject-specific calibration procedures. Key Words: Res-
onant voice--Laryngeal adduction--Voice therapy--Electroglottography--
Closed quotient.

Accepted for publication March 10, 1996. Previous computational work has indicated that a
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Katherine barely abducted, or a barely adducted, laryngeal con-
Verdolini, Ph.D., CSD, MGH IHP 101 Merrimac Street, figuration may be favorable in the treatment of voice
Boston, MA 02114 U.S.A.
An earlier version of this work was presented at the 22nd disorders arising from a range of etiologies (1).
Annual Symposium: Care of the Professional Voice, June 7-12, Specifically, barely abducted vocal folds have been
1993; Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. proposed to produce maximum "vocal economy"

315
316 KATHERINE VERDOLINI, DAVID G. DRUKER, PHYLLIS M. PALMER, AND HANI SAMAWI

(Ev-max) defined as the maximized ratio between bined with a complete glottal closure" (4, p. 559). In-
voice output (dB) and intraglottal impact stress (kPa) spection of inverse-filtered waveforms for flow
under constant subglottic pressure and frequency phonation in fact reveals a slightly positive minimum
conditions. Ev-max may be conceptually relevant for flow offset, implying barely abducted vocal folds (4,
many cases of voice therapy because it theoretically p. 558).
represents an optimum tradeoff between voice output A second voicing pattern that may involve a bare-
(maximized) and intraglottal impact stress (mini- ly ab/adducted laryngeal configuration is "resonant
haized), which is a desirable objective for many pa- voice." This general type of phonation mode has
tients. According to the model, the objective in ther- been used in theatre and singing pedagogy for gener-
apy for hyperadducted conditions, such as nodules, ations (5) and more recently has also been used in
would be to train the barely ab/adducted laryngeal speech pathology (6). In the present context, resonant
configuration by decreasing adduction. For condi- voice is defined as a voicing pattern involving oral vi-
tions involving hypoadduction, including paralysis bratory sensations, particularly on the alveolar ridge
and bowing, the objective would be to train a similar and adjacent facial plates, in the context of what sub-
configuration by increasing adduction. Thus, the jects perceive as "easy" phonation. One previous
same physiological goal might be used in therapy for study indicated that vocally healthy, trained subjects
different patient populations. produced resonant voice with barely abducted, or
Speculations about Ev-max are based on a combi- barely adducted, vocal folds, and thus a configuration
nation of simulated and excised larynx data (1,2). within the range of those producing Ev-max (7). A
The model has not yet been tested in human subjects preliminary efficacy study indicated some evidence
because the technology for reliably measuring intra- of a therapeutic benefit from resonant voice for pa-
glottal impact stress in humans is still under develop- tients with nodules, presumably because adduction,
ment (3). Awaiting that technology, we propose that and thus intraglottal impact, was reduced compared
the Ev model may be a reasonable one to use as a with a previous pathogenic voice pattern, while voice
framework in some voice therapy studies for three output intensity remained adequate for functional
reasons. First, other global, quantitative, theoretical- purposes (6).
ly and empirically based models of voice therapy are The purpose of the present study is to further pur-
not available. Reference to an incompletely verified sue the "resonant voice" phenomenon as a possible
model provides a better rational framework for ther- example of a voice type involving barely ab/adduct-
apy investigations than no model at all. Second, by ed vocal folds. Specifically, two questions were
pursuing simulated and canine data, internal validity asked. The first and primary question was if subjects
is enhanced due to a level of experimental control not with nodules produce resonant voice with a barely
possible in awake humans. Third, at least some stud- abducted (or barely adducted) laryngeal configura-
ies conducted within the model's framework should tion at conversational pitches, as do vocally normal
provide evidence regarding the model's viability, thus subjects. Laryngeal adduction was estimated using
constraining further theoretical developments. visual perceptual ratings from videoscopic views of
Given these considerations, it would seem helpful the larynx.
to identify voicing patterns that may involve a barely The second question was whether resonant voice
abducted or barely adducted laryngeal configuration, could be noninvasively identified using the EGG CQ.
and then to study the effect of their training in con- Assuming that resonant voice would indeed be pro-
trolled clinical trials. Two voice types possibly in- duced with the barely abducted or barely adducted
volving this target configuration, "flow phonation" laryngeal configuration of interest as in a previous
and "resonant voice," have been previously described study (7), the identification of a noninvasive tool to
in the literature. Flow phonation has been formally detect it might be clinically useful. For both ques-
defined as "that type of phonation that has the high- tions, pressed, normal, and breathy voice types were
est possible glottogram amplitude that can be corn- used as comparative voicing modes.

Journal ofVoice, Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


LARYNGEAL ADDUCTION IN RESONANT VOICE 317

METHODS The remaining six subjects, also including 3 males


and 3 females, were recruited from the voice case-
Subjects load in our clinic among patients with laryngeal nod-
Twelve adult, vocally trained singers or actors par- ules. These subjects had completed at least a portion
ticipated in the experiment as volunteers. Trained of voice therapy, and were also trained singers (N =
subjects were used in this study because we antici- 5) or actors (N = 1). They complained of persisting
pated that they would require relatively little training or recurring hoarseness despite improvements made
to produce valid samples of the voice types to be in therapy, they demonstrated some degree of hoarse-
evaluated, and that they would produce the voice ness on the day of participation, and videoscopic ex-
types with minimal variability as compared with un- aminations during the experiment confirmed the per-
trained subjects. sistence of nodules. The average age in this subject
Subject characteristics are shown in Table 1. Sub- group was 29 years (range = 20-39). Although gen-
der and age characteristics were similar across the
jects were divided into two groups. Six subjects, 3
two groups, subjects were not specifically matched in
males and 3 females, were vocally healthy. They de-
a pairwise fashion.
nied any history of a voice disorder, their voices were
In addition to age and gender, Table 1 also displays
judged as normal by a professor of clinical voice on the
years of prior voice training, duration of prior voice
day of the experiments, and videoscopic examinations therapy, and for subjects with nodules, the number of
during the protocol (described later) further confirmed therapy sessions received and change in auditory-
normal laryngeal status for these subjects. These sub- perceptual status of voice following therapy, based
jects were current or former graduate students in Voice on clinical records. For this change score, a 5-point
in the Department of Music at our university (N = 5 scale was used to indicate severe (5), moderately se-
and N = 1, respectively). The average age in this sub- vere (4), moderate (3), and mild dysphonia (2), and
ject group was 27 years (range = 22-30 yr). normal voice (1). A unit change of "1" would indi-

T A B L E 1. Subject characteristics including age, gender, years of prior voice training, duration of prior voice
therapy and number of therapy sessions, and unit change in voice with therapy~
Training Therapy Voice
Subject Age Gender (yr) (mo) # Sessions change

01 = Healthy 30 M 12 NA NA NA
02 = Healthy 29 F 15 NA NA NA
03 = Healthy 22 F 9 NA NA NA
04 = Healthy 23 M 6 NA NA NA
05 = Healthy 28 F 13 NA NA NA
06 = Healthy 28 M 12 NA NA NA
07 = Nodules 30 F 6 2.00 5 2 (S - M)
08 = Nodules 25 M 7 8.00 22 I (Mi-N)
09 = Nodules 20 M 8 5.00 7 0 (Mi-Mi)
10 = Nodules 20 F 4 7.00 23 3 (MS-N)
11 = Nodules 39 M 6 0.75 3 2 (MS-M)
12 = Nodules 38 F 8 2.00 5 2 (S - M)

t5 = S = Severe; 4 = MS = Moderately Severe; 3 = M = Moderate; 2 = Mi = Mild; 1 = N = Normal.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


318 KATHERINE VERDOLINI, DAVID G. DRUKER, PHYLLIS M. PALMER, AND HANI SAMAWI

cite an improvement from any one of these scaler Assuming that direct, perceptual measures of ad-
values to the next, less severe grade of dysphonia (for duction would identify resonant voice as associated
example, improvement from moderately severe to with barely ab/adducted vocal folds, a secondary,
moderate). noninvasive estimate of adduction was also assessed
for its potential to identify resonant voice, and the
Measures of Laryngeal Adduction target laryngeal configuration, in clinical situations.
The primary, direct measure of laryngeal adduction This was the EGG CQ. The EGG signal reflects the
qnvolved an ordinal rating of adduction based on amount of translaryngeal current passage between
videoscopic views of the larynx during vowel pro- bilateral surface electrodes, typically during phona-
duction. Specifically, expert judges who were unin- tory tasks. This signal is thought to be a good esti-
formed about subjects' experimental conditions and mate of changes in vocal fold contact area during
about the experimental hypotheses were shown 5-sec phonation (8,9). The CQ, derived from the EGG sig-
segments of videotaped segments of the larynx during nal, reflects the proportion of vocal fold closed ver-
vowel productions using pressed, resonant, normal, sus vocal fold open time during the glottal cycle. The
and breathy voice. Segments were presented without CQ is anticipated to increase as glottal adduction in-
the audio channel, and multiple views were offered if
creases, because of greater glottal resistance to sub-
the judges required them. Judges were instructed to
glottic pressure and thus, longer closed phases. Pre-
rate each segment on an ordinal, visual-analogue
vious empirical studies confirm this prediction by
scale on which - 5 indicated "extreme hypoadduc-
showing a covariance of the CQ with direct, visually
tion" and +5 indicated "extreme hyperadduction." In-
based measures of adduction (7,10). In the present
termediate, noninteger values were allowed and en-
study, the question was whether the EGG CQ would
couraged. A rating of zero was to indicate a "neutral"
be sensitive enough to distinguish resonant voice
adduction rating, or barely adducted vocal folds.
from other voice types.
Judges were told to use their own internal criteria for
the ratings. Although changes in anteroposterior and
Equipment
supraglottic activity may accompany vocal fold
Videoscopic images of subjects' larynges were ob-
changes, judges' attention was directed to the vocal
tained using a 90 ° R. Wolf 4450.47 rigid telescopic
folds' medial dimension. No other instructions were
provided, nor were examples of the various ratings endoscope, a Karl Storz 9000 Mini Solid State CCD
provided in this study. The segments were presented Video Camera, and a Bruel and Kj~ier Rhino-Larynx
to judges blocked by subject, in randomized order Stroboscope light source, Type 4914. Images were
within subjects. After all the segments had been pre- later played back to judges on a large screen color
sented the first time, they were presented again using television monitor, Sony Ideo Projection System KP-
new random orders within and across subjects. 5000, using a Sony U-matic videocassette recorder.
Perceptual, ordinal measures of laryngeal adduc- A SynchroVoice Electroglottograph was used to
tion were used in this study for a combination of rea- collect EGG signals. Simultaneous audio signals
sons. First, interval and ratio measures using metric were collected with an AKG C 460 B condenser mi-
(ram) scales are not yet available in live humans. Per- crophone powered by a Symetrix SX202 Dual Mic
cent scores of relative intralaryngeal distances (e.g., preamplifier. Aerodynamic signals were also collect-
intervocal process distance divided by length of the ed at the same time as EGG and audio signals for
membranous vocal folds) were inappropriate, be- analysis purposes that are not further described here.
cause these measures are made at discrete moments All signals were recorded on a PC-108M SONY
in time, whereas time-integrated measures were sought Digital Audio Tape (DAT) recorder with DC fre-
in the present study. Also, the precision of such rela- quency response capabilities, and stored for later
tive measures is unknown and may be no different analysis. Signals were also monitored online using a
from the precision obtained with ordinal measures. Data 6000 digital oscilloscope. Following data col-
Ultimately, the ordinal measures provided the inte- lection, a Hewlett Packard 350D attenuator was used
grated assessments of laryngeal adduction that were to attenuate files that were clipped during initial dig-
pertinent to the study questions. itizing, but were not clipped during data collection.

Journal of Voice. VoL 12, No. 3, 1998


L A R Y N G E A L A D D U C T I O N IN R E S O N A N T V O I C E 319

Because subjects would produce constant pitches Once the window of stable phonation was identi-
during EGG data collection, a Casio keyboard was fied for analysis, the initial period of one EGG cycle
used to provide pitches for critical experimental tri- was measured using cursors provided by the wave-
als. A Wittner Taktall Piccolo metronome was used form editor. Starting and ending times of acceptable
to indicate seconds elapsed for the experimental tri- phonation and an estimate of the pitch period were
als, which involved constant durations across all entered into the program. The program then found
conditions. the maximum and minimum signal levels for each
cycle, and calculated the signal level at vocal fold
EGG Analysis Software closure on the nth cycle so(n) as
After data collection was completed, EGG signals
were digitized at 12.5 kHz with a 16-bit resolution s,.(n) = min(n) + 0.35 (max(n) - min(n) )
Ariel DSP 32-c coprocessor board for analogue-to-
digital conversion, using Hypersignal software (1989) where min(n) and max(n) are the minimum and max-
installed in a Gateway 386 computer. As noted, the imum EGG signal levels during the nth cycle. Signal
EGG signal is thought to be a good estimate of changes level at vocal fold opening So(n) was similarly calcu-
in vocal fold contact area during closure (8,9). By lated as
setting an appropriate criterion level, a single cycle of
EGG signal can be divided into open and closed so(n) = min(n + 1) + 0.35 (max(n) - min(n + 1) )
phases. The CQ is then calculated as "glottal closed
phase divided by period" (9, p. 343). A locally writ- where min(n + 1) is the minimum EGG signal level
ten computer program calculated the closed quotient of the subsequent cycle. Cycles were assumed to be-
for each cycle (pitch period) of the EGG signal (Fig. gin and end with vocal fold closure. It is necessary to
1). This procedure is similar to the one described by separately calculate the closing and opening signal
Rothenberg and Mahshie using the 35% criterion levels by using the closest maximum and minimum,
they suggest, but more complex due to the conisder- to compensate for low frequency components in the
ation of possible signal drift as discussed below. EGG signal. Rothenberg and Mahshie (9) discuss the
Each utterance was analyzed as follows. The in- causes of these components, including artifacts from
vestigators first examined the signal from each utter- movement of electrodes and the structure of the neck.
ance in its entirety using a waveform editor and iden- Low frequency components, which appear as "DC
tified the portion of the waveform with stable shift" of the average signal level, were present in all
phonation. In most cases this was almost the entire the signals collected in this study. It is possible to at-
duration of voicing, or 4 sec for each utterance. In tenuate these components by high-pass filtering the
such cases the program was able to analyze all cy- signal with a linear phase filter; the technique used
cles, which included about 300-600 successive cy- here produces similar results (9).
cles depending on the fundamental frequency (f0). Using so(n) and So(n), the program located the clos-
However, some utterances were contaminated by ing and opening times for each cycle and calculated
noise. In some cases, no stable phonation could be the closed quotient as
identified for analysis, or the computer program was
otherwise unable to perform analyses (N = 25/144 ut- closed duration to(n) - tc(n)
CQ--
terances for the vowel/i/, but no cases for the vowel total period tc(n+ l) - tc(n)
/a/). In other, rare cases, only a relatively small num-
ber (as few as 30) of cycles could be analyzed for a where to(n) and to(n) are the closing and opening
given utterance. It should be noted, however, that times for the nth cycle, respectively. As the program
with one exception, for all subjects at least one token proceeds through the signal it readjusts the pitch pe-
was partially or fully available for analysis for each riod estimate using the opening and closing times of.
of the four voice types produced and described later. the preceding cycle. Because subjects were instruct-
The exception was Subject 12, for whom no token of ed to maintain constant pitch during each utterance,
resonant voice was analyzable for/i/. these adjustments were small. All times were inter-

Journal of Voice, Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


320 KATHERINE VERDOLINI, DAVID G. DRUKER, PHYLLIS M. PALMER, AND HANI SAMAWI

A
I

<
*6
t-
O
0
-0
sc(n)
0
It.

so(n)
0
>
v Sc(n+ l)
"o
-I

Tx So(n+1)
I
E I I
< I I
(.9 I I
I I
(.9 I I
UJ ! I
I l
I I
'! closed I'
a duration t

I I
I J
, total period

I ! !

tc(n) to(n) to(n+1) tc(n+ l)


FIG. 1. This figure depicts two cycles of EGG oriented with vocal fold contact area (maximum
closure) up. Closing and opening events for each cycle determined by 35% criterion levels are in-
dicated by dots and labeled by so(n), s,,(n), s,:(n+l), and s,,(n+l). Corresponding closing and open-
ing times are marked on the abscissa with to(n), to(n), t,.(n+l), and t,,(n+l), respectively. The mean
voltage level of the waveform descends over time due to low frequency components in the signal,
but criterion levels are calculated separately for each glottal closure and opening. This yields good
estimates of closed duration and total period.

polated for greater accuracy. The program wrote the using 20 dB attenuation. This manipulation did not af-
average closed quotient for all analyzed cycles as its fect CQ estimates, which are time-based estimates
output. that do not depend on absolute signal magnitudes.
During data analysis, it was noted that 13 of the dig-
itized EGG files f o r / i / w e r e clipped. However, the Procedures
corresponding utterances were unclipped in their Data were collected in a quiet room. Subjects were
original format on the DAT tape, and were redigitized seated throughout the experiment. EGG data were

Journal ofVoice, Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


LARYNGEAL ADDUCTION IN RESONANT VOICE 321

collected first, followed by laryngeal videoscopy. Specific verbal descriptions of the voice types were
First, each subject's typical conversational fo was es- somewhat challenging, and they were demonstrated
timated because all EGG trials would be completed rather than verbally described to each subject. Pressed
at the conversational pitch. These pitches were ex- voice was demonstrated as a high effort phonation
amined because they are usually the most relevant for mode, as if pushing with a relatively closed airway.
the greatest proportion of voice therapy tasks• Con- Normal voice corresponded to a conversational voic-
stant pitches within subjects were further necessary ing mode. Resonant voice was modeled as a low ef-
because laryngeal configuration is known to vary fort phonation mode with a "ringing" or "focused"
with fo changes (see for example, Hollien, 1974), and voice quality, accompanied by oral vibratory sensa-
we did not want the interpretation of EGG data to be tions. Breathy voice was also demonstrated as a low
confounded by fo-related factors. effort phonation mode, relatively quiet, with audible
To determine each subject's typical conversational air escapage during phonation.
pitch, the subject was asked to count out loud from 1 Following training (about 10 min), subjects pro-
to 5 in a normal conversational voice, sustaining the duced the actual experimental tokens by sustaining
vowel in the word "three." The fo for that vowel was the vowels/a/,/i/, a n d / u / i n blocked fashion, with
determined from an oscilloscopic display, and the vowel order counterbalanced across subjects. Within
corresponding pitch was used for all subsequent each vowel, subjects produced 3 tokens each of pressed,
EGG trials for that subject• normal, resonant, and breathy voice, with the order
After the subject's conversational pitch had been of the 12 tokens (4 voice types × 3 tokens) random-
determined, the subject received training for the ex-
ly ordered within each subject. The order of vowel by
perimental procedures• The training period was de-
voice-type trials was indicated on a sheet of paper
signed to be brief, to minimize fatigue before the ex-
individually supplied to each subject. Subjects were
perimental protocol• For that reason, a single vowel
instructed to repeat any trial that they considered an
was chosen for training• This vowel was different
invalid example of the target voice type, and the ex-
from the ones used in later data collection (/iL/a/,
perimenter also requested repeated tokens for trials
and/u/), to avoid a differential training effect for one
that she considered poor exemplars. Including 3 vow-
of those vowels• Specifically, for training, subjects
els by 4 voice types by 3 repetitions, each subject
were instructed to produce the vowel/o/continuous-
produced a total of 36 tokens considered valid by the
ly, at the identified conversational pitch provided
subject and the experimenter•
with a keyboard, for 4 seconds following a "Ready
• .n o w . . , b e g i n . . . " statement• A metronome set
.
A speech-language pathology doctoral student
at 60 beats per minute indicated the number of sec- with an emphasis in clinical voice science and who
onds elapsed. The subject practiced producing Io/in was uninformed about the order of the voice types
this fashion using pressed, normal, resonant, and provided a further, independent estimate of the to-
breathy voice, following demonstrations by the ex- kens' validity by indicating which voice type she per-
perimenter. Training trials were repeated until both ceived for each token• This judge was also allowed to
the subject and the experimenter were satisfied that request repeats of any token. However, she did not re-
the target voice types were consistently produced. veal her judgments nor request verification regarding
Because of the subjects' prior voice training and sta- voice type during data collection, because she was
tus as singers or actors, no more than a few training informed about the number of intended tokens for
trials were required for each voice type, for any sub- each voice type within vowel (three). Verification
ject. Although the subjects' skilled status likely af- procedures would have allowed her to progressively
fected the speed with which they could be prepared narrow down the possible voice types remaining
for the experimental protocol, based on our experi- within each vowel and thus compromise her unin-
ence the qualities of the voice types were similar to formed status. Subsequent analyses revealed that
those produced by nonsinger, nonactor subjects who there was agreement between this listener's percep-
have received sufficient prior training in the voice tions and the subject's intended voice type on 89% of
types in voice therapy or elsewhere. trials for/a/, with 72% and 76% agreement rates for

Journal of Voice. Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


322 KATHERINE VERDOLINI, DAVID G. DRUKER, PHYLLIS M. PALMER, AND HANI SAMAWI

/i/ and /u/, respectively. The agreement level may Videotaping continued for each voice type pro-
have been affected by a degradation in the acoustic duced until the examiner considered than an ade-
signal due to the placement of a face mask used for quate view of the larynx had been obtained for each
the collection of aerodynamic data not described token, and the subject and the examiner both consid-
here. The degradation effect was apparently stronger ered that a valid exemplar of the voice type had been
for this judge than for the primary experimenter, who produced. Again, a listener who was uninformed about
also verified trials; because the judge was seated the intended order of voice types (either a postdoc-
about 5 ft from the subject, whereas the experimenter toral otolaryngology fellow or a speech-language
was seated within about 1 ft. All tokens were retained pathology professor of voice disorders) further indi-
for statistical analyses; the reason is that assuming a cated on a sheet of paper the voice type that she per-
generally valid data set, the exclusion of not fully ceived for each trial. For these trials, there was 100%
corroborated trials would introduce a bias in favor of agreement between the intended and perceived voice
the experimental hypothesis, in this case a difference type, across the subject, the experimenter, and the lis-
in CQs as a function of voice type. tener-judge. Thus, without a face mask in place as for
Microphone signals were also recorded during EGG trials, the voice types were clearly and consis-
EGG trials. These signals were used to provide a tently distinguished by subjects and listeners.
"chatter channel" during later data analysis to aid in After the experiment was completed, four indepen-
identifying tokens to be analyzed. However, no dent judges (two otolaryngologists and two speech
acoustic analyses were performed for the tokens be- pathologists, all with extensive experience in video-
cause of the acoustic degradation across the face scopic imaging of the larynx) evaluated videotaped
mask, which has been already noted. segments for each token produced under videoscop-
Following this first part of the experiment, which ic examination. For each token, a 5-sec segment was
lasted no more than about 20 min including training selected as the "best view of the larynx" by a re-
and data collection, videoscopic images of subjects' search assistant specialized in voice disorders who
larynges were obtained using a rigid endoscope dur- was uninformed about the order of the voice types
ing sustained/i/, produced with pressed, normal, res- and was further naive to the experimental hypothe-
onant, and breathy voice in randomized order across ses. The selected segments were presented in random
subjects. EGG and videoscopic data were not ob- order to the judges, without audio and with tokens
tained in tandem because the positioning of the face blocked by subject. After all tokens had been pre-
mask during EGG trials prohibited simultaneous sented once to the judges in this way, the tokens were
rigid endoscopy. The v o w e l / i / w a s used for video- presented again without audio in a different random
scopy because this vowel facilitates a full view of the order. Judges were instructed to rate each segment on
larynx during phonation. a visual-analogue scale described in a previous sec-
For videoscopic trials, subjects used a consistent, tion to indicate amount of perceived adduction. Mul-
comfortable conversational pitch that allowed for ad- tiple viewings were allowed, if the judges required
equate views of the larynx. This pitch was the same them.
used for EGG trials or within a maximum of three Although rigid endoscopy can affect laryngeal
whole tones above or below that pitch. Slight differ- configuration, this possible source of distortion is
ences between pitches for video versus EGG trials probably not extremely relevant for the present in-
were required in some cases to obtain an adequate la- vestigation. First, as noted, videoscopic examinations
ryngeal view. These differences did not pose a prob- continued until a valid exemplar of each voice type
lem for the experimental design because the point had been produced. Presumably, such exemplars re-
was not to directly compare video and EGG mea- quired representative laryngeal physiologies for the
sures, but was to obtain different estimates of laryn- voice types, and adduction ratings were based on
geal adduction for the voice types examined within these final samples. Further, the potential problem of
the conversational pitch range: a direct measure for different camera angles affecting perceived adduc-
investigatory purposes and an indirect one for poten- tion was a self-limiting one within this experimental
tial clinical application. situation. If reliable differences in adduction ratings

Journal of Voice, Vok 12, No. 3, 1998


LARYNGEAL ADDUCTION IN RESONANT VOICE 323

were detected as a function of voice type, then effects Statistical analyses first involved a Cochran-Man-
of this type were smaller than those occurring for tel-Haenszel Test of Association (1 I, pp. 873-874) to
changes in voice type. determine the degree of agreement between judges
for the ratings. The Row Means Scores Difference
Statistical Analyses from this test indicated that judges scores were asso-
Statistical analyses focused on the results for the ciated (p < .04), showing good agreement. Therefore,
vowel /i/, which was consistent across videoscopic scores from all judges could be included together in
and EGG tasks. For EGG, the results for/a/were al- subsequent analyses. A Multivariate Analysis of Vari-
so analyzed and reported for all subjects because of ance (MANOVA) treating each judge's score as a
the relatively good agreement rates between subjects
separate outcome, with group (healthy larynx versus
and examiners for this vowel (89%; agreement for/i/
nodules) as a between-subjects factor and voice type
was only 72%). Because/u/productions were neither
(pressed, resonant, normal, and breathy) as a within-
recapitulated under videoendoscopy, nor were agree-
subjects factor, was conducted to assess a possible
ment scores particularly compelling (76%), the re-
suits for this vowel were inspected and reported for group times voice type interaction in the results. This
two randomly selected subjects. analysis failed to reveal any interaction (F(3,184) =
0.28, p < .85), and thus a subsequent Repeated Mea-
sures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with group and
voice type was conducted without the interaction in
RESULTS
the equation to avoid overfitting the model. In this
Laryngeal Adduction Ratings analysis, the main effect of group was unreliable I
Average adduction ratings are shown in Table 2, (F(i,187) = 0.63, p < .44), but the main effect of voice
which shows that the largest ratings were obtained type was reliable (F(3,187) = 29.67, p < .0001). Thus,
for pressed voice, with those ratings indicating a rel- subjects with nodules and subjects with healthy la-
ative hyperadduction. Lower ratings consistent with rynges showed similar effects. Post-hoc comparisons
a barely adducted or barely abducted vocal fold con- with all subjects pooled indicated that the adduction
figuration were obtained for resonant and normal ratings for all voice types reliably differed from each
voice types. The lowest ratings, indicating laryngeal other (p < .05), except normal and resonant voice, for
hypoadduction, were obtained for breathy voice. which ratings were equivalent.

T A B L E 2. Average lalyngeal adduction ratings as a function of voice ~,pe, from videoscopic


views, on lip

Voice type group Pressed Resonant Normal Breathy Average

Healthy 1.23 0.04 -0.36 - 1.33 -0.10


(I.45) (1.04) (0.85) (1.97)
Nodules 1.31 -0.40 -0.46 - 1.52 -0.27
(1.43) (1.36) (1.64) (1.36)
Average 1.27 - 0.18 - 0.41 - 1.43 - 0.19

~ +5 = extreme hyperadduction; 0 = barely adducted; - 5 = extreme hypoadduction. Standard deviations indi-


cated in parentheses.

tln this article, the term "statistically reliable" is used in place of the more common term "statistically significant." The reason is related to a point
made in an editorial in Journal of Experimental Psychology (12). Reliability refers to the likelihood of replicating the results relative to the null hy-
pothesis, given the amount of variability present in the data, whereas significance implies a conceptual importance that may or may not be present
with reliable data.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


324 KATHERINE VERDOLINI, DAVID G. DRUKER, PHYLLIS M. PALMER, AND HANI SAMAWI

T A B L E 3. Average EGG closed quotients (CQs) as a fimction of voice O'pe, for /iP

Voice type group Pressed Resonant Normal Breathy Average

Healthy 0.572 0.555 0.530 0.493 0.538


(0.044) (0.079) (0.060) (0.123)
Nodules 0.574 0.445 0.446 0.420 0.47 I
(0.052) (0.095) (0.068) (0.034)
Average 0.573 0.500 0.488 0.457 0.505

*Standard deviations indicated in parentheses.

EGG Closed Quotient MANOVA failed to indicate any group × voice in-
Average CQs for/i/are shown in Table 3. This table teraction (Wilks' Lambda F(9.92.63 ) = 0.66, p < .75),
shows that for both subject groups, subjects produced and the subsequent Repeated Measures ANOVA
the greatest CQs for pressed voice, with smaller CQs again confirmed a reliable main effect of both group
for resonant and normal voice, and the smallest CQs (Ftl.139) = 22.64, p < .0001 ) and voice type (Ft3.139) =
for breathy voice. On average, CQs for subjects with 10.43, p < .0001). As for/i/, post-hoc Tukey paired
nodules were lower than CQs for laryngeally healthy comparisons indicated reliable differences between
subjects. This result is attributable to poorer transla- CQs for pressed and all other voice types, but also a
ryngeal electrical conductivity in subjects with nod- distinction between breathy and resonant voice.
ules due to persisting glottal spaces surrounding le- EGG results f o r / u / were inspected for two ran-
sions during phonation. A MANOVA treating each domly selected subjects, one with a healthy larynx
subject as a separate outcome, and with group and one with nodules. The pattern of results was en-
(healthy larynx vs. nodules) as a between-subjects tirely consistent with those reported f o r / i / and /a/
factor and voice type (pressed, resonant, normal, and (data not shown).
breathy) as a within-subjects factor, was conducted to
assess the interaction of group and voice type. An in- DISCUSSION
teraction was not confirmed by this analysis (Wilks'
Lambda F(9,2.582) = 3 . 5 8 , p < . 19). Thus, a subsequent Question 1
Repeated Measures ANOVA was performed with on- The primary question in this study was whether
ly group and voice type in the model. In this test, re- resonant voice would be produced with a similar la-
liable main effects were obtained for both group ryngeal configuration by subjects with nodules and
(Ft l.114) = 20.72, p < .0001) and voice type (FI3.114) = subjects with healthy larynges. Specifically, the
12.75, p < .0001). Thus, although CQs were con- question was whether resonant voice would be pro-
finned as generally smaller for subjects with nodules, duced with a barely abducted or barely adducted la-
the same pattern of CQs was obtained for both subject ryngeal configuration in both groups. The data indi-
groups across the voice types. Post hoc Tukey paired cated that performer subjects with nodules, as well as
comparisons confirmed reliable differences between performer subjects with healthy larynges, produced
the CQs for pressed versus all other voice types (p < resonant voice with the anticipated barely abducted
.05). However, none of the other comparisons indicat- or barely adducted laryngeal posturing during rigid
ed reliable differences for/i/. videoendoscopy. For both subject groups, this pos-
A similar pattern of results was obtained for/a/. ture was reliably distinguished from byperadducted
The average findings for this vowel are shown in ones used in pressed voice, and from hypoadducted
Table 4. Again, the greatest CQs were produced for ones used in breathy voice. Adduction for resonant
pressed voice, smaller CQs were produced for reso- voice was no different from adduction for normal
nant and normal voice, and the smallest CQs were voice (see [ 1O] for similar results for pressed, normal,
produced for breathy voice. As for /i/, an initial and breathy voice in healthy singers).
2This test uses adjusted degrees of freedom in order to evaluate the results against the F-distribution.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 12, No. 3, 1998


LARYNGEAL ADDUCTION IN RESONANT VOICE 325

T A B L E 4. Average EGG closed quotients (CQs) as a fimction of voice t3,pe, f o r /c~

Voice type group Pressed Resonant Normal Breathy Average

Healthy 0.578 0.547 0.531 0.453 0.527


(0.063) (0.065) (0.075) (0.095)
Nodules 0.529 0.467 0.464 0.435 0.474
(0.054) (0.073) (0.046) (0.066)
Average 0.554 0.507 0.498 0.444 0.501

+Standard deviations indicated in parentheses.

Before discussing the results, there are two impor- Returning to the main points of interest, the poten-
tant caveats. First, as noted, the present findings were tial clinical relevance of the present findings was in-
obtained during rigid endoscopy. It is possible that dicated in the Introduction. The barely abducted, or
the endoscopic procedure affected laryngeal configu- barely adducted, laryngeal configuration used by both
rations. This possibility should be explored further. healthy subjects and subjects in this study to produce
In the meantime, we can say that the results obtained resonant voice may be clinically pertinent. The rea-
were based on valid tokens of the voice types exam- son is that these configurations may subsume those
ined. Thus, the configurations described in this study used to produce maximum "vocal economy" Ev-max,
are certainly included in the range of configurations under constant subglottic pressure and frequency con-
that may be used for the voice types. ditions. Theoretically, Ev-max involves an optimal
A second caveat is that the findings were obtained tradeoff between voice output intensity (maximized)
in trained voice users. The generalizability of the re- and intraglottal impact stress (minimized), which,
sults to other populations is not known and should be computationally, should be produced with barely ab-
explored in further studies. In the meantime, two ob- ducted vocal folds ( 1). The barely adducted laryngeal
servations suggest that the findings might extend to configuration used for resonant voice appears to in-
other populations. First, in a previous study evaluat- clude the range needed for maximum vocal economy.
ing pressed, normal, and breathy voice types, direct Ev-max may be a therapeutic objective for many
adduction measures showed a similar pattern of re- voice patients. For virtually all patients, output inten-
sults as reported here (10). Although those findings sity needs to be strong enough for functional purpos-
were also based on singers, the findings correlated es. At the same time, intraglottal impact stress should
well with abduction quotients (Qas; half of the be limited because of its probable causal relation to
prephonatory distance between the vocal folds divid- laryngeal trauma (13). The maximum ratio of output-
ed by vocal fold vibrational amplitudes) for non- to-impact stress or Ev-max represents an optimal re-
singer/actor subjects producing the same voice types. lation between these parameters. Patients with hy-
Thus, there is some evidence that adduction-based peradducted disorders including nodules might be
measures for pressed, normal, and breathy voice may trained to reduce adduction towards the configuration
be similar for singer/actor and nonsinger/actor sub- producing Ev-max by using resonant voice, with po-
jects. Second, and related to the first point, our clini- tential therapeutic benefit. The notion that Ev-max,
cal experience suggests that also resonant voice is and resonant voice, may be therapeutic found some
usually produced with similar laryngeal configura- support in preliminary data indicating a benefit from
tions by singers/actors and by others, given training resonant voice training in patients with nodules (6).
in this voice type. Thus, although generalization of On the other hand, patients with hypoadducted disor-
the present results to nonsinger/actor groups cannot ders, including bowing and paralysis, might benefit
be assumed, there are some reasons to think that the from increased adduction towards Ev-max by attempt-
results might be generalizable. ing to produce resonant voice. No data are yet avail-

Jounaal of Voice, Vol. 12. No. 3, 1998


326 KATHERINE VERDOLINI, DAVID G. DRUKER, PHYLLIS M. PALMER, AND HANI SAMAWI

able regarding the potential therapeutic benefits of Question 2


this approach for glottic insufficiencies. However, A second question was whether the EGG CQ might
our clinical experiences indicate that this may be a be used to noninvasively distinguish resonant voice,
reasonable idea to pursue with formal studies. and thus a barely abducted or barely adducted laryn-
The barely ab/adducted laryngeal configuration geal configuration. The answer to this question was
used for resonant voice may have further benefits, not entirely straightforward. For both/i/and/aJ, av-
beyond the potential for producing Ev-max. Within erage CQs were reliably smaller for resonant as com-
this range of glottic configurations, the subglottic pres- pared with pressed voice. However, average CQs for
sures required for vocal fold oscillation are smaller resonant voice were not reliably different from those
than for any other configurations, assuming constant for breathy voice for/i/, although they were for/a/.
Thus, the CQ appeared somewhat less sensitive than
f0 and constant tissue viscosity conditions. This pre-
direct adduction measures for distinguishing reso-
diction is based on prior computational as well as
nant voice, and thus for identifying a barely abduct-
empirical work indicating a direct reliance of phona-
ed or barely adducted laryngeal configuration.
tion threshold pressure on glottal width (4,14).
There is further concern that CQs were quantita-
Two important questions remain. First, since reso- tively different for healthy subjects as compared with
nant and normal voice appear to be produced with a subjects with nodules. For subjects with nodules, CQs
similar laryngeal configuration, barely abducted or were reliably smaller. This result is likely due to in-
barely adducted, what is the potential therapeutic creased resistance to electrical current passage across
utility of training resonant voice? Why not simply air spaces surrounding lesions. The implication is
train normal voice? Second, if the amount of adduc- that a specific range of CQs cannot be identified for
tion is similar for resonant and for normal voice, what resonant voice across all subjects. The range for every
is the difference between these voice types? The an- subject should be individually calibrated. Such a pro-
swers to these questions are interrelated. The poten- cedure is conceptually possible. However, it would
tial therapeutic value of resonant over normal voice be clinically cumbersome. It would be particularly
partly depends on how "normal voice" is produced. cumbersome because recalibration would be literally
For many subjects, particularly clinically hyperad- required for every training session: As laryngeal con-
ducted or hypoadducted subjects, normal voice may ditions change across time, for example with de-
not be produced with a barely ab/adducted laryngeal creased or increased nodule size, the CQs correspond-
configuration without prior training. Clinical subjects ing to resonant voice would be expected to change.
in our study did use this configuration, perhaps as the At this point, such procedures do not seem practical
result of training. For others, the institution of reso- for most clinical situations.
nant voice might be therapeutic as compared with
hyper- or hypoadducted presenting conditions. Re- SUMMARY
garding the difference between resonant and normal
In summary, in the present study, singer and actor
voice, it is likely that resonant voice is often pro-
subjects with and without nodules consistently pro-
duced with subtle supraglottic adjustments not exam- duced resonant voice with a barely abducted or bare-
ined in this study to enhance the glottal spectrum and ly adducted laryngeal configuration. This configura-
thus increase oral vibratory sensations. Such adjust- tion may subsume a configuration with therapeutic
ments are in fact frequently a part of resonant voice value for patients with both hyper- as well as hy-
training (5), and can boost oral output by a maximum poadducted conditions. The generalizability of the
of about 6 dB with no change in subglottic pressure results should be assessed relative to nonsinger/actor
or glottal resistance (15). Thus, resonant voice train- populations. The EGG CQ reliably distinguished res-
ing might accomplish output advantages not present onant from pressed voice, but did not consistently
in normal voice without additional glottal loading. distinguish resonant from other voice types evaluat-

Journal of Voice, VoL 12, No. 3, 1998


LARYNGEAL ADDUCTION IN RESONANT VOICE 327

ed (normal and breathy). Because of this finding, and 5. Lessac A. The use and training of the human voice: a prac-
because of distinct differences in CQs across healthy tical approach to speech and voice dynamics. Mountain
subjects and subjects with nodules, the results do not View, Calif." Mayfield Publishing; 1967.
6. Verdolini-Marston K, Burke MD, Lessac A, Glaze L, Cald-
strongly support the use of the CQ as a noninvasive
well E. A preliminary study on two methods of treatment for
indicator of resonant voice or laryngeal configuration laryngeal nodules. J Voice 1995;9:74--85.
in most clinical situations unless ongoing within-sub- 7. Peterson KL, Verdolini-Marston K, Barkmeier JM, Hoffman
jects calibration procedures are used. HT. Comparison of aerodynamic and electroglottographic
parameters in evaluating Clinically relevant voicing patterns.
Acknowledgments: The study was supported by Grant Ann Otol Rhinol La~. ngol 1994; 103:335-46.
No. P60 DC00976 and from the National Institute on 8. Childers DG, Hicks DM, Moore GP, AlaskaYA. A model for
Deafness K08 DC00139 and Other Communication Dis- vocal fold vibratory motion, contact area, and the elec-
orders. The authors acknowledge Dr. Linnea Peterson for troglottogram. J Acoust Soc Am 1986;80:1309-20.
her assistance with the experiment and Dr. Kenneth Moll 9. Rothenberg M, Mahshie JJ. Monitoring vocal fold abduction
for his comments on an earlier version of the paper. through vocal fold contact area. J Speech Heal" Res 1988;
31:338-51.
10. Scherer R, Vail V. Measures of laryngeal adduction. J Acoust
REFERENCES Soc Am 1988;84(Suppl 1):58 I(A).
I I. SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT: Users guide, Version 6 4th ed.
I. Verdolini K, Titze IR. The application of laboratory formulas Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc. 1989;1:873-4.
to clinical voice management. Am J Speech-Lang Pathol 12. Editorial. Exp Psych:General 1990;119:3-4.
1995 ;4:62-9. 13. Jiang JJ, Titze IR. Measurement of vocal fold intraglonal
2. Berry DA, Verdolini K, Chan RW, Titze IR. Indications of an
pressure and impact stress. J Voice 1994;8:145-56.
optimum glottal width in vocal production. J Speech-Lang-
14. Titze IR. The physics of small-amplitude oscillation of the
Hear Res (in review).
vocal fold. J Acoust Soc Am 1988;83:15361-52.
3. Hess MM, Verdolini K, Bierhals W, Mansmann U, Gross M.
Endolaryngeal contact pressures, J Voice 1998;12:50--67. 15. Titze IR. Principles of voice production. Englewood Cliffs,
4. Gauffin J, Sundberg J. Spectral correlates of glottal voice NJ: Prentice Hall, 1994.
source waveform characteristics. Speech Hear Res 1989;
32:556--650.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 12, No. 3. 1998

You might also like