You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/223633020

Public Perceptions of Police Misconduct and Discrimination: Examining the


Impact of Media Consumption

Article  in  Journal of Criminal Justice · March 2007


DOI: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2007.01.006

CITATIONS READS

136 5,752

2 authors, including:

Kenneth Dowler
Wilfrid Laurier University
17 PUBLICATIONS   854 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Kenneth Dowler on 31 July 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193 – 203

Public perceptions of police misconduct and discrimination:


Examining the impact of media consumption
Kenneth Dowler a,⁎, Valerie Zawilski b
a
Department of Criminology, Wilfrid Laurier University at Brantford, 73 George Street, Brantford, Ontario, Canada N3T 2Y3
b
Department of Contemporary Studies, Wilfrid Laurier University at Brantford, 73 George Street, Brantford, Ontario, Canada N3T 2Y3

Abstract

The media portrayal of policing is juxtaposed with both positive and negative representations. As a result, a complex
relationship exists between media consumption and public attitudes towards the police. The purpose of this study was to test the
impact that media consumption had on attitudes toward police misconduct and discrimination. The findings revealed that heavy
consumers of network news were more likely to believe that police misconduct was a frequent event. This was especially true for
minority respondents. Similarly, minority respondents that frequently viewed network news were more likely to believe that Whites
received better treatment by the police. Finally, the findings revealed that frequent viewers of police dramas believed that the
wealthy received preferential treatment from the police. Conversely, frequent viewers of crime solving shows believed that the
wealthy did not receive preferential treatment.
© 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction less research on media's influence on citizens' attitudes


toward police misconduct and discrimination. As a result,
The popular media is of fundamental importance in the the purpose of this research study was to address the gap
construction of attitudes toward criminal justice and by testing the impact of media consumption on public
criminal justice agents. The majority of public knowledge attitudes toward the police.
about crime and justice is derived from media consump- The majority of research conducted on the police-
tion (Ericson, Baranek, & Chan, 1987; Graber, 1980; media relationship was centered on determining the
Roberts & Doob, 1990; Surette, 2007). As such, the portrayal of police within the media. Literature on media
perception of victims, criminals, and law enforcement portrayals of police revealed two contradictory observa-
officials is largely determined by their portrayal within the tions. Some research revealed that the police were
media. There was a plethora of research studies that presented favorably, while other research implied that
examined attitudes toward the police (Schafer, Huebner, the police were negatively represented (Surette, 2007).
& Bynum, 2003). There were, however, relatively few Various researchers posited that the news media engaged
studies that examined the media impact on those attitudes in negative presentations of policing. For example, print
(Weitzer & Tuch, 2004, 2005a, 2005b). There was even and broadcast media characterized the police as in-
effective and incompetent, while reality police shows
and news tabloid programs presented the police as heroes
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 519 756 5372x5754. who fight evil (Surette, 2007). The media offered little
E-mail address: kdowler@wlu.ca (K. Dowler). information to evaluate police, and the focus was on
0047-2352/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2007.01.006
194 K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203

negative events rather than positive or successful crime the proportion of crimes solved were overrepresented on
prevention (Graber, 1980). Essentially, crime presented these shows. Other research argued that stereotypes
by the media was punished; however, the police were about racial minorities and crime were common in police
rarely the heroes (Lichter & Lichter, 1983). reality shows (Kooistra et al., 1998; Oliver & Armstrong,
Nevertheless, the majority of research suggested that 1998). Finally, Surette (2007, p. 108) argued that the end
the police were favorably presented. For instance, in result was that “crime control is applauded, due process
crime dramas, crimes were solved and suspects were is disparaged. Individual causes of crime, assumed guilt
successfully arrested (Carlson, 1985; Dominick, 1973; of suspects, and an ‘us’ versus them portrait dominates”
Doyle, 2003; Estep & MacDonald, 1984; Kooistra, the construction of crime and justice.
Mahoney, & Westervelt, 1998; Zillman & Wakshlag, Although there were mixed views about whether the
1985). Similarly, television news exaggerated the pro- police were negatively or positively presented by the
portion of arrests, which portrayed the police as more various media types, some researchers posited that
effective than official statistics exhibited (Marsh, 1991; public attitudes toward the police were influenced by
Roshier, 1973; Sacco & Fair, 1988; Skogan & Maxfield, exposure to media (Huang & Vaughn, 1996; Maguire,
1981). Public relation strategies endorsed by police 1988). There were very few studies, however, that tested
agencies might partially explain a favorable view of the this assumption. In one study, viewing police reality
police. The presentation of proactive police activity shows and television news increased confidence in
builds the image that the police were effective and police. Racial differences, however, existed among
efficient investigators of crime (Christensen, Schmidt, & respondents. White viewers of “reality” police programs
Henderson, 1982). The media-police relationship is were more likely to have positive attitudes toward the
mutually beneficial. The police have an interest in police, while there was no relationship with African
preserving a positive public image, while the reporters American viewers. Conversely, an increase in confi-
require quick, reliable, and relatively easy sources of dence towards the police was exhibited in both African
crime news (Ericson et al., 1987; Fishman, 1981; Hall, Americans and Whites after viewing the news (Eschholz,
Critcher, Jefferson, Clarke, & Roberts, 1978). Blackwell, Gertz, & Chiricos, 2002). Moreover, Dowler
More recent research suggested that images of (2002) found that media consumption had a limited
policing created unrealistic public expectations about impact on attitudes toward the police. Heavy television
real policing and disappointment when police did not viewers with previous police contact were more likely to
perform like their media portrayals (Perlmutter, 2000). have negative attitudes toward the police.
Surette (2007) revealed nine police narratives within In terms of police misconduct, there were a handful
fictional police images, which included rogue cops, of important studies that examined public attitudes
corrupt cops, honest cops, buddy cops, comedy cops, toward police transgressions. Many of these studies
action comedy cops, female cops, and aging cops. tested the impact of a single, publicized incident of
Surette (2007) argued that with the exception of female police corruption or misconduct on public perceptions
police narratives, hyper masculinity was the familiar of police. In brief, they found that news coverage of
pattern within the narratives and the narratives were brutality incidents or police corruption increased neg-
individualized. In addition, police reality programs have ative attitudes toward the police (Kaminski & Jefferis,
become highly popular in television programming and 1998; Sigleman, Welch, Bledsoe, & Combs, 1997; Tuch
provided different images of policing. According to & Weitzer, 1997; Weitzer, 2002). To further test this
Cavender and Fishman (1998), the popularity of police relationship, Weitzer and Tuch (2004) examined the
reality programs was due to the social context, in which impact that frequent exposure of separate incidents of
crime was viewed as a serious social problem that was police misconduct had on citizen attitudes toward the
getting steadily more problematic and dangerous, while police. They found that repeated media exposure to
Surette (2007) reported that the attraction of these shows police abuse increased respondents' beliefs in the fre-
was voyeuristic. The final edit of these shows was quency of police misconduct. This was true for Whites,
controlled by the cooperating police department, thus African Americans, and Hispanics, however, minorities
any negative portrayals of the police would not be were more strongly affected. In another study, Weitzer
included in the final product. Donovan (1998) suggested and Tuch (2005b) found that exposure to media ac-
that reality television programs recreated the law and counts of police misconduct increased perception of
order ideology and that the police were portrayed as police bias against minorities. Despite the importance
knowledgeable, sensitive, caring, and competent. More- of their findings, Weitzer and Tuch (2004, 2005b)
over, Doyle (2003) argued that crimes of violence and employed a single-item self-report measure of media
K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203 195

exposure. It is vital that a media exposure is measured of research: the vulnerability thesis, the substitution
with a multi-measure item that explores different types thesis, the resonance thesis, and the affinity thesis.
of media. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to test Research studies concluded that the vulnerability the-
the impact that varying types of media consumption had sis, which stated that women and elderly populations
on attitudes toward police misconduct and discrimina- were generally more likely to feel at risk than younger
tory practices in policing. males, had not been well supported (Skogan & Max-
field, 1981). Conversely, there was some support for
Theoretical orientation the substitution thesis which suggested that people
such as high income elderly women who have not been
The textual encoding of a media presentation and the exposed to criminal situations, might, after watching
subsequent polysemic decoding of the text by the the news or crime shows, substitute the reality of their
audience generated an array of diverse interpretations by lives with the idea that they will be future victims of
the viewing audience (Hall, 1980). Media reception criminal behavior (Gunter, 1987). Other studies had
research proposed that the cultural system of codes and found substantially significant findings, which sup-
conventions used by media producer and audience ported the resonance theory, that male viewers from
needed to be understood. Fiske (1986), however, took low income backgrounds exposed to criminal situa-
this further, by suggesting that there were eight positions tions would have greater feelings of resonance with
of subjectivity that should be considered when studying people involved in arrests on television than other
an audience reaction to the media. These positions people (Chiricos, Padgett, & Gertz, 2000; Doob &
included: self, gender, age, group, family, class, nation, Macdonald, 1979; Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, & Sign-
ethnicity, and sexual orientation (Fiske, 1986). In orielli, 1980). Finally, the affinity thesis had been
contrast, earlier approaches to media studies such as supported by evidence that individuals that viewed a
the hypodermic needle theory that regarded audiences as preponderance of characters with similar demographic
passive and heterogeneous and the two step flow or characteristics, that were victimized on television, sub-
limited effects model in which opinion leaders acted as sequently would be more likely to fear being victims of
mediators which interpreted media texts and events for a criminal behavior themselves (Chiricos, Eschholz, &
mass audience, underestimated the intellectual capabil- Gertz, 1997; Gerbner, Gross, Jackson-Beeck, Jeffries-
ities of its viewers (McQuail, 1987). Fox, & Signorielli, 1978).
Blumler and Katz (1974) proposed that while the While social location research examined the viewer's
media essentially entertained its audience, it also served interpretations of a media text in relation to his or
as an important tool of cultural transmission that was herself, the mirror image of social location research
employed by corporations and the state to teach indivi- might be found within the framework of ecological
duals about the hegemonic values of the state, interper- theorizing. This area of research inquiry examined the
sonal relationships, individual and collective identities, roles that social economic status, residential stability,
and the identities of ‘the other(s).’ This complex process and the racial/ethnic/cultural composition of a neigh-
of interpreting, regulating, and appropriating meaning borhood's population might have on an individual's
from multimedia texts was an imperfect process which perceptions about crime rates and criminal activity in the
M. M. Bakhtin described as heteroglossia (Maybin, area that they reside. Several studies had concluded that
2001). there was a significant positive correlation between the
Since the 1980s, reception research had focused on percentage of minorities in a neighborhood and the rate
heteroglossia or the meaning of television messages that of police brutality and aggression. Social conflict theory
were mediated by the subjectivity of its viewers (Carra- explained this social phenomenon by proposing that
gee, 1990; Fiske, 1986; Gunter, 1987; Jensen, 1990; threats to the status quo by minority males who pose a
Perse, Ferguson, & McLeod, 1994). Multimedia view- psychological threat to the White male dominated
ers constructed meanings according to their social lo- hegemonic state was played out in the power relation-
cation in society, which in turn informed and activated ship between police forces and subordinate groups
its meaning for them. Thus, social location research (Kane, 2003).
analyzed the relationship between the subject's life Other factors such as crime rates in a community, the
circumstances, demographic characteristics, and media size of the population living in a community, and the
presentations (Fiske, 1986). general rental and residential turnover rates indicated
Four categories of social location research have that poverty indicators in combination with police
been identified and they included the following areas workload issues and the percentage of minorities living
196 K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203

in a community contributed to the development of the How often do you watch shows like these: more than
minority threat hypothesis that was outwardly mani- once a week, about once a week, one to three times a
fested in the social construction of the symbolic as- month, less than once a month, or never?” (2) “Some
sailant. The symbolic assailant was most often identified shows on television try to solve actual crimes, such as
by the general public as a low income African-American America's Most Wanted and Unsolved Mysteries. How
male who lived in a community that had high crime often do you watch shows like these: more than once
rates. While Americans were confounded by a multi- a week, about once a week, one to three times a
tude of media images of the symbolic assailant, mem- month, less than once a month, or never?” (3) “Some
bers of this minority group might, due to perceptions of shows on television feature footage of police situation
resonance and affinity, feel antagonistic towards the and chases, such as COPS, Highway Patrol, and World's
police force in their neighborhood. Essentially, they Wildest Police Chases. How often do you watch shows
might regard the police as an occupying army. These like these: more than once a week, about once a week,
feelings of antagonism precipitate conflict as the autho- one to three times a month, less than once a month,
rity of the police force was challenged on a community or never?” The frequency of television news consump-
level. Subsequently, evidence that supported the mino- tion was measured by asking respondents how often
rity threat thesis and the police response to minority they watched each of the following television news
threat thesis was perpetuated by the multimedia in- programs: (1) network newscasts such as NBC Nightly
dustry and consumed by a mass audience (Jackson & News, CBS Evening News, and ABC World News
Boyd, 2005). Tonight, (2) local newscasts hosted by anchors from
your own area, (3) Newshour with Jim Lehrer on PBS,
Methodology (4) news magazine programs like 20/20, 60 Minutes or
Dateline, and (5) all news cable stations like CNN,
Sample MSNBC, or FOXNEWS. Each of these variables were
reverse-coded, to have the higher number represent
The data were derived from the 2000 Law and Media increased consumption.
Survey. The Law and Media Survey was a nationally
representative, random telephone survey of adults (n = Dependent variables
1,011) who resided in the United States. The project was
designed and conducted by the Center for Survey Attitudes towards police were divided into two distinct
Research and Analysis, and the Roper Center for areas, which included police misconduct and discrimi-
Opinion Research provided the data. The purpose of natory police practices. Attitudes toward police miscon-
the Law and Media Survey was to examine American duct were measured with three questions: (1) “How often
attitudes toward crime and justice, exploring issues such do you think police officers distort the truth while testi-
as courts, corrections, and police. In addition, the survey fying in a trial to help get a conviction? Is it often,
provided a comprehensive assessment of respondents' sometimes, rarely, or never?” (2) “How often to do you
media consumption. This was essential, as prior research believe police officers will bend the rules to get a
on attitudes toward police lacked a detailed measure of confession from a person accused of a crime? Is it often,
media consumption (Dowler, 2002). sometimes, rarely, or never?” (3) “How often do think
police officers physically abuse those who are accused of
Measurement a crime? Is it often; sometimes, rarely, or never?” The
responses were employed to create a composite measure
Independent variables of attitudes toward police misconduct, which ranged from
3 = low degree of police misconduct to 12 = high degree
There were several different media forms, and each of police misconduct. The alpha reliability coefficient for
type might portray the police in different manners. As a the respondents' perception of the amount of police mis-
result, this research was unique in that media consump- conduct was .70.
tion included different types of crime shows and Discriminatory police practices were measured with
television newscasts. Exposure to media was separated two questions: (1) “Please tell me whether you strongly
into crime show and news consumption. Crime shows agree, agree, disagree, or strongly disagree with the
were measured with three questions which included: following statement. Police officers are more likely to
(1) “Some shows on television feature police officers treat Whites better than minorities.” (2). “Police officers
and prosecutors such as NYPD Blue and Law and Order. are more likely to treat wealthy people better than the
K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203 197

Table 1
Descriptive characteristics of the sample
Variable f % Mean Variable f % Mean
Race Crime drama 2.74
White 807 79.8 Never 358 35.5
Non-White 204 20.2 Less than once a month 124 12.3
One to three times a month 134 13.3
Gender About once a week 212 21.0
Male 494 48.9 More than once a week 181 17.9
Female 517 51.1

Age 45.55 Crime solving 2.54


18–29 201 20.2 Never 386 38.2
30–39 190 19.1 Less than once a month 131 13.0
40–49 223 22.4 One to three times a month 190 18.8
50–59 169 17.0 About once a week 169 16.7
60+ 212 21.3 More than once a week 134 13.3

Education 3.97 Police reality 2.43


Grade school (0–8) 17 1.7 Never 430 42.6
Some high school 73 7.2 Less than once a month 115 11.4
High school 305 30.2 One to three times a month 184 18.2
Some college 269 26.6 About once a week 159 15.7
College graduate 218 21.6 More than once a week 122 12.1
Post-graduate 127 12.6

Income 2.62 Network news 3.37


Less than $30,000 200 23.1 Never 176 17.4
$30,000 to $50,000 260 30.1 Less than once a month 122 12.1
$50,000 to $70,000 181 20.9 One to three times a month 161 15.9
$70,000 to $100,000 115 13.3 About once a week 250 24.8
$100,000+ 108 12.5 More than once a week 301 29.8

Victim of crime Local news 3.90


Yes 492 48.7 Never 109 10.8
No 519 51.3 Less than once a month 82 8.1
One to three times a month 105 10.4
Victim of violent crime About once a week 215 21.3
Yes 76 7.5 More than once a week 498 49.4
No 935 92.5

Charged or arrested News magazines 2.71


Yes 215 21.3 Never 186 18.4
No 796 78.7 Less than once a month 257 25.4
One to three times a month 286 28.3
Concern for violent crime 2.44 About once a week 230 22.8
Not at all 179 17.7 More than once a week 51 5.0
Not too concerned 367 36.3
Somewhat concerned 301 29.8 Cable 3.05
Very concerned 163 16.1 Never 254 25.2
Less than once a month 141 14.0
Concern for property crime 2.81 One to three times a month 159 15.8
Not at all 82 8.1 About once a week 202 20.1
Not too concerned 314 31.1 More than once a week 250 24.9
Somewhat concerned 359 35.5
Very concerned 256 25.3 PBS 1.56
Never 716 71.0
Serious crime in country 3.65 Less than once a month 134 13.3
Not at all serious 3 0.3 One to three times a month 73 7.2
Not too serious 39 3.9 About once a week 53 5.3
(continued on next page)
198 K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203

Table 1 (continued )
Variable f % Mean Variable f % Mean
Serious crime in country PBS
Somewhat serious 269 26.7 More than once a week 32 3.2
Very serious 695 69.1

Serious crime in neighborhood 2.66 Preferential treatment of Whites


Not at all serious 71 7.0 Yes 560 70.1
Not too serious 369 36.6 No 239 29.9
Somewhat serious 395 39.2
Very serious 173 17.2 Preferential treatment of wealthy
Yes 761 77.6
Accuracy of crime shows 2.27 No 220 22.4
Very inaccurate 187 21.6
Somewhat inaccurate 313 36.2 Perceived police misconduct 8.68
Somewhat accurate 308 35.6
Very accurate 56 6.5

less well-off.” To conduct logistic regression, these ities and the poor. To check for multicollinearity,
questions were divided into two categories, which re- all models included collinearity diagnostics. None
presented either agree (coded 1) or disagree (coded 0) of the variance inflation factors (VIF) were greater
with the statement about preferential treatment. than 2, indicating the absence of multicollinearity
(Lewis-Beck, 1990).
Control/demographic variables
Results
There were several demographic variables that
were included in the analysis, which included race (1 = Table 1 provides the descriptive characteristics of the
White, 0 = non-White), gender (1 = male, 0 = female), age sample.
in years, household income measured on a five-step ladder Table 2 presents multivariate results. Model 1 exa-
ranging from less than $30,000 per year (coded 1) to more mined the full sample and the results indicated that two
than $100,000 per year (coded 5), education measured media variables were related to perceptions of police
on a six-step ladder ranging from grade school or less misconduct.
(coded 1) to post-graduate (coded 6), victim of crime (1 = Frequent viewers of network news and crime solving
victim, 0 = non-victim), victim of violent crime (1 = violent shows were more likely to report that police misconduct
crime victim, 0 = nonviolent crime victim), and charged/ was a common or frequent occurrence. The findings also
arrested for a crime (1 = charged/arrested, 0 = never revealed that race and experience within criminal justice
charged/arrested), concern about property crime and fear system were significant predictors of attitudes toward
of violent crime measured on a four-step ladder ranging police misconduct. White respondents were more likely
from not at all concerned (coded 1) to very concerned to report that police misconduct was rare, whereas
(coded 4), perception of serious crime in America and respondents that had been charged or arrested believed
within the neighborhood measured with a four-step ladder that police misconduct was common. In addition, male
ranging from not at all serious (coded 1) to very serious respondents, higher educated respondents, and respon-
(coded 4), and accuracy of crime shows measured on a dents concerned about property crime were more likely
four-step ladder ranging from very inaccurately (coded 1) to perceive lower levels of police misconduct. Con-
to very accurately (coded 4). versely, respondents who reported that there were se-
rious crime problems within their neighborhood were
Analytic induction more likely to believe that police misconduct was
plentiful.
Linear regression (OLS) was employed to test the To enhance the analysis, interaction effects for race
relationship between media consumption and police and income were examined more closely. The results for
misconduct. Binomial (or binary) logistic regression race were presented in Models 2 and 3, and revealed that
was used to test the relationship between media con- non-White respondents that were frequent viewers of
sumption and discriminatory practices toward minor- network news were more likely to believe that police
K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203 199

Table 2
OLS estimates for the regression of perceived police misconduct on predictors
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Full sample White Non-White N$50,000 b$50,000
SE β SE β SE β SE β SE B
Crime shows
Police drama .03 −.01 .04 .02 .08 − .08 .06 .02 .04 − .02
Crime solving .05 .08⁎ .05 .06 .10 .13 .07 .16⁎⁎ .06 .03
Police reality .05 −.04 .05 −.04 .10 − .06 .08 − .04 .06 − .03

Television news
Network .04 .09⁎ .05 .01 .09 .35⁎⁎ .07 .02 .06 .12⁎
Local .04 −.04 .05 −.04 .09 − .09 .07 − .06 .06 − .01
News magazines .05 .02 .06 .05 .12 − .14 .09 .01 .02 .02
Cable .04 .01 .04 −.00 .08 .07 .06 .00 .05 .01
PBS .05 −.03 .06 −.03 .11 .04 .08 − .03 .07 − .03

Demographic
Age of respondent .00 −.00 .00 .01 .01 − .02 .01 .05 .00 − .07
White .13 −.18⁎⁎ – – – – .21 − .23⁎⁎ .17 − .15⁎⁎
Male .11 −.07⁎ .12 −.08⁎ .25 − .05 .18 − .09 .14 − .06
Years of education .05 −.09⁎⁎ .05 −.09⁎ .11 − .12 .08 − .02 .06 − .11
Income .05 .03 .05 .02 .12 .07 – – – –

Control
Victim of crime .11 .03 .12 .01 .25 .16⁎ .17 − .00 .14 .05
Victim of violent crime .20 .03 .22 .04 .46 .02 .35 − .02 .24 .05
Charged or arrested .13 .16⁎⁎ .15 .18⁎⁎ .29 .10 .21 .16⁎⁎ .17 .15⁎⁎
Concern for violent crime .06 .03 .07 .01 .11 − .07 .35 − .02 .07 .02
Concern for property crime .05 −.12⁎⁎ .06 −.13⁎⁎ .13 − .06 .08 − .17⁎⁎ .07 − .08
Serious crime in country .10 .04 .11 .04 .21 .06 .15 .02 .13 .06
Serious crime in neighborhood .07 .09⁎⁎ .07 .08⁎ .14 .12 .10 .10 .09 .08
Accuracy of crime shows .06 −.04 .07 −.05 .15 .05 .10 − .01 .09 − .06
R-square .14 .10 .19 .16 .14
Adjusted R-square .12 .08 .10 .12 .11
N 996 796 199 397 598
⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .01.

misconduct was common. The findings for income were of Whites by the police. Not surprisingly, White res-
presented in Models 4 and 5. The results showed that pondents believed that Whites were not treated pre-
respondents that earned more than $50,000 who ferentially by the police, while non-Whites disagreed.
watched crime solving shows were more likely to Male respondents believed that Whites received no
believe that police misconduct was frequent. Similarly, preferential treatment, while higher educated respon-
respondents that earned less than $50,000 who watched dents disagreed. Finally, respondents charged or arrested
high levels of network news were more likely to be- were more likely to agree that Whites were treated better
lieve that police misconduct was frequent. Interestingly, by the police. Interaction effects were examined in
income level had no effect for White respondents, who Models 2 and 3. Interestingly, minority respondents that
regardless of income level, believed that police mis- frequently watched network news were more likely to
conduct was infrequent. believe that Whites were treated favorably by the police,
Table 3 presents the logistic regression results. while non-White respondents that frequently watched
Model 1 included the full sample and the results showed police reality programs disagreed that Whites were
no significant relationships between preferential treat- treated favorably by the police.
ment of Whites and media consumption. There were, Model 4 examined perceived treatment of the wealthy
however, significant relationships between race, gender, by the police. Multivariate findings revealed that fre-
education, charged/arrested, and preferential treatment quent viewers of police drama shows were more likely to
200 K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203

Table 3
Odds ratios (Exp. B) for perceived preferential treatment of Whites and the wealthy on predictors
Whites treated better by police Wealthy treated better by police
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
Full sample White Non-White Full sample N$50,000 b$50,000
Crime shows Exp. B Exp. B Exp. B Exp. B Exp. B Exp. B
Police drama 1.06 1.07 1.03 1.13⁎ 1.24⁎ 1.08
Crime solving 0.98 0.94 1.19 0.85⁎ 0.77⁎ 0.89
Police reality 0.89 0.95 0.66⁎ 1.01 1.23 0.91

Television news
Network 1.07 0.99 1.56⁎ 1.01 0.89 1.08
Local 0.91 0.91 0.80 1.02 1.03 1.05
News magazines 1.13 1.19 0.87 1.00 1.04 0.95
Cable 1.12 1.10 1.17 0.99 1.06 0.98
PBS 0.92 0.89 1.16 1.00 1.13 0.94

Demographic
Age of respondent 0.99 1.01 0.97⁎ 1.01 1.03 1.00
White 0.43⁎⁎ – – 0.78 0.32⁎⁎ 1.15
Male 0.69⁎ 0.65⁎ 1.16 1.31 1.68 0.98
Years of education 1.20⁎ 1.25⁎⁎ 1.09 1.22⁎⁎ 1.23 1.19
Income 0.97 0.96 0.98 0.89 – –

Control
Victim of crime 0.92 0.89 1.10 1.23 0.83 1.72⁎
Victim of violent crime 0.88 0.90 0.85 0.80 1.43 0.61
Charged or arrested 1.57⁎ 1.83⁎⁎ 0.96 1.56⁎ 1.60 1.76
Concern for violent crime 0.89 0.93 0.75 0.91 0.75 0.97
Concern for property crime 0.92 0.89 1.26 0.88 0.76⁎ 0.96
Serious crime in country 1.25 1.21 1.47 1.39⁎ 1.44 1.50⁎
Serious crime in neighborhood 0.97 0.97 0.90 1.02 1.29 0.88
Accuracy of crime shows 1.00 1.01 1.25 0.87 0.42 0.85
Cox and Snell R-square 0.06 0.05 0.12 0.04 0.11 0.05
Nagelkerke R-square 0.08 0.07 0.20 0.07 0.16 0.09
N 790 628 162 969 393 576
⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .01.

believe that police treated the wealthy better, while Discussion


frequent viewers of crime solving shows believed
that there was no preferential treatment given to the Overall, the results suggested that the media had little
wealthy. impact on attitudes toward police misconduct and
Other significant relationships included education, discrimination. Nevertheless, there were several find-
charged/arrested, and perception of crime in the country. ings that require further explanation.
Respondents that were charged by the police, highly Compared to other types of news, the consumption of
educated, and believed that crime was a serious problem network news had the most impact on respondents'
in the country were more likely to believe that the wealthy attitudes toward police misconduct and discrimination.
received better treatment by the police. Interaction effects Heavy viewers of network news were more likely to
for income were examined in Models 5 and 6, and believe that police misconduct was a frequent occurrence.
revealed that the previous media effects were stronger for Police abuse or misconduct was frequently reported by the
respondents who earned more than $50,000. Respondents media (Lawrence, 2000). It was highly likely that heavy
who earned more than $50,000 who frequently watched viewers of network news would be more aware of these
police dramas were more likely to agree that the wealthy incidents. In terms of non-White respondents, it appeared
were treated better by police, while those that frequently that the consumption of network news influenced their
watched crime solving shows disagreed. attitudes more than White respondents, which was
K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203 201

consistent with prior research (Weitzer & Tuch, 2004). As a result, these types of shows might reproduce the
Many high-profile incidents of police misconduct in- minority threat hypothesis in a manner that seemed fair,
volved non-White victims. Consequently, it was possible unbiased, and palatable to some minority viewers.
that non-White audience members might be more affected In a similar vein, older non-Whites were more likely
by these incidents. In essence, including race was an to disagree that Whites were treated better by the police.
important element in the examination of media effects. This supported both the minority threat hypothesis and
The results showed that minority respondents who the resonance/affinity thesis. Generally, older minorities
reported heavy consumption of network news were do not experience as much negative contact with the
more likely to believe that the police treated Whites better police, which might explain their more positive view of
than minorities. This finding was very important and police treatment towards minorities and helped support
might partially explain the gap between White and the resonance/affinity hypothesis. Similarly, as younger
minority attitudes toward the police. Consequently, it is minorities were often considered the symbolic assailant,
vital that future research include media consumption as a older minorities might also subscribe to these stereo-
possible determinant within racial differences in attitudes types and believe that non-Whites were not treated un-
toward the police, as it is often overlooked (Weitzer & fairly within the system, which supported the minority
Tuch, 2004). threat hypothesis.
As previously suggested, the minority threat hypoth- The viewing of crime shows had little impact on
esis and the subsequent police response might create attitudes toward police misconduct and discrimination.
hostility and antagonism towards police by minority There was, however, an interesting relationship between
community members. The findings revealed that non- the viewing of police dramas, crime solving shows, and
Whites that watched television news were more sus- attitudes toward police treatment of the wealthy. Respon-
ceptible to these messages, in that they were more likely dents that were heavy consumers of police dramas were
to believe that police misconduct was frequent and that more likely to believe that the wealthy were treated fa-
Whites were treated better by the police. As a result, vorably by the police. Conversely, respondents that were
both the resonance and affinity thesis might partially heavy consumers of crime solving shows were less likely
explain the racial differences in media effects. to believe that the wealthy were treated favorably by the
Conversely, the resonance/affinity thesis was reversed police. Interestingly, the media effects were stronger for
for non-White viewers of police reality shows. Non- respondents who earned more than $50,000, which sup-
Whites who frequently watched police reality shows were ported the resonance/affinity thesis. Wealthier respon-
more likely to disagree that Whites were not treated better dents might have a better understanding of their treatment
by the police. The content of police reality shows might by the police and might be more impacted by media
partially explain why non-Whites felt this way. The ‘law presentations because they could relate to characters that
and order’ approach was a prominent theme within this had higher social status.
genre (Cavender, 1998; Cavender & Fishman, 1998; The difference in media effects between police drama
Donovan, 1998; Doyle, 2003; Surette, 2007). In effect, and crime solving shows was an interesting contradiction,
Doyle (1998) argued that viewers who watched police which might be explained by the varying content within
reality shows saw these types of programs as more crime shows. Crime solving shows such as America's
informative than as entertainment. As mentioned previ- Most Wanted or Unsolved Mysteries might provide
ously, the common theme in these ‘reality’ shows was that audience members with economically diverse suspects
aggressive law enforcement was required because of the or offenders. The viewing audience might believe that
increasing dangerousness and prevalence of crime higher social class offenders were not provided with any
(Surette, 2007). As a result, non-Whites that watched legal breaks or favors. Essentially, the purpose of these
these types of shows might believe that society needed to programs were to apprehend suspects, therefore, prefer-
have tougher crime control and that due process and civil ential treatment would be absent. Conversely, crime
rights might be part of the problem. In addition, a highly drama audience members might be exposed to episodes in
satisfying form of justice was played out within these which wealthy or high status offenders received better
shows, essentially these reality shows provided a form of treatment. For instance, the suspects/offenders might have
justice that lulled the general public into thinking the received better legal representation or the police/prose-
police officers were not biased, and treated all criminals in cutor had more difficulty in the arrest or prosecution.
a similar manner regardless of race. The producers of There had been a number of important studies
these shows, however, could easily manipulate the seg- that examined the content of police related dramas
ments to make it appear more balanced (Donovan, 1998). (see Dominick, 1973; Eschholz, Mallard, & Flynn,
202 K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203

2004; Inciardi & Dee, 1987; Lichter & Lichter, 1983; Doob, A., & Macdonald, G. (1979). Television viewing and fear of
Soulliere, 2004) and police reality shows (see Doyle, victimization: Is the relationship causal? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 37, 170−179.
2003; Fishman & Cavender, 1998; Oliver, 1994). There Dowler, K. (2002). Media influence on citizen attitudes toward police
is, however, a need for more research within this area, effectiveness. Policing and Society: An International Journal of
especially given the proliferation of crime shows on Research and Policy, 12, 227−238.
television (Eschholz et al., 2004). Therefore, it is pru- Doyle, A. (1998). “Cops”: Television policing as policing reality. In
M. Fishman & G. Cavender (Eds.), Entertaining crime: Tele-
dent to conduct systematic content analyses of tele-
vision reality programs (pp. 95−116). New York: Aldine de
vision crime shows, to help explain the validity of the Gruyter.
above conclusions. Essentially, theories about televi- Doyle, A. (2003). Arresting images: Crime and police in front of the
sion police portrayals need to be fully developed within television camera. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of
the context of police dramas, police reality, and crime Toronto Press.
solving shows. Ericson, R., Baranek, P., & Chan, J. (1987). Visualizing deviance.
Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press.
In conclusion, any examination of media impact on Eschholz, S., Blackwell, B., Gertz, M., & Chiricos, T. (2002). Race and
public attitudes is complicated by the perception of attitudes toward the police: Assessing the effects of watching
media content. It is inherently difficult to sort through the “reality” police programs. Journal of Criminal Justice, 30, 327−341.
complexities in police portrayals on television. Without Eschholz, S., Mallard, M., & Flynn, S. (2004). Images of prime time
doubt, police portrayals are juxtaposed with both nega- justice: A content analysis of “NYPD Blue” and “Law and Order.”
Journal of Criminal Justice and Popular Culture, 10, 161−180.
tive and positive images, and it is within this contradic- Estep, R., & MacDonald, P. (1984). How prime-time crime evolved
tory framework that public attitudes toward police abuse on TV, 1976–1983. In R. Surette (Ed.), Justice and the media
or misconduct are shaped. Interestingly, this study found (pp. 110−123). Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas.
that media consumption, in the form of network news, Fishman, M. (1981). Police news: Constructing an image of crime.
was related to attitudes toward the frequency of police Urban Life, 9, 371−394.
Fishman, M., & Cavender, G. (1998). Entertaining crime: Television
misconduct. Similarly, consumption of crime drama and reality programs. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
crime solving shows had opposite effects on attitudes Fiske, J. (1986). Television: Polysemy and popularity. Critical Studies
toward the preferential treatment of the wealthy. in Mass Communication, 3, 391−408.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., &
Signorielli, N. (1978). Cultural indicators: Violence profile number
References nine. Journal of Communication, 28, 176−180.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1980). The
Blumler, J. G., & Katz, E. (1974). The uses of mass communication. mainstreaming of America: Violence profile number eleven.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Journal of Communication, 30, 10−29.
Carlson, J. (1985). Prime time law enforcement. New York: Praeger. Graber, D. (1980). Crime news and the public. New York: Praeger.
Carragee, K. M. (1990). Interpretive media study and interpretive Gunter, B. (1987). Television and the fear of crime. London: John
social science. Critical Studies in Mass Communication, 7, 81−95. Libbey.
Cavender, G. (1998). In the shadow of shadows: Television reality Hall, S. (1980). Culture, media and language. London: Hutchinson.
crime programming. In M. Fishman & G. Cavender (Eds.), En- Hall, S., Critcher, C., Jefferson, T., Clarke, J., & Roberts, B. (1978).
tertaining crime: Television reality programs (pp. 79−94). New Policing the crisis: Mugging, the state, and law and order. London:
York: Aldine de Gruyter. Macmillan.
Cavender, G., & Fishman, M. (1998). Television reality crime programs: Huang, W. S. W., & Vaughn, M. S. (1996). Support and confidence:
Context and history. In M. Fishman & G. Cavender (Eds.), Enter- Public attitudes toward the police. In T. J. Flanagan & D. R.
taining crime: Television reality programs (pp. 1−18). New York: Longmire (Eds.), Americans view crime and justice: A national
Aldine de Gruyter. public opinion survey (pp. 31−45). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Chiricos, T., Eschholz, S., & Gertz, M. (1997). Crime, news, and fear Inciardi, J., & Dee, J. (1987). From the Keystone Cops to Miami Vice:
of crime: Toward an identification of audience effects. Social Images of policing in American popular culture. Journal of
Problems, 44, 342−357. Popular Culture, 21, 84−102.
Chiricos, T., Padgett, K., & Gertz, M. (2000). Fear, TV news and the Jackson, A. L., & Boyd, L. M. (2005). Minority-threat hypothesis and the
reality of crime. Criminology, 38, 755−785. workload hypothesis: A community-level examination of lenient
Christensen, J., Schmidt, J., & Henderson, J. (1982). The selling of the policing in high crime communities. Criminal Justice Studies, 18,
police: Media, ideology and crime control. Contemporary Crises, 29−50.
6, 227−239. Jensen, K. B. (1990). Television futures: A social action methodology
Dominick, J. R. (1973). Crime and law enforcement on prime time for study interpretive communities. Critical Studies in Mass
television. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 241−250. Communication, 7, 129−146.
Donovan, P. (1998). Armed with the power of television: Reality crime Kaminski, R., & Jefferis, E. (1998). The effect of a violent televised
programming and the reconstruction of law and order in the United arrest on public perceptions of the police. Policing, 21, 683−706.
States. In M. Fishman & G. Cavender (Eds.), Entertaining crime: Kane, R. J. (2003). Social control in the metropolis: A community-
Television reality programs (pp. 117−140). New York: Aldine de level examination of the minority group-threat hypothesis. Justice
Gruyter. Quarterly, 20, 265−295.
K. Dowler, V. Zawilski / Journal of Criminal Justice 35 (2007) 193–203 203

Kooistra, P. G., Mahoney, J. S., & Westervelt, S. D. (1998). The world Roberts, J., & Doob, A. (1990). News media influences on public
according to “Cops.” In M. Fishman & G. Cavender (Eds.), En- views of sentencing. Law and Human Behavior, 12, 451−478.
tertaining crime: Television reality programs (pp. 141−158). New Roshier, B. (1973). The selection of crime news by the press. In S.
York: Aldine de Gruyter. Cohen & J. Young (Eds.), The manufacture of news (pp. 28−39).
Lawrence, R. (2000). The politics of force: Media and the construction Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
of police brutality. Berkley: University of California Press. Sacco, V., & Fair, B. (1988). Images of legal control: Crime news and
Lewis-Beck, M. (1990). Applied regression analysis. Beverly Hills, the process of organizational legitimation. Canadian Journal of
CA: Sage. Communication, 32, 114−123.
Lichter, L. S., & Lichter, S. R. (1983). Prime time crime. Washington, Schafer, J. A., Huebner, B. M., & Bynum, T. S. (2003). Citizen
DC: Media Institute. perceptions of police services: Race, neighborhood context, and
Maguire, B. (1988). Image vs. reality: An analysis of prime-time community policing. Police Quarterly, 6, 440−468.
television crime and police programs. Crime and Justice, 11, Sigleman, L., Welch, S., Bledsoe, T., & Combs, M. (1997). Police
165−188. brutality and public perceptions of racial discrimination. Political
Marsh, H. (1991). A comparative analysis of crime coverage in Research Quarterly, 50, 777−791.
newspapers in the United States and other countries from 1960 to Skogan, W., & Maxfield, M. (1981). Coping with crime. Beverly Hills,
1989: A review of the literature. Journal of Criminal Justice, 19, CA: Sage.
67−80. Soulliere, D. (2004). Policing on prime time: A comparison of TV
Maybin, J. (2001). Language, struggle and voice: The Bakhtin/ policing and real world policing. American Journal of Criminal
Volosinov writings. In M. Wetherall, S. Tayler, & S. J. Yates (Eds.), Justice, 28, 215−234.
Discourse theory and practice: A reader (pp. 64−71). Thousand Surette, R. (2007). Media, crime, and criminal justice: Images,
Oaks, CA: Sage. realities and policies (3rd ed.). New York: Wadsworth.
McQuail, D. (1987). Mass communication theory: An introduction Tuch, S., & Weitzer, R. (1997). Racial differences in attitudes toward
(2nd ed.). London: Sage. the police. Public Opinion Quarterly, 61, 642−663.
Oliver, M. (1994). Portrayals of crime, race, and aggression in reality Weitzer, R. (2002). Incidents of police misconduct and public opinion.
based police shows. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, Journal of Criminal Justice, 20, 397−408.
38, 179−192. Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. A. (2004). Race and perceptions of police
Oliver, M. B., & Armstrong, G. B. (1998). The color of crime: misconduct. Social Problems, 51, 305−325.
Perceptions of Caucasians' and African Americans' involvement Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. A. (2005a). Determinants of public satisfaction
in crime. In M. Fishman & G. Cavender (Eds.), Entertaining with the police. Police Quarterly, 8, 279−297.
crime: Television reality programs (pp. 19−36). New York: Aldine Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. A. (2005b). Racially biased policing:
de Gruyter. Determinants of citizen perceptions. Social Forces, 83, 1009−1030.
Perlmutter, D. (2000). Policing the media: Street cops and public Zillman, D., & Wakshlag, J. (1985). Fear of victimization and the
perceptions of law enforcement. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. appeal of crime drama. In D. Zillman & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective
Perse, E., Ferguson, D., & McLeod, D. (1994). Cultivation in the new exposure to communication (pp. 141−156). Hillsdale, NJ:
media environment. Communication Research, 21, 79−104. Lawrence Erlbaum.

View publication stats

You might also like