You are on page 1of 19

Personal Relationships, 10 (2003), 475–493. Printed in the United States of America.

Copyright # 2003 IARR. 1350-4126/02

Attachment insecurity, depression, and the


transition to parenthood

JUDITH FEENEY,a RICHARD ALEXANDER,a PATRICIA NOLLER,a AND


LYDIA HOHAUSb
a
Family Centre, School of Psychology, University of Queensland; bSchool of Applied
Psychology, Griffith University

Abstract
In a longitudinal study of adult attachment and depression during the transition to parenthood, 76 couples
completed questionnaires on three occasions: during the second trimester of pregnancy, and six weeks and six
months postbirth. On the first and second occasions, the couples were also interviewed about their experiences of
pregnancy and parenthood, respectively. Measures were also completed at similar time intervals by a comparison
group of 74 childless couples. Attachment security was assessed in terms of the dimensions of discomfort with
closeness and relationship anxiety. Relationship anxiety was less stable for transition wives than for other
participants. Relationship anxiety also predicted increases in new mothers’ depressive symptoms, after control-
ling for a broad range of other risk factors. However, the association between relationship anxiety and maternal
depression was moderated by husbands’ caregiving style. Maternal depression was linked to increases in hus-
bands’ and wives’ attachment insecurity and marital dissatisfaction. Results are discussed in terms of the impact
of depression and negative working models of attachment on couple interaction.

The transition to parenthood is a develop- not apply to all couples (Belsky & Rovine,
mental phase marked by significant per- 1990; Terry, McHugh, & Noller, 1991).
sonal, familial, and social change (Levy-Shiff, In terms of individual well-being, mater-
1994). Given the diverse demands asso- nal depression has emerged as an important
ciated with new parenthood, it is not sur- issue for researchers and clinicians. Studies
prising that studies have demonstrated suggest that 8% to 15% of new mothers are
changes in couple relationships and individ- affected by postnatal depression and many
ual well-being across the transition period. others experience depressive symptoms that
In terms of couple relationships, research are milder or less enduring in nature
indicates that parenthood often results in a (O’Hara, 1997). Further, depressed mood
more traditional division of labor, together is usually accompanied by other problems
with a lessening of shared leisure activities, such as extreme fatigue, feelings of guilt,
joint decision-making, and general compan- and disturbances of sleep and appetite. Col-
ionship (e.g., Huston & Vangelisti, 1995; lectively, these problems have serious impli-
Sanchez & Thomson, 1997). Further, there cations for women’s well-being, for couple
is substantial evidence that, on average, relationships, and for infant care (Murray
relationship satisfaction declines across the & Cooper, 1997).
transition period, although this decline does
Maternal depression
Correspondence should be addressed to Judith Feeney, Researchers have examined a wide range of
School of Psychology, University of Queensland,
Queensland 4072, Australia; e-mail:j.feeney@psy.uq. risk factors for postpartum depression,
edu.au. including early family-of-origin experiences,

475
476 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

personal adjustment difficulties (especially romantic love as an attachment process,


prior depression), hormonal and obstetric research has demonstrated the effects of
factors, infant health and temperament, attachment security on relationship quality
stressful life events, aspects of the couple and stability (see Feeney, 1999, for a review).
relationship, and social support (Beck, Studies have also supported the relevance of
1996). None of these variables has received attachment to adults’ psychological well-
unambiguous support as a risk factor, but being. For example, attachment difficulties
some appear to be more important than have been linked with depression (Carnelley,
others. For example, there has been limited Pietromonaco, & Jaffe, 1994; Dozier,
support for the role of obstetric factors and Stovall, & Albus, 1999; Strahan, 1995). This
family-of-origin experiences, but consider- link seems to be quite robust, given that
able support for the role of personal adjust- attachment difficulties have been variously
ment difficulties, infant temperament, and defined in terms of attachment-related
stressful life events (Da Costa, Larouche, experiences (e.g., early death of a parent),
Dritsa, & Brender, 2000; Gotlib, Whiffen, adults’ state of mind regarding early parental
Wallace, & Mount, 1991; O’Hara, 1997). relationships, and discomfort with closeness
Even stronger evidence exists for the and anxiety over relationships (the key
importance of relationship factors, both dimensions of romantic attachment).
within and beyond the marriage. With Despite abundant evidence that attach-
regard to relationships beyond the mar- ment security predicts individual and couple
riage, postnatal depression is more likely functioning, it is important to note that the
to occur when mothers receive, or perceive, degree of security is not immutable. In fact,
little support from friends and family mem- researchers have noted that measures of
bers (Beck, 1996; O’Hara, 1997). This find- adult attachment show considerable
ing can be understood in terms of the effects instability over time (Baldwin & Fehr,
of social support on mothers’ emotions, 1995). Stability rates increase when unreli-
cognitions, and actual parenting inter- ability of measures is taken into account
actions (Rutter, 1997). In terms of factors (Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1994), and when
within the marriage, postnatal depression items with optimal psychometric properties
has been linked to marital conflict and dis- are used (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000),
satisfaction (Beck, 1996; O’Hara, 1997). In but some instability nevertheless remains.
addition, longitudinal studies have reported Consistent with the concept of working
higher rates of postnatal depression when models of attachment, it seems that attach-
husbands are perceived as uncaring or over- ment security can be influenced by important
controlling (Boyce, Hickie, & Parker, 1991; relational events, such as the development
Matthey, Barnett, Ungerer, & Waters, and the dissolution of couple bonds
2000). The link between marital quality (Feeney & Noller, 1992; Kirkpatrick &
and maternal depression is likely to be com- Hazan, 1994).
plex, however, and qualitative data suggest Also consistent with attachment theory
reciprocal relations between maternal (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980), recent studies
depression and the state of the couple rela- suggest that the effects of adult attachment
tionship (Mauthner, 1998). Given the role style (or attachment dimensions) are more
of relationship factors in postnatal depres- pronounced under stressful conditions.
sion, it is important to consider the rele- Much of this research has focused on con-
vance of attachment insecurity. ditions that challenge the attachment bond,
such as major relationship conflict (Feeney,
1998; Rholes, Simpson, & Stevens, 1998).
Attachment, caregiving, and depression
Results of these studies support Mikulincer
during the transition to parenthood
and Florian’s (1998) claim that attachment
Since 1987, when Hazan and Shaver security is a resource that helps individuals
reported their groundbreaking studies of cope more effectively with challenging
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 477

situations. These researchers present addi- tion to parenthood: Women still tend to be
tional evidence for this claim, including a the primary caregivers of young children,
study of first-time mothers and a matched but partners can play a key role in meeting
sample of childless married women. In this their needs for care and reassurance. Hence,
study, insecure attachment was associated husbands who provide effective spousal
with psychological distress, but only among care may buffer the potentially negative
the new mothers. This finding supports the effects of wives’ insecurity.
widely held view, noted earlier, that new As noted already, studies of adult attach-
parenthood is a very challenging time. ment have linked insecurity to depressive
Unfortunately, however, the cross-sectional symptoms, and to less constructive
nature of Mikulincer and Florian’s data responses to stressful situations. To our
limits their interpretability. knowledge, the only empirical data linking
Other researchers have recently argued attachment insecurity and postnatal depres-
that adult attachment theory is uniquely sion are those of Meredith and Noller (in
suited to the study of the transition to par- press). In this study, mothers identified as
enthood. New parenthood is a time when suffering from postnatal depression rated
attachment issues should be particularly themselves higher on preoccupied attach-
salient, as the couple bond must accommo- ment than other mothers. Although this
date the presence of a new and highly finding suggests that attachment insecurity
dependent individual (Alexander, Feeney, may be a risk factor for postnatal depres-
Hohaus, & Noller, 2001; Rholes, Simpson, sion, the study had several limitations: Only
Campbell, & Grich, 2001). For some mothers (and not their partners) were stud-
spouses, attachment-related concerns (dis- ied, no comparison group of childless
comfort with closeness, relationship anx- women was included, and attachment and
iety) may be exacerbated by the demands adjustment (depression) were again meas-
of parenthood; for others, the experience ured concurrently. The concurrent meas-
of new parenthood (including the reactions urement is particularly problematic, as it
of the partner) may strengthen a sense of precludes establishing causal relations
security. Hence, like relationship develop- among these variables.
ment and dissolution, the transition to par-
enthood may be a relatively powerful
The present study: Research questions and
influence on attachment security.
hypotheses
Further, initial levels of attachment
security may predict spouses’ adjustment The present study focuses on couple rela-
to the transition to parenthood. In a longi- tionships across the transition to parent-
tudinal study of new parenthood, Rholes hood. Like Rholes et al. (2001), we were
et al. (2001) focused on the implications interested in assessing the effects of adult
of wives’ anxious-ambivalence. These partners’ attachment security on adjust-
researchers found that highly ambivalent ment to parenthood. However, our focus
women reported declines in marital satisfac- was on depressive symptoms, rather than
tion across the transition period, particu- marital satisfaction. As well as drawing
larly if they perceived their husbands as together two significant research areas
providing insufficient help and support pre- (adult attachment and maternal depres-
natally. The moderating effect of husbands’ sion), our study has other important
provision of help and support is consistent strengths. First, it uses a major, naturally
with the argument that attachment and care- occurring event to assess attachment-
giving are core, interrelated features of related issues. Second, it includes a sample
couple bonds (Hazan & Shaver, 1994; of childless couples; hence, we are able to
Shaver, Hazan, & Bradshaw, 1988). Further, compare the stability of attachment and the
both attachment and spousal caregiving predictive relations between attachment
are of prime importance during the transi- and depression in couples facing first-time
478 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

parenthood and those not facing a common important predictor of adjustment during
stressful event. Third, couples were assessed this period. To examine this question, we
on three occasions, allowing tests of causal assessed a broad range of risk factors for the
relations between attachment, depressive onset of depressive symptoms. Specifically,
symptoms, and later outcomes. Finally, we included measures of marital satisfaction,
questionnaire and interview data were gath- social support, psychological adjustment
ered from both husbands and wives, provid- (prenatal depression and general anxiety),
ing a comprehensive assessment of risk factors related to pregnancy and birth, and
factors for the onset of depressive symp- infant temperament. Our aim was to examine
toms. Together, these features allow a these variables as controls, to determine the
strong test of theory-based predictions rele- unique ability of attachment to predict
vant to three research questions. changes in maternal depression. Our expect-
ation is stated in the following hypothesis.
Stability of attachment. First, we were
interested in the stability of attachment. As H2a: Relationship anxiety will predict
noted earlier, attachment-related concerns increased depressive symptoms
may be either alleviated or exacerbated by among new mothers, after control-
the demands of parenthood, depending on ling for other risk factors, such as
specific experiences. Hence, attachment marital dissatisfaction.
security may be less stable among first-time
parents than among childless spouses. However, husbands who provide effec-
Further, the personal and relational changes tive spousal care may buffer the effects of
that accompany parenthood are generally wives’ insecurity. Hence, we assessed theo-
more marked for women than for men. We retically important dimensions of spousal
therefore made the following hypothesis. caregiving (sensitivity, proximity, coopera-
tion, and compulsive caregiving), and
H1: Attachment dimensions will be less formed another hypothesis.
stable over the course of the study
for wives in the transition group H2b: The link between relationship
than for other spouses. anxiety and depressive symptoms
will be stronger for wives whose
Note that we did not expect parenthood husbands reported providing less
to produce change in mean levels of secur- beneficial care.
ity: Relational events that are primarily
negative (e.g., breakups) increase insecur- Given previous research linking attach-
ity, but events such as the formation of ment insecurity and depression in general
new relationships predict instability of samples, no hypothesis was formulated
attachment, rather than change in a parti- concerning the relative strength of this asso-
cular direction (Feeney & Noller, 1992; ciation among comparison and transition
Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994). groups; however, this research question
was addressed. Similarly, for completeness,
Predicting depression. Second, we were we assessed whether husbands’ beneficial
interested in the role of attachment insecur- caregiving might buffer the effects of wives’
ity in predicting depressive symptoms insecurity within comparison couples.
among new mothers. Spouses who feel
unworthy of love and who lack confidence Implications of depression for later
in their partner’s commitment are likely to functioning. Third, we examined the
find parenthood particularly stressful, and implications of new mothers’ depression
to have difficulty renegotiating the couple for later individual and couple functioning.
relationship. Hence, the relationship anx- Previous researchers have noted the high
iety dimension of attachment may be an levels of interdependence that characterize
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 479

marital bonds (Berscheid, 1998), and the who had not had any children before; these
likelihood of reciprocal relations between conditions were imposed to enhance the com-
maternal depression and the state of the cou- parability of the groups and to ensure that
ple relationship (Mauthner, 1998). Further, transition couples were new to the experiences
given that the transition to parenthood is a of pregnancy and parenthood.
complex and ongoing process, variables Transition couples were recruited during
such as attachment insecurity may be both the second trimester of pregnancy (14th to
cause and consequence of adjustment 26th weeks). These couples were contacted
difficulties. Hence, it is crucial to examine through radio interviews and media releases
the impact of depression on the later attach- in local newspapers and magazines, by
ment security of both spouses, as well as on approaching relevant health services (e.g.,
overall evaluations of the marriage. women’s hospitals, antenatal clinics), and
through the psychology subject pool at the
H3: Maternal depression will be asso- University of Queensland. At the beginning
ciated with increases in attachment of the study, the transition group consisted
insecurity and relationship dissatis- of 107 couples. The comparison group con-
faction across the transition period sisted of couples who reported that they
for both husbands and wives. were not planning to have children in the
near future. These couples were contacted
Again, no hypothesis was formulated in similar ways to the transition couples,
concerning the relative effects of depression except that health services were not used as
in the comparison and transition groups, a recruitment source. This group contained
but for completeness this question was also 100 couples when the study began.
addressed. Both transition and comparison groups
varied widely in terms of age, length of mar-
riage, religiosity, education, and occupation
Method
(see Table 1). Almost all couples were of
Anglo-European background, with English
Participants
as their first language. The comparability of
Two groups of married couples were recruited the transition and comparison groups,
for the study. Both groups were restricted to together with issues concerning attrition,
couples who were in their first marriages and are addressed in the Procedure section.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of transition and comparison wives and husbands

Wives Husbands
Transition Comparison Transition Comparison
(n ¼ 76) (n ¼ 74) (n ¼ 76) (n ¼ 74)
Age (in years) 29.0 (4.7) 28.5 (5.1) 31.6 (6.1) 30.5 (5.9)
Length of marriage 43.3 (36.8) 41.9 (35.2) 43.3 (36.8) 41.9 (35.2)
(in months)
Religiosity 1.8 (0.7) 1.7 (0.7) 1.7 (0.7) 1.6 (0.7)
(from 1 ¼ not at all, to 3 ¼ very)
Percentage with a 75.0% 77.1% 68.8% 74.0%
postschooling qualification
Percentage holding 57.1% 60.0% 50.1% 62.4%
professional/managerial positions

Note. The table shows mean scores and standard deviations (in parentheses) for continuous variables, and
percentages for categorical variables.
480 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

Questionnaire measures involving the tendency to become over-


involved in the partner’s problems (e.g., ‘‘I
Attachment security. This construct was
create problems by taking on my partner’s
assessed using the Attachment Style Question-
troubles as if they were my own’’). Item
naire (ASQ; Feeney, Noller, & Hanrahan,
response format is from 1 ¼ not at all like
1994). Although the ASQ can yield scores
me to 6 ¼ completely like me. Although both
on five attachment scales, the two major
spouses completed these scales, the focus in
factors are discomfort with closeness
this paper is on the link between husbands’
and relationship anxiety (Alexander et al.,
caregiving style and wives’ depression.
2001). Discomfort with closeness (16 items)
Alpha coefficients for husbands’ data
measures the tendency to be uncomfortable
ranged from .72 (compulsive care) to .88
with high levels of intimacy, and to have
(sensitivity).
difficulty in depending on relationship
partners. A sample item is ‘‘I find it rela-
tively easy to get close to other people’’ Marital satisfaction. Satisfaction with
(reverse scored). Relationship anxiety (13 the marriage was assessed using items from
items) measures concerns about whether Snyder’s Marital Satisfaction Inventory
partners’ feelings of love and commitment (1979). Given the length of the original
are deep and lasting, together with fears of inventory (280 items), it was not feasible to
being rejected or abandoned (e.g., ‘‘I worry use all the items in the present multiple-
that others won’t care about me as much as measure study. Hence, we used 35 items
I care about them’’). The response range from scales that have proved particularly
was from 1 ¼ totally disagree to 6 ¼ totally useful in distinguishing between happy and
agree. The mean scores, standard devi- unhappy spouses (Snyder, 1979): global dis-
ations, and alpha coefficients for these tress (10 items; e.g., ‘‘My marriage has been
scales, broken down by group, gender, disappointing in several ways’’), affective
and time, appear in Table 2 (this table pre- communication (8 items; e.g., ‘‘There is a
sents descriptive statistics for focal vari- great deal of love and affection expressed in
ables assessed at all three times). our marriage’’), problem-solving communi-
cation (9 items; e.g., ‘‘Even when angry with
me, my spouse is able to appreciate my
Spousal caregiving. Ways of responding
viewpoints’’), and time together (8 items;
to the spouse’s needs for care and support
e.g., ‘‘My spouse seems to enjoy just being
were assessed using four 8-item scales devel-
with me’’). Items were selected on the basis
oped by Kunce and Shaver (1994): sensitiv-
of their item-total correlations (Snyder,
ity, proximity, cooperation, and compulsive
1979) and were combined to give a reliable
caregiving. Sensitivity refers to the ability to
overall measure of marital satisfaction (see
notice and interpret the partner’s needs and
Table 2). Items employ a true-false format.
feelings (e.g., ‘‘I am very attentive to my
partner’s nonverbal signals for help and
support’’). The proximity scale assesses Psychological adjustment: Depression and
physical and psychological accessibility to anxiety. Depression and anxiety were
the partner in times of need (e.g., ‘‘When assessed using 7-item scales from the
my partner seems to want or need a hug, Short-Form Depression Anxiety Stress
I’m glad to provide it’’). Cooperation refers scales (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995),
to the ability to support the partner’s own which are designed to tap core clinical
problem-solving efforts, rather than taking symptoms of these emotional states. (The
control (e.g., ‘‘I always respect my partner’s Stress scale was also used as part of a larger
ability to make his/her own decisions study, but is not included in this report.)
and solve his/her own problems’’). Finally, The Depression scale taps symptoms of
the compulsive caregiving scale measures depressed mood, feelings of hopelessness,
a maladaptive approach to caregiving, and difficulty in becoming involved in
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the focal variables at each measurement time

Wives Husbands
Transition Comparison Transition Comparison
T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3
Discomfort with 45.59 44.27 45.76 44.77 43.38 43.28 50.87 49.19 48.94 49.12 48.12 49.50
closeness 10.47 10.90 10.51 11.33 11.81 11.12 11.09 11.11 10.38 9.99 9.95 9.88
.85 .87 .86 .86 .88 .85 .86 .89 .89 .83 .81 .87
Relationship 37.42 36.99 38.10 37.12 36.58 36. 93 37.30 36.36 36.33 35.87 35.80 36.33
anxiety 10.01 9.35 10.80 11.88 12.01 11.64 10.53 11.01 10.73 9.36 9.78 10.73
.84 .82 .86 .85 .89 .86 .85 .88 .82 .83 .85 .85
Marital satisfaction 31.77 30.63 29.73 32.10 31.43 31.56 31.70 31.56 30.39 32.29 31.59 31. 61
4.21 4.21 5.54 4.19 4.56 5.21 4.20 3.99 4.63 3.98 4.60 4.99
.87 .92 .91 .87 .87 .86 .88 .91 .89 .89 .84 .90
Depression 2.41 2.43 2.99 2.54 2.29 2.77 2.25 2.20 2.10 2.24 2.11 2.71
2.73 3.02 3.71 3.38 2.69 3.99 2.94 3.01 3.11 2.99 3.10 3.21
.80 .86 .81 .81 .83 .79 .88 .83 .84 .85 .85 .88
General anxiety 2.23 1.48 1.48 1.88 1.20 1.75 1.36 1.32 1.24 1.19 1.01 1.42
2.59 2.61 1.75 2.99 2.18 3.03 2.45 2.19 2.55 2.33 1.80 2.81
.78 .80 .80 .81 .85 .79 .80 .82 .84 .80 .79 .87

Note. For each measure, the cell entries show the mean score (Row 1), standard deviation (Row 2), and alpha coefficient (Row 3).

481
482 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

everyday activities (e.g., ‘‘I felt that life was to obtain more spontaneous and detailed
meaningless’’). In contrast, the anxiety scale accounts of pregnancy and parenthood
focuses on physiological signs of anxiety, than could be elicited by standardized
such as rapid breathing, and on the general questionnaires. To control for the amount
subjective experience of being anxious and of contact that couples had with members
panicky (e.g., ‘‘I was worried about situa- of the research team, interviews were con-
tions in which I might panic and make a ducted with both transition and comparison
fool of myself’’). Items focus on the indivi- couples (comparison couples were inter-
dual’s experiences over the past week; as a viewed about their relationship history and
result, they are useful for detecting changes recent relationship experiences). However,
in adjustment that may be linked to recent for the purposes of this report, only
events. Items are answered on a 4-point interview data from the transition group
scale, from 0 ¼ did not apply to me at all to will be considered. The interviews were
4 ¼ applied to me very much or most of the tape-recorded and two research assistants
time. The scales showed high internal (advanced graduate students in psychology)
consistency (again, see Table 2). coded the responses, as detailed below.

Experiences of pregnancy. At the begin-


Social support. We assessed individuals’
ning of the study, transition couples were
perceptions of the level of support they
interviewed about four issues: planning and
received from members of their social net-
decision-making in relation to the preg-
work (spouse, friends, and family), using
nancy, reactions to the news of impending
Brown’s (1986) Support Behaviors Inven-
parenthood, experiences of pregnancy itself,
tory. This 33-item measure was originally
and plans for paid and unpaid work. In
developed for a study of social support dur-
coding responses to the first two issues, the
ing pregnancy, although it can be applied
coders derived two variables separately for
more generally. Items include ‘‘Members of
husbands and wives (both partners were
my family help me out when I need it,’’ and
asked to comment on these issues). First,
are answered on a 6-point scale, from
perceptions of the pregnancy as planned or
1 ¼ strongly disagree to 6 ¼ strongly agree.
unplanned were coded on a 3-point scale
The measure showed very high reliability
(1 ¼ planned to become pregnant at this
(a ¼ .92 for wives and .93 for husbands).
time, 2 ¼ planned to become pregnant but
not right now, and 3 ¼ not planned at all).
Infant temperament. Eight items from the Second, reactions to the pregnancy were
Infant Characteristics Questionnaire (Bates, coded on a 4-point scale: 1 ¼ negative (e.g.,
Freeland, & Lounsbury, 1979) were used to ‘‘I was distraught’’), 2 ¼ mixed (e.g., ‘‘I was
assess perceptions of infant temperament. happy in one way but also worried’’),
The eight items form a scale that represents 3 ¼ positive (e.g., ‘‘I was pleased by the
the main dimension of infant temperament: news’’), and 4 ¼ highly positive (e.g., ‘‘I
infant difficulty (Bates et al., 1979). Items are was just ecstatic!’’). Questions concerning
answered on a 7-point Likert scale (the verbal experiences of pregnancy were directed at
anchors vary from item to item), with higher wives only, and responses were coded as
scores indicating greater infant difficulty 1 ¼ no problems, 2 ¼ relatively minor pro-
(a ¼ .89 for wives and .87 for husbands). blems such as nausea and moodiness, and
3 ¼ major problems, including severe anx-
iety or illness. The research assistants were
Interview measures
given a coding scheme developed by the
Couples were also interviewed on two occa- researchers, which provided guidelines for
sions (during the second trimester of preg- all coding decisions. One assistant coded all
nancy, and six weeks post-birth). The of the tapes, and the other coded a random
interviews were used to build rapport, and sample of 25%. Interrater reliability of the
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 483

codes was assessed using Kappa coeffi- the second trimester of pregnancy (Time
cients, which assess the extent of agreement 1), six weeks postbirth (Time 2), and six
above that occurring by chance. All coeffi- months postbirth (Time 3). Time 1 inter-
cients exceeded .79. views took place about one week before
completion of Time 1 questionnaires,
Finally, the questions concerning work-
whereas Time 2 interviews took place
related plans focused primarily on leave
about one week after the completion of
from paid work. Almost all wives
Time 2 questionnaires. This research report
(N ¼ 102) reported that they would take
focuses on attachment security, marital
time off from work after the birth, with
satisfaction, social support, depression,
the most common period of leave being
anxiety, and experiences of pregnancy
one year. In contrast, only 31 husbands
(assessed at Time 1), and infant tempera-
reported that they would seek leave from
ment and experiences of birth (assessed at
work, and none reported seeking more
Time 2), as predictors of wives’ depression
than 8 weeks leave. These responses indicate
(Time 2); the moderating effect of husbands’
that couples generally saw the wives as
caregiving style (Time 1) is also assessed.
adopting the role of primary caregiver, and
Finally, the report focuses on the effects of
support our decision to focus on wives’
maternal depression on final levels of attach-
postbirth depression and husbands’ provi-
ment security and marital satisfaction.
sion of spousal care.

Experiences of birth. Six weeks after the


Comparability of the groups. To assess
babies were born, transition couples were
whether the two groups of couples were
interviewed about their experiences of
comparable at the beginning of the study,
labor and birth, and of new parenthood.
we first compared them on the background
For the purposes of this report, only the
variables of age, length of marriage, reli-
first section of this interview is relevant.
giosity, education (up to 10 years of school-
Questions in this section were designed to
ing, 11–12 years of schooling, technical
assess the relative ease or difficulty of the
college certificate or diploma, university
birth. In particular, we asked couples to
degree), and occupation (professional/man-
report on the length of labor (in hours),
agerial, clerical/technical, unskilled, not in
the extent of pain relief required (from
paid employment). For wives, there were no
1 ¼ only natural methods such as massage,
differences between the groups. For hus-
to 4 ¼ epidural), the extent of surgical inter-
bands, there was a difference in occupational
vention (1 ¼ none, 2 ¼ minor intervention,
status only, w2(3) ¼ 11.65, p < .01; profes-
3 ¼ caesarean section), and the extent of pre-
sional and managerial positions were held
maturity (in weeks). Because responses to
by more comparison than transition hus-
these questions were straightforward, the
bands (see Table 1). This difference probably
research assistants worked together to
reflects patterns of recruitment: Many transi-
achieve consensus coding (independent cod-
tion couples heard about the study through
ing was not used).
the public hospital system, whereas most
comparison couples were recruited through
media outlets or the psychology subject
Procedure
pool. We also compared transition and com-
As noted earlier, both questionnaires and parison couples on questionnaire scores
interviews were used to assess partners’ obtained at Time 1 (attachment security, mar-
adjustment to parenthood. The variables ital satisfaction, depression, anxiety, and
discussed in this report come from a larger social support). Univariate and multivariate
study (Feeney, Hohaus, Noller, & Alexander, analyses of variance showed that the groups
2001), in which most questionnaires could not be differentiated on any of these
were administered on three occasions: in measures.
484 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

Attrition. To minimize attrition, we kept Results


in touch with couples in between the assess-
ment points, sending them greeting cards on Stability of attachment
their wedding anniversaries and when their
To assess the stability of attachment,
babies were born. Where possible, we also
test-retest correlations were calculated for
ensured that the same interviewer visited a
relationship anxiety and discomfort with
couple on each occasion, to build rapport
closeness, across each time interval. These
and maintain a sense of involvement. The
correlations were calculated separately by
overall rate of attrition was similar for both
group and gender, and are shown in
groups. Of the original 107 couples in the
Table 3. Fisher tests for the overall interval
transition group, 92 were interviewed at
of Time 1–Time 3 indicated that the test-
Time 2, and 76 completed the final phase.
retest correlation for relationship anxiety
Similarly, of the 100 comparison couples, 85
was lower for transition wives than for all
were interviewed at Time 2, and 74 completed
other spouses. Specifically, comparisons
the entire study. The main reason that couples
with transition wives yielded the following
did not complete the study was that they
z values and one-tailed p values (adopted
moved without providing a forwarding
because of the directional nature of
address. A few couples reported being too
Hypothesis 1): for comparison wives,
busy to continue their involvement, and
z ¼ 1.87, p ¼ .03; for transition husbands,
some were no longer appropriate for the
z ¼ 2.11, p ¼ .02; and for comparison hus-
study (e.g., some comparison couples became
bands, z ¼ 3.22, p < .001. The test-retest
pregnant during the study).
correlation for discomfort with closeness
To explore whether attrition was random was lower for transition wives than for
or systematic, we compared the couples comparison wives (z ¼ 3.50, p < .001), but
who withdrew and those who remained, on transition wives did not differ from either
all measures completed at Time 1. Of the group of husbands. These results support
five background variables, only education Hypothesis 1, although mainly with regard
level predicted attrition: In both groups, to relationship anxiety. Further, for transi-
those with university education were some- tion wives only, stability of relationship
what less likely to withdraw (w2(3) ¼ 11.14 anxiety was lower for the longer period
and 8.32 for transition and comparison than for the two shorter segments, a finding
groups, respectively, p < .05 in each case). which suggests that this instability is unlikely
None of the questionnaire measures pre- to stem purely from measurement error.
dicted attrition, supporting the validity of It was also considered important to
comparisons to be made between the two examine possible change in mean levels
groups of couples over time. Nevertheless, of security, although as noted earlier, we
data analyses were restricted to the 150 did not expect the transition to parenthood
couples who completed all assessments. to produce such change. A 2  2  3

Table 3. Test-retest correlations for the two attachment dimensions

Wives Husbands
Dimension Transition Comparison Transition Comparison
Relationship anxiety .54a (.74, .63) .72b (.86, .78) .74b (.79, .77) .81b (.78, .80)
Discomfort with closeness .61a (.69, .68) .86b (.86, .89) .70a (.76, .67) .68a (.70, .76)

Note. In each cell, the main entry refers to the overall interval (Time 1–Time 3); entries in parentheses refer to the
Time 1–Time 2 and Time 2–Time 3 intervals. In each row, correlations with different subscripts (for the overall
interval) differ at least at p < .05 (one-tailed).
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 485

MANOVA (Group  Gender  Time) was husbands’ and wives’ general anxiety); in
conducted on the attachment scales. Analysis 3, Time 1 interview reports of the
Group was varied between-couple, and gen- pregnancy (spouses’ emotional reactions,
der and time were varied within-couple. The planning for pregnancy, pregnancy-related
only significant effect was the main effect of problems); and in Analysis 4, Time 2
gender, which was restricted to discomfort interview reports of the birth (length of
with closeness, F(1, 134) ¼ 20.20, p < .001; labor, extent of pain relief and surgical
husbands reported higher discomfort than intervention, degree of prematurity) and
wives (see Table 2 for mean scores). There questionnaire reports of infant tempera-
were no main or interactive effects of group, ment. Finally, at Step 3, spouses’ initial
indicating that the transition to parenthood levels of discomfort with closeness and
was not associated with an overall increase relationship anxiety were entered. (These
or decrease in levels of security. analyses were restricted to transition
couples, as some predictor variables did
not apply to comparison couples—the next
Predicting maternal depression
section examines initial depression and
Maternal depression was defined in terms of attachment security as predictors of later
transition wives’ questionnaire reports, com- depression, for both groups.) Correlations
pleted six weeks postbirth. Although the among the questionnaire-based predictor
mean score on this variable was low (see variables appear in Table 4.
Table 2), it is important to note that this At Step 1, wives’ initial depression did
measure of depression focuses on core clin- not predict their Time 2 depression
ical symptoms, whereas most questionnaire (b ¼ .09, ns). At Step 2, none of the four
measures inflate the extent of depression in blocks of predictors added significantly to
new mothers by including items that tap the explained variance; however, regression
normal postnatal changes (e.g., tiredness, coefficients indicated that maternal depres-
sleep disturbance). Extensive normative sion was related to wives’ reports of infant
data on the current measure indicate that difficulty (r ¼ .23, b ¼ 26, p < .05), and
scores higher than 4 lie beyond the normal weakly related to wives’ reports of their
range (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). reactions to the pregnancy (r ¼  .18,
b ¼  .24, p < .08). At Step 3, spouses’
Predicting maternal depression: Main effects attachment dimensions added significantly
of attachment. To predict mothers’ to the explained variance in all analyses
depression at Time 2, four hierarchical (F change values ranged from 3.78 to 4.46
regression analyses were conducted. In line across the analyses, p < .01 in each case).
with Hypothesis 2a, these analyses assessed More specifically, wives’ prenatal relation-
the ability of attachment dimensions to pre- ship anxiety consistently predicted their
dict changes in maternal depression, after postbirth depression, after controlling for
controlling for other risk factors. Hence, in the other risk factors; in the full models,
each case, wives’ initial depression was the beta weight for relationship anxiety
entered at Step 1. Step 2 comprised risk ranged from .41 to .45 (p < .01 in each case).
factors assessed by questionnaire and inter- Note that in the regression analyses
view, with the four subsets chosen to tap a reported above, depression was treated as
range of theoretical constructs while main- a continuous variable. Given that studies of
taining an adequate ratio of cases to vari- postnatal depression generally focus on
ables. Specifically, these predictors were fairly severe symptoms of distress, it was
entered at Step 2: in Analysis 1, relationship considered important to assess the conver-
factors (husbands’ and wives’ initial scores gent validity of the results by distinguishing
on marital satisfaction and social support); between normal and high levels of depres-
in Analysis 2, measures of initial psycho- sion. Hence, logistic regression analysis
logical adjustment (husbands’ depression, was also used to assess the links between
486 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

Table 4. Correlations among focal variables (questionnaire-based predictor variables and


wives’ Time 2 depression) for transition and comparison couples

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1 w discomfort –

2 h discomfort .25 –
.09 –
3 w relat. anxiety .44 .16 –
.37 .03 –
4 h relat. anxiety .33 .39 .09 –
.08 .33 .10 –
5 w marital satisfaction .24 .02 .30 .03 –
.29 .06 .29 .11 –
6 h marital satisfaction .11 .29 .07 .31 .45 –
.15 .19 .16 .15 .64 –
7 w depression .10 .11 .26 .06 .42 .30 –
.30 .03 .38 .06 .16 .14 –
8 h depression .30 .17 .14 .42 .15 .26 .09 –
.13 .22 .09 .32 .13 .24 .18 –
9 w general anxiety .16 .09 .38 .06 .22 .17 .43 .26 –
.20 .02 .40 .07 .31 .22 .53 .16 –
10 h general anxiety .32 .08 .10 .42 .12 .26 .06 .49 .10 –
.13 .24 .05 .40 .01 .10 .02 .63 .01 –
11 w social support .39 .09 .42 .18 .26 .02 .12 .05 .17 .01 –
.40 .19 .41 .05 .47 .22 .19 .14 .23 .12 –
12 h social support .14 .32 .20 .39 .01 .25 .03 .34 .16 .21 .28 –
.02 .32 .08 .14 .11 .13 .04 .26 .08 .23 .31 –
13 w infant temperament .11 .05 .01 .08 .09 .11 .03 .03 .04 .24 .07 .16 –
– – – – – – – – – – – – –
14 h infant temperament .04 .01 .01 .15 .18 .01 .03 .06 .04 .03 .08 05 .67 –
– – – – – – – – – – – – – –
15 w Time 2 depression .10 .17 .35 .02 .06 .03 .09 .02 .14 .01 .02 .07 .24 .10
.15 .14 .32 .29 .30 .21 .50 .41 .39 .15 .22 .24 – –

Note. w ¼ wives; h ¼ husbands. Top and bottom cell entries are for transition and comparison couples, respec-
tively. Boldface entries are significant; critical values ¼ .23 (p < .05), .30 (p < .01), .37 (p < .001).

transition wives’ Time 2 depression and the only one of these women reported serious
predictor variables. In these analyses, the depression at Time 1.)
dependent variable was dichotomized; Four logistic regression analyses were
based on extensive normative data (Lovibond conducted, with the dichotomous measure
& Lovibond, 1995), scores of 5 and above of depression being related to the same sets
were taken as indicative of depression. of predictor variables. Findings from these
According to this criterion, 13 of the analyses generally supported those already
mothers experienced serious depressive reported. The Wald statistic for wives’ initial
symptoms.1 (Using the same criterion, relationship anxiety was significant in three
of the four full models (all except the
model including interview reports of the preg-
1. It should be noted that members of the research
team informed all participants of appropriate nancy), ranging from 4.51 to 7.97 (p from .05
sources of counseling and other support. to .01). Mean initial scores on relationship
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 487

anxiety were higher for depressed mothers the hypothesized interactive effect of wives’
(M ¼ 43.60, SD ¼ 12.59) than for other relationship anxiety and husbands’ caregiv-
mothers (M ¼ 36.43, SD ¼ 9.70). ing (Hypothesis 2b), another hierarchical
regression analysis was conducted to pre-
The role of prior depression. Interestingly, dict transition wives’ postbirth depres-
we found no link between prenatal and sion.2 Six predictor variables were entered
postnatal depressive symptoms. To clarify at Step 1: wives’ initial depression (as a
this null result, additional analyses were control variable), wives’ initial relationship
conducted to assess the role of initial anxiety, and husbands’ initial scores on the
depression and attachment security as pre- four caregiving scales (sensitivity, proxi-
dictors of later depression, for transition mity, cooperation, and compulsive care).
and comparison wives and husbands. Product terms were formed by multiplying
Hence, four hierarchical regression analyses wives’ scores on relationship anxiety by
were conducted, one for each combination husbands’ scores on each caregiving scale,
of group and gender: Husbands’ and wives’ after mean-centering the scores to avoid
initial scores on depression were entered at multicollinearity. These terms were entered
Step 1, and the four attachment scales were at Step 2.
entered at Step 2. The results of these Although the overall prediction was reli-
analyses are summarized in Table 5. able at Step 1 (R2 ¼ .16, p < .01), only wives’
relationship anxiety was a significant pre-
At Step 1, spouses’ initial scores on
dictor of depressive symptoms (b ¼ 36,
depression provided strong prediction of
p < .001). At Step 2, the product terms pro-
Time 2 depression, except for transition
vided a reliable increase in explained vari-
wives. The most robust finding was the effect
ance (R2 change ¼ .10, p < .05), with a
of own depression, although husbands’
significant interaction between wives’ rela-
initial depression also predicted Time 2
tionship anxiety and husbands’ compulsive
depression for comparison wives. At Step 2,
care (b ¼ 26, p < .02). This interaction was
addition of the attachment scales provided a
further explored using procedures recom-
strong increase in explained variance for
mended by Aiken and West (1991). Specifi-
transition wives only, with own relationship
cally, equations were derived for the
anxiety linked to Time 2 depression. Fisher
regression of wives’ depression on wives’
tests indicated that the bivariate correlation
relationship anxiety, at high (þ1 SD) and
between Time 1 and Time 2 depression was
low (1 SD) levels of husbands’ compulsive
significantly lower for transition wives
care. As Figure 1 shows, maternal depres-
(r ¼ .09) than for all other combinations of
sion was associated with wives’ relationship
group and gender (rs ranging from .45 to
anxiety, only when husbands were high in
.62). In contrast, the correlation between
compulsive care. In other words, wives who
Time 1 relationship anxiety and Time 2
were anxious about their relationships and
depression did not differ across the four
whose husbands became overinvolved with
combinations (rs ranged from .32 to .35).
their concerns reported higher maternal
depression than all other couples.
Interactive effect of wives’ relationship
A similar analysis was conducted for com-
anxiety and husbands’ caregiving. To test
parison wives. Prediction was reliable at Step
1 (R2 ¼ .32, p < .001), with wives’ initial
2. For completeness, hierarchical regression analysis depression predicting their later depressive
was also used to test for possible interactive effects symptoms (b ¼ 40, p < .001). At Step 2, the
of spouses’ attachment dimensions (Wives’
Relationship Anxiety  Husbands’ Relationship product terms did not add to the prediction
Anxiety, Wives’ Relationship Anxiety  Husbands’ (R2 change ¼ .03, ns). Hence, the interactive
Discomfort, Wives’ Discomfort  Husbands’ effect of wives’ relationship anxiety and hus-
Discomfort, and Wives’ Discomfort  Husbands’
Relationship Anxiety). However, all four bands’ compulsive care was restricted to
interactive effects were nonsignificant. those wives experiencing new parenthood.
488 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

Table 5. Summary of explained variance and significant effects in the prediction of Time 2
depression from Time 1 depression (Step 1) and Time 1 attachment (Step 2)

Wives Husbands
Transition Comparison Transition Comparison
Step 1 R2 ¼ .01, ns R2 ¼ .28*** R2 ¼ .21*** R2 ¼ .40***
F(2, 73) ¼ 0.30 F(2, 71) ¼ 15.25 F(2, 73) ¼ 11.48 F(2, 71) ¼ 27.06
No significant Ws’ depression Hs’ depression Hs’ depression
predictors b ¼ .43** b ¼ .45** b ¼ .62***
Hs’ depression
b ¼ .25**
Step 2 R2 change ¼ .14** R2 change ¼ .07, ns R2 change ¼ .04, ns R2 change ¼ .06y
F change ¼ 3.51 F change ¼ 2.45 F change ¼ 1.06 F change ¼ 2.54
Ws’ relationship Ws’ depression Hs’ depression Hs’ depression
anxiety b ¼ .36** b ¼ .37** b ¼ .55***
b ¼ .38** Hs’ depression
b ¼ .19y

Note. yp < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Consequences of maternal depression esis, we conducted hierarchical regression


analyses predicting each outcome variable:
We expected that maternal depression would
transition husbands’ and wives’ Time 3
be associated with increased insecurity and
scores on discomfort with closeness, relation-
marital dissatisfaction across the transition
ship anxiety, and marital satisfaction. Time 1
period (Hypothesis 3). To test this hypoth-
scores on the corresponding variable were
entered at Step 1, and wives’ Time 2 depres-
High anxious wives sion scores were entered at Step 2. Hence,
Low anxious wives
these analyses assessed whether wives’
7 depressive symptoms were associated with
6
changes in attachment security and marital
satisfaction across the course of the study.
Wives’ depression, Time 2

5 In three of the six analyses, wives’ Time 2


depression added to the prediction of the
4
outcome measure. In predicting wives’ dis-
3 comfort with closeness, 37% of the variance
was explained at Step 1; at Step 2, wives’
2 depression explained an additional 5% of
the variance (F change ¼ 5.45, p < .05,
1
b ¼ .22). For the prediction of husbands’
0 relationship anxiety, 54% of the variance
Low High was explained at Step 1; at Step 2, wives’
Husbands’ compulsive care depression explained an additional 10%
(F change ¼ 18.11, p < .001, b ¼ .32). Finally,
Figure 1. Interaction of wives’ relationship for husbands’ marital satisfaction, 37% of
and husbands’ compulsive care in the the variance was explained at Step 1;
prediction of maternal depression. at Step 2, wives’ depression explained an
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 489

additional 4% (F change ¼ 2.91, p < .05, the relationship anxiety scale was less stable
b ¼  .18). For completeness, similar analyses for transition wives than for other partici-
were conducted for the comparison group. pants. On the other hand, transition wives’
However, in this group, wives’ Time 2 scores on discomfort with closeness were
depression did not predict changes in less stable than those of comparison wives,
either husbands’ or wives’ attachment but did not differ from those of transition
security or marital satisfaction. or comparison husbands. As previous
researchers have noted, measures of adult
attachment show varying degrees of stabil-
Discussion
ity. Single-item ratings show test-retest cor-
Before discussing the results of this study, it relations around .6, whereas multiple-item
is important to note its limitations and and interview-based measures (which are
strengths. Given that the first assessment generally more reliable) are relatively stable
of transition couples occurred during the (e.g., Feeney & Noller, 1996; Scharfe &
second trimester of pregnancy, we have no Bartholomew, 1994). Further, both theory
information about previous levels of individ- and research suggest that attachment meas-
ual and couple adjustment. For example, ures are more stable when the individuals
we cannot be certain that women who studied are in stable relationships (Feeney &
reported low levels of depression at Time 1 Noller, 1996). Given the high reliability of
had not experienced any prior episodes of the attachment scales used in the present
depression. However, given the difficulties study, transition wives’ scores on relation-
associated with recruiting samples of ship anxiety showed more instability than is
couples prior to pregnancy, the longitudinal typical in samples of intact couples.
design employed in this study is typical of Further, for transition wives only, relation-
‘‘prospective’’ studies of parenthood, and ship anxiety was less stable over the longer
allowed us to assess hypothesized patterns period than over the two shorter intervals.
of predictive relations. Because we con- This result suggests that the instability
ducted several analyses, the possibility of observed among transition wives probably
Type 1 errors should be kept in mind. At reflects some change in the underlying
the same time, the limited sample size would construct, in addition to measurement
have reduced the ability to detect significant error. This finding concerning the malle-
effects; hence, Type 2 errors may also have ability of attachment dimensions adds to
occurred. Notwithstanding this issue, it is previous work that highlights the transition
important to note that the transition and to parenthood as a crucial time in couple
comparison couples were similar in terms relationships (e.g., Cox, Paley, Burchinal, &
of their background experiences, relation- Payne, 1999).
ship length, and initial functioning both as As expected, however, the transition
individuals and as couples. Further, attri- to parenthood was not associated with
tion rates were similar for the two groups, change in mean levels of attachment
and education level was the only variable security, for either husbands or wives.
that predicted attrition (in both groups). Together, these results suggest that attach-
Hence, we can be relatively confident that ment issues are salient during this transition
any differences between transition and period, but that the experience of first-time
comparison couples later in the study are parenthood does not necessarily pose a
due to the experience of first-time parenthood. threat to individuals’ sense of security.
Rather, consistent with previous research, it
appears that working models of attachment
Stability of attachment
can be influenced for either better or worse,
There was some support for Hypothesis 1, depending on the specific nature of relation-
which predicted lower stability of attach- ship experiences (Hammond & Fletcher,
ment among transition wives. Specifically, 1991; Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994).
490 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

participants for whom negative affect is an


Predicting maternal depression
ongoing problem, again inflating the link
Wives’ prenatal relationship anxiety emerged between prenatal and postnatal depression.
as a reliable predictor of increases in their (In contrast, the present study was described
depressive symptoms. Of the wide range of in relatively broad terms.) More generally,
predictor variables assessed in this study, the researchers have noted that no risk factor
only other variables that predicted increases for postpartum depression has received
in maternal depression were wives’ reports unambiguous support, that some factors
that they reacted negatively to the preg- may have complex effects because they fluc-
nancy, and wives’ perceptions of infant diffi- tuate over time, and that different factors
culty. Importantly, the link between prenatal may predict depressive symptoms versus
relationship anxiety and later depression was clinical diagnosis of depression (Beck, 1996;
significant even when controlling for these O’Hara, 1997).
variables, and for variables that correlate Regardless of these issues, we found that
with relationship anxiety and have emerged it was only in the context of new parent-
as risk factors in some studies (e.g., marital hood that attachment insecurity appeared
dissatisfaction, lack of social support). to trigger depression in women who showed
Further, this link held, regardless of whether no initial signs of depression. (When Time 1
depression was conceptualized as a continu- depression was controlled, Time 1 relation-
ous or dichotomous variable. These findings ship anxiety predicted later depression for
offer strong support for Hypothesis 2a. transition, but not comparison, wives.)
Interestingly, wives who experienced post- These findings suggest that anxiety over
birth depression were not distinguished from relationships, when coupled with a major
other transition wives by their prenatal depres- transition such as first-time parenthood
sion levels (despite their prenatal anxiety about (and the associated demands of primary
relationships). Further, the predictive effects of caregiving), predisposes some women to
prior depression and relationship anxiety were become depressed. This result is consistent
different for transition wives than for other with the proposition that the effects of
participants: For other participants, Time 2 insecurity become more apparent when
depression was strongly predicted by prior people face stressful or challenging situations.
depression, with minimal additional variance This proposition has already been supported
explained by attachment security. by studies of environmental stressors (e.g.,
At this stage, it is unclear why prenatal Mikulincer, Florian, & Weller, 1993) and
depression failed to predict postbirth depres- by laboratory studies of relationship conflict
sion. Meta-analysis suggests a large effect (Feeney, 1998; Rholes et al., 1998). However,
size for the relation between prenatal and the present work is important in extending
postnatal depression (Beck, 1996); however, these findings to a major, naturally occurring
several studies fail to report the predictive event that affects most married couples, and
effect of prenatal depression, and others that impacts directly on the couple bond.
report it to be nonsignificant (e.g., Kumar Critics might argue that the link between
& Robson, 1984). The strength of the link initial relationship anxiety and maternal
between prenatal and postnatal depression depression is not causal in nature; rather, it
may depend on complex factors, including simply shows that some individuals are high
the timing of the assessments and the focus in a general factor that can be labeled ‘‘nega-
of the study. For example, some studies tive affectivity,’’ or neuroticism. In other
assess prenatal depression quite late in preg- words, it could be argued that this link sim-
nancy; this practice may inflate the link with ply reflects substantial overlap among such
postnatal depression, as concerns related to constructs as relationship anxiety, general
birth and child-rearing may be salient at anxiety, and depression. It is true that these
both times. Further, studies focusing expli- three constructs, when assessed concur-
citly on depressed mood may attract some rently, show a degree of overlap. However,
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 491

in the present study, only about 15% to 20% Although this finding was gender specific,
of the variance in any of these measures was it is congruent with Mauthner’s (1998)
shared with either of the others. Moreover, qualitative data, which suggest reciprocal
the link between relationship anxiety and relations between postnatal depression and
maternal depression remained highly signifi- the state of the couple relationship. For
cant, even controlling for wives’ initial example, depression is usually associated with
depression and general anxiety, and percep- emotional withdrawal and social isolation,
tions of infant temperament. Collectively, which in turn are likely to impact on the
these findings suggest that relationship anx- quality of the marriage (Mauthner, 1998).
iety predicts maternal depression, not As expected, maternal depression was
because it signals general negativity or anx- also associated with increases in attachment
iety but because it raises basic issues about insecurity across the transition period.
the partner’s love and commitment and the However, the findings were again gender
future of the couple relationship. Being specific: Maternal depression was linked to
based on longitudinal data, this finding increases in husbands’ relationship anxiety
strengthens the evidence from Meredith and and wives’ discomfort with closeness. At
Noller’s (in press) study linking attachment this stage, it is not clear why these effects
insecurity and postnatal depression. should be gender specific. However, by defi-
At the same time, further analyses indi- nition, relationship anxiety is associated
cated that the association between transi- with concerns about self-worth, about part-
tion wives’ relationship anxiety and ners’ love and commitment, and about the
postbirth depression was moderated by hus- stability of intimate relationships. Hence,
bands’ caregiving style. Consistent with we can speculate that if wives’ relationship
Hypothesis 2b, this finding suggests that anxiety is an important causal factor in their
partners’ provision of appropriate care can postbirth depression, the concerns that
buffer, to a large extent, the potentially these wives experience are often communi-
negative effects of wives’ attachment cated, either directly or indirectly, to their
insecurity. This effect was restricted, however, partners. Given the difficult emotional
to the measure of compulsive care. Hence, it climate associated with spousal depres-
appears that husbands can best support sion and new parenthood, husbands may
their wives throughout this period by being eventually come to share these concerns. In
attentive to their needs and concerns, while addition, as noted earlier, wives who are
taking care not to become overinvolved and depressed are likely to withdraw emotion-
burdened by them. Given that wives who ally from their partners; over time, this
are anxious about their relationships crave emotional distancing may result in wives’
affection and reassurance, it is not surpris- increasing discomfort with couple intimacy.
ing that husbands who tend to become It is worth noting that the hierarchical
overwhelmed by their partners’ needs find regression analyses used to test the third
it difficult to help them deal with their dis- hypothesis provide a very stringent test of
tress. The moderating role of husbands’ the effects of maternal depression. As pre-
spousal caregiving also provides indirect vious studies indicate, measures of attach-
support for the claim that attachment and ment security and relationship quality are
caregiving are interrelated systems of beha- relatively stable over time in established
vior that are central to the development of couple relationships, leaving little additional
the pair bond (Hazan & Shaver, 1994). variance to be explained when earlier scores
are statistically controlled (Noller & Feeney,
2002; Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1994).
Consequences of maternal depression
Although maternal depression was
As hypothesized, maternal depression associated with changes in wives’ discomfort
was associated with an increase (albeit with closeness, husbands’ relationship anx-
modest) in husbands’ marital dissatisfaction. iety, and husbands’ marital satisfaction, these
492 J. Feeney, R. Alexander, P. Noller, and L. Hohaus

same variables were not reliable predictors of and this finding reinforces the view that this
wives’ postbirth depression. These results transition period is a crucial time in the life
suggest the importance of a process view of cycle of couple relationships. In terms of
the transition to parenthood (and, in particu- research, there is a need for further prospec-
lar, of maternal depression), which focuses tive studies of adult attachment during the
on both the predictors and outcomes of transition to parenthood. Larger samples
adjustment difficulties. Clearly, difficulties would be particularly useful, allowing
that affect one spouse have an impact on researchers to use methods such as struc-
the other and on the couple relationship. As tural equation modeling to test integrative
suggested earlier, even relatively happy cou- models that incorporate risk factors, mater-
ples may experience some erosion in marital nal depression, and outcome variables.
satisfaction, if issues concerning insecurity Finally, with regard to practice, the results
and emotional withdrawal are not addressed. highlight the ongoing process of change
that characterizes the transition to parent-
hood, and suggest ways in which individual
Summary and Conclusions
and couple adjustment may be promoted.
The results of this study have important For example, early intervention can help
implications for theory, research, and prac- individuals to address basic insecurities,
tice. The findings clearly support the rele- and to develop a strong relationship before
vance of attachment theory to couple embarking on parenthood. In addition, hus-
relationships, and specifically, to the transi- bands clearly play a key role in supporting
tion to parenthood. It seems that new par- their wives both before and after the birth,
enthood is a time when working models of and programs that facilitate this process are
attachment may be particularly malleable, likely to facilitate a smoother transition.

References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Boyce, P., Hickie, I., & Parker, G. (1991). Parents,
Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury partners or personality? Risk factors for post-
Park, CA: Sage. natal depression. Journal of Affective Disorders,
Alexander, R. P., Feeney, J. A., Hohaus, L., & Noller, P. 21, 245–255.
(2001). Attachment style and coping resources as Brown, M. A. (1986). Social support during preg-
predictors of coping strategies in the transition to nancy: A unidimensional or multidimensional con-
parenthood. Personal Relationships, 8, 137–152. struct? Nursing Research, 35, 4–9.
Baldwin, M. W., & Fehr, B. (1995). On the instability Carnelley, K. B., Pietromonaco, P. R., & Jaffe, K.
of attachment style ratings. Personal Relationships, (1994). Depression, working models of others,
2, 247–261. and relationship functioning. Journal of Personality
Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. B., & Lounsbury, M. L. and Social Psychology, 66, 127–140.
(1979). Measurement of infant difficultness. Child Cox, M. J., Paley, B., Burchinal, M., & Payne, C. C.
Development, 50, 794–803. (1999). Marital perceptions and interactions across
Beck, C. T. (1996). A meta-analysis of predictors the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage
of postpartum depression. Nursing Research, 45, and the Family, 61, 611–625.
297–303. Da Costa, D., Larouche, J., Dritsa, M., & Brender, W.
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1990). Patterns of marital (2000). Psychosocial correlates of prepartum and
change across the transition to parenthood: postpartum depressed mood. Journal of Affective
Pregnancy to three years postpartum. Journal of Disorders, 59, 31–40.
Marriage and the Family, 52, 5–19. Dozier, M., Stovall, K. C., & Albus, K. E. (1999).
Berscheid, E. (1998). A social psychological view of mari- Attachment and psychopathology in adulthood.
tal dysfunction and stability. In. T. N. Bradbury In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The handbook
(Ed.), The developmental course of marital of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical appli-
dysfunction (pp. 441–459). Cambridge, UK: cations (pp. 497–519). New York: Guilford.
Cambridge University Press. Feeney, J. A. (1998). Adult attachment and relationship-
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol.1. Attach- centered anxiety: Responses to physical and emo-
ment. New York: Basic Books. tional distancing. In J. A. Simpson & W. S Rholes
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol.2. Separ- (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships
ation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books. (pp. 189–218). New York: Guilford.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol.3. Loss. Feeney, J. A. (1999). Adult romantic attachment and
New York: Basic Books. couple relationships. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver
Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood 493

(Eds.), The handbook of attachment: Theory, cations. Journal of Reproductive and Infant
research, and clinical applications (pp. 355–377). Psychology, 16, 157–175.
New York: Guilford. Meredith, P., & Noller, P. (in press). Attachment and
Feeney, J. A., Hohaus, L., Noller, P., & Alexander, R. infant difficultness in postnatal depression. Journal
(2001). Becoming parents: Exploring the bonds of Family Issues.
between mothers, fathers, and their infants. Cam- Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. between adult attachment styles and emotional and
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1992). Attachment style cognitive reactions to stressful events. In J. A. Simpson
and romantic love: Relationship dissolution. & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and
Australian Journal of Psychology, 44, 69–74. close relationships (pp. 143–165). New York: Guil-
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1996). Adult attachment. ford.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Weller, A. (1993).
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Hanrahan, M. (1994). Attachment styles, coping strategies, and posttrau-
Assessing adult attachment: Developments in the matic psychological distress: The impact of the
conceptualization of security and insecurity. In Gulf War in Israel. Journal of Personality and
M. B. Sperling & W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attach- Social Psychology, 64, 817–826.
ment in adults: Theory, assessment, and treatment Murray, L., & Cooper, P. J. (1997). The role of infant
(pp. 128–152). New York: Guilford. and maternal factors in postpartum depression,
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). mother-infant interactions, and infant outcome.
An item response theory analysis of self-report In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum
measures of adult attachment. Journal of Person- depression and child development (pp. 111–135).
ality and Social Psychology, 78, 350–365. New York: Guilford.
Gotlib, I. H., Whiffen, V. E., Wallace, P. M., & Noller, P., & Feeney, J. A. (2002). Communication,
Mount, J. H. (1991). Prospective investigation of relationship concerns, and satisfaction in early
postpartum depression: Factors involved in onset marriage. In A. L. Vangelisti, H. T. Reis, &
and recovery. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, M. A. Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Stability and change in
100, 122–132. relationships (pp. 129–155). Cambridge, UK:
Hammond J. R., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1991). Attach- Cambridge.
ment styles and relationship satisfaction in the O’Hara, M. W. (1997). The nature of postpartum
development of close relationships. New Zealand depressive disorders. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper
Journal of Psychology, 20, 56–62. (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love (pp. 3–31). New York: Guilford.
conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Grich, J.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524. (2001). Adult attachment and the transition to
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an parenthood. Journal of Personality and Social
organizational framework for research on close Psychology, 81, 421–435.
relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 1–22. Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Stevens, J. G. (1998).
Huston, T. L., & Vangelisti, A. L. (1995). How par- Attachment orientations, social support, and con-
enthood affects marriage. In M. A. Fitzpatrick & flict resolution in close relationships. In J. A. Simpson
A. L. Vangelisti (Eds.), Explaining family interac- & W. S Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close
tions (pp. 147–176). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. relationships (pp. 166–188). New York: Guilford.
Kirkpatrick, L. E., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment Rutter, M. (1997). Maternal depression and infant
styles and close relationships: A four-year prospec- development: Cause and consequence; sensitivity
tive study. Personal Relationships, 1, 123–142. and specificity. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper
Kumar, R., & Robson, K. M. (1984). A prospective (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development
study of emotional disorders in childbearing (pp. 295–315). New York: Guilford.
women. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 35–47. Sanchez, L., & Thomson, E. (1997). Becoming
Kunce, L. J., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). An attachment- mothers and fathers: Parenthood, gender, and the
theoretical approach to caregiving in romantic division of labor. Gender and Society, 11, 747–772.
relationships. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Reliability and
(Eds.), Advances in personal relationships Vol.5: stability of adult attachment patterns. Personal
Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 205–237). Relationships, 1, 23–43.
London: Jessica Kingsley. Shaver, P. R., Hazan, C., & Bradshaw, D. (1988).
Levy-Shiff, R. (1994). Individual and contextual correlates Love as attachment: The integration of three beha-
of marital change across the transition to parenthood. vioral systems. In R. J. Sternberg & M. Barnes
Developmental Psychology, 30, 591–601. (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68–99). New
Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) Snyder, D. K. (1979). Multidimensional assessment of
(2nd ed.). Sydney, Australia: Psychology Founda- marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the
tion of Australia. Family, 41, 813–823.
Matthey, S., Barnett, B., Ungerer, J., & Waters, B. Strahan, B. J. (1995). Predictors of depression: An
(2000). Paternal and maternal depressed mood dur- attachment theoretical approach. Journal of Family
ing the transition to parenthood. Journal of Affect- Studies, 1, 33–47.
ive Disorders, 60, 75–85. Terry, D. J., McHugh, T. A., & Noller, P. (1991). Role
Mauthner, N. S. (1998). Re-assessing the importance dissatisfaction and the decline in marital quality
and role of the marital relationship in postnatal across the transition to parenthood. Australian
depression: methodological and theoretical impli- Journal of Psychology, 43, 129–132.

You might also like