You are on page 1of 5

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Prelinguistic Communication and Subsequent


Language Acquisition in Children
With Cochlear Implants
Mary O’Leary Kane, MA, CCC-SLP; Betty Schopmeyer, MS, CCC-SLP; Nancy K. Mellon, MS;
Nae-Yuh Wang, PhD; John K. Niparko, MD

Objective: To investigate the relationship between pre- Results: We found positive, though weak, correlations
linguistic communication behaviors and subsequent lan- between prelinguistic communication skills (CSBS scores)
guage development after cochlear implantation in deaf chil- and language learning after cochlear implantation (RDLS
dren. Evaluative tools with predictive validity for language scores). Linear correlation between test results failed to
potential in very young deaf children remain elusive. reach statistical significance (receptive comparisons,
P =.17; expressive comparisons, P=.13).
Setting: A tertiary care cochlear implant center and a
preschool setting of spoken language immersion in which Conclusions: Evaluating the quality of prelinguistic
oral language development is emphasized through au- communication behaviors potentially adds important
ditory and oral motor subskill practice. predictive information to profiles of children who are
candidates for cochlear implantation. Correlative analy-
Subjects: Eighteen prelingually deaf children who un- sis suggests that early CSBS testing may provide useful
derwent unilateral implantation at an average age of 15 clinical information. Poor CSBS scores may serve as a
months also underwent testing with the Communica- precaution: if children lack an appropriate prelinguistic
tion and Symbolic Behavior Scales (CSBS) before device behavioral repertoire, the emergence of age-appropriate
activation and with the Reynell Developmental Lan- formal language may be at risk. Observations suggest
guage Scales (RDLS) at an average of 20 months after coch- that symbolic prelinguistic behaviors are necessary, but
lear implantation. not sufficient, for the development of strong linguistic
skills. The variability of behavioral measures in very
Methods: A prospective study correlated preoperative young deaf children poses challenges in designing ob-
communication behavior assessments of 18 children who jective measures with predictive value for later lan-
were candidates for cochlear implantation. We exam- guage level.
ined the value of prelinguistic behavioral testing with the
CSBS in predicting later language level after cochlear im-
plantation as reflected in RDLS scores. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2004;130:619-623

A
S DEAF CHILDREN UN- early communicative competence. A thor-
dergo implantation at ough speech and language assessment is
younger ages, tools for as- a crucial component of the cochlear im-
sessing baseline status of plant candidacy process because it guides
the ability to communi- consideration of both the appropriate-
cate with predictive value become harder ness of surgical intervention and the for-
to find. Assessing the language skills of a mat for postimplantation rehabilitation.
deaf child who has been diagnosed as hav- Therein lays the challenge: to reliably as-
From the River School, ing a severe-to-profound hearing loss re- sess a child’s communication skills at an
Washington, DC (Mss Kane, quires evaluation of communication skills age when communication behaviors are
Schopmeyer, and Mellon); and that are subserved by mid- and high- difficult to quantify reliably. If valid base-
the Departments of Medicine frequency hearing, sensory capabilities that line metrics could be found, results could
(Dr Wang) and
are invariably absent in young candi- serve as a baseline measure of communi-
Otolaryngology–Head and
Neck Surgery (Dr Niparko), dates for cochlear implantation.1 Variabil- cation, predict further growth, and direct
The Johns Hopkins University, ity in hearing, when superimposed on the course and intensity of therapy.
Baltimore, Md. The authors natural variability in developmental learn- Because of their young age and lack
have no relevant financial ing rates, presents methodological chal- of auditory experience, most deaf chil-
interest in this article. lenges in designing valid measures of very dren enter an evaluation of cochlear im-

(REPRINTED) ARCH OTOLARYNGOL HEAD NECK SURG/ VOL 130, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHOTO.COM
619
Downloaded from www.archoto.com on January 22, 2011
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
plant candidacy with little or no formal language, METHODS
signed or spoken. Ideally, at least rudimentary commu-
nication patterns develop between the child and care- The present study included 18 profoundly deaf children who
giver in naturally occurring situations such as meal- underwent implantation at the average age of 14.2 months. All
times and in play. Observation of the child in a play children received their implants at the Listening Center, The
interaction can lend considerable insight into commu- Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Md. The CSBS and the
nication skills. Prelinguistic communication behaviors, Reynell Developmental Language Scales (RDLS) were admin-
istered at the mean ages of 16 months (range, 10-22 months)
such as pointing, gesturing, object manipulation, affec-
and 36 months (range, 17-56 months), respectively. Each of
tive signaling, turn-taking, and joint attention, can be the subjects received either a Clarion device (platinum series;
assessed in play. Verbalizations and imitations may also Advanced Bionics Corp, Sylmar, Calif) or a Nucleus 24-
be analyzed as approximations of true words. In a play channel device (Cochlear Corp, Englewood, Colo). All chil-
environment, receptive and expressive language can be dren were prelingually deaf and participated in a preimplan-
evaluated when the examiner gives directions, asks for tation trial period with binaural hearing aids. The children in
labels, and encourages verbal interactions.2 the study presented without gross motor difficulties, and En-
Without access to sound, rudimentary means are glish was the primary language in all of the households. Cog-
used to establish a topic and to maintain an interaction, nitive levels were not assessed. Given the range of develop-
often with a physical object used to cue the interaction. mental levels of the subjects, speech recognition levels were
classified for means of comparison.
For example, a child may request milk by holding a cup
Therapeutic intervention varied. All children participated
toward his or her mother or engage in a playful ex- in weekly postimplantation therapy with an oral (re)habi-
change when bubbles are blown by looking toward the litation specialist for at least 12 months. Some children con-
bubbles and then giggling and looking at his or her tinued therapy for longer periods. Family involvement and
mother. Both examples offer information about the support crossed a wide range. Therefore, the variation in the
child’s ability to connect socially and to communicate intensity of both formal rehabilitative and informal family-
basic needs and emotions. Data can be gleaned through based communication training was considerable, as is repre-
observation of a child’s variety of strategies and number sentative of all cases involving children who receive implant
of exchanges. For the very young child with hearing services in our center and educational training in our school.
loss who lacks proficiency in sign or spoken language, However, all children received school-based support of their
spoken language learning. The central research question we
play offers a practical and theoretically compelling ve-
pursued related to within-subject baseline vs posttreatment
hicle with which to observe natural interactions. In the scores using age-appropriate tests. Thus, we believe that this
present study, we used the Communication and Sym- analysis withstands potential intersubject variability in experi-
bolic Behavior Scales (CSBS) to assess constructs re- ence and environmental factors related to oral language learn-
lated to modeling communication behaviors reflecting ing.
communicative function, gestural means, vocalizations, All subjects were evaluated using the CSBS and the RDLS.
and verbalizations. The CSBS assesses communicative, social-affective, and sym-
Later, when the child is a cochlear implant user, it bolic abilities of children whose functional communication age
is imperative to assess his or her skills in developing lin- is between 8 months and 2 years.2 Prelinguistic communica-
guistic abilities. Activation of the cochlear implant tion is rated in 22 scales measuring parameters of communi-
cative competence and aspects of symbolic development. A struc-
should allow the child access to auditory information
tured play session is videotaped, and the child’s behaviors are
and provide benefit from verbal language models. Lan- analyzed to assess communicative function, noting gestural
guage therapy in the context of home or school experi- means, vocalizations, verbalizations, reciprocity, social-
ence can support receptive and expressive language affective signaling, and symbolic expressions in play. Stan-
growth of the new implant user. However, to gauge a dard scores are compared to norms for hearing children based
child’s progress, formal evaluations are needed. For ex- on chronological age or language stage. Six months after acti-
ample, a measure that indicates that a child is not mak- vation (and at subsequent 6-month intervals), each child was
ing 6 months’ progress in 6 months’ time can prompt tested using the RDLS.
critical assessment of the environment in which the de- The RDLS measures verbal comprehension and expres-
vice is being used, as well as possible changes in device sive language abilities in children between the ages of 12 months
and 5 years.3 Pictures, objects, and observations are used to as-
programming. Information gathered in subsequent test-
sess each child’s understanding of verbal preconcepts, at-
ing after implantation can direct language intervention, tributes, and verbal reasoning. Expressively, each child’s re-
particularly when baseline preintervention information sponses are analyzed with respect to the structure, vocabulary,
is available. and content of the message. Results report standard scores, per-
The objective of the present study was to investi- centile rank, and developmental levels.3 The RDLS has been used
gate the relationship between prelinguistic communica- successfully in this population to track language development
tion behaviors in implantation candidates and subse- after implantation. Robbins et al4 and Svirsky et al5 applied the
quent language development after cochlear implant use. RDLS to populations of deaf children, with and without coch-
We evaluated metrics that are amenable to early child- lear implants, and found that the rate of language develop-
hood stages, when candidacy evaluation for cochlear im- ment in children with implants exceeded that expected from
children without implants.
plantation in prelingual deafness is commonly per-
We hypothesized that there would be correlations be-
formed, and later, with cochlear implant experience. We tween early communication behaviors (CSBS) and later lan-
determined the predictive validity of prelinguistic com- guage (RDLS) scores. That is, performance on the CSBS would
munication behaviors in forecasting language outcomes likely correlate with both receptive and expressive standard
after implantation. scores on the RDLS. Specifically, we postulated that the CSBS

(REPRINTED) ARCH OTOLARYNGOL HEAD NECK SURG/ VOL 130, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHOTO.COM
620
Downloaded from www.archoto.com on January 22, 2011
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
verbal and vocal subtest scores would correlate with the over-
130
all expressive language score on the RDLS. The reciprocity sub-
test scores on the CSBS was predicted to correlate with the re-
ceptive language scores on the RDLS. Statistical treatment of 120
CSBS and RDLS scores was designed to assess linear correla-
tions and to derive Pearson correlation coefficients between test
measures. 110

Receptive RDLS Score


RESULTS
100
The mean CSBS standard score was 84.5, and the mean
overall RDLS standard score was 69.5 (mean RDLS re-
90
ceptive score, 66.0; mean RDLS expressive score, 73.1).
The CSBS scores are plotted against the receptive and ex-
pressive scores on the RDLS in Figure 1 and Figure 2, 80
respectively. The Pearson correlation coefficients that were
observed between the CSBS and RDLS subtest results are
shown below. 70

Test RDLS Receptive RDLS Expressive


CSBS vocal 0.09 60
CSBS verbal 0.31 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 100 105
CSBS reciprocity 0.13 Overall CSBS Score
CSBS overall 0.41 0.36
Figure 1. Bivariate analysis of overall scores on the Communication and
The CSBS scores, when compared with the later re- Symbolic Behavior Scales (CSBS) and the receptive Reynell Developmental
ceptive RDLS scores, demonstrated a correlation coeffi- Language Scales (RDLS) showing the calculated line of regression and 95%
confidence limits (curvilinear lines). These measures demonstrated the
cient of 0.41 (P=.17). Comparing the overall CSBS scores highest correlation (correlation coefficient, 0.41; P = .17).
with the expressive RDLS scores generated a lower but
still positive correlation coefficient of 0.36 (P=.13). Within
subtests, we observed positive correlations between the in early language development rather than measure-
overall scores on each test and between the verbal CSBS ment deficiencies.7
subtest and the expressive RDLS scores. Weak but posi- High variability in testing communication capabili-
tive correlations were observed in the CSBS vocaliza- ties is particularly likely when early childhood subjects
tion subtest and expressive RDLS scores and in the reci- exhibit severe-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss, as
procity subtest of the CSBS and receptive RDLS scores. these capabilities normally rely on a sense that is vari-
Correlations between CSBS and RDLS overall and ably impaired across subjects. Furthermore, the study de-
subtest means failed to reach statistical significance. How- sign could not exclude variables related to differences in
ever, coefficients were consistently positive. Consider- cognition and different stages of development assessed
ing the difficulty in quantifying end points related to pre- before and after intervention, although age-norming of
linguistic behaviors, we considered the consistently scores ameliorates the latter concern. Thus, we believe
positive correlation coefficients to be potentially mean- that the positive correlations that were observed in our
ingful. Correlation coefficients approaching or exceed- study provide grounds for the continued assessment of
ing 0.40 are considered as 1 criterion of a meaningful as- the CSBS metric of early communication behaviors in
sociation between developmental milestones in common, young implantation candidates.
multimodal testing of 6- to 42-month-old children.6 Pragmatic skills enable dynamic communication
exchanges and thus would seem to be a key predictor of
COMMENT succeeding language learning potential. By 8 or 9
months of age, based on a foundation of phonological
The present study assessed the feasibility and accuracy awareness, children enter the stage of development
of an early characterization of communication behav- when they intentionally perform specific behaviors to
iors in predicting later language level. In general, we accomplish desired social functions.2,8,9 Nonverbal
observed positive correlations between early, baseline mea- means are used by children to communicate intention
sures and later, postinterventional outcomes; ie, age- (eg, looking at a ball, reaching out, and then looking
normed early CSBS scores and later RDLS scores showed again toward the parent), joint attention (eg, smiling
positive linear correlations. The variability in test re- while looking at the face of a parent and then looking at
sults, both before and after intervention, was substan- a shared toy/action), and protest (eg, showing dissatis-
tial. Clearly, such variance is to be expected in testing faction through facial expressions and vocalizations).
highly integrated functions, such as receptive and ex- Formal interactive speech and language skills begin to
pressive language, in young listeners. Investigators have replace prelinguistic behaviors. In normal develop-
observed high variability in assessing other inventories ment, competence with prelinguistic communication
of early childhood development. The high variance that appears to be both a precursor and a prerequisite to ap-
has been observed with some metrics of language level propriate speech and language use.10,11 In the present
is thought to accurately represent individual differences study, we assessed constructs related to pragmatics of

(REPRINTED) ARCH OTOLARYNGOL HEAD NECK SURG/ VOL 130, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHOTO.COM
621
Downloaded from www.archoto.com on January 22, 2011
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
105
between the CSBS and the RDLS within the context of
the present study.
Poor performance on the CSBS may serve as a pre-
100
caution. Very low CSBS scores tended strongly toward
low RDLS scores. These preliminary observations sug-
95
gest that if children lack appropriate prelinguistic
behaviors, the emergence of age-appropriate formal lan-
90
guage may be at risk. Low scores on the CSBS may
Expressive RDLS Score

reflect limitations in symbolic representations and a


85
need for significant therapeutic support of behaviors
that facilitate communication. Postimplantation habili-
80
tation should address prelinguistic skills to foster prag-
matic as well as auditory and linguistic development.
75
Multidisciplinary evaluations are recommended to
assess cognitive, physical, and other sensory delays on
70 an individual basis.
The converse was not true, however. High perfor-
65 mance on the CSBS showed poor predictive value for sub-
sequent RDLS scores. One possible explanation for this
60
65 70 75 80 85 90 95 100 105
observation is that CSBS and RDLS measures reflect ca-
Overall CSBS Score pabilities that, though age-appropriate, represent differ-
ent receptive and expressive capabilities. For example,
Figure 2. Bivariate analysis of overall scores on the Communication and approximately 15 of the 22 subtests on the CSBS reflect
Symbolic Behavior Scales (CSBS) and the expressive Reynell Developmental visual abilities, while performance on the RDLS de-
Language Scales (RDLS) showing the calculated line of regression and 95% pends more heavily on auditory abilities. Adding other
confidence limits (curvilinear lines). These measures demonstrated a
correlation coefficient of 0.36 (P=.13). measures of auditory performance to the assessment pro-
tocol after implantation may lend further insight into fac-
tors that contribute to postimplantation language growth.
communication, including reciprocity, social-affective Future assessments of the CSBS should better account
signaling, and symbolic expressions in play, as inte- for developmental variables related to cognition and en-
grated into the CSBS battery of tests. We therefore vironment. For example, it would be helpful to identify
tested the hypothesis that preimplantation pragmatics, characteristics that contrast the subgroup scores with high
as tested with the CSBS battery of tests, would predict CSBS, low RDLS scores, thus enabling stronger conclu-
oral language learning after implantation. sions about conditions in which performance on the CSBS
Later, skills were assessed with the RDLS through predicts RDLS scores.
observation and documentation of formal language skills. The present study investigated the prelinguistic be-
The examiner used miniature figurines to enact familiar haviors that predict later communicative competence. Be-
scenes (eg, a mother comforting her baby, a parent serv- haviors, including joint attention, positive affect, and ges-
ing dinner, or a farmer caring for animals), and the child tures, should be considered in cochlear implantation
was expected to answer questions or to describe the scenes candidacy evaluations. However, such behaviors are
depicted. Basic understanding of social norms is needed often subtle and compose a challenging set of measures
to perform these tasks. Engagement with the examiner for formal, quantitative assessment. The analysis of
is also required to complete the RDLS. The test is lengthy, functional communication through emergent play and
as props are presented. Without appropriate eye con- social-interactive skills appears to be an informative al-
tact, eye gaze, and interest in the examiner, the child will ternative, particularly in the assessment of children with
have difficulty following the standardized guidelines. severe-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss. Use of a
Therefore, the CSBS and the RDLS assess corollary be- standardized measure to quantify prelinguistic skills al-
haviors that, in the context of normal developmental prac- lows examiners to formally assess very young children.
tice and learning, contribute to verbal communication The information provided by the CSBS results reflected
competence. the importance of symbolic prelinguistic behaviors in the
We hypothesized that the children who performed development of strong linguistic skills after implanta-
well on the CSBS would demonstrate accelerated lan- tion. Preliminary data indicate that poor performance on
guage learning after cochlear implantation. Prelinguis- the CSBS is a “red flag” to members of the implantation
tic communication skills would seem to serve as a foun- team regarding later language acquisition.
dation for formal language to be learned and used Tools for measuring prelinguistic communication
effectively. As new vocabulary replaced gestures, a more behaviors may lend insight into strategies of therapeutic
sophisticated, rule-based language could be acquired. Sup- intervention. Prelinguistic assessments potentially guide
port for this hypothesis can be considered weak based the intensity of therapeutic intervention and can help to
on the present data, and further investigation is needed. set parental expectations by identifying deficits in inte-
As overall correlations of CSBS and RDLS scores did not grative or psychosocial functioning at an early stage.
reach statistical significance (P= .13), linear correlation Early assessment may also implement parent-based
does not appear to be a good measure of the association interventions that are designed to bolster prelinguistic

(REPRINTED) ARCH OTOLARYNGOL HEAD NECK SURG/ VOL 130, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHOTO.COM
622
Downloaded from www.archoto.com on January 22, 2011
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
behaviors in young children. Facilitating prelinguistic REFERENCES
communication skills in children with developmental
disabilities has been found to have a positive impact on 1. Schopmeyer B. Professional roles in multidisciplinary assessment of candi-
dacy. In: Niparko JK, Kirk KI, Mellon NK, McConkey Robbins AM, Tucci DL, Wil-
later language acquisition.12 Improvements in prelin- son BS, eds. Cochlear Implants: Principles and Practices. Philadelphia, Pa: Lip-
guistic behaviors were found to have reciprocal effects pincott Williams & Wilkins; 2000:178-181.
with teachers and other communication partners, 2. Wetherby AM, Prizant BM. Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales. Chi-
cago, Ill: Applied Symbolix; 1993.
resulting in improved communicative competence. 3. Reynell JK, Gruber CP. Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Los Angeles,
Investigators have also observed positive relationships Calif: Western Psychological Services; 1990.
between prelinguistic vocalization and later expressive 4. Robbins AM, Svirsky M, Kirk KI. Children with implants can speak, but can they
communicate? Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1997;117:155-160.
vocabulary13 and between parental responsiveness and 5. Svirsky MA, Robbins AM, Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Miyamoto RT. Language development
the effectiveness of prelinguistic communication inter- in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants. Psychol Sci. 2000;11:153-158.
vention in exerting measurable effects on language.14 6. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. San Diego, Calif: Har-
court Brace & Co; 1993.
These findings suggest that the facilitation of prelin- 7. Fenson L, Bates E, Dale P, Goodman J, Reznick JS, Thal D. Measuring variability
guistic skills in the context of the parent-child relation- in early child language: don’t shoot the messenger. Child Dev. 2000;71:323-328.
8. Thal D, Bates E. Language and communication in early childhood. Pediatr Ann.
ship is likely to carry a positive longitudinal benefit of 1989;18:299-305.
later communicative competence. 9. Jaffe J, Beebe B, Feldstein S, Crown CL, Jasnow MD. Rhythms of dialogue in
infancy: coordinated timing in development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2001;
66:i-viii, 1-132.
Submitted for publication January 16, 2004; final revision 10. Piaget J. Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. New York, NY: WW Norton;
received January 27, 2004; accepted February 2, 2004. 1954.
This study was supported in part by the Deafness Re- 11. Peller L. Libidinal phases, ego development, and play. Psychoanal Stud Child.
1954;9:178-198.
search Foundation, Washington, DC. 12. Yoder PJ, Warren SF, Kim K, Gazdag GE. Facilitating prelinguistic communica-
This study was presented at the Ninth Symposium on tion skills in young children with developmental delay, II: systematic replication
and extension. J Speech Hear Res. 1994;37:841-851.
Cochlear Implants in Children; April 25, 2003; Washing- 13. McCathren RB, Yoder PJ, Warren SF. The relationship between prelinguistic vo-
ton, DC. calization and later expressive vocabulary in young children with developmental
Corresponding author and reprints: Mary O’Leary delay. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:915-924.
14. Yoder PJ, Warren SF. Maternal responsivity predicts the prelinguistic commu-
Kane, MA, CCC-SLP, 4880 MacArthur Blvd NW, Wash- nication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional communication. J Speech
ington, DC 20007 (e-mail: molearykane@riverschool.net). Lang Hear Res. 1998;41:1207-1219.

(REPRINTED) ARCH OTOLARYNGOL HEAD NECK SURG/ VOL 130, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHOTO.COM
623
Downloaded from www.archoto.com on January 22, 2011
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

You might also like