You are on page 1of 4

Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 1101–1104

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Shorter communication

Words may not be enough! No increased emotional Stroop effect in obsessive–


compulsive disorder
Steffen Moritz*, Benny-Kristin Fischer, Birgit Hottenrott, Michael Kellner, Susanne Fricke, Sarah Randjbar,
Lena Jelinek
University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Martinistrasse 52, 20246 Hamburg, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Conflicting evidence has been obtained whether or not patients diagnosed with obsessive–compulsive
Received 6 February 2008 disorder (OCD) share an attentional bias towards disorder-related stimuli. Some of these inconsistencies
Received in revised form 27 April 2008 can be accounted for by suboptimal stimuli selection. In consideration of the heterogeneity of OCD, we
Accepted 1 May 2008
investigated Stroop interference effects for two classes of OCD items (i.e., washing and checking) in 23
OCD patients and 23 healthy controls. In order to cover prevalent OCD concerns, item compilation was
Keywords: based on experts’ appraisals. Patients neither displayed greater immediate as well as delayed Stroop
Obsessive–compulsive disorder
interference nor any bias for OCD and subtype-congruent stimuli. On the contrary, for washing-related
Stroop
Washer
items, OCD patients, and here especially washers, displayed facilitation relative to healthy controls. Al-
Checker though the present study at first sight refutes the notion of an attentional bias in OCD in contrast to other
Attentional bias anxiety disorders, several potential moderators need to be considered before this account is ultimately
Interference dismissed. In particular, an attentional bias may only be elicited using visual material that is more
attention-grabbing than verbal stimuli. Finally, blockwise instead of random item administration and
greater consideration of individual relevance may be crucial prerequisites for the effect to emerge.
Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction eliciting concern or arousal largely differ even among patients who
share the same subtype (i.e., washing, checking, ordering, hoard-
There is equivocal evidence whether or not patients with ing). While one checker may only be concerned when leaving his
obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) share an attentional bias for house (i.e., worry that the door has not been properly locked),
concern-related material, a response pattern consistently reported another patient may only worry while driving (i.e., fear to have hit
for patients with other anxiety disorders and depression (Mathews someone over). In contrast, in phobias, for example spider phobia,
& MacLeod, 2005; Williams, Meathews, & MacLeod, 1996). While the set of fear-eliciting stimuli is more homogeneous. Moreover,
OCD is classified among the anxiety disorders, the absence of such not all prior relevant studies have looked at OCD-relevant stimuli
a bias in many studies (Kampman, Keijsers, Verbraak, Naring, & but some administered general anxiety, panic, or depression
Hoogduin, 2002; Kyrios & Iob, 1998; McNally, Riemann, Louro, stimuli (McNally et al., 1992, 1994; McNeil et al., 1999; Moritz,
Lukach, & Kim, 1992; McNeil, Tucker, Miranda, Lewin, & Nordgren, Jacobsen et al., 2004). As many patients do not experience fear/
1999; Moritz, Jacobsen et al., 2004; Moritz & von Muhlenen, 2005; panic but disgust, tension, ‘‘not just right’’ experiences (Coles,
Moritz & von Mühlenen, 2008; Unoki, Kasuga, Matsushima, & Ohta, Heimberg, Frost, & Steketee, 2005) or an unspecified urge pre-
1999: supraliminal presentation) but not all studies (Foa, Ilai, ceding compulsions, such material may not be best suited to cap-
McCarthy, Shoyer, & Murdock, 1993; Lavy, van Oppen, & van den ture an attentional bias.
Hout, 1994; Unoki et al., 1999: subliminal presentation) has been A prominent test to elicit an attentional bias is the emotional
interpreted (Summerfeldt & Endler, 1998) as a further evidence to Stroop paradigm (MacLeod,1991). Whereas in a conventional Stroop
segregate OCD from the anxiety disorder spectrum besides differ- task (Stroop, 1935), the print-colour of an incongruently typed col-
ences regarding phenomenology and treatment. our word has to be named resulting in strong interference (i.e.,
Alternatively, problems to detect an attentional bias in patients reading is more automatized than the target response colour nam-
with OCD may be rooted in the idiosyncratic nature of OCD: Stimuli ing), the emotional Stroop uses disorder-related words. As person-
ally or disorder-relevant items are attention-grabbing, the subject’s
attention is diverted from the primary task towards the distractor,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ49 40 42803 6565; fax: þ49 40 42803 7566. thus delaying the response. As an alternative account, rumination or
E-mail address: moritz@uke.uni-hamburg.de (S. Moritz). a lowered threshold for disorder-relevant material has been

0005-7967/$ – see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.005
1102 S. Moritz et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 1101–1104

postulated to explain this response pattern in mental disorders. Experimental task


Response latencies in the experimental condition are compared to
a control condition (neutral stimuli, colour bars, or meaningless The emotional Stroop task was constructed using SuperLabÒ
strings of characters). Usually, the emotional Stroop effect is weaker software and was individually presented via a Macintosh computer.
than interference effects elicited by colour words. Following a short practice task with six items to familiarize subjects
For the present study, we compared OCD and healthy subjects with the task requirements, 15 stimuli from each of the nine dif-
on an emotional variant of the Stroop task involving OCD-relevant ferent conditions were presented in random order.
stimuli. We looked separately at items relevant for the two most Words for the non-OCD conditions were similar to stimuli used
prevalent OCD subtypes (checking and washing). This was done, as in a prior experiment (Moritz, Jacobsen et al., 2004) and compiled
an attentional bias may not operate for all subtypes alike and to following consensus ratings by experienced clinicians. The OCD
take into account the heterogeneous phenomenology of OCD (e.g., items were newly collected after an iterative reduction process
the word ‘‘dirt’’ is likely more arousing for washers than checkers). guided by ratings from six clinical psychologists or psychiatrists
Stimuli were chosen via expert ratings to cover the most prevalent with extensive experience with OCD patients. The final set of words
concerns for each subtype. Apart from subtype other prominent was rated OCD-relevant by the majority of assessors. The conditions
moderators for group differences in OCD apart from subtypes, such were similar according to word length, initial letters (plosive vs.
as severity of OCD as well as comorbid affective symptoms, were non-plosive) and word frequency (all contrasts p > 0.1). The emo-
carefully considered. Finally, in view of recent studies (McKenna & tional word conditions were as follows: OCD-checking relevant
Sharma, 2004; for a discussion see Phaf & Kan, 2007) that have (e.g., lock, accident), OCD-washing relevant (e.g., blood, dirt), anxiety
detected slow but not fast emotional Stroop interference, we have (e.g., anxiety, panic), depression (e.g., weakness, loneliness), positive
looked at reaction times both for immediate (trial N) and sub- (e.g., beauty, success), and paranoia-relevant (e.g., spy, surveillance).
sequent trials (N þ 1). To the best of our knowledge, it has not been There were two non-affective control conditions: neutral (e.g.,
investigated whether interference effects need more time to evolve bag, table) and Stroop interference (e.g., incongruently written
in OCD patients. colour words). Naming of horizontal colour bars served as baseline
condition. As noted, the non-OCD conditions were similar to
Methods a previous experiment with one exception: in the neutral condi-
tion, two formerly presented items that could be considered OCD-
Participants relevant (soap, sink) were substituted for the present experiment.
Colors were evenly distributed across all conditions: red (Ger-
Twenty-three patients meeting DSM-IV criteria for OCD were man: rot), green (grün), yellow (gelb), and blue (blau). Words were
recruited from the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of presented in German language; all participants were German na-
the University Medical Center Hamburg/Germany (gender: 11 tive speakers.
male, 12 female; age: M ¼ 35.57, SD ¼ 10.54; years of formal school Each trial consisted of the following sequence: a small fixation
education: M ¼ 11.04, SD ¼ 1.69). Presence of OCD was confirmed point (300 ms) was first followed by a 200 ms blank trial. Sub-
with the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI, sequently, the target stimulus (font 30, type Geneva) was shown
Sheehan et al., 1998). Any history of psychotic and manic symptoms until the individual triggered the voice-key via microphone. Par-
(grandiose or paranoid ideas, hallucinations of any modality) led to ticipants were instructed to respond as fast as possible and to avoid
exclusion. Most patients were medicated with psychotropic drugs making mistakes. Following each response, the experimenter
(15 patients were prescribed antidepressant drugs, six of these also pressed a button to denote correct (i.e., ‘‘C’’) and incorrect re-
with a neuroleptic agent). Eleven patients had a depressive episode sponses (i.e., ‘‘X’’) which initiated the next trial.
secondary to OCD and eight patients were co-diagnosed with an-
other anxiety disorder. Strategy of data analysis
Twenty-three subjects served as healthy controls who were
recruited via an established subject pool or word-of-mouth (gender: Reaction time latencies were recorded for correct trials only.
12 male, 11 female; age: M ¼ 31.39, SD ¼ 10.66; years of formal Reaction times faster than 300 ms and slower than 4000 ms were
school education: M ¼ 11.74, SD ¼ 1.48). The MINI interview verified excluded from analyses. As dependent variables, we computed
absence of any axis I psychiatric disorder in controls. None of the reaction times from the median response latencies for target words
participants suffered from neurological disorders (e.g., stroke, epi- (N) as well as subsequent words (trial N þ 1), as we were interested
lepsy) including OCD spectrum disorders (e.g., Tourette’s syndrome). if an attentional bias persists over time and/or manifests after
Written informed consent was obtained from all participants a delay due to, for example, rumination. A 2  9 mixed analysis of
prior to baseline assessment. The German version of the Yale- variance (ANOVA) was conducted with the experimental condi-
Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) (Goodman et al., tions as within-subject and Group (healthy, OCD) OCD as between-
1989; German translation by Hand & Büttner-Westphal, 1991) subject factor. Since our hypothesis was undirected, for exploratory
served as an index of OCD severity (total: M ¼ 22.73; SD ¼ 6.61). For purposes we also computed single comparisons.
assessing depression, the Hamilton depression rating scale (HDRS;
17 item version; Hamilton, 1960) was administered blind to neu- Results
rocognitive status (total score: M ¼ 12.18, SD ¼ 7.08). Subscores for
these scales were computed using algorithms derived from factor Sociodemographic background variables and immediate
analytic studies (Moritz et al., 2002; Moritz, Meier, Hand, Schick, & interference
Jahn, 2004). We administered the Hamburg Obsessional Compul-
sive Inventory (HOCI; Klepsch, Zaworka, Hand, Lünenschloss, & Samples did not differ with respect to sociodemographic back-
Jauernig, 1991) to specify OCD subtypes. The HOCI is a self-rating ground variables (age, gender, school education; all p > .1).
instrument that assesses core obsessions (e.g., thoughts of doing In the mixed ANOVA neither the effect of Group, F(1,44) ¼ 0.84,
harm to self/others) and compulsions (e.g., checking) along six p > .3, h2partial ¼ .02, nor the interaction of Group  Condition,
scales. Seven of the patients displayed both checking and washing F(8,352) ¼ 0.78, p > .3, h2partial ¼ .02, surpassed the level of signifi-
symptoms, while each three patients only showed checking or cance. As can be seen in Fig. 1, the significant effect of Condition is
washing symptoms. mainly owing to discrepant reaction times in the control condition
S. Moritz et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 1101–1104 1103

850 group differences were rather small and for the washing-related
healthy words even a counter-intuitive facilitation occurred: here, espe-
reaction times in ms

800
OCD cially washers, displayed less interference!
750 While it is thus tempting to infer that OCD is not associated with
an attentional bias, particularly in view of many studies unable to
700 detect such a cognitive preference in OCD (see introduction) and in
line with claims made by Summerfeldt and Endler (1998), we
650
should not jump to conclusions. First, although we gathered stimuli
600 that deal with the prominent concerns of OCD washers and
checkers, we cannot rule out that for a subgroup of patients sharing
550
idiosyncratic and very isolated worries these items were not at-
g

ng

id

ve

ty

op
ra

tention-grabbing enough thus attenuating group differences. The


in

io

m r
no

na colo
ti
hi

ut

ro
g
ck

ss

xi
si

in
as

ra
ne

St
an
po
e

e counter-intuitive facilitation for washing-related material could be


pa
ch

pr
w

de

owed to greater familiarity in patients with such stimuli acting


Fig. 1. The two-way ANOVA did not yield any group differences regarding condition. against a pop-out effect of the distractor, which may only be found
However, post hoc analyses revealed a slight speeding of OCD patients for washing- for the most fear-evoking stimuli. We would like to point out that
relevant words in the colour bar naming condition but not in the neutral condition
Kyrios and Iob (1998) also found facilitation for OCD and other
relative to controls.
anxiety words in both OCD and healthy subjects. Interestingly, Lavy
et al. (1994) reported greater interference for negatively valenced
colour naming versus the conventional Stroop interference condi- OCD words while for positively valenced OCD words the response
tion, F(8,352) ¼ 28.50, p < .001, h2partial ¼ 0.39. When we subtracted pattern reversed in OCD patients. Nonetheless, future studies may
reaction times in the two OCD and the Stroop interference condi- benefit from greater consideration of individual concerns and may
tion from the neutral word and non-word control condition (colour also employ paradigms that may tease apart the relative contri-
naming), no evidence for greater interference emerged for OCD bution of vigilance and disengagement problems in the emergence
patients even before Bonferroni-correction for multiple compari- of Stroop interference (Amir, Elias, Klumpp, & Przeworski, 2003).
sons. On the contrary, for washing-relevant material, the difference If the attentional bias in OCD is existent, it is apparently more
achieved significance (washing vs. colour bars, p ¼ .03) or trend subtle than in PTSD patients or patients with simple phobias.
level (washing vs. neutral word, p ¼ .09) indicating facilitation in Therefore, a verbal paradigm may not be sufficiently sensitive to
patients. For checking-relevant items (p > .3) and conventional elicit a heightened attentional bias, especially when involving ran-
Stroop interference (p > .1) no such effects emerged. dom item administration precluding prolonged confrontation with
disorder-related items. The worries of OCD patients are mostly
triggered by visual cues or images (e.g., dirt on the table, key lock)
Delayed interference
and a visual paradigm may thus be fairer to put this hypothesis to
test. Indeed, a recent study using neutral, anxiety and OCD pictures
For trials following the critical trials (N þ 1) groups displayed
in the context of a modified inhibition of return (IOR) paradigm was
a similar response pattern: the main effect of Group and the in-
able to show greater distractibility of OCD patients for OCD-relevant
teraction did not produce significant effects (both p > 0.3, both
but not neutral and anxiety material relative to controls (Moritz
h2partial < 0.028). Moreover, none of the post hoc comparisons (e.g.,
et al., submitted for publication). Therefore, while we are safe to
washing condition – neutral) yielded significance (p > 0.3).
assume that any attentional bias in OCD is less pronounced than in
other anxiety disorders, especially PTSD, researchers should con-
Subgroup analysis tinue to pursue this account with other paradigms before ultimately
dismissing it. Moreover, subjective appraisal ratings may help to
According to HOCI scores, the OCD sample was split into determine whether stimuli sufficiently cover patients’ concerns. In
washers and checkers. None of the ANOVA results yielded signifi- line with this, we found that washers but not checkers were slowed
cance except for the washing condition with colour naming as on a primed Stroop task for OCD-relevant targets when the sample
baseline, F(2,41) ¼ 3.34, p < .05. Post hoc comparisons showed that was split in subjects endorsing more than one-third of the items as
washers (M ¼ 48 ms) showed significantly less interference than personally relevant versus those that did not meet this criterion
healthy participants (M ¼ 124 ms). OCD-non-washers performed (Fischer, 2007). So far, stimuli of past research were either rated by
intermediate (M ¼ 92 ms). Finally, we investigated whether a sub- other subjects for relevance (Foa et al., 1993; Lavy et al., 1994; Unoki
group of patients would display strong interference. To our sur- et al., 1999), experts (Moritz, Jacobsen et al., 2004, and present
prise, for washing-relevant material six healthy subjects were study) or not at all. None of these studies performed a subanalysis of
among the 10 participants with the highest interference latencies, items with special personal relevance. Finally, the blockwise ad-
whereas for checking-relevant items both five healthy and OCD ministration of conditions may be more powerful to elicit effects
patients displayed highest interference. (e.g., Lavy et al., 1994) if an attentional bias in OCD needs time to
evolve and/or needs multiple triggers and prolonged exhibition (see
Discussion Phaf & Kan, 2007), although the present analysis of delayed effects in
trial N þ 1 did not provide evidence for this assumption.
The present study revealed no evidence for greater interference
on any of the experimental conditions of an emotional Stroop task
in OCD. Specifically, no retardation was detected for OCD-relevant
References
material – irrespective of subtype. The OCD stimuli were collected
after a thorough iterative reduction process involving experts’ Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in
opinion to assure that central OCD concerns were covered. More- social phobia: facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention
over, special care was devoted to equate conditions for several from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1325–1335.
Coles, M. E., Heimberg, R. G., Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (2005). Not just right
important moderators such as words length and language fre- experiences and obsessive–compulsive features: experimental and self-
quency. Our findings unlikely reflect a lack of statistical power, as monitoring perspectives. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 153–167.
1104 S. Moritz et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 1101–1104

Fischer, B.-K. (2007). Untersuchung kognitiver Einbahnstraßen bei Zwang unter McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., Louro, C. E., Lukach, B. M., & Kim, E. (1992). Cognitive
Verwendung einer semantischen Priming-Aufgabe mit störungsrelevantem processing of emotional information in panic disorder. Behaviour Research
Material [Investigation of cognitive ‘‘one-way streets’’ in obsessive–compulsive Therapy, 30, 143–149.
disorder using a semantic priming-task with disorder-relevant material]. McNeil, D. W., Tucker, P., Miranda, R., Jr., Lewin, M. R., & Nordgren, J. C. (1999).
Master thesis, University of Hamburg, Hamburg. Response to depression and anxiety Stroop stimuli in posttraumatic stress
Foa, E. B., Ilai, D., McCarthy, P. R., Shoyer, B., & Murdock, T. (1993). Information disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and major depressive disorder. Journal
processing in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 512–516.
173–189. Moritz, S., Jacobsen, D., Kloss, M., Fricke, S., Rufer, M., & Hand, I. (2004). Examination
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. of emotional Stroop interference in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour
L., et al. (1989). The Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale. I. Development, Research and Therapy, 42, 671–682.
use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1011. Moritz, S., Kloss, M., Jacobsen, D., Wein, C., Fricke, S., & Hand, I. (2002). Dimensional
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosur- structure of the Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale (Y-BOCS). Psychiatry
gery and Psychiatry, 23, 56–62. Research, 109, 193–199.
Hand, I., & Büttner-Westphal, H. (1991). Die Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale Moritz, S., Meier, B., Hand, I., Schick, M., & Jahn, H. (2004). Dimensional structure of
(Y-BOCS): Ein halbstrukturiertes Interview zur Beurteilung des Schweregrades the Hamilton depression rating scale in patients with obsessive–compulsive
von Denk- und Handlungszwängen. Verhaltenstherapie, 1, 223–225. disorder. Psychiatry Research, 125, 171–180.
Kampman, M., Keijsers, G. P., Verbraak, M. J., Naring, G., & Hoogduin, C. A. (2002). Moritz, S., Mühlenen, A. v., Jelinek, L., Randjbar, S., Ruhe, C., Fischer, B.-K., et al.
The emotional Stroop: a comparison of panic disorder patients, obsessive– Evidence for an attentional bias for washing and checking relevant stimuli in
compulsive patients, and normal controls, in two experiments. Journal of obsessive–compulsive disorder, submitted for publication.
Anxiety Disorders, 16, 425–441. Moritz, S., & von Muhlenen, A. (2005). Inhibition of return in patients with ob-
Klepsch, R., Zaworka, W., Hand, I., Lünenschloss, K., & Jauernig, G. (1991). Derivation sessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 117–126.
and validation of the Hamburg obsession/compulsion inventory-short form Moritz, S., & von Mühlenen, A. (2008). Investigation of an attentional bias for fear-related
(HOCI-S): first results. Psychological Assessment, 3, 196–201. material in obsessive–compulsive checkers. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 225–229.
Kyrios, M., & Iob, M. A. (1998). Automatic and strategic processing in obsessive– Phaf, R. H., & Kan, K. J. (2007). The automaticity of emotional Stroop: a meta-
compulsive disorder: attentional bias, cognitive avoidance or more complex analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 184–199.
phenomena? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12, 271–292. Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al.
Lavy, E., van Oppen, P., & van den Hout, M. (1994). Selective processing of emotional (1998). The MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The
information in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview.
32, 243–246. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl. 20), 22–33.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of
review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203. Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Summerfeldt, L. J., & Endler, N. S. (1998). Examining the evidence for anxiety-
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167–195. related cognitive biases in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety
McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals Disorders, 12, 579–598.
that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. Journal of Unoki, K., Kasuga, T., Matsushima, E., & Ohta, K. (1999). Attentional processing of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 382–392. emotional information in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical
McNally, R. J., Amir, N., Louro, C. E., Lukach, B. M., Riemann, B. C., & Calamari, J. E. Neuroscience, 53, 635–642.
(1994). Cognitive processing of idiographic emotional information in panic Williams, J. M. G., Meathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task
disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 119–122. and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.

You might also like