You are on page 1of 8

Effect of Vacation on Health: Moderating Factors

of Vacation Outcome
Gerhard Strauss-Blasche, Barbara Reithofer,Wolfgang Schobersberger,
Cem Ekmekcioglu, and Wolfgang Marktl

Background: Vacation has recently become a topic of interest in health research as both beneficial and adverse health
effects have been documented. The present study was aimed at identifying vacation characteristics predicting health-
related vacation outcome.
Methods: One hundred ninety-one predominantly white-collar employees (109 female, 82 males; mean age 37.8 yr, range
16–62 yr) received a questionnaire in the week after vacation assessing subject characteristics, physical vacation char-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


acteristics, the individual structuring of the day, health and social behavior, and stress during vacation as well as the
perceived change of recuperation and exhaustion from before to after a vacation. Regression analysis was used to iden-
tify variables predicting vacation outcome.
Results:Twenty-seven percent of the variance of the change of recuperation and 15% of the change of exhaustion could
be explained. Recuperation was facilitated by free time for one’s self, warmer (and sunnier) vacation locations, exercise
during vacation, good sleep, and making new acquaintances, especially among vacationers reporting higher levels of
prevacation work strain. Exhaustion was increased by vacation-related health problems and a greater time-zone differ-
ence to home, and was reduced by warmer vacation locations.
Conclusion: Health-related vacation outcome is significantly affected by the way an individual organizes his or her vacation.

Vacation, defined by Lounsbury and Hoops as a removal of work stress and an increase of personally
temporary respite from work lasting several days to sev- available time,5,6 are as yet unclear. On the other hand,
eral weeks, has presently become a topic of interest in vacation may also be associated with adverse health
health research.1 A recent study suggests that the frequency effects for some individuals, potentially leading to leisure
of annual vacations for men at risk for coronary heart dis- sickness or cardiac incidence.7,8
ease (CHD) is associated with a reduced risk of mortal- Therefore, the aim of the present study was to iden-
ity, which is predominantly attributed to the CHD.2 tify variables relevant to the individual’s vacation that affect
Also, leisure vacation has been reported to be negatively both positive and negative health-related vacation out-
associated with depression,3 a potential risk factor for come.The study is exploratory as little information on
CHD,4 in a sample of Japanese male white-collar work- the effects of specific vacation attributes on health-
ers. In longitudinal research with healthy employees, we related vacation outcomes is provided in literature. Our
and others found vacation to be associated with at least approach was to study physiologic and psychological
short-term improvements of health-related variables factors that have a known potential effect on well-being
such as burnout, mood, and physical complaints.5,6 To date, and health in addition to vacation-specific variables
the mechanisms leading to these results, except for a potentially moderating vacation outcome. Four aspects
of vacation were studied: (1) physical aspects of vacation,
including vacation duration assuming vacation benefits
to increase with longer vacation,9 the average tempera-
Gerhard Strauss-Blasche, PhD, Cem Ekmekcioglu, MD, and
Wolfgang Marktl, MD: Department of Physiology, Medical
ture at the vacation site as a climatic variable,10 the time-
University of Vienna, Vienna, and Ludwig Boltzmann Institute zone difference to account for jet lag,11 and the travel time
for Biological Rhythm Research, Bad Tatzmannsdorf, Austria; to account for travel stress12; (2) the organization of daily
Barbara Reithofer, MA: Department of Physiology, Medical routine, including the number of provided meals and the
University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria; Wolfgang planning of the day, both potential exogenous pace-
Schobersberger MD: Institute for Leisure, Travel and Alpine makers affecting health,13 self-determination of daily
Medicine, University for Health Sciences, Medical
actions, and time for one’s self and needs as leisure fac-
Informatics and Technology Tyrol, Innsbruck, Austria.
tors contributing to psychological well-being 14,15 ;
Reprint requests: Gerhard Strauss-Blasche, PhD, (3) health and social behavior including the amount of
Department of Physiology, Medical University of Vienna, physical activity,16 the duration and quality of sleep,17 and
Schwarzspanierstrasse 17, 1090 Vienna, Austria.
making new acquaintances as known factors improving
J Travel Med 2005; 12:94–101. health and mood18; and (4) potential stressors during

94
S t r a u s s - B l a s ch e e t a l , M o d e r a t i n g F a c t o r s o f Va c a t i o n 95

vacation including health problems and interpersonal con- .91. Scale 2 (“exhaustion”) was composed of three items
flicts.19,20 (“In comparison to the 2 weeks before vacation, I now
feel more indifferent and apathetic, more depressed, and
Methods more exhausted”) with a scale reliability of Cronbach’s
 = .73.To describe subject characteristics, age, sex, and
Subjects and Procedure an estimate of physical as well as mental strain of daily
Subjects were employees of a large travel agency and work were assessed, the latter two because we assumed
their relatives.A questionnaire, including a cover letter that more strained individuals would show a greater
asking for participation in a study of the effect of vaca- improvement during vacation.23
tion type and vacation activities on recuperation and The independent variables described four different
well-being, was e-mailed or handed to approximately aspects of vacation: (1) physical vacation characteristics
500 recipients. Subjects were asked to fill out the ques- (duration, average temperature, time-zone difference,
tionnaire within the 2 weeks following their vacation. time to reach destination), (2) organization of the day
Of the 295 returned questionnaires (response rate 59%), (number of provided meals, self-determination of one’s

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


only the 239 filled out by occupationally active indi- daily actions, planning of day ahead, time for one’s self
viduals were included in the study. Of these, 38 had to and needs), (3) health and social behavior (physical activ-
be rejected owing to incomplete answers or delayed ity during vacation, average sleep duration during vaca-
responses, and 10 were rejected owing to a vacation dura- tion, sleep quality, making new acquaintances), and
tion > 4 weeks.The latter criterion was established as (4) stress during vacation (health problems, interper-
vacations > 4 weeks are unusual and the frequency of sonal conflicts).A complete list of items and their response
these vacations was small in the current sample. Of 191 frequencies is presented in Table 1.
remaining study participants, 109 were females and 82
were males with a mean age of 37.8 years (SD = 11.3; Statistics
range 16–62 yr). Most study participants had partners The intercorrelation between the subject charac-
(72.8%); 24% had children younger than 14 years.The teristics and the independent variables as well as their cor-
study reflected a variety of locations, most commonly relation with the two outcome measures was calculated.
in Austria (27%), followed by Italy (17%), Greece (13%), The predictive effect of these variables on the two out-
northern Europe (11%), and Croatia (8%).About 5% of come measures was analyzed using two separate linear
the subjects traveled to other continents.Approximately regression analyses. Vacationer characteristics and the
80% of the subjects filled out their questionnaire within four groups of items representing the different aspects
the first week of return; 30% completed it during the of vacation were entered in five separate blocks.Total R2,
first 3 days after vacation. R2 change for each block, and  values for every vari-
able were listed.None of the variables had a variance infla-
Measures tion factor of greater than 1.5.24
Selected items of a standardized German quality-of-
life questionnaire were used as outcome measure.21 This Results
quality-of-life measure, although initially constructed
for hypertensive patients, encompasses a broad variety of The variables of subject and vacation description and
items related to positive and negative well-being and self- their frequency distribution are illustrated in Table 1.The
perceived fitness and is quite sensitive to change. From intercorrelation of these variables is illustrated in Table 2.
these three scales, 11 items were chosen, and subjects were To predict vacation outcome, we conducted two regres-
asked to rate their current state of well-being in com- sion analyses for both outcome measures—recupera-
parison to the state 2 weeks before their vacation.This tion and exhaustion (Table 3). Five groups of variables
approach is a modification of the postintervention “then were entered consecutively as separate blocks.The first
rating” devised to minimize errors in the measurement block encompassed variables describing the vacationers,
of change.22 Two scales were derived from the 11 items the second block variables described physical vacation
by factor analysis, together explaining 60.3% of the vari- characteristics, the third block described the way vaca-
ance. One item was discarded owing to a low factor load- tioners organized their day, the fourth block related to
ing.Scale 1 (“recuperation”) was composed of seven items health and social behavior, and the fifth block related to
(“In comparison to the 2 weeks before vacation, I now stressful events during vacation.
feel mentally fitter, feel more balanced and relaxed, can Twenty-seven percent of the change of recupera-
concentrate better during work, feel physically fitter, do tion could be explained by the descriptive variables.
my work more easily, am in a better mood, and feel more The vacationer characteristics (block 1) explained 7% of
recuperated”) with a scale reliability of Cronbach’s  = the variance, with the mental strain of prevacation daily
96 J o u r n a l o f Tr a v e l M e d i c i n e , Vo l u m e 1 2 , N u m b e r 2

Table 1 Subject Characteristics, Variables Describing Vacation, and Outcome Measures


Characteristic/Variable Range Response Frequency (%)
Age (yr) 16–24 12
25–30 15.7
30–39 28.3
40–49 26.2
50–62 17.8
Physical strain of daily work (scale*) Low 37.6
2 40.7
3 11.2
4 7.8
High 2.7
Mental strain of daily work (scale*) Low 3.5
2 17.4
3 5.4

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


4 33.3
High 40.3
Duration of vacation (d) 3–7 34.9
8–14 43.8
15–21 12.8
22–28 8.5
Time zone difference to home (h) 0 64.3
1–2 33
5–8 2.7
Average temperature at vacation site (C) 15–20 14.3
21–25 27.9
26–30 40
30–45 17.8
Time to travel to vacation site (h) 1–3 36.4
4–7 37.6
8–12 16.3
13–48 9.7
Provided meals (number) None 34
1 (breakfast) 27.2
2 22.5
3 16.2
Self-determination of daily actions (scale*) Not at all 2.1
2 11.0
3 2.1
4 16.1
Very much so 68.6
Planning the day ahead (scale*) Not at all 25.7
2 40.3
3 9.4
4 20.4
Very much so 4.2
Time for one’s self and needs (scale*) None 2.6
2 16.2
3 2.6
4 31.9
Adequate 46.6
Physical activity during vacation (scale*) None 20.4
2 26.2
3 24.1
4 22.0
Very often 7.3
Average duration of sleep (h) 5–7 8.9
8 24.1
9 39.3
10–11 27.7
Sleep quality (scale*) Bad 1.6
Continued
S t r a u s s - B l a s ch e e t a l , M o d e r a t i n g F a c t o r s o f Va c a t i o n 97

Table 1 (Continued)
Characteristic/Variable Range Response Frequency (%)
2 15.7
3 3.7
4 25.1
Good 53.9
Making new acquaintances (scale*) Not at all 27.2
2 32.5
3 6.3
4 19.4
Very much so 14.7
Health problems during vacation (scale*) None 78.0
2 13.6
3 5.2
4 3.1

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


Very often 0
Interpersonal conflicts (scale*) Not at all 69.6
2 20.9
3 3.7
4 3.1
Very much so 2.6
Recuperation (scale 0–100) 0–19 5.2
20–39 12.6
40–59 24.6
60–79 41.9
80–100 15.7
Exhaustion (scale 0–100) 0–19 71.2
20–39 17.3
40–59 9.4
60–79 2.1
80–100 0
*Five-point Likert scale.

work positively predicting outcome; that is, individuals variance. Interpersonal conflicts were negatively related;
with high job strain experienced greater recuperation health problems were unrelated to recuperation.
during vacation. Age, sex, and physical strain were not Overall, 15% of the second outcome measure, the
significantly associated with recuperation. Physical vaca- perceived change of exhaustion, could be explained by
tion characteristics (block 2) explained 6% of the vari- the descriptive variables.The vacationer characteristics
ance, with warm temperature at the vacation site (block 1) did not predict exhaustion. Physical vacation
predicting recuperation. Despite a single-order correla- characteristics (block 2) predicted 7% of the change of
tion, the duration of vacation did not predict recuper- exhaustion,with a greater time-zone difference being pos-
ation in the regression analysis nor did time-zone itively related to exhaustion and the average tempera-
difference or travel time. ture at the vacation site negatively related to exhaustion.
The way vacationers organized their day (block 3) The duration of vacation and the time to travel to the
explained 8% of the variance.Whereas the number of pro- vacation destination were unrelated. The individual
vided meals,the extent of self-determination of one’s daily organization of daily vacation activities (block 3) did not
activities, and the extent vacationers tended to plan their predict exhaustion. Also, health and social behaviors
day ahead did not predict recuperation, the amount of (block 4) were unrelated to exhaustion.Stress during vaca-
time vacationers perceived to have for themselves and their tion (block 5) explained 3% of the variance, with health
needs was positively and strongly associated with recu- problems, but not interpersonal conflicts, predicting
peration. Health and social behaviors (block 4) explained exhaustion.
7% of the perceived change of recuperation.Vacationers
engaging in more physical activity, having a better qual- Discussion
ity of sleep, and making new acquaintances perceived
greater recuperation after vacation. Sleep duration dur- Even though studies in tourism and leisure research
ing vacation did not significantly predict recuperation. have assessed the effect of vacation-related factors on
Potential vacation stressors (block 5) predicted 2% of the vacation satisfaction,25 the present study is, to our knowl-
98 J o u r n a l o f Tr a v e l M e d i c i n e , Vo l u m e 1 2 , N u m b e r 2

Table 2 Intercorrelation of Subject and Vacation Characteristics*


Characteristic 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
1 Age — .21 .24 .29 .19 .16 .19
2 Sex (1= m, 2 = f) .21 — .17
3 Physical strain of
daily work —
4 Mental strain of
daily work — .14 .15
5 Duration of
vacation (d) .24 — .26 .27 .21 .25
6 Time-zone
difference to
home (h) .26 — .22 .32 .30 .17
7 Average temperature
at vacation site .29 .22 — .18 .27 .19

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


8 Time to travel to
vacation site (h) .19 .27 .32 — .18
9 Number of provided
meals .30 .18 — .18
10 Self-determining one’s
daily actions .16 .27 — .37
11 Planning the day ahead .17 — .17
12 Time for one’s self
and needs .14 .21 .37 — .21
13 Physical activity
duringvacation —
14 Average duration
of sleep (h) .19 .25 —
15 Sleep quality .15 .17 .21 — .22
16 Make new
acquaintances .17 .18 —
17 Health problems
during vacation .19 .18 .22 —
18 Interpersonal conflicts
*Only significant correlations (p < .05) are listed.

edge, the first one investigating the effect of vacation greater improvement of burnout in females has been
descriptors on health-related vacation outcome. Relevant reported elsewhere.5 Age was associated with a number
vacationer characteristics were thereby considered simul- of differences in vacation organization, such as vacation
taneously.Health-related vacation outcome was measured duration and travel time, but did not affect outcome on
using two variables derived by factor analysis of rephrased its own. Higher mental prevacation work strain was
items from a standardized German quality-of-life ques- associated with greater vacation-related recuperation, a
tionnaire21: recuperation, reflecting positive mood, well- result that is in accordance with the removal of work strain
being and perceived mental and physical fitness; and hypothesis of vacation,27 and has been found for biologic
exhaustion, reflecting depressive mood and fatigue.The and nonbiologic markers.8,23 Individuals experiencing
distinction between these two measures can be seen as high levels of work strain show greater improvements of
similar to the established distinction between positive and well-being when relieved from this strain (eg,during vaca-
negative well-being.26 tion) than do those who do not experience high work
Overall, a qualified proportion of the perceived strain. A similar result was not found for physical work
change of recuperation during vacation could be pre- strain, presumably because of its small prevalence in the
dicted, indicating that the vacation descriptors did actu- predominantly white-collar-employee study sample.
ally tap relevant aspects of vacation. In contrast, only a Of the physical vacation characteristics, vacation
relatively small amount of the change of exhaustion duration did not predict vacation outcome despite a
could be explained, presumably owing to the small positive single-order correlation. This is a surprising
increase of exhaustion reported. result, as one would assume that a longer vacation would
Of the variables describing the vacationers, sex did be more recuperating. It has been found that quality of
not affect vacation outcome in this study, although a sleep increases throughout vacation, but mood essentially
S t r a u s s - B l a s ch e e t a l , M o d e r a t i n g F a c t o r s o f Va c a t i o n 99

Table 3. Regression Analysis Predicting Vacation Outcome†


 Values Single-Order Correlations
Block Characteristics/Variables Recuperated Exhausted Recuperated Exhausted
Total R2 .30*** .15*
1. Subject characteristics
(R2  change recuperation: .08**; R2  change
exhaustion: .00)
Age .12 .09 .11 .03
Sex .10 .05 .04 .05
Physical strain of daily work .12 .00 .09 .00
Mental strain of daily work .17** .02 .20** .02
2. Physical vacation characteristics
(R2  change recuperation: .06*; R2  change
exhaustion: .07**)
Duration of vacation (d) .09 .06 .18* .03

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


Average temperature at vacation site .20** .22** .16* .13
Time-zone difference to home (h) .16* .20** .01 .17*
Time to travel to vacation destination (h) .01 .06 .07 .09
3. Individual organization of day
(R2  change recuperation: .08**; R2  change
exhaustion: .03)
Number of provided meals .08 .00 .08 .05
Self-determination of one’s daily activities .08 .13 .03 .18*
Planning the day ahead .09 .07 .17* .03
Time for one’s self and needs .21** .05 .29*** .13
4. Health and social behavior
(R2  change recuperation: .07**; R2  change
exhaustion: .01)
Physical activity during vacation .14* .08 .17* .08
Average sleep duration during vacation (h) .08 .01 .16* .03
Sleep quality .14* .04 .24* .05
Making new acquaintances .16* .02 .12 .09
5. Stress during vacation
(R2  change recuperation: .02; R2  change
exhaustion: .03*)
Health problems during vacation .07 .17* .06 .17*
Interpersonal conflicts .11 .08 .11 .08

Variables were entered in five blocks; R2  change of each block (the amount of variance explained by those variables) both for recuperation and
exhaustion as well as  values and single-order correlations are displayed (*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001).

finds its peak after 7 days.9,28 As both variables show the hand, the travel time, which ranged from 1 to 48 hours,
greatest improvement in the first 3 days, several-day did not affect vacation outcome.Although related to the
vacations may also have a relevant impact on well-being strain of travel (r = .31), the type of transportation may
and health, although vacation-related factors such as be more relevant than the duration, as has been found
travel strain and altitude have to be taken into account.28 in a recent study investigating vacation-related factors on
Another surprising result is the strong positive effect of myocardial infarction.7
the average temperature at the vacation site on both out- The individual organization of the day, for exam-
come measures. As most vacations in warmer climates ple, how vacationers planned their time, was the strongest
took place in the summer in the Mediterranean area, the predictor of recuperation among the four studied aspects
effect presumably is primarily due to sun exposure, a vari- of vacation.This was owing to the large positive impact
able known to affect mood positively.10,29 In addition, of having enough free available time for one’s self and
lifestyle factors also may contribute to this result, illus- one’s needs, which is in accordance with the notion that
trated by the association of average temperature with some relaxation and freedom of obligations are important
variables of vacation organization. aspects of leisure leading to well-being.15,30 The result
An expected result is that a greater time-zone dif- replicates a previous result obtained by our group,6 and
ference was related to greater exhaustion, pointing to the it can be seen as related to the finding that low-effort,
well-known phenomenon of jet lag,11 even though the that is, relaxing, undemanding activities, enhance after-
number of time zones crossed was moderate.On the other work recovery.31 A greater self-determination of one’s
100 J o u r n a l o f Tr a v e l M e d i c i n e , Vo l u m e 1 2 , N u m b e r 2

daily activities, however, was not related to recuperation, a style of vacation organization, rather than an effect of
as has been suggested in leisure research.14 Possibly, self- vacation organization on perceived health, thereby revers-
determination or self-control is only important in the ing the expected direction of causation.This explanation
presence of stress, as underlined by Karasek’s demand- cannot be completely ruled out but seems unlikely
control model and illustrated in the negative single- owing to the positive association of perceived workload
order correlation with postvacation exhaustion in the and positive vacation outcome. If individuals were rat-
present study.32 Also contrary to our expectations,the vari- ing their current state, then a negative association should
ables associated with how individuals structured their day be present, if any, with high workload potentially being
(number of provided meals, planning of day ahead) did associated with poorer mood and health.36,37 A report-
not affect vacation outcome. Based on our knowledge ing or memory error cannot be ruled out, but as only
of residential spa therapy and on the presumed positive effects in relation to vacation organization were assessed,
effect of exogenous pacemakers,13,33 we had imagined that a systematic memory or reporting error should not have
a moderate amount of daily structure would positively affected the results.
affect well-being and health, but this hypothesis was not

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


supported. Conclusions
Health and social behaviors also predicted recu-
peration strongly. The amount of physical activity, the The study implies that the effect of vacation can-
quality of sleep, and the extent of making new acquain- not merely be explained by the removal of work stress,
tances were all positively associated with recuperation. as has been indicated in previous studies.5,27 Rather, the
Physical activity is a known factor improving well-being way an individual organizes his or her vacation makes a
and health.16 In a recent study we found a vacation difference in regard to health-related vacation outcome.
incorporating regular walking tours to improve per- Having enough time for one’s self and one’s needs, exer-
ceived health in individuals with metabolic syndrome.28 cising, getting good sleep, and socializing in a warm
Quality of sleep not only affects well-being but is also vacation climate facilitate recuperation, especially in
affected by well-being.17 Therefore, the positive effect of vacationers reporting higher levels of prevacation men-
good sleep should be interpreted cautiously as good tal strain. On the other hand, larger time-zone differences
sleep may also be a result of greater recuperation. Sleep to home and health problems affect vacation outcome
quantity, on the other hand, did not predict vacation out- negatively.As has been stressed by Eden in a recent arti-
come, a result also in line with other research finding sleep cle,30 further studies are necessary to establish more con-
duration less relevant for health and mood,34 although sistent criteria regarding vacation description and
a positive single-order correlation was present. Making individual vacation organization and their effects on
new acquaintances was associated with greater recuper- well-being and health.
ation, presumably reflecting the well-known mood-
enhancing effect of social interaction.18 Declaration of Interests
Stress in the form of health problems—but not in
the form of interpersonal conflicts—negatively affected The authors have no financial or other conflicts of
vacation outcome, with health problems leading to interest to disclose.
increased postvacation exhaustion. Health problems are
a known phenomenon during vacation and travel and
Acknowledgment
are attributable to both environmental and psycholog-
ical factors.8,19 Although interpersonal conflicts are an
We would like to express our gratitude to Mrs.
obvious stressor affecting well-being,their impact on vaca-
Barbara Hadek for proofreading the final version of the
tion outcome could not be detected in the present
manuscript.
study.20
There are some limitations to this study that have
to be addressed. Asking the vacationers to estimate the References
degree of change from 2 weeks before to after vacation,
1. Lounsbury JW, Hoops LL.A vacation from work: changes in
may be subject to two forms of measurement error:
work and non-work outcomes. J Appl Psychol 1986;
(1) individuals may not be rating change but may be rat- 71:392–401.
ing their current state, and (2) individuals who are 2. Gump BB, Matthews KA. Are vacations good for your
appraising change may be subject to reporting or mem- health? The 9-year mortality experience after the multiple
ory error.35 If the first error applied, the results would risk factor intervention trial. Psychosom Med 2000;
reflect an association between a general mood state and 62:608–612.
S t r a u s s - B l a s ch e e t a l , M o d e r a t i n g F a c t o r s o f Va c a t i o n 101

3. Tarumi K, Hagihara A. An inquiry into the causal relation- 22. Howard GS, Ralph KM, Gulanick NA, et al. Internal inva-
ship among leisure vacation, depression, and absence from lidity in pretest-posttest self-reporting evaluations and a
work. Sangyo Ika Daigaku Zasshi 1999; 21:289–307. reevaluation of retrospective pretests. Appl Psychol Meas
4. Kubzansky LD, Kawachi I. Going to the heart of the mat- 1979; 3:1–23.
ter: do negative emotions cause coronary heart disease? J Psy- 23. Strauss-Blasche G, Ekmekcioglu C, Marktl W. Serum lipid
chosom Res 2000; 48:323–337. responses to a respite from occupational and domestic
5. Westman M, Eden D. Effects of a respite from work on demands in subjects with varying levels of stress. J Psycho-
burnout: vacation relief and fade-out. J Appl Psychol 1997; som Res 2003; 55:521–524.
82:516–527. 24. Miles J, Shevlin M.Applying regression and correlation. Lon-
6. Strauss-Blasche G, Ekmekcioglu C, Marktl W. Does vacation don: Sage, 2001.
enable recuperation? Changes in well-being associated with 25. Lounsbury JW, Hoops LL.An investigation of factors asso-
time away from work. Occup Med (Lond) 2000; 50:167–172. ciated with vacation satisfaction. J Leisure Res 1985;
7. Kop WJ,Vingerhoets A, Kruithof GJ, Gottdiener JS. Risk fac- 17(1):1–13.
tors for myocardial infarction during vacation travel. Psy- 26. Chamberlain K. On the structure of subjective well-being.
chosom Med 2003; 65:396–401. Soc Ind Res 1988; 20:581–604.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jtm/article/12/2/94/1863838 by guest on 25 June 2021


8. Vingerhoets AJ,Van Huijgevoort M,Van Heck GL. Leisure 27. Westman M, Etzion D. The impact of vacation and job
sickness: a pilot study on its prevalence, phenomenology, and stress on burnout and absenteeism. Psychol Health 2001;
background. Psychother Psychosom 2002; 71:311–317. 16:595–606.
9. Roth S, Silberer G. Urlaubsstimung und Tourismusmarket- 28. Strauss-Blasche G, Riedmann B, Schobersberger W, et al.Vaca-
ing. [Holiday mood and tourist marketing.] Planung und tion at moderate and low altitude improves perceived health
Analyse 2000; 2000(2):77–83. in individuals with metabolic syndrome. J Travel Med 2004;
10. Molin J, Mellerup E, Bolwig T, et al.The influence of climate 11:300–306.
on development of winter depression. J Affect Disord 1996; 29. Magnusson A, Boivin D. Seasonal affective disorder: an
37:151–155. overview. Chronobiol Int 2003; 20:189–207.
11. Loat CE, Rhodes EC. Jet-lag and human performance. 30. Eden D. Job stress and respite relief: overcoming high-tech
Sports Med 1989; 8:226–238. tethers. Explor Theoret Mech Perspect 2001; 1:143–194.
12. Raggatt PT, Morrissey SA.A field study of stress and fatigue 31. Sonnentag S.Work, recovery activities, and individual well-
in long-distance bus drivers. Behav Med 1997; 23:122–129. being: a diary study. J Occup Health Psychol 2001; 6:196–210.
13. Piggins HD.Human clock genes.Ann Med 2002;34:394–400. 32. de Lange AH,Taris TW, Kompier MA, et al.“The very best
14. Coleman D,Iso-Ahola SE.Leisure and health:the role of social of the millennium”: longitudinal research and the demand-
support and self-determination.J Leisure Res 1993;25:111–128. control-(support) model. J Occup Health Psychol 2003;
15. Caldwell LL, Smith EA. Leisure: an overlooked component 8:282–305.
of health promotion.Can J Public Health 1988;79(2):S44–S48. 33. Hildebrandt G, Gutenbrunner C. Die Kur—Kurverlauf,
16. Scully D, Kremer J, Meade MM, et al. Physical exercise and Kureffekt und Kurerfolg. [Spa therapy—its course, effects and
psychological well being: a critical review. Br J Sports Med health improvements.] In: Gutenbrunner C, Hildebrandt G,
1998; 32:111–120. eds. Handbuch der Balneologie and medizinischen Klima-
17. Pilcher JJ, Ott ES.The relationships between sleep and meas- tologie. Berlin: Springer, 1998:85–186.
ures of health and well-being in college students: a repeated 34. Pilcher JJ, Ginter DR, Sadowsky B. Sleep quality versus sleep
measures approach. Behav Med 1998; 23:170–178. quantity: relationships between sleep and measures of health,
18. Argyle M, Martin M.The psychological causes of happiness. well-being and sleepiness in college students. J Psychosom
In: Starck F, Argyle M, Schwarz N, eds. Subjective well- Res 1997; 42:583–596.
being. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 1991:74–100. 35. Allison PJ, Locker D, Feine JS. Quality of life: a dynamic con-
19. Rogers HL, Reilly SM. A survey of the health experiences struct. Soc Sci Med 1997; 45:221–230.
of international business travelers. Part one—physiological 36. Repetti RL. The effects of workload and the social envi-
aspects.AAOHN J 2002; 50:449–459. ronment at work and health. In: Goldberger L, Breznitz S,
20. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R, Cacioppo JT, Malarkey WB. Mar- eds. Handbook of stress. New York: The Free Press,
ital stress: immunologic, neuroendocrine, and autonomic 1993:368–385.
correlates.Ann N Y Acad Sci 1998; 840:656–663. 37. Strauss-Blasche G, Ekmekcioglu C, Marktl W. Moderating
21. Siegrist J, Broer M, Junge A. Profil der Lebensqualitaet chro- effects of vacation on reactions to work- and domestic
nisch Kranker (PLC). Goettingen: Beltz, 1996. stress. Leisure Sci 2002; 24:1–13.

You might also like