You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [University Of Pittsburgh]

On: 26 November 2014, At: 11:23


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Personality Assessment


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

The Role of Defense Mechanisms in Borderline and


Antisocial Personalities
a a b
Michelle D. Presniak , Trevor R. Olson & Michael Wm. MacGregor
a
Institute of Community and Family Psychiatry at SMBD Jewish General Hospital,
Department of Psychiatry , McGill University , Montreal, Quebec, Canada
b
Department of Psychology , University of Saskatchewan , Saskatoon, Saskatchewan,
Canada
Published online: 10 Feb 2010.

To cite this article: Michelle D. Presniak , Trevor R. Olson & Michael Wm. MacGregor (2010) The Role of Defense Mechanisms
in Borderline and Antisocial Personalities, Journal of Personality Assessment, 92:2, 137-145, DOI: 10.1080/00223890903510373

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223890903510373

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Personality Assessment, 92(2), 137–145, 2010
Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0022-3891 print / 1532-7752 online
DOI: 10.1080/00223890903510373

The Role of Defense Mechanisms in Borderline and Antisocial


Personalities
MICHELLE D. PRESNIAK,1 TREVOR R. OLSON,1 AND MICHAEL WM. MACGREGOR2

1
Institute of Community and Family Psychiatry at SMBD Jewish General Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University,
Montreal, Quebec, Canada
2
Department of Psychology, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada

We examined whether borderline personality disorder (BPD) and antisocial personality disorder (APD) could be differentiated based on defense
mechanisms as measured by observer (Defense-Q; MacGregor, Olson, Presniak, & Davidson, 2008) and self-report (Defense Style Questionnaire;
Andrews, Singh, & Bond, 1993) measures. We conducted 2 studies whereby nonclinical participants were divided into borderline and antisocial
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

groups based on scores from the Personality Assessment Inventory (Morey, 1991). Multivariate analysis of variance results revealed significant
overall group differences in defense use. Univariate analyses further showed group differences on several individual defenses (e.g., acting out,
denial, and turning against self). Together, the findings suggest that in BPD, the defenses may emphasize interpersonal dependency and a tendency
to direct aggression toward the self; whereas in APD, the defenses may emphasize egocentricity, interpersonal exploitation, and a tendency to
direct aggression toward others. Overall, this study demonstrates important differences in defense use between borderline and antisocial personality
groups across both observer and self-report measures.

Recently, there has been a renewed interest in the empirical study cessive anxiety (e.g., Vaillant, 1994). Defenses are viewed as
of defense mechanisms and the role they play in many aspects both protectors against unconscious feelings and as cognitive
of psychosocial functioning (e.g., Hilsenroth, Hibbard, Nash, and relational behaviors that protect the self (Cooper, 1998).
& Handler, 1993; MacGregor, Davidson, Barksdale, Black, & At present, there is no consensus on the number of defenses
MacLean, 2003). Defenses are now included in the Diagnos- that exist. Anna Freud (1936/1986) discussed 10 defenses in
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text “The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense,” and there has since
rev. [DSM–IV–TR]; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) been an expansion of recognized defenses. For example, Vaillant
as a diagnostic axis for further study. Empirical evidence has (1977) included 18 in his original hierarchy of defenses, whereas
emerged demonstrating the role of defenses in childhood and the DSM–IV–TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) in-
adolescent development (Cramer, 1997), adult social function- cludes a list of 31 defenses. Despite disagreement in the number
ing (Vaillant, 1977), physical health (MacGregor et al., 2003), of defenses that exist, most authors agree on the role of defenses.
and psychopathology (Andrews, Singh, & Bond, 1993). De- Defense mechanisms work to alter internal psychological states
fenses may also be helpful in differentiating between similar (e.g., emotions, thoughts). They alter meaning or significance
mental disorders (Spinhoven & Kooiman, 1997). Cluster B per- of perceived threats, create the perception of control, reduce or
sonality disorders are some of the most difficult disorders to eliminate the experience of conscious anxiety, and thus protect
differentiate, with literature having shown overlapping criteria, and enhance the self (Ihilevich & Gleser, 1995).
high comorbidity, and low discriminant validity (e.g., Becker,
Grilo, Edell, & McGlashan, 2000). One of the highest comor-
bidity rates within this cluster is between borderline person- Assessment of Defense Mechanisms
ality disorder (BPD) and antisocial personality disorder (APD; One area of debate in defense mechanism literature centers
Becker et al., 2000; Zanarini et al., 2004). The goal of these stud- on how best to assess defenses. For example, theorists who
ies was to investigate the utility of defenses in differentiating see defenses predominantly as intrapsychic processes have ar-
borderline and antisocial personalities. gued against the use of self-report measures to assess defenses,
whereas others argue that self-report assessment has some utility
CONCEPTUALIZATION AND ASSESSMENT OF DEFENSE (Bond, Gardener, Christian, & Sigal, 1983; Cooper, 1998).
MECHANISMS Self-report defense measures require individuals to report on
Conceptualization their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, which are believed to
Current psychodynamic authors have defined defenses as un- represent aspects of various defenses (Cramer, 2006). Advan-
conscious mental operations that function to defend against ex- tages of this method are that it limits researcher bias and allows
timely collection of data in large samples. A criticism, however,
is that individuals must consciously report on their unconscious
Received February 1, 2009; Revised October 21, 2009.
behavior (Shedler, Mayman, & Manis, 1993). Because self-
Address correspondence to Michelle D. Presniak, Institute of Community and report assessment requires an awareness of thoughts, feelings,
Family Psychiatry at SMBD Jewish General Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, and behaviors, these measures are problematic for assessment
McGill University, 4333 Chemin de la cote Ste-Catherine, Montreal, Quebec, of individuals with limited self-awareness or who frequently use
H3T 1E4, Canada. Email: michelle.presniak@usask.ca defenses that distort self-evaluation (e.g., denial; Shedler et al.,
137
138 PRESNIAK, OLSON, MACGREGOR

1993). As such, most self-report defense measures use items in- differ in the expression of the underlying dynamics. For exam-
tended to assess conscious derivatives of defenses (Bond, 2004). ple, Perry and Cooper (1986) suggested that BPD is expressed
Other researchers have focused on objective observational through interpersonal dependency, whereas APD is expressed
assessment of defenses (e.g., MacGregor, Olson, Presniak, & through interpersonal exploitation and antisocial behaviors (e.g.,
Davidson, 2008; Perry, 1990). This usually involves the use of stealing). Also, although both disorders are characterized by
coding manuals to assist trained observers in the identification manipulative behavior, in BPD, it is used in interpersonal rela-
and assessment of the defenses. These measures allow for use tionships to gain concern from others; whereas in APD, it is used
of some inference by coders, which facilitates the assessment of to establish and gain material goods and power (American Psy-
unconscious material. One limitation of this approach, however, chiatric Association, 2000). Finally, although both disorders are
is that it is often labor intensive and makes data collection in characterized by aggression, Gacono, Meloy, and Berg (1992)
large samples difficult. Additionally, although good interrater suggested that the aggression may be more strongly directed
reliability can often be achieved for overall defensive function- toward the self in BPD and toward others in APD.
ing or defense levels, it can be difficult to achieve good reliability Although individuals with BPD and APD both exhibit prim-
at the individual defense level. itive defense use (e.g., splitting, denial, devaluation), many au-
Assessing defenses with more than one method of measure- thors have also stated that these groups will differ in their relative
ment can provide additional information regarding an individ- use of some defenses. For example, BPD is considered more se-
ual’s defense use. However, problems may arise when trying vere than APD within borderline personality organization and
to compare results from different measures. One common dif- should therefore be associated with higher reliance on maladap-
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

ficulty is differences in the conceptualization of a particular tive defenses (Cramer, 1999). Perry and Cooper (1986), how-
defense mechanism between the measures. For example, de- ever, proposed that two dimensions of primitive defenses exist.
valuation, as assessed by some measures (e.g., Defense Style The first dimension includes splitting of self, splitting of others,
Questionnaire [DSQ]; Andrews et al., 1993), combines both and projective identification and would be more associated with
the devaluation of self and the devaluation of others, whereas BPD because it leads to interpersonal instability and depen-
other measures only include one direction of devaluation, such dency. The second dimension includes omnipotence, primitive
as devaluating others (e.g., Defense-Q; MacGregor et al., 2008). idealization, and devaluation and would be more associated with
Additionally, the behaviors an individual can or will self-report APD because it protects the self from low self-esteem and feel-
in the context of privately filling out a questionnaire may dif- ings of anger.
fer from behaviors evident to an external observer viewing a Gacono et al. (1992) suggested some differences in the way
video-recorded interview (Bond, 1990). In turn, these behaviors these defenses relate to BPD and APD. First, Gacono et al.
may differentially relate to various aspects of psychopathology suggested that projection may be more characteristic of APD
or personality to which the defense scores are compared. Be- because the primary way these individuals can relate to others
cause of these conceptualization, measurement, or contextual is through a projection of themselves. Second, unlike Perry and
differences, it is difficult to generalize results across measures. Cooper (1986), Gacono et al. argued that many of the broader
As such, it is important to utilize multiple methods of assessing conceptualized defenses will be associated with both BPD and
defenses in any study (Davidson & MacGregor, 1998). APD such as splitting and devaluation. However, Gacono et al.
suggested that the way in which the defense is utilized differs be-
DIFFERENTIATING BPD AND APD BASED ON tween groups. For example, although both groups use splitting,
DEFENSE MECHANISMS those with BPD tend to alternate between idealization of others
and devaluation. The devaluation, however, is predominantly
Since the introduction of personality disorders into the DSM–
turned inward with negative self-thoughts, feelings, and behav-
III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), there have been
iors (i.e., devaluation of self, turning against self). Although at
many criticisms of the diagnoses, especially of those disor-
times aggression in BPD can be directed toward others, this
ders within Cluster B (e.g., Herkov & Blashfield, 1995; Widi-
often results in an exacerbated view of the self as damaged. In
ger, 1993). One major criticism is the high comorbidity and
those with APD, splitting is evident through a poorly integrated
poor discriminant validity between the Cluster B personality
identity in which they present with an overt grandiosity of the
disorders due to their overlapping criteria (Blais & Norman,
self and tendency to devalue others. Splitting, therefore, occurs
1997). BPD has one of the highest comorbidity rates with other
between a grandiose and devalued self, but this latter portion is
personality disorders, especially with APD (e.g., 26%: Becker
defended against through grandiosity and devaluation of others.
et al., 2000; Herkov & Blashfield, 1995).
Gacono and Meloy (1988) also argued that denial is an es-
pecially integral defense for individuals with APD because it
Theoretical Role of Defenses in BPD and APD prevents them from thinking about the consequences of their
According to psychodynamic theory, BPD and APD share antisocial behaviors. They additionally note that, in its more
many similarities in personality structure. Both disorders fall developmentally advanced form, denial may develop into ra-
within Kernberg’s (1984) borderline personality organization, tionalization. This occurs as the individuals mature and, rather
thus sharing primary use of primitive defenses, a lack of an in- than deny their behaviors, they will justify them through dis-
tegrated identity, and maintained reality testing. The primitive torted explanations.
defenses are thought to protect the ego by separating contra-
dictory experiences of self and others, thus preventing anxiety
related to these conflicts (Kernberg, 1984). Empirical Evidence of Defense Use in BPD and APD
Despite the similarities in psychodynamics (e.g., identity dis- Research has supported Kernberg’s (1984) hypotheses that
integration) of BPD and APD, many argue that these groups BPD is positively associated with primitive defenses (e.g.,
DEFENSE MECHANISMS AND PERSONALITY 139

denial, devaluation, idealization, projective identification, split- Only Gacono et al. (1992) previously examined differences in
ting; Berg, 1990; Hilsenroth et al., 1993). Studies have also individual defense mean scores between BPD and APD groups.
shown a positive association between APD and primitive de- In their study, however, Gacono et al. acknowledged that they
fense use (e.g., devaluation, projective identification, splitting; were limited by the defense measure used, the Lerner Defense
Chabrol & Leichsenring, 2006; Leichsenring, Kunst, & Hoyer, Scales, because the measure requires human responses on the
2003). Although both disorders are associated with primitive Rorschach. Gacono et al. (1992) noted that “too few scores were
defenses, some findings have supported that BPD may demon- produced to analyze these indices meaningfully” (p. 41).
strate higher levels of maladaptive defenses, such as acting out In these studies, we examined mean differences in individual
and passive aggression, compared to other personality disorder defense mechanisms between BPD and APD groups. In Study 1,
groups (Bond, 1990; Perry & Cooper, 1986). we assessed defenses using a self-report measure, the DSQ;
Few studies have examined the association of defenses with and in Study 2, the DSQ and an observer report measure, the
BPD and APD concurrently (e.g., Cramer, 1999; Gacono et al., Defense-Q, were used. In both studies, mean defense scores
1992). The results of these studies have shown that BPD is were compared between BPD and APD groups using multivari-
positively associated with acting out, denial, principalization, ate ANOVA (MANOVA) analyses. To avoid the small sample
reversal, and turning against self (TAS), and APD is positively limitation of previous studies, we used a self-report personal-
associated with acting out, denial, intellectualization, princi- ity measure in two large samples to identify potential partici-
palization, projection, and turning against object (Berman & pants that might approximate participants diagnosed with BPD
McCann, 1995; Cramer, 1999; Lingiardi et al., 1999; Perry & or APD. The Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI; Morey,
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

Cooper, 1986). In addition, Perry and Cooper (1986) tested their 1991) was chosen based on its good overall validity and reliabil-
hypothesis that two dimensions of primitive defenses existed. ity indexes as well as its demonstrated validity at assessing BPD
They found that the first dimension (i.e., splitting of self, split- pathology in nonclinical samples (Boone, 1998; Kurtz, Morey,
ting of others, and projective identification) was associated with & Tomarken, 1993; Trull, 1995). Group membership was as-
BPD symptoms (r = .36), whereas the second (i.e., omnipo- signed based on clinical cutoff scores from the PAI. Defense
tence, primitive idealization, and devaluation) was associated use comparisons between groups were conducted to extend pre-
with APD symptoms (r = .23). Perry and Cooper (1986) also vious research, which has primarily examined the correlations
found that the action defense level (most maladaptive level) was between many defenses and personality disorders.
associated with BPD symptoms (r = .26), and the disavowal
defense level (e.g., denial) was associated with APD symptoms Hypotheses
(r = .22).
Despite different patterns of defense use emerging in these Based on the theoretical and empirical literature described
correlational studies, only two studies have statistically com- previously, 12 hypothesized defense use differences were tested
pared the mean differences in defense use between BPD and between the BPD and APD groups. Five defenses were pro-
APD groups. Perry and Cooper (1986) compared defenses be- posed to be higher in the BPD group (acting out, idealization,
tween 10 patients with BPD and 8 patients with APD. Defenses passive aggression, splitting, and TAS), and seven defenses were
were assessed with an early version of the Defense Mechanism proposed to be higher in the APD group (denial, devaluation
Rating Scales (DMRS; Perry, 1990). Results from a canonical of other, grandiosity, intellectualization, projection, rationaliza-
discriminant function analysis revealed no significant differ- tion, and turning against others [TAO]).
ences between groups. In 1992, Gacono et al. examined the
results of previous studies that used the Lerner Defense Scales STUDY 1
(Lerner & Lerner, 1980) to assess defenses. One-way analy- Method
ses of variance (ANOVAs) revealed no significant differences
Participants. A total of 674 undergraduate students from
between BPD (n = 18) and APD (n = 21) groups, although
a Canadian university participated in this study. All students
there was a trend toward higher primitive idealization scores in
were enrolled in an introductory psychology class and received
the BPD group. Together, the results of these two studies sug-
partial course credit for participation. All participants provided
gest that these disorders could not be differentiated based on
informed consent, and the study was approved by the Research
defenses.
Ethics Board of the University of Saskatchewan. The mean age
of the sample was 19.4 years (range = 17–40; SD = 3.0). The
CURRENT RESEARCH STUDIES sample was 71.0% female, and 87.1% of the participants were
Caucasian.
Most studies that have investigated defenses and personality
disorders have examined correlations between disorders and de-
fenses. To date, however, only Gacono et al. (1992) and Perry Measures
and Cooper (1986) have statistically explored differences in PAI (Morey, 1991). The PAI is a 344-item self-report mea-
defense use between BPD and APD groups. Although no sig- sure of mental health and personality functioning, which in-
nificant results were found in either study, both studies had very cludes four Validity scales, 11 Clinical scales, five Treatment
small samples sizes. Additionally, the chosen analyses may have scales, and two Interpersonal scales (Morey, 1991). The Bor-
restricted potential findings. For example, Perry and Cooper derline Features (BOR) and Antisocial Features (ANT) Clini-
(1986) compared the groups on five defense styles rather than cal scales were used in this study. The PAI has demonstrated
individual defenses, whereas many argue that differences be- good reliability and validity in census, college, and clinical sam-
tween the groups will be seen on individual defenses and not on ples (e.g., Boyle & Lennon, 1994; Kurtz et al., 1993; Morey,
broad defense styles (e.g., Gacono et al., 1992). 1991; Walters, Duncan & Geyer, 2003). The ANT and BOR
140 PRESNIAK, OLSON, MACGREGOR

TABLE 1.—Study 1 means and standard deviations for the Defense Style Questionnaire.

Defense BPD M (SD)a APD M (SD)b F p d Reliability (No. of Items)c

Acting Out 33.68 (8.07) 27.58 (6.12) 13.23 .001 +.87 .69 (6)
Denial 5.82 (2.84) 7.92 (3.32) 8.18 .006 −.68 .19 (2)
Idealization 15.68 (4.72) 13.95 (4.82) 2.35 .129 +.37 .32 (3)
Passive Aggression 38.91 (9.13) 32.34 (7.59) 11.10 .001 +.80 .54 (8)
Projection 33.44 (9.17) 29.53 (10.84) 2.70 .105 +.39 .74 (10)
Rationalization 4.20 (2.25) 5.34 (2.35) 4.38 .040 −.50 — (1)
Splitting 11.65 (4.78) 11.42 (4.98) 0.04 .845 +.05 .56 (3)

Note. BPD = borderline personality disorder group; APD = antisocial personality disorder group.
a
n = 34. b n = 38. c Reliability calculated using Cronbach’s alpha. Reliability for rationalization is not calculated because it consists of one item.

scales have demonstrated good concurrent validity. For exam- the ANT scale), and 38 participants met criteria for the APD
ple, the BOR scale correlated with borderline scales from other group (i.e., score ≥ 70t on the ANT scale and < 70t on the
self-report measures (e.g., Minnesota Multiphasic Personality BOR scale). We excluded participants who had scores ≥ 70t on
Inventory [Hathaway & McKinley, 1943]; Kurtz et al., 1993), both the ANT and BOR scales.
and the ANT scale correlated with the Psychopathy Checklist– The mean t score for BOR in the BPD group was 74.1 (SD =
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

Revised (Hare, 1991) total scale, Factor 1 (Affective and Inter- 4.0), and in the APD group it was 58.4 (SD = 7.3). The mean t
personal Features), and Factor 2 (Behavioral Features; Walters score for ANT in the APD group was 76.1 (SD = 5.3); and in the
et al., 2003). Finally, the PAI has demonstrated good validity BPD group, it was 56.5 (SD = 7.4). The mean age of the grouped
in nonclinical samples (Boone, 1998; Kurtz et al., 1993; Trull, sample was 20.0 years (range = 18–33; SD = 3.0), and 93.2%
1995). For example, nonclinical participants who scored ≥70t were Caucasian, with no significant age or racial differences
on the BOR scale demonstrated more interpersonal problems, between groups, t(58) = 1.62, p = .11 and χ 2 (2, N = 72) =
maladaptive coping methods, academic difficulties, and psy- 2.31, p = .32, respectively. There were significant differences
chopathology in general compared to those scoring <70t (Trull, in percentage of each sex between groups, χ 2 (1, N = 72) =
1995; Trull, Useda, Conforti, & Doan, 1997). 17.51, p < .001, with more women in the BPD group (85.3%)
compared to the APD group (36.8%). Many of the participants
DSQ (Andrews et al., 1993). The DSQ is a 72-item self- in each group had elevations on other scales on the PAI. The
report questionnaire that assesses the use of 20 defense mech- most common concurrent elevations in the BPD group were on
anisms categorized into three defense styles: mature, neurotic, the Suicide (38.2%) and Depression scales (47.1%); and in the
and immature (Andrews et al., 1993). Participants rate their APD group, the most common elevations were on the Alcohol
agreement of each item on a scale ranging from 1 (no agreement) Problems (31.6%) and Aggression (15.8%) scales.
to 9 (strong agreement). Only the hypothesized defenses of act-
ing out, denial, idealization, passive aggression, projection, ra- Results
tionalization, and splitting were used in analyses. Devaluation MANOVA results. Due to the significant differences in sex
was not included because the DSQ combines both devaluation between the two groups, we conducted a preliminary factorial
toward others and toward the self; therefore, the directional dif- ANOVA whereby the interaction between sex and defenses was
ferences proposed by Gacono et al. (1992) could not be tested. tested. There was no significant interaction, F (7, 62) = 0.22,
Other defenses hypothesized to be related to BOR and ANT p = .98; therefore, the MANOVA was conducted with men and
(e.g., TAS) are not assessed by the DSQ. Internal consistency women combined.
for individual defenses has ranged from .07 to .82 (Andrews We conducted a MANOVA between the APD and BPD groups
et al., 1993). Concurrent validity of the DSQ is evidenced by on the following defenses: Acting Out, Denial, Idealization, Pas-
differences in individual defense use between individuals with sive Aggression, Projection, Rationalization, and Splitting. The
clinical disorders and healthy controls (Andrews et al., 1993). mean and standard deviation for each defense mechanism is pre-
sented in Table 1.1 Wilks’ Lambda indicates there was an overall
Procedure significant difference between the BPD and APD groups on de-
fenses, F (7, 64) = 5.01, p < .001. This effect accounted for
We asked participants to complete the PAI and the DSQ. 35.4% (η2 ) of the variance. The univariate results revealed that
Participants who did not complete both measures were not in- Acting Out and Passive Aggression were significantly higher
cluded, resulting in a sample of 446 participants. These par- in the BPD group, and Denial and Rationalization were signif-
ticipants were screened using PAI scores. Invalid profiles were icantly higher in the APD group. Acting Out accounted for the
identified according to Morey (1991). We removed any profiles most variance (partial ω2 = 15.9%) followed by Passive Ag-
that evidenced inconsistent (Inconsistency scale; t score > 73), gression (13.7%), Denial (10.5%), and Rationalization (5.9%).
careless (Infrequency scale; t score > 74), exaggerated nega-
tive (Negative Impression Management scale; t score > 91), or
exaggerated positive responding (Positive Impression Manage- 1 After examining the variables for normal distribution, one variable (Pro-
ment scale; t score > 68), resulting in a sample of 428. Second, jection) was significantly skewed (z > 3.33). We conducted a square root trans-
we divided participants into BPD and APD groups based on formation, which resulted in a normal distribution. Because the results with
the following criteria: 34 participants met criteria for the BPD the transformed score confirmed the results with the nontransformed score, the
group (i.e., scored ≥ 70t on the PAI BOR scale and < 70t on latter results were reported.
DEFENSE MECHANISMS AND PERSONALITY 141

TABLE 2.—Study 1 correlations between seven Defense Style Questionnaire TABLE 3.—Study 2 means and standard deviations for the Defense-Q.
defenses and the Borderline Features and Antisocial Features scales from the
Personality Assessment Inventory. BPD M APD M
Defense (SD)a (SD)b F p d Reliabilityc
Defense Borderline Features Antisocial Features z p
Acting Out 2.84 (0.83) 3.27 (1.05) 3.92 .051 −.45 .73
Acting Out .53∗∗ .35∗∗ 4.05 <.001 Devaluation 3.86 (0.80) 4.32 (0.91) 5.74 .019 −.54 .54
Denial −.00 .22∗∗ 4.29 <.001 Grandiosity 3.94 (0.76) 4.33 (0.81) 4.90 .030 −.50 .64
Idealization .01 −.10∗ 2.11 .017 Idealization 3.82 (0.68) 3.84 (0.48) 0.03 .857 −.04 .52
Passive Aggression .47∗∗ .30∗∗ 3.54 <.001 Intellectualization 3.07 (1.23) 3.55 (1.54) 2.28 .135 −.34 .85
Projection .50∗∗ .34∗∗ 4.36 <.001 Neurotic Denial 4.18 (1.14) 4.63 (1.22) 2.79 .099 −.38 .72
Rationalization .08 .23∗∗ 2.96 .002 Passive Aggression 4.18 (0.86) 4.18 (0.77) 0.00 .995 .00 .60
Splitting .31∗∗ .25∗∗ 1.28 .100 Projection 4.31 (0.57) 4.01 (0.55) 6.16 .015 +.56 .45
Rationalization 5.41 (0.86) 5.26 (0.84) 0.66 .420 +.18 .52
Note. N = 428. z scores indicate differences in magnitude of correlations according to Splitting 3.39 (0.84) 3.19 (0.69) 1.37 .246 +.26 .28
Meng, Rosenthal, and Rubin (1992). Turning Against Others 4.66 (0.77) 4.36 (0.79) 2.89 .093 +.38 .51

p < .05. ∗∗ p < .001. Turning Against Self 4.62 (0.97) 4.12 (0.99) 5.31 .024 +.52 .62

Note. BPD = borderline personality disorder group; APD = antisocial personality


Correlation results. Because a larger sample of partici- disorder group.
a
n = 34. b n = 49. c Defense-Q reliability calculated using one-way random effects
pants had completed both the PAI and DSQ (of whom many did model intraclass correlation coefficients.
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

not meet inclusion criteria), we also conducted correlation anal-


yses between the individual defenses with the BOR and ANT
scales of the PAI in this larger sample (n = 428). The results of overall defense use. The individual defense scores from the
of the Pearson correlations are reported in Table 2.2 Results in- Defense-Q coders were averaged together to produce a mean
dicated four defenses were positively correlated with the BOR score for each of the 12 defenses used in this study. We used
scale: Acting Out, Passive Aggression, Projection, and Splitting. the defenses of Acting Out, Devaluation, Grandiosity, Idealiza-
Six defenses were positively correlated with the ANT scale: tion, Intellectualization, Neurotic Denial, Passive Aggression,
Acting Out, Denial, Passive Aggression, Projection, Rational- Projection, Rationalization, Splitting, TAO, and TAS in this
ization, and Splitting. One defense, Idealization, was negatively study.
correlated with the ANT scale. The Defense-Q has demonstrated good convergent and con-
current validity (see MacGregor & Olson, 2005). Interrater reli-
STUDY 2 ability for the Defense-Q has been shown to range from poor to
excellent for individual defenses and excellent for profile com-
Method
parisons (MacGregor & Olson, 2005). The interrater reliability
Participants. A total of 1,539 undergraduate students from for each defense from this study are reported in Table 3.
a Canadian university participated in this study. All participants
provided informed consent, and the study was approved by the Procedure
Research Ethics Board of the University of Saskatchewan. The
mean age of the participants was 19.7 years (range = 17–40; Participants completed the PAI in the initial meeting. Of the
SD = 2.7); 68.0% were female, and 87.0% were Caucasian. All 1,539 participants, we called back 171 for this study. We con-
participants received partial course credit for their participation. tacted participants if they had elevated scores on BOR or ANT.
We asked participants to return to complete the DSQ and the Ex-
Measures panded Structured Interview (ESI; Hall, Davidson, MacGregor,
The PAI and DSQ as described in Study 1 were used in & MacLean, 1998). The ESI is a 12 to 15 min semistructured in-
Study 2. terview designed to elicit information related to stress, anxiety,
and coping (Hall et al., 1998). Participants are asked questions
Defense-Q (MacGregor et al., 2008). The Defense-Q is related to school and work and how they react in certain stress-
an observational measure used to assess the relative use of 25 ful situations. The ESI is based on a Type A interview (used to
defense mechanisms (MacGregor & Davidson, 1998; MacGre- assess hostility, competitiveness, time urgency, etc.) adapted for
gor et al., 2008). It is a Q-sort instrument based on a system use with a university population and modified to assess defense
of rank ordering: one defense is identified as most characteris- use and emotional expression. Defenses have been assessed us-
tic, two as quite characteristic, five as somewhat characteristic, ing the ESI in previous research on defenses, personality, and
nine as neither uncharacteristic nor characteristic, five as some- health (e.g., MacGregor et al., 2003). The entire interview is
what uncharacteristic, two as quite uncharacteristic, and one as viewed separately by each coder, providing them with a sam-
most uncharacteristic. The categorization results in a seminor- ple of responses and behaviors from which they observationally
mal distribution representing a participant’s overall pattern (or assess defenses using the Defense-Q.
profile) of defense use. The Defense-Q allows for the exami- Of the 171 participants who were called back, 92 participants
nation of individual defense use as well as provides a profile returned to complete the ESI and DSQ. The most common rea-
sons for not returning were a failure to respond to repeated
contact attempts or a response that they had already reached
2 Three variables (denial, projection, and splitting) were significantly skewed their maximum allotted research credits. Once we removed par-
(z > 3.33) and we conducted transformations, resulting in a normal distribution. ticipants for incomplete data or invalid PAI profiles (see Study 1
Because the results with the transformed scores confirmed the results with the for criteria), the resulting sample was 83 participants, of which
nontransformed scores, the latter results were reported. 34 met criteria for the BPD group (i.e., scored ≥ 70t on the PAI
142 PRESNIAK, OLSON, MACGREGOR

TABLE 4.—Study 2 means and standard deviations for the Defense Style Questionnaire.

Defense BPD M (SD)a APD M (SD)b F p d Reliability (No. of Items)c

Acting Out 33.77 (7.27) 29.27 (7.09) 7.92 .006 +.64 .55 (6)
Denial 5.97 (3.08) 6.84 (3.09) 1.58 .212 −.28 .07 (2)
Idealization 15.62 (4.57) 15.45 (4.26) 0.30 .864 +.04 .14 (3)
Passive Aggression 38.77 (7.92) 33.88 (6.44) 9.57 .003 +.70 .22 (8)
Projection 35.06 (12.24) 30.76 (12.71) 2.37 .127 +.35 .83 (10)
Rationalization 5.06 (2.51) 5.45 (2.07) 0.60 .442 −.18 − (1)
Splitting 12.29 (4.47) 11.86 (5.05) 0.17 .686 +.09 .44 (3)

Note. BPD = borderline personality disorder group; APD = antisocial personality disorder group.
a
n = 34. b n = 49. c Reliability calculated using Cronbach’s alpha. Reliability for rationalization is not calculated because it consists of one item.

BOR scale and < 70t on the ANT scale), and 49 met criteria for This effect accounted for 18.8% (η2 ) of the variance. Univariate
the APD group (i.e., score ≥ 70t on the ANT scale and < 70t results revealed that Acting Out and Passive Aggression were
on the BOR scale). All variables were normally distributed with higher in the BPD group compared to the APD group. Passive
no outliers. Of the total sample, 93.4% were Caucasian, and Aggression accounted for 10.6% (partial ω2 ) of the variance,
the mean age was 19.5 years (range = 18–26; SD = 1.6), with
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

and Acting Out accounted for 8.9%.


no significant age, t(59) = 0.04, p = .97, or racial differences,
χ 2 (3, N = 61) = 3.88, p = .28 between the groups. There Defense-Q results. Due to the significant differences in sex
were significant differences in the sex of participants between between the two groups, we conducted a preliminary factorial
groups, χ 2 (1, N = 61) = 8.15, p = .006, with more women in ANOVA whereby we tested the interaction between sex and
the BPD group (84.6%) compared to the APD group (48.4%). defenses. There was no significant interaction for the Defense-
Many of the participants in each group had elevations on other Q analyses, F (12, 42) = 0.58, p = .85; therefore, we conducted
scales on the PAI. The most common concurrent elevations in the MANOVA with men and women combined.
the BPD group were on the Suicide (26.5%) and Depression The mean and standard deviation for each defense mechanism
scales (38.2%); and in the APD group, the most common eleva- used in the Defense-Q analysis is presented in Table 3. There
tions were on the Alcohol Problems (34.7%) and Drug Problems was an overall significant difference between the BPD and APD
(26.5%) scales. groups on defenses, F (12, 70) = 3.08, p = .002. This effect
accounted for 34.5% (η2 ) of the variance. Results from univari-
Defense coder training. Data were collected over 4 con- ate analyses revealed that Projection and TAS were higher in
secutive years. Each year’s participant interviews were coded by the BPD group, and Devaluation and Grandiosity were signifi-
three Defense-Q coders, some of whom coded multiple years. cantly higher in the APD group. Devaluation accounted for the
The coders from the initial 2 years had additionally coded de- most variance (partial ω2 = 6.6%), followed by TAS (6.2%),
fense mechanism data for another published manuscript (Olson, Grandiosity (5.7%), and Projection (1.5%).
Presniak, & MacGregor, 2009). All coders were senior under- DISCUSSION
graduate students who received standard training for the Defense
Q. This included a minimum of 20 hr of training, which involved Overall group differences were demonstrated across the two
a discussion of theory, how to rate each defense mechanism (i.e., studies. Of the five defenses hypothesized to be higher in the
definition, examples of its manifestation, how to look for evi- BPD group, two were supported (passive aggression and TAS),
dence of its presence or absence), and observation of trainers one was inconsistently supported (acting out), and two were not
(current authors) coding defenses from video-recorded inter- supported (idealization and splitting). Of the seven defenses hy-
views as well as coders independently practicing coding and pothesized to be higher in the APD group, two were supported
then reviewing their assessment in a group meeting (see David- (devaluation of others and grandiosity), one was partially sup-
son & MacGregor, 1996, or MacGregor & Olson, 2005, for a ported (denial; all effects in the right direction, but only one
more detailed discussion of training procedure). After coding of three was significant), one was inconsistently supported (ra-
defenses for this data began, coders met together with trainers tionalization), and three were not supported (intellectualization,
weekly to discuss coding and maintain reliability. TAO, and projection).
Consistent with our hypothesis and previous theory and re-
search (e.g., Bond, 1990; Cramer, 1999), we found partial sup-
Results port that the BPD group would use the maladaptive defenses,
DSQ results. Due to the significant differences in sex be- acting out, and passive aggression more than the APD group.
tween the two groups, we conducted a preliminary factorial These results, however, were not consistent across measure-
ANOVA whereby we tested the interaction between sex and ment type.3 The Defense-Q results indicated no difference in
defenses. There was no significant interaction for the DSQ anal- Passive Aggression scores and showed a moderate effect size
yses, F (7, 47) = 0.58, p = .77; therefore, we conducted the for lower Acting Out scores in the BPD group. One explanation
MANOVA with men and women combined. for this discrepancy could be the differences in the measurement
The mean and standard deviation for each defense in the anal-
ysis is presented in Table 4. Results of the Wilks’ Lambda multi-
variate test indicated a significant overall difference between the 3 For a correlation table of the convergence between the DSQ and Defense-Q

BPD and APD groups on defenses, F (7, 75) = 2.68, p = .016. defenses from this study, please contact M. D. Presniak.
DEFENSE MECHANISMS AND PERSONALITY 143

of these defenses between the DSQ and Defense-Q. For exam- is characteristic of both BPD and APD, but that when required
ple, the DSQ’s Acting Out predominantly assesses impulsivity to choose one’s most likely reaction to situations, participants
and limited aggression, which may be more likely to be self- with APD are more likely to choose TAO. Our results also did
reported by individuals with BPD. In comparison, APD groups not support our hypothesis that idealization would be used more
may be less likely to admit exploitative and criminal acts on by the BPD group. However, a moderate effect size emerged
self-report measures, but their acting out behaviors may be ob- indicating idealization scores on the DSQ were higher in the
served by coders. As such, it would be valuable to further assess BPD group and, in addition, there was a significant negative
the types of acting out and passive aggressive behaviors used correlation between Idealization on the DSQ and the ANT scale
by each group and to examine whether these differences are of the PAI. This latter finding supports Gacono et al.’s (1992)
reflected in measurement type or differentially related to each suggestions that individuals with APD are incapable of idealiz-
group. ing others. Therefore, there is partial evidence that idealization
Contrary to our hypothesis, splitting was not used signif- may not be characteristic of APD and may be used more by
icantly more by the BPD group. Our results, however, were the BPD group. The findings are consistent with the results of
consistent with researchers who previously have found no dif- Gacono et al. (1992), but additional research is needed to further
ference in splitting use between BPD and APD groups (Gacono understand whether differences in the use of idealization exist
et al., 1992). Another explanation for the unsupported hypoth- between BPD and APD groups.
esis could be related to the low reliability of Splitting scores. We hypothesized that denial would be used more by the APD
In Study 2, interrater reliability of Splitting on the Defense-Q group based on Gacono and Meloy (1988) who suggested that
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

was poor (.28), therefore likely affecting the validity of these denial is an important defense for APD. Gacono and Meloy
results. The hypotheses, however, were not supported with the explained that intrinsic to APD is a “shut-off” mechanism that
DSQ either. The construct validity of assessing splitting through allows the exclusion of personality aspects that could diminish
self-report may have impacted the findings. For example, the their grandiose self-view or lead to a recognition of their ex-
DSQ asks participants to report the extent to which they view ploitative and criminal behaviors. Denial is a key component of
things as polar opposites (e.g., “Sometimes I think I’m an angel this proposed mechanism. In our studies here, Denial and Neu-
and other times I think I’m a devil”). Accurate self-report of this rotic Denial scores were higher, with small to medium effect
behavior may be challenging for individuals who rely heavily sizes, in the APD group compared to the BPD group, although
on splitting because it requires a self-awareness of an inability the difference was statistically significant in only one of the three
to integrate that is inherently problematic for them. tests. These findings provide partial support for the hypothesis
Gacono et al. (1992) also proposed an alternate theory of and are consistent with previous findings showing APD to be
splitting in BPD and APD, and our results provide support for associated with disavowal level defenses including denial (Perry
this conceptualization. First, Grandiosity scores were higher in & Cooper, 1986).
the APD group, supporting the notion that this group splits their We also hypothesized that the APD group would use more
identity into a grandiose and devalued self, of which only the rationalization. Gacono and Meloy (1988) suggested that, in its
grandiose self is evident. Second, our findings also partially developmentally advanced form, denial would develop into ra-
supported Gacono et al.’s (1992) suggestion that the direction tionalization. This occurs when individuals become more aware
aggression is aimed will differ between groups. TAS was sig- of their antisocial behaviors but justify them through distorted
nificantly higher in the BPD group compared to the APD group. self-serving explanations. The results supported this hypothesis
This finding was consistent with previous findings showing that in Study 1 but not in Study 2. The Defense-Q results showed that
BPD is positively associated with TAS (Berman & McCann, both groups were rated by observers as using the defense above
1995). In addition, devaluation of others was used significantly average (M > 4). It has been noted by many that college stu-
more by the APD group. Lingiardi et al. (1999) had previously dents tend to rationalize maladaptive behaviors (e.g., cheating
found no correlation between APD scores and devaluation on on tests, violence; McCabe, 1992; Pezza & Bellotti, 1995). It
the DMRS, although this research was conducted using a pre- may be that elevated use of rationalization in this university sam-
vious version of the DMRS that combined the constructs of ple obscured our ability to examine this hypothesis. Lingiardi
devaluation of self and others, thus obscuring the directional et al. (1999) also suggested that intellectualization can capture
relationships. the tendency to minimize negative experiences. Lingiardi et al.
Although a large portion of Gacono et al.’s (1992) theory previously found that use of intellectualization positively cor-
on splitting was supported (i.e., use of devaluation, grandiosity, related with APD in a sample of clinical outpatients. Based on
and TAS), our results did not show support for significant dif- this finding, we expected intellectualization on the Defense-Q
ferences between the groups for use of idealization or TAO. The to be higher in the APD group; however, the hypothesis was
results for TAO, which showed no significant differences but a not supported. Further research is needed to explore the use of
small to medium effect size indicating a lower score in the APD intellectualization and rationalization in APD.
group, were contrary to previous theory and research. Using Finally, the hypothesis that projection would be used more by
the self-report Defense Mechanisms Inventory (DMI; Ihilevich the APD group was not supported. This result is also inconsistent
& Gleser, 1986), Berman and McCann (1995) previously found with the theory proposed by Gacono et al. (1992) that, in APD,
APD was positively associated with the individual defense Turn- the world is seen through a projected view of themselves as well
ing Against Object. The DMI, however, uses a forced-choice as inconsistent with the results of Cramer (1999) who found that
self-report format in which participants are required to choose projection was a predictor of APD but not BPD. Our results are
between five reactions to multiple situations. Each reaction rep- consistent, however, with the theory and findings of Perry and
resents a defense, and as such, participants are unable to have Cooper (1986), who suggested that projective identification,
elevated scores on all five defenses. It may be that blaming others which is a more primitive form of projection, was associated
144 PRESNIAK, OLSON, MACGREGOR

with BPD. Therefore, additional research is needed to examine Becker, D. F., Grilo, C. M., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (2000). Comor-
the role of projection in both of these groups. bidity of borderline personality disorder with other personality disorders in
Overall, although all hypotheses were not supported, the find- hospitalized adolescents and adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157,
ings support the conceptualization of BPD and APD as having 2011–2016.
Berg, J. L. (1990). Differentiating ego functions of borderline and narcissistic
separate and diverse defensive profiles. Supporting Perry and
personalities. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 537–548.
Cooper’s (1986) proposition, the defenses in BPD appear to Berman, S. M., & McCann, J. T. (1995). Defense mechanisms and personal-
emphasize interpersonal relatedness or dependency. These in- ity disorders: An empirical test of Millon’s theory. Journal of Personality
clude defenses that lead to difficulties expressing one’s needs Assessment, 64, 132–144.
and wishes to others (passive aggression), projecting internal Blais, M. A., & Norman, D. (1997). A psychometric evaluation of the DSM–
selves onto others (projection), and the splitting of the ego, IV personality disorder criteria sets. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11,
which involves predominantly a devaluation of the self (TAS). 168–176.
Supporting many of Gacono et al.’s (1992) and Gacano and Bond, M. (1990). Are “borderline defenses” specific for borderline personality
Meloy’s (1988) theoretical work, the defensive profile in APD, disorders? Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 251–256.
on the other hand, appears to emphasize egocentricity, inter- Bond, M. (2004). Empirical studies of defense style: Relationships with psy-
personal exploitation, and the expression of aggression toward chopathology and change. Harvard Review Psychiatry, 12, 263–278.
Bond, M., Gardener, S. T., Christian, J., & Sigal, J. J. (1983). Empirical study
others. Specifically, the defenses contribute to a splitting of the
of self-rated defense styles. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 333–338.
ego, which results in a grandiose view of the self (grandiosity); Boone, D. (1998). Internal consistency reliability of the Personality Assessment
the tendency to minimize, distort, or avoid the consequences of
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

Inventory with psychiatric patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54, 839–


their behavior (denial and rationalization); and a tendency to 843.
protect the self by devaluing others (devaluation). Boyle, G. J., & Lennon, T. J. (1994). Examination of the reliability and validity
of the Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and
Strengths and Limitations Behavioral Assessment, 16, 173–187.
One limitation to these studies is the weaknesses in reliabil- Chabrol, H., & Leichsenring, F. (2006). Borderline personality organization
and psychopathic traits in nonclinical adolescents: Relationships of identity
ity and validity that is characteristic of some defense measures,
diffusion, primitive defense mechanisms and reality testing with callousness
which may have decreased the likelihood of finding significant and impulsivity traits. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 70, 161–170.
results. For example, the interrater reliability for Splitting on Cooper, S. H. (1998). Changing notions of defense within psychoanalytic theory.
the Defense-Q was poor for Study 2, and the Acting Out items Journal of Personality, 66, 947–964.
and behaviors measured by the DSQ and Defense-Q may cap- Cramer, P. (1997). Evidence for change in children’s use of defense mechanisms.
ture different aspects of acting out. Additionally, some of the Journal of Personality, 65, 233–247.
group comparisons made using the DSQ may have been atten- Cramer, P. (1999). Personality, personality disorders, and defense mechanisms.
uated by the low internal consistency observed for several of Journal of Personality, 67, 535–554.
these scales. These difficulties capturing defenses can compro- Cramer, P. (2006). Protecting the self: Defense mechanisms in action. New
mise our ability to accurately assess defenses and their relation York: Guildford.
Davidson, K., & MacGregor, M. Wm. (1996). The reliability of the Defense-Q.
to personality disorders, which complicates the comparison of
Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 624–639.
results across studies. To mitigate this problem, we used both Davidson, K., & MacGregor, M. Wm. (1998). A critical appraisal of self-report
observer and self-report measures of defenses in this study to defense mechanism measures. Journal of Personality, 66, 965–992.
get a more complete and detailed defense assessment. However, Freud, A. (1986). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. In J. D. Sutherland
further measurement development is still needed to improve the (Ed. & Trans.), The writings of Anna Freud (Vol. 2). London, England:
assessment of defenses. Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1936)
Another limitation to these studies is the use of a nonclinical Gacono, C. B., & Meloy, J. R. (1988). The relationship between cognitive style
sample for the BPD and APD groups. Nonclinical samples may and defensive process in the psychopath. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 15,
not display the degree of symptom severity and behaviors that 472–483.
would be found in a clinical group. Although it will be impor- Gacono, C. B., Meloy, J. R., & Berg, J. L. (1992). Object relations, defen-
sive operations, and affective states in narcissistic, borderline, and antisocial
tant to replicate these results in a clinical sample, our findings
personality disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 59, 32–49.
suggest that in BPD, the defenses may emphasize interpersonal Hall, P., Davidson, K., MacGregor, M. Wm., & MacLean, D. (1998). Expanded
relatedness or dependency, whereas in APD, the defenses may Structured Interview administration training manual (Tech. Rep. No. 1).
emphasize egocentricity and interpersonal exploitation. Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Heart Health of Nova Scotia.
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. North
ACKNOWLEDGMENT Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
This study was conducted with the support of a Canada Grad- Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1943). The Minnesota Multiphasic Per-
sonality Inventory. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
uate Scholarship awarded to M. D. Presniak by the Social Sci-
Herkov, M. J., & Blashfield, R. K. (1995). Clinician diagnoses of personal-
ences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. ity disorders: Evidence of a hierarchical structure. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 65, 313–321.
REFERENCES Hilsenroth, M. J., Hibbard, S. R., Nash, M. R., & Handler, L. (1993). A
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of Rorschach study of narcissism, defense, and aggression in borderline, narcis-
mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. sistic, and cluster c personality disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment,
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of 60, 346–361.
mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Ihilevich, D., & Gleser, G. C. (1986). Defense mechanisms: Their classifica-
Andrews, G., Singh, M., & Bond, M. (1993). The Defense Style Questionnaire. tion, correlates, and measurement with the Defense Mechanism Inventory.
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, 246–256. Owosso, MI: DMI associates.
DEFENSE MECHANISMS AND PERSONALITY 145

Ihilevich, D., & Gleser, G. C. (1995). The defense mechanisms inventory: Its Morey, L. C. (1991). The Personality Assessment Inventory. Lutz, FL: Psycho-
development and clinical applications. In H. R. Conte & R. Plutchik (Eds.), logical Assessment Resources.
Ego defenses: Theory and development (pp. 221–246). New York, NY: Wiley. Olson, T. R., Presniak, M. D., & MacGregor, M. Wm. (2009). Differentiation of
Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders: Psychotherapeutic strate- depression and anxiety groups using defense mechanisms. Journal of Nervous
gies. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. and Mental Disease, 197, 834–840.
Kurtz, J. E., Morey, L. C., & Tomarken, A. J. (1993). The concurrent validity of Perry, J. C. (1990). The Defense Mechanism Rating scales (5th ed.). Cambridge,
three self-report measures of borderline personality. Journal of Psychopathol- MA: Cambridge Hospital.
ogy and Behavioral Assessment, 15, 255–266. Perry, J. C., & Cooper, S. H. (1986). A preliminary report on defenses and con-
Leichsenring, F., Kunst, H., & Hoyer, J. (2003). Borderline personality organi- flicts associated with borderline personality disorder. Journal of the American
zation in violent offenders: Correlations of identity diffusion and primitive Psychoanalytic Association, 34, 863–893.
defense mechanisms with antisocial features, neuroticism, and interpersonal Pezza, P. E., & Bellotti, A. (1995). College campus violence: Origins, impacts,
problems. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 67, 314–327. and responses. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 105–123.
Lerner, P., & Lerner, H. (1980). Rorschach assessment of primitive defenses in Shedler, J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). The illusion of mental health.
borderline personality structure. In J. Kwawer, A. Sugarman, P. Lerner, & H. American Psychologist, 48, 1117–1131.
Lerner (Eds.), Borderline phenomena and the Rorschach test (pp. 257–274). Spinhoven, P., & Kooiman, C. G. (1997). Defense style in depressed and anxious
New York, NY: International Universities Press. psychiatric outpatients: An explorative study. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Lingiardi, V., Lonati, C., Delucchi, F., Fossati, A., Vanzulli, L., & Maffei, C. Disease, 185, 87–94.
(1999). Defense mechanisms and personality disorders. Journal of Nervous Trull, T. J. (1995). Borderline personality disorder features in nonclinical
and Mental Disease, 187, 224–228. young adults: 1. Identification and validation. Psychological Assessment, 7,
Downloaded by [University Of Pittsburgh] at 11:23 26 November 2014

MacGregor, M. Wm., & Davidson, K. (1998). The Defense-Q: An idiographic Q- 33–41.


Sort measure of defense mechanisms. A manual for clinicians and researchers Trull, T., Useda, J. D., Conforti, K., & Doan, B. (1997). Borderline personality
(Tech. Rep. No. 4). Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Heart Health of Nova disorder features in nonclinical young adults: 2. Two-year outcome. Journal
Scotia. of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 307–314.
MacGregor, M. Wm., Davidson, K., Barksdale, C., Black, S., & MacLean, D. Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
(2003). Adaptive defense use and resting blood pressure in a population-based Press.
sample. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55, 531–541. Vaillant, G. E. (1994). Ego mechanisms of defense and personality psy-
MacGregor, M. Wm., & Olson, T. R. (2005). Defense mechanisms: Their rela- chopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 44–50.
tion to personality and health: An exploration of defense mechanisms assessed Walters, G. D., Duncan, S. A., & Geyer, M. D. (2003). Predicting disciplinary
by the Defense-Q. In A. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research adjustment in inmates undergoing forensic evaluation: A direct comparison
(Vol. 36, pp. 95–141). New York, NY: Nova Science. of the PCL-R and the PAI. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology,
MacGregor, M. Wm., Olson, T. R., Presniak, M. D., & Davidson, K. (2008). 14, 382–393.
The Defense-Q: An idiographic Q-Sort measure of defense mechanisms: A Widiger, T. A. (1993). The DSM–III–R categorical personality disorder diag-
manual for clinicians and researchers (2nd ed.). Unpublished manuscript. noses: A critique and an alternative. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 75–90.
McCabe, D. L. (1992). The influence of situational ethics on cheating among Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Dubo, E. D., Sickel, A. E., Trikha, A., Levin,
college students. Sociological Inquiry, 62, 365–374. A., & Reynolds, V. (2004). Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality
Meng, X., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated corre- disorder: Description of 6-year course and prediction to time-to-remission.
lation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 172–175. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110, 416–420.

You might also like