You are on page 1of 15

Regular Article

Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319– Received: August 16, 2012


331 DOI: 10.1159/000348452 Accepted after revision: January 21, 2013
Published online: August 9, 2013

Individual Music Therapy for Mental


Health Care Clients with Low Therapy
Motivation: Multicentre Randomised
Controlled Trial
Christian Golda Karin Mösslera Denise Grockeh Tor Olav Heldala
Lars Tjemslande Trond Aarref Leif Edvard Aarøb, d Hans Rittmannsbergerg
Brynjulf Stigec Jörg Assmusa Randi Rolvsjordc
a
GAMUT, Uni Health, Uni Research and b Department of Health Promotion and Development, c GAMUT, University of
Bergen, Bergen, and d Division of Mental Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, e Stavanger University
Hospital, Stavanger, and f Nordfjord Psychiatric Centre, Nordfjordeid, Norway; g State Psychiatric Clinic Wagner-Jauregg,
Linz, Austria; h University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Vic., Australia

Key Words tion-to-treat basis using generalised estimating


Motivation · Effectiveness · Pragmatic trial · equations in longitudinal linear models, controlling for
Resource-oriented music therapy · Serious mental diagnosis, site and time point. Results: MT was superior
disorders to TAU for total negative symptoms (SANS, d = 0.54, p
< 0.001) as well as functioning, clinical global
impressions, social avoidance through music, and
Abstract
vitality (all p < 0.01). Conclusion: Indi- vidual MT as
Background: Music therapy (MT) has been shown to be
conducted in routine practice is an effective addition
ef- ficacious for mental health care clients with
to usual care for mental health care clients with low
various disor- ders such as schizophrenia, depression
motivation. Copyright © 2013 S. Karger AG,
and substance abuse. Referral to MT in clinical Basel
practice is often based on other factors than diagnosis.
We aimed to examine the ef- fectiveness of resource-
oriented MT for mental health care clients with low
motivation for other therapies. Method: This was a Introduction
pragmatic parallel trial. In specialised centres in Norway,
Austria and Australia, 144 adults with non-organic mental Despite significant progress in the development of
disorders and low therapy motivation were ran- both psychosocial and pharmacological treatments for
domised to 3 months of biweekly individual, resource- people with serious mental disorders, there are still
ori- ented MT plus treatment as usual (TAU) or TAU many who do not benefit sufficiently from the usually
alone. TAU was typically intensive (71% were available therapies. Low motivation, sometimes but not
inpatients) and included the best combination of necessar- ily related to lack of insight, and usually
therapies available for each par- ticipant, excluding MT. leading to poor therapy outcome, has been described in
Blinded assessments of the Scale for the Assessment of people with seri- ous mental disorders such as
Negative Symptoms (SANS) and 15 sec- ondary schizophrenia [1–4], de- pression and bipolar disorder
outcomes were collected before randomisation and after [2, 5], and psychosomatic disorders [6, 7]. Motivation
1, 3 and 9 months. Changes were analysed on an inten- plays a central role in numer-
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel Christian Gold
0033–3190/13/0825–0319$38.00/0 GAMUT, Uni Health, Uni Research
Lars Hilles gt. 3
E-Mail karger@karger.com
NO–5015 Bergen (Norway)
www.karger.com/pps
E-Mail christian.gold @ uni.no

ous mental disorders, but its potential for treatment and Sandane, Norway, and moved to Uni Research, Bergen, Norway
for understanding mental disorders has not been fully in 2006. The trial was registered before recruitment began (trial
utilised [8, 9]. It is also related to dropout from therapy, registration No. NCT00137189) and the full study protocol pub-
which can be a significant problem [10, 11]. lished [25]. The first author had full access to all the data in the
study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for
Music therapy (MT) is an intervention where clients publication.
are encouraged to express themselves and to relate to
others through musical means. There is an emerging ev- Participants
idence base that this form of therapy can improve symp- The study included adults in mental health care who had a
toms and functioning of people with serious mental dis- low motivation for therapy. In addition to this main eligibility
criterion, we aimed to formulate as few restrictions (exclusion
orders such as schizophrenia, depression and substance- criteria) as reasonably possible. In a pragmatic trial, eligibility
related disorders [12–16]. However, music therapists do criteria should be as wide as in clinical practice, ideally including
not normally focus primarily on the client’s diagnosis. all participants with the condition of interest ‘regardless of their
Other criteria may be more important in defining the anticipated risk, responsiveness, co-morbidities or past compli-
population for whom MT is most indicated. From a re- ance’ [31]. Participants were recruited between September 2005
and December 2009 in specialised mental health care centres
source-oriented perspective, the client’s resources, (Nordfjord Psychiatric Centre, Sogn og Fjordane, Norway;
strengths and potentials, more than problems and symp- Stavanger University Hospital and Jæren District Psychiatric
toms, should be emphasised, as well as collaboration Centre, herein combined as Rogaland, Norway; State Psychiatric
and equal relationships [17, 18]. Such a perspective to Clinic Wagner-Jauregg, Linz, Austria, and Sunshine Hospital,
MT builds on a contextual understanding of therapeutic Melbourne, Australia). Ethical approval for the study was grant-
ed by all relevant committees (Regional Committee for Medical
pro- cesses [19–21], the philosophy of empowerment and Health Research Ethics Western Norway – REK Vest, Ethics
[22, 23] and positive psychology [24]. Committee of Upper Austria and Melbourne Health Ethics). All
Several clinical and research reports have described participants provided written informed consent to participate in
clients who were not able to improve through other ther- the study. Criteria for inclusion and exclusion were assessed by
apies but benefited from MT [14, 25–30]. Low the clinician who had the primary responsibility for the patient
at the hospital unit, based on information collected by the clinical
motivation for other therapies appears to be a common team. Eligible participants were informed about the study by a
reason for referral to MT [26]. After previous trials have team member, using both oral and written information, and in-
demon- strated the efficacy of MT for serious mental vited to participate. The information provided included the title
disorders, broad, practice-based trials are now needed to and aim of the study, why they were eligible, what participation
confirm its effectiveness with the populations and would comprise for them, that they would be randomised, what
information would be collected, and that they would be free to
settings where it is commonly applied. withdraw at any time without having to specify reasons.
The present study aimed to examine the effectiveness
of MT compared to treatment as usual (TAU) to Inclusion Criteria
improve the ability of clients to express themselves In line with the goal of pragmatic trials of effectiveness [31]
emotionally, to build satisfying social relationships and as well as the focus of the resource-oriented MT approach [17,
18], we selected a broad range of diagnoses. Participants were
to improve their general motivation. To answer the inpa- tients, outpatients or day patients in mental health care,
question of effective- ness, we aimed to emphasise with a primary diagnosis of a non-organic mental disorder (F1–
strongly those features that characterise clinical practice F6 ac- cording to ICD-10 criteria), as assessed by a psychiatrist
in MT in all study dimen- sions, ranging from at the study site. In addition, participants had to meet at least one
participants through interventions and comparisons to of the following criteria: (1) low motivation due to lacking or
insufficient insight; (2) difficulties talking about feelings or
outcomes [31]. problems; (3) wants a ‘medication cure’ or ‘does not believe in
talking’; (4) has not achieved sufficient improvement in previous
psychotherapy, or (5) other type of low motivation (specified).
Methods These criteria were chosen because they reflect common reasons
for referral to MT [26]. Participants could still receive other
Trial Design therapies, regardless of their motivation for or perceived benefit
This was a pragmatic [31], single-blind, multicentre ran- from them.
domised trial with two parallel groups of equal size. Treatment
duration was 3 months and duration of follow-up 9 months (fig. Exclusion Criteria
1). The study was originally planned to be conducted in Norway As the assessments for the study included several self-report
but was later extended to an international multicentre trial measures [25], people who were unable to complete these could
with four sites: Nordfjord (Norway), Rogaland (Norway), Linz not be included. Furthermore, people with a severe life-threaten-
(Austria) and Melbourne (Australia). The central randomisation ing somatic illness were not included because that would alter
Downloaded by:

office was first located at Sogn og Fjordane University College, the goals and process of therapy significantly.
UniversityDownloaded
McMaster University

320 Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 Gold et al.


DOI: 10.1159/000348452
271 assessed for eligibility

127 excluded:
29 did not meet inclusion criteria 88 did not consent
10 did not attend first contact

144 randomised

72 allocated to MT, biweekly sessions over 3 months


72 allocated to TAU
48 received 18–25 sessions
16 received 10–17 sessions
8 received 2–9 sessions

67 completed 1-month follow-up 2 lost 59 completed 1-month follow-up 11 lost


3 missing but available later 2 missing but available later

Fig. 1. Flow chart of study participants. 65 completed 3-month follow-up 7 lost 54 completed 3-month follow-up 17 lost
‘Lost’ indicates participants with missing 0 missing but available later 1 missing but available later
data at the given time point and all later
time points (including those who were lost
at a previous time point). ‘Missing’ indi-
52 completed 9-month follow-up 20 lost 48 completed 9-month follow-up 24 lost
cates participants with missing data on the
given time point but with available data
on a later time point.

Interventions
ceptable but not necessary’ and ‘proscribed’ ones [17]. This
Participants were randomly assigned to MT or TAU over a
system allowed us to describe both the similarities with
pe- riod of 3 months.
(‘essential but not unique’) and the differences to (‘unique and
essential’) other MT approaches. For example, one ‘unique and
Music Therapy
essential’ principle is en- titled ‘collaborating with the client
Participants assigned to this group were offered individual
concerning goals of therapy and methods of working’. It is
MT sessions, 2 sessions per week of 45 min each, for a period of
described that this ‘implies a shared re- sponsibility of the
3 months from the date of randomisation. Previous research has
process of therapy in which both the therapist and the client are
suggested that about 20 sessions may be needed for MT to have
using their competences towards their common goal/ agreement
an effect on people with schizophrenia [12], and that a medium
to help the client’. The relationship between therapist and client
effect of MT will be reached after 10–24 sessions on symptoms
should be ‘based upon equality and mutuality’ [17]. Oth- er
and functioning of people with serious mental disorders [14].
approaches might use a more prescriptive strategy where the
The maximum number of sessions in this study was 26, and we
ther- apist tends to make such decisions for, rather than with, the
encouraged therapists to ensure that clients received at least 18
client. By contrast, essential but not unique principles include
sessions within the 3-month period when this was possible. Ten
items that are typical for MT in general, such as ‘engaging the
academically and clinically qual- ified music therapists with
client in music interplay (such as musical improvisation, creating
clinical experience in MT in psychiatry provided MT in
songs, playing pre-composed music or listening to music)’,
accordance with a flexible therapy manual devel- oped for this
‘tuning into the client’s musical expressions’, and also ‘reflecting
study [17]. Outlining 21 principles of resource-orient- ed MT,
verbally on music and mu- sical interplay’. It is, however,
this manual describes general therapeutic attitudes and be-
important to note that the differences between approaches might
haviours (e.g. focusing on the client’s strengths and potentials)
be relatively gradual – while approaches differ in the extent to
as well as specific attitudes within the musical interaction (e.g.
which they explicitly emphasise and focus on resources, it would
tuning into the client’s musical expression). In accordance with
not be appropriate to say that some approaches only focus on
recommen- dations for psychotherapy research [32], ‘unique and
resources while others do not do so at all [17].
essential’ and ‘essential but not unique’ principles were
In training sessions as well as regular supervision meetings
described as well as ‘ac-
with therapists it was emphasised that the goal was flexible
McMaster University Downloaded by:

appli-

Music Therapy for Clients with Low Motivation Psych


other Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 DOI: 10.1159/000348452 321
cation, and that deviations may be appropriate and desirable
using one-way mirrors or video recordings as appropriate. Test
de- pending on the individual client and his/her therapeutic
ratings were continued until familiarity and agreement was
process. Therapists self-rated their own adherence and
achieved. Rating was based on a 1-hour interview with the par-
competence at the end of each session. In addition, in a
ticipant. Internal consistency in this sample was high
randomly selected 50% sub- sample of all clients in this
(Cronbach’s alpha 0.95), confirming the reliability values
condition, a randomly selected session was videotaped and
reported in earlier studies.
the therapist’s adherence and competence was assessed by
three independent observers (qualified music therapists with
Secondary Outcomes
training in resource-oriented MT principles). Each item was
As secondary outcomes we chose further measures that were
rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very
either of general relevance or that were assumed to be linked to
much), and means were calculated across the items of each
how MT works. As the goals of resource-oriented MT are gener-
category.
ally broad, following the client’s individual goals and wishes
rath- er than targeting a selected outcome, we aimed to collect a
Treatment as Usual
broad variety of outcome domains.
In line with pragmatic trials of effectiveness [31], all partici-
General symptoms were assessed using the BSI-18 self-report
pants were allowed to continue or begin any other treatment
scale, with 18 items addressing anxiety, depression and somatic
during their participation in the study. The best combination of
complaints [45]. It has demonstrated concurrent and predictive
known and available medical and non-medical therapies, as de-
validity as well as internal consistency in clinical and community
cided by the multidisciplinary team at the institution where the
samples. Functioning was measured using a blind rating with the
participant received mental health care, was offered and provid-
GAF [46]. The GAF is a widely used single-item scale which has
ed to all participants. Such concomitant treatment was moni-
demonstrated good predictive validity and interrater reliability.
tored. This excluded any MT outside the study during the first
Clinical global impressions were evaluated by a blinded assessor
3 months after randomisation. After that, participants were al-
us- ing the CGI Scale [47]. It consists of two 1-item scales –
lowed to receive some MT. The number of MT sessions during
severity (CGI-S) and change (CGI-C) – and has been widely
that period (i.e. from the 4th to the 9th month) was also moni-
used to assess treatment outcomes in mental health because of its
tored.
simplicity and intuitiveness.
Secondary outcomes specifically linked to the assumed
Outcomes
mecha- nisms of MT were as follows: the Interest in Music (IiM)
Primary Outcome
Scale, developed and validated for the present study [48], is a
Selecting an appropriate primary end point is a challenge in a
self-report measure consisting of 12 Likert-scaled items. Its two
study that is broad in terms of diagnoses. We chose a measure of
subscales, ac- tivity and engagement in music (IiM A&E) and
negative symptoms as this concept encompasses areas that are
social avoidance through music (IiM SA), have demonstrated
highly relevant for MT: the ability (or its absence) to express
reliability and valid- ity in the study sample [48].
one- self emotionally (affective flattening), the ability to form
Motivation for change was measured using a modified
and sus- tain satisfactory relationships (anhedonia/asociality) and
version of the two URICA subscales, precontemplation and
general motivation (avolition/apathy). Although negative
contemplation [49, 50]. Predictive validity, reliability and
symptoms are usually associated with schizophrenia, there is
sensitivity for change of this scale have been shown for a variety
evidence that they are relevant for other disorders as well,
of mental disorders [51, 52]. We added 2 items, ‘I hope that I
notably for affective dis- orders [33–37]. Negative symptoms in
could learn how to better in- teract with other people’ and ‘I need
themselves are strongly predictive of a need for care, irrespective
to find something meaningful to do’ to the existing 16 items. We
of the primary diagnosis or whether the negative symptoms are
used the sum score (with pre- contemplation items reversed) as a
considered primary or sec- ondary [38]. For the present study, it
simple continuous measure be- cause the categorical concept of
was important to note that people with negative symptoms often
stages of change has been criticised [53]. Internal consistency of
have little ability to lead a creative, active, self-determined and
our 18-item version of the URICA in this sample was good
social life, and that MT has previously been found to improve
(Cronbach’s alpha 0.88).
these aspects of mental health [12, 14]. Music therapists do not
Motivation was assessed by a blinded rater using the SANS
merely attempt to remove symp- toms, but try to help clients to
Avolition/Apathy Scale [41]. Self-efficacy was assessed using the
get in touch with abilities that may be lost, inaccessible or not yet
General Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale (GES) [54]. This is a self-
fully developed [39]. The concept of negative symptoms actually
re- port measure with 10 Likert-scaled items that has
describes abilities, even though it is their absence that is assessed
demonstrated test-retest reliability and internal consistency in
[40]. The primary outcome was the composite score of the Scale
both clinical and non-clinical samples. Self-esteem was measured
for the Assessment of Negative Symp- toms (SANS) [41], as
using the Rosen- berg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) [55], a self-
completed by a trained assessor blinded to randomisation status.
report measure with 10 items. The scale has been used in many
This is a sum score of 24 items with a total possible range from 0
studies. Discriminant va- lidity, test-retest reliability and internal
to 120. Validity has been demonstrated for a variety of mental
consistency have been shown for patients with mental disorders.
disorders [33, 35]. Several studies have con- firmed interrater
Vitality was assessed using the vitality subscale of the SF-36
reliability, with intraclass correlation coeffi- cients ranging from
[56]. This is a self-report scale with 4 items. It has demonstrated
0.60 to 0.92 [41]. Test-retest reliability and internal consistency
discriminant validity and sensitivity to change in schizophrenic
have been demonstrated in people with schizophrenia [41], and
patients, and internal con- sistency and test-retest reliability have
the scale has also shown sensitivity to change in previous MT
also been confirmed. Affect regulation was measured by a
trials [12, 14, 42–44]. Reliability within and across sites was
blinded rater using the SANS sub- scale, affecting flattening and
McMaster University Downloaded by:

ensured through test ratings and discussions


blunting [41]. The 7-item scale has

322 Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 DOI: 10.1159/000348452


Gol d et al.
good internal consistency; interrater reliability is moderate.
Statistical Analysis
Sensi- tivity to change following MT has been demonstrated in
Success of randomisation was verified by testing equality of
people with schizophrenia. Relational competence was assessed
means or proportions of baseline characteristics. In addition, we
using the IIP-32 [57]. This self-report scale contains 32 items
used the reverse propensity score [62] to examine any relation
describing a variety of interpersonal problems. It has
be- tween baseline SANS values and the probability of
demonstrated internal consistency in psychotherapy patients and
assignment to MT. The reverse propensity score can detect
test-retest reliability in a non-clinical sample.
baseline imbalances even if they are not associated with any
Social relationships were assessed using both a self-report
observed covariates and is therefore a powerful tool for detecting
and a blinded assessment. First, we used the social relationships
unblinding of the allocation sequence and selection bias [62].
sub- scale of the Q-LES-Q [58] to elicit the view of the
Analyses of effects were on an intention-to-treat (ITT) basis
participants themselves on the quality of their social
to provide a conservative estimate of effectiveness. This means
relationships. It has 11 face-valid items and has demonstrated
that participants who were randomised and had available
sensitivity to change, test- retest reliability and internal
outcome data were included in the analysis regardless of whether
consistency in major depression. Sec- ond, we used the SANS
or not they re- ceived the complete intervention. The ITT
anhedonia/asociality subscale [41] to ob- tain a blinded
analysis is an important feature which affects the population to
assessment of the same domain. The 5-item scale has
which results can be gener- alised. In psychotherapy studies, it is
demonstrated satisfactory interrater reliability, internal consisten-
typical that as many as 30– 60% do not complete therapy as
cy, and sensitivity to change following MT.
intended [10, 11]; in a recent study on art therapy, 40% did not
attend any session [63]. Because we intended to generalise our
Sample Size
effect estimates to all those who are of- fered MT, as opposed to
The required sample size was calculated for the primary out-
the smaller group of those who complete it, we chose an ITT
come. We assumed an effect size slightly smaller than medium (f
analysis.
= 0.20, equivalent to d = 0.40), based on the results of our
We excluded the first 10 participants from the analysis
Cochrane review [59]. For α = 0.05, 80% power, and an assumed
because recent research has indicated that these may not
36% vari- ance explained by the baseline value, the required
represent the true effects of therapy adequately due to the impact
usable sample size was calculated as 130. To allow for 10%
of implementing trial procedures [64]. We repeated all analyses
dropouts, we aimed to randomise 144 participants. The protocol
with these partici- pants included to examine the impact of trial
considered that the ac- tual power may be somewhat higher
procedures in this study. Further sensitivity analyses are
because of the multiple fol- low-up time points [25, 60].
described below.
Conversely, power was probably re- duced again through the
Distributions of data were examined graphically. For
extension to an international multicentre trial, as some clustering
normally distributed continuous variables, effects of MT versus
according to site was likely to occur [61].
TAU were analysed using generalised estimating equations
(GEE) [65] with gaussian distribution, identity link function, and
Randomisation and Masking
exchangeable correlation structure (function geeglm in R
After informed consent and baseline assessment, participants
package geepack) in longitudinal linear models. Centred baseline
were allocated to conditions using a computerised randomisation
SANS scores, diagno- sis (psychotic, F2, or other), site
procedure, stratified by treatment centre and type of disorder
(Nordfjord, Rogaland, Austria or Australia) and time (1, 3 or 9
(primary diagnosis of a psychotic disorder, F2 in ICD-10, vs.
months) were included as confound- ing variables. The
oth- er). A block size of two was chosen to balance limited
interactions of these variables with treatment group were also
resources of music therapists. As block size and the stratification
included. Effects from GEE models were trans- formed into
by diagnosis were not known to clinicians, we anticipated that
effect sizes to facilitate clinical interpretation and were
guessing future allocations would be unlikely. To conceal
interpreted in accordance with existing guidelines (i.e. 0.2 –
allocation from the in- volved clinicians and the participants, a
small effect, 0.5 – medium effect, 0.8 – large effect [66, 67]).
person who had no con- tact with the participants kept the
The CGI-C scale was not normally distributed and was analysed
randomisation list and provid- ed individual allocations of
as a dichoto- mous outcome (improved vs. not improved), using
included participants, who were iden- tified by number, age, sex
a longitudinal linear model as above but with a binomial
and diagnosis, either by e-mail or by text message.
distribution and logit link function. We applied a two-sided 5%
This was a single-blind study. The person who assessed out- significance level for the primary outcome and a two-sided 1%
comes was not involved in the daily work at the participant’s for secondary outcomes to account for multiple testing.
hospi- tal unit and was therefore not aware of the assigned
Several sensitivity analyses were conducted for the primary
treatment. Suc- cess of blinding was verified by asking assessors
outcome. First, we added age and sex as additional confounders
whether or not they had inadvertently become aware of the
into the model. Second, we assessed the potential impact of
participant’s allocation.
miss- ing data in two sensitivity analyses. Where the outcome
was unob- served, we assumed either no change from the last
Data Management
observed value (last observation carried forward), or no change
A local site investigator was appointed at each clinical site to from the baseline value. Another sensitivity analysis was planned
help to administrate the data collection and to ensure data for any unusual outliers that might have been found in the
quality. This person supervised and facilitated the data collection bivariate distribution of baseline scores and follow-up scores, but
process, ensured that assessments were reliable and timely, and was not necessary since no such outliers were found. Third, we
was respon- sible for transferral of information between the analysed therapist effects (i.e. the effects that the individual
clinical site and the central office. An administrative person at person conducting the therapy might have, as opposed to the
the central office dou- ble-checked that data entry was correct therapy method) as they may be im- portant in psychotherapy
and returned to the site investigator with questions if necessary. [21]. For this purpose, we calculated a linear mixed-effects
model (function lmer in R package lme4) for
Music Therapy for Clients with Low Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 323
Motivation DOI: 10.1159/000348452
the MT group, with client and therapist entered as nested random worked well. Assessors
factors. The remaining variables were the same as in the longitu-
dinal model. The variance due to client or therapist was
calculated from that model. R version 2.11.1 was used for all
analyses.

Results

Flow of Participants through the Study


The flow of participants is shown in figure 1. Of 88
who did not consent, 44 would have had difficulties
attending regular therapy sessions as an outpatient (i.e.
these people were either outpatients or going to be
discharged soon). The target number of 144 included
participants was ex- actly reached; 72 were randomised
to MT and 72 to TAU (Nordfjord 17 + 16, Rogaland 25
+ 27, Australia 7 + 7, Austria 23 + 22). A small number
of participants missed intermediate assessments but
were available later; the number of dropouts was
reasonable overall (details in fig. 1). All those
randomised to MT received at least some sessions (fig.
1).

Baseline Characteristics
Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the sam-
ple. About half of the participants were female. Age
ranged from 18 to 76 years. The most frequent
diagnostic groups were psychotic disorders (42%) and
mood (affec- tive) disorders (31%; table 1). Other
primary diagnoses included neurotic, stress-related and
somatoform disor- ders (F4, n = 16), personality
disorders (F6, n = 14), sub- stance-related mental
disorders (F1, n = 6), and behav- ioural syndromes
associated with physiological distur- bances (F5, n = 3);
64 participants (44%) also had one or more secondary
diagnoses and these were almost always also non-
organic mental disorders (F1–F6). More than half of the
participants were referred due to ‘difficulties talking
about feelings or problems,’ and almost half of them
had experienced limited benefit in a previous psy-
chotherapy (table 1). At the time of referral, 3 of 4 par-
ticipants (76%; table 1) had an interest in music.
Negative symptoms at baseline were at mean 38 (SD =
22; table 1), somewhat lower compared to previous
trials of MT for schizophrenia [12, 43, 44].
None of the baseline variables showed a significant
im- balance between the two study groups (all p > 0.05;
ta- ble 1). Furthermore, the reverse propensity score also
McMaster University Downloaded by:

showed no association between probability of


assignment to MT and SANS score and therefore no
indication of any otherwise unobserved selection bias. It
can be confident- ly concluded that randomisation
became aware of the participant’s correct allocation in Ten music therapists (Nordfjord 2, Rogaland 5,
8% of all cases; in another 2% assessors reported that Australia 1, Austria 2) provided MT. Eight of them were
they had become aware of the allocated treatment but female; all had completed specialised MT training as
guessed in- correctly. In no case was blinding broken was common in their country (5 bachelor, 4 master, 1
intentionally. PhD or equivalent), and all had experience with
mental health care clients. Caseload within the study
Treatments Received and Treatment Fidelity varied
At baseline, standard treatments as provided by the from 1 to 16.
mental health care institution included for most par- Participants randomised to MT received a mean
ticipants some medication (antipsychotic, n = 88, 61%; number of 17.5 (SD = 5.5) sessions during the 3
antidepressive, n = 78, 54%; mood stabilisers, n = 49, months following randomisation (means by site:
34%; other psychotropic drugs, n = 54, 38%; other Nordfjord 16, Rogaland 15, Australia 24, Austria 19;
med- ication, n = 53, 37%). In addition, 63 (44%) thereof mean = 6.4, SD = 3.8 and median = 6 during
received psychotherapy or other psychological the first month). The mean number of sessions
treatment, 6 (4%) electroconvulsive therapy, and 59 received per week dropped slightly from 1.5 during
(41%) other non- pharmacological interventions. These the first 30 days after randomi- sation to 1.1 during
latter interven- tions included supportive talks and the next 60 days. Two thirds of all participants
counselling not oth- erwise specified, occupational received at least the minimum target of 18 sessions
therapy, physiotherapy, sport therapy, art therapy, body (fig. 1). None of those randomised to TAU re- ceived
therapy, gardening, mi- lieu therapy, social work or any MT during that period. Between 3 and 9
educational programs. At later time points, we found months after randomisation, a small number of MT
similar rates with psycho- therapy/psychological sessions were provided to participants of both groups
treatment, but rates of psycho- tropic drugs and other (MT group: mean = 1.3, SD 2.8, n = 66; TAU group:
non-pharmacological interven- tions were reduced. The mean = 5.0, SD 5.8, n = 61).
number of inpatients decreased from 102 (71%) at Treatment fidelity was assessed as adherence and
baseline (table 1) to 66 (46%), 48 competence in the use of therapeutic principles. Means
(33%) and 35 (24%) at 1, 3 and 9 months, respectively. were calculated across all 1,229 MT sessions for self-

324 Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 Gold et al.


DOI: 10.1159/000348452

McMaster University Downloaded by:


Table 1. Baseline characteristics of 144 participants randomised to MT or TAU

Characteristic Total (n = 144) MT (n = 72) TAU (n = 72)


Age 33.99±11.33 33.81±11.03 34.17±11.70
Male 75 (52) 43 (60) 32 (44)
Setting
Inpatient 102 (71) 55 (76) 47 (65)
Day patient 6 (4) 2 (3) 4 (6)
Outpatient 36 (25) 15 (21) 21 (29)
Diagnosis
F2 Psychotic 61 (42) 31 (43) 30 (42)
F3 Affective 44 (31) 26 (36) 18 (25)
Other1 39 (27) 15 (21) 24 (33)
Type of low motivation2
Insufficient insight 35 (24) 16 (22) 19 (26)
Difficulty talking about feelings/problems 85 (59) 40 (56) 45 (63)
Does not believe in talking 10 (7) 3 (4) 7 (10)
Insufficient improvement in psychotherapy 71 (49) 40 (56) 31 (43)
Other 15 (10) 4 (6) 11 (15)
Prior interest in music 119 (83) 64 (89) 55 (76)
Scores
Negative symptoms (SANS, n = 141)3 37.67±22.28 39.71±23.98 35.54±20.32
General symptoms (BSI-18, n = 140)3 42.3±15.68 42.97±14.96 41.63±16.45
Functioning (GAF, n = 143)4 48.15±10.99 47.1±10.85 49.23±11.1
Clinical global impressions (CGI-S, n = 142)3 4.2±1.16 4.18±1.18 4.21±1.14
Activity and engagement in music (IiM A&E, n = 141)4 36.72±8.68 36.25±8.55 37.19±8.85
Social avoidance through music (IiM SA, n = 141)3 5.04±2.29 5.08±2.03 4.99±2.55
Motivation for change (URICA, n = 141)4 73.23±12.02 73.15±11.17 73.3±12.91
Motivation (SANS, n = 142)3 7.71±4.18 7.78±4.41 7.64±3.95
Self-efficacy (GSE, n = 141)4 30.59±9.42 29.89±9.47 31.3±9.39
Self-esteem (RSE, n = 140)4 30.49±10.13 30±9.58 30.99±10.71
Vitality (SF-36, n = 139)4 10.59±4.31 10.48±4.34 10.71±4.31
Affect regulation (SANS, n = 141)3 10.11±8.6 11.34±9.1 8.86±7.94
Relational competencies (IIP-32, n = 139)3 77.96±17.43 78.82±18.09 77.06±16.8
Social relationships (self; Q-LES-Q, n = 140)4 33.99±7.86 33.03±8.09 34.94±7.55
Social relationships (observer; SANS, n = 140)3 10.44±5.9 11.38±6.24 9.46±5.4

Values are means ± SD or n (%).


1
Including primary diagnoses within F1 (n = 6, 4%), F4 (n = 16, 11%), F5 (n = 3, 3%) and F6 (n = 14, 10%).
2
Overlapping categories, not mutually exclusive.
3
Negative outcome: high scores indicate poor clinical outcome.
4
Positive outcome: high scores indicate good clinical outcome.

rated adherence and competence, and across 34 ran-


observer-rated: mean = 0.78). ‘Proscribed’ principles
domly selected videotaped sessions for observer-rated
were rarely used (self-rated: mean = 0.06; observer-rat-
adherence and competence [25]. In terms of adherence,
ed: mean = 0.09). Competence ratings were high for all
‘unique and essential’ principles were commonly used
categories of principles (ranging from 2.70 to 3.07 for
(self-rated: mean = 2.74; observer-rated: mean = 2.72;
self-rated and from 2.63 to 3.08 for observer-rated com-
all on a scale from 0 to 4), as were ‘essential but not
petence). This indicates that the principles were typi-
unique’ principles (self-rated: mean = 2.11; observer-
cally applied in a competent way [17]. Contents of MT
rated: mean = 2.14). ‘Acceptable but not necessary’
sessions provided as part of this study have also been
principles were less common (self-rated: mean = 0.75;
analysed elsewhere [68, 69].
McMaster University Downloaded by:

Music Therapy for Clients with Low Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 325
Motivation DOI: 10.1159/000348452
Table 2. Effects of MT vs. TAU on continuous outcomes

Variable Excluding first 10 participants Including all participants

difference p value effect size difference p value effect size


between d (95% CI)2 between d (95%
groups (SE)1 CI)2 groups (SE)1

Negative symptoms (SANS)3 –12.09 (3.43) 0.000*** –0.54 (–0.84,–0.24) –8.54 (3.62) 0.018* –0.38 (–0.7,–0.06)
General symptoms (BSI-18)3 –2.57 (2.87) 0.371 –0.16 (–0.52, 0.2) –0.3 (2.94) 0.919 –0.02 (–0.39, 0.35)
Functioning (GAF)4 6.99 (2.38) 0.003** 0.64 (0.21, 1.06) 4.9 (2.35) 0.037* 0.45 (0.03, 0.86)
Clinical global impressions (CGI-S)3 –1.06 (0.25) 0.000*** –0.91 (–1.33,–0.5) –0.56 (0.31) 0.065 –0.49 (–1.01, 0.03)
Activity and engagement in music (IiM A&E) 4 –1.45 (1.65) 0.381 –0.17 (–0.54, 0.21) –1.9 (1.49) 0.201 –0.22 (–0.56, 0.12)
Social avoidance through music (IiM SA)3 –1.22 (0.45) 0.007** –0.53 (–0.92,–0.15) –0.73 (0.44) 0.095 –0.32 (–0.69, 0.06)
Motivation for change (URICA)4 –4.45 (3.15) 0.157 –0.37 (–0.88, 0.14) –2.6 (3.26) 0.426 –0.22 (–0.75, 0.32)
Motivation (SANS)3 –2.24 (1) 0.026* –0.54 (–1.01,–0.06) –1.12 (1.04) 0.281 –0.27 (–0.76, 0.22)
Self-efficacy (GSE)4 2.12 (1.96) 0.280 0.23 (–0.18, 0.63) 1.72 (2) 0.389 0.18 (–0.23, 0.6)
Self-esteem (RSE)4 0.83 (1.72) 0.631 0.08 (–0.25, 0.42) –0.09 (1.73) 0.960 –0.01 (–0.34, 0.33)
0.34 0.87) 4
(–0.18,(SF-36)
Vitality 2.87 (1.08) 0.008** 0.67 (0.18, 1.16) 1.48 (1.15) 0.196
Affect
–0.28 regulation
(–0.57, 0) (SANS)3 –3.03 (1.27) 0.017* –0.35 (–0.64,–0.06) –2.45 (1.26) 0.051
Relational competencies (IIP-32)3 –0.64 (3.45) 0.854 –0.04 (–0.42, 0.35) –1.27 (3.15) 0.687 –0.07 (–0.43, 0.28)
Social relationships (self; Q-LES-Q)4 2.91 (1.81) 0.108 0.37 (–0.08, 0.82) 2.8 (1.9) 0.140 0.36 (–0.12, 0.83)
Social relationships (observer; SANS)3 –3.16 (1.59) 0.047* –0.54 (–1.06,–0.01) –2.52 (1.43) 0.078 –0.43 (–0.9, 0.05)

All analyses ITT.


1
Estimate from GEE.
2
Effect sizes were calculated by dividing the difference between groups from this table by the common SD from table 1.
3
Negative outcome: high scores indicate poor clinical outcome; negative differences indicate effects in favour of MT.
4
Positive outcome: high scores indicate good clinical outcome; positive differences indicate effects in favour of MT.
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Effects of MT
cial relationships (observer-rated) and social avoidance
The GEE results for continuous outcomes are shown
through music. Effects on the remaining variables were
in table 2. The effect on the primary outcome, negative
smaller than medium (d < 0.50; fig. 2, left panel). The
symptoms, was significant in favour of MT (p < 0.001)
right panel of figure 2 shows how including the first
with an effect size in the medium range (mean
10 participants reduced the effect sizes.
difference in SANS scores 12.09, d = 0.54; table 2).
No adverse events were observed. We noted that
Among the sec- ondary outcomes, effects on
drop- out rates were lower in MT than in TAU at 1 and
functioning, clinical global impressions, social
3 months (1 month: 2 vs. 11, OR 6.24, p = 0.02; 3
avoidance through music, and vitality were all
months: 7 vs. 17, OR 2.85, p = 0.04; see numbers lost in
significant (all p < 0.01; table 2). Tendencies were
fig. 1) but not at 9 months.
found for motivation, affect regulation and social rela-
tionships (all p < 0.05; table 2). Including the first 10
Sensitivity Analyses and Therapist Effects
par- ticipants reduced the effects considerably, but the
Model diagnostic plots for the primary outcome
effect on the primary outcome remained significant (p =
showed no associations between fitted values and
0.018; table 2). The GEE model for improvement in
residu- als. The effect of MT remained significant: (1)
clinical global impressions (CGI-C) showed a tendency
when age and sex were entered as additional
in favour of MT (log odds 1.45, SE 0.58, p = 0.013).
confounders, (2) when the last observed value was
The effect on this outcome corresponded to an odds
inserted for missing data (last observation carried
ratio (OR) of 4.28 (95% CI 1.37–13.42). Figure 2 shows
forward), (3) when the baseline value was inserted for
all effect sizes and confidence intervals. It can be seen
missing data and (4) when unblinded participants were
that the largest effect (d > 0.80) was observed on
excluded. Therapist effects were negli- gible: less than
clinical global impressions, followed by medium-to-
0.1% of the variance was due to site and therapist.
large effects (0.50 < d < 0.80) on vitality, functioning,
negative symptoms, motivation, so-
McMaster University Downloaded by:

326 Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 Gold et al.


DOI: 10.1159/000348452
Clinical global impressions
Vitality Functioning Negative symptoms
Motivation Social relationships (observer) Social avoidance through music Social relationships (self)
Affect regulation Self-efficacy General symptoms
Self-esteem Relational competencies
Activity and engagement in music
Motivation for change

–0.8–0.5–0.2 0.20.50.8 –0.8–0.5–0.2


Effect size (excl. first 10)Effect size (incl. all participant

Fig. 2. Effect sizes of MT vs. TAU. All analyses were ITT. Effect sizes and 95% CIs were reversed where necessary so that a
positive effect size always indicates an effect in favour of MT. Effect sizes excluding the first 10 participants and effect sizes
including all participants are shown.

Discussion
Our results on attendance rates are also interesting.
All participants allocated to MT received at least some
Following previous trials documenting the effects of
sessions; two thirds of them received at least 18
MT in specific serious mental disorders such as schizo-
sessions. Dropout rates as well as non-completer rates
phrenia [12], depression [13] and substance abuse [16],
were gen- erally low. This compares favourably to
this practice-based trial suggests the effectiveness of
previous studies [10, 11, 63, 70], especially when
MT in mental health care clients with low therapy
considering that this study was focused on clients with
motiva- tion. Compared to previous trials, this trial was
low motivation for therapy. The comparison of dropout
more pragmatic [31], i.e. more targeted at effectiveness
rates between groups (where dropout was defined as not
in real clinical practice than at efficacy in optimal
attending the psychiatric assessment) also suggested that
circumstanc- es. This was reflected in several
MT might help to keep clients in contact with
dimensions: inclusion criteria were broad and in line
psychiatry.
with common referral criteria for MT; procedures for
assessment and therapy were placed in a usual clinical
For Whom Does MT Work?
setting; the control condi- tion was the best usually
One of the most difficult questions in MT is which
available treatment, and out- comes were also broad.
cli- ents will benefit from it the most. Previous studies
Thus, although the question of efficacy versus
have typically focused on medical diagnosis, which has
effectiveness is not a dichotomy [31], the present study
obvi- ous advantages when communicating findings
demonstrated the effectiveness of MT in routine adult
within a medical system. Factors such as treatment
mental health care. It should be noted that this
history are only beginning to be incorporated into trial
effectiveness was found in addition to an al- ready
designs [71]. Without aiming to diminish the importance
highly developed and often intensive standard
of medical diagnoses, the present study adds other
treatment.
dimensions that previous authors have suggested may
McMaster University Downloaded by:

be of importance.

Music Therapy for Clients with Low Motivation Psych


other Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 DOI: 10.1159/000348452 327
Namely, MT may be most important for those who do due to the impact of implementing trial procedures [64].
not benefit from other approaches, for example because Like the previous trials, the present study also used
they are unable to use verbal discourse therapeutically expe- rienced therapists, so experience with the therapy
[14, 26, 29, 30]. This is not just a ‘residual category’ but method does not explain the shifting effects. Instead, the
may tell us something about the nature of the (collec- tive) experience of study therapists with
therapeutic poten- tial of music. conducting ther- apy in the context of a trial is what
matters. Resource- oriented MT is characterised by a
How Does MT Work? strong emphasis on the emerging process between
Music is a social art [39]. The social aspects of therapist and client, as op- posed to fixed therapeutic
creating music together in a therapeutic setting are techniques. It may therefore be particularly vulnerable
emphasised by many MT approaches, but especially so to disruptions caused by trial pro- cedures, and
in resource-ori- ented MT [18]. Social abilities, often especially so when delivered in a one-to-one setting.
quite basic ones, are reflected in the outcomes negative Further research will be necessary to fully under- stand
symptoms, social re- lationships and social avoidance this phenomenon of ‘shifting effects’ [64], and also to
through music, but also more globally in clinical global examine more precisely the number of initial partici-
impressions, functioning, motivation and vitality. The pants after whom effects are likely to stabilise at the
present study suggests that MT helps clients to improve nor- mal level.
and develop these abilities. This is important because
impairments in the ability to build and sustain satisfying Limitations
relationships with others are at the core of many mental Since the majority of participants were inpatients at
health problems. the time of their enrolment, and most had either a
Musical elements are important in early develop- psychotic or affective disorder, the study is
ment of communication skills in humans [72]. The generalisable primarily to similar settings and
abil- ity to communicate musically is engraved early diagnoses. The sample size in this study was sufficient
in our ontogenetic development and precedes the to detect an effect on a distal end point in an overall
acquisition of verbal language. The concept of analysis, but limited our ability to perform more
vitality [72], which can be described in musical detailed analyses. Another factor that limited test power
terms, is much more com- prehensive than emotional was the clinical heterogeneity of our sample in terms of
expression or communica- tion alone. The potential diagnoses. Additionally, our study did not at- tempt to
of music in this area is reflect- ed in the outcomes of control for the unspecific or ‘common’ effects of
vitality and affect regulation, but also in some of the therapeutic attention that are shared by all bona fide
broader outcomes such as negative symptoms. mod- els of psychotherapy [21], including MT.
The social and the developmental perspective togeth- Resource-ori- ented MT is, however, informed by
er may also explain why music is often felt to be common factors re- search in psychotherapy [17, 18,
motivat- ing by clients [30]. The motivating feature of 21]. In this approach, im- portance is assigned to factors
MT is re- flected in this study in several ways. First, the that might be seen as coincidental in other approaches
study showed effects with a population that experiences such as credibility, au- thenticity and belief. Empirical
little benefit from and motivation for other therapies. and theoretical research suggests that it is difficult to
Second, we found MT to have an effect on motivation. separate ‘active’ from ‘non- active’ (or specific from
Third, the delayed dropout in the MT group compared non-specific) ingredients in psy- chotherapy where both
to TAU in- dicated that MT helped to keep participants types of ingredients are of psycho- logical nature [21].
in contact with mental health care for a longer time, Therefore, it is logically impossible to construct a
which is again likely to reflect their increased convincing ‘placebo’ therapy that contains ex- actly the
motivation. In contrast, motivation to change showed a same amount of all non-specific ingredients as the
small decreasing ten- dency, as might be expected at the experimental intervention and none of the specific in-
end of a successful therapy. gredients [73]. In another equally important line of
argu- ment, ‘usual care’ or the ‘best available
Impact of Implementing Trial Procedures management strat- egy’ characterises a pragmatic trial
The present study confirmed previous findings that whereas ‘restricted flexibility’ and ‘placebo’ would be
the first participants in a trial of a complex intervention more characteristic of an explanatory trial [31]. TAU (or
may not reflect the typical effects of that intervention standard care) is used as a relevant comparison in many
well, contemporary trials [13, 16, 74–78]. It is generally
considered as ‘a sensible control in many settings,
(although) it is (by definition) a
328 Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 Gold et al.
DOI: 10.1159/000348452

heterogeneous intervention’ [79] (p. 20). It was also the and may not be meaningful if done too rigorously [17,
most appropriate control condition in this study because 83], but in spite of differences in philosophical and
we aimed to examined effectiveness under usual prac- tical approaches that inevitably exist across
circum- stances in order to ‘help users choose between countries, it was possible to find a common basis in this
options for care’ [31]. study. This might pave the way for further international
randomised controlled trials of psychosocial
Implications for Practice interventions in the fu- ture [84].
Mental health care clients with low motivation are
of- ten referred to MT in clinical practice [26].
Indications for MT, as for other psychotherapies, may
Acknowledgements
be related more to such ‘soft’ factors than to a particular
psychiatric diagnosis [14]. The findings from the This study was supported by grants from the Research
present study sug- gest that MT may be a valuable Council of Norway (project No. 186025 in the Mental Health
addition to usual mental health care for clients where programme and project No. 158700/V10 in the SHP
verbal psychotherapy is of limited help. Related programme), the Western Norway Regional Health Authority,
Førde Health Trust, Norway, and the University of Melbourne,
findings from observational studies [68], qualitative Australia (Melbourne Research Office, International
research [18] and clinical case studies [18, 69] suggest Collaborative Award). Intramural support was provided by the
that active involvement of clients is cru- cial and that participating hospitals: Nordfjord Psychiatric Centre (Sogn og
using music may facilitate the active engage- ment of Fjordane, Norway); Stavanger University Hospi- tal and Jæren
clients in a therapeutic process. The concrete form in District Psychiatric Centre (Rogaland, Norway); State
Psychiatric Clinic Wagner-Jauregg Linz (Austria), and Sun-
which music is used, therefore, needs to be cho- sen shine Hospital Melbourne (Australia). The funding sources had
together with the client. In addition to traditional no role in the study design, in the collection, analysis and
modalities such as improvisation and listening to music, interpreta- tion of data, in the writing of the report, and in the
there is now some research to suggest that reproducing decision to submit the paper for publication.
music (i.e. singing or learning to play existing pieces) Aslaug Frafjord, Katharina Fuchs, Inger Marie Karterud,
Jason Kenner, Marianne Lygren, Sissel Næsheim and Ingrid
may be both particularly common and successful with Petersen (in addition to co-authors T.O.H., K.M. and R.R.)
this client group [68]. The meaning that is ascribed to conducted MT. Stig Heskestad helped to implement the study at
the music may also vary. It has been discussed in terms Jæren DPS, Rogaland, Norway. Bruce Wampold provided
of concepts such as analogy or metaphor [80], but other valuable insights on the analy- sis and interpretation of therapist
factors such as enjoyment, engagement and meaning are effects. We would like to thank all participants for their
willingness to contribute to this research, and all collaborators
also important [81]. For the therapist, it is helpful to fo- for their help with collecting data and ensur- ing data quality.
cus therapeutic attention on the strengths and potentials
of the clients rather than directing too much attention
towards problems and symptoms [17, 18].
Disclosure Statement
Implications for Research C.G., K.M., D.G., T.O.H., B.S. and R.R. are clinically trained
More research is needed to understand the mecha- music therapists. L.T., T.A., L.E.A., H.R. and J.A. declare that
nisms of change in MT. It will be relevant to they have no conflicts of interest.
determine how variation in treatment fidelity as
measured in the present study might affect its
effectiveness. Future stud- ies of therapies for mental
health care clients with low motivation should further References 1 David AS: Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychi-
atry 1990;156:798–808.
aim to include cost-effective- ness and head-to-head 2 Markova IS, Berrios GE: The assessment of
comparisons, for example to cognitive behaviour insight in clinical psychiatry: a new scale.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1992;86:159–164.
therapy or to pharmacological treatments (both of 3 Markova IS, Roberts KH, Gallagher C, Boos
which seem to have limited effects on negative H, McKenna PJ, Berrios GE: Assessment of
symptoms) [82]. Also of interest would be studies insight in psychosis: a re-standardization of a
new scale. Psychiatry Res 2003;119:81–88.
comparing resource-oriented MT to other re- source- 4 McEvoy JP, Aland J, Wilson WH, Guy W,
oriented therapy and/or to other MT. The pres- ent Hawkins L: Measuring chronic
McMaster University Downloaded by:

study has demonstrated that international multicen- schizophrenic patients’ attitudes toward
their illness and treatment. Hosp
tre trials of psychosocial interventions are possible. Community Psychiatry 1981; 32:856–858.
Standardization of psychosocial therapies is difficult
Music Therapy for Clients with Low Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 329
Motivation DOI: 10.1159/000348452
5 Ghaemi SN, Stoll AL, Pope HG: Lack of in-
22 Fitzsimons S, Fuller R: Empowerment and 36 Kitamura T, Suga R: Depressive and
sight in bipolar disorder: the acute manic epi-
its implications for clinical practise in negative symptoms in major psychiatric
sode. J Nerv Ment Dis 1995;183:464–467.
mental health: a review. J Ment Health disorders. Compr Psychiatry 1991;32:88–
6 Breisacher S, Ries H, Bischoff C, Ehrhard M:
2002;11:481– 499. 94.
Evaluation of the ‘Psychosomatic Group
23 Sprague J, Hayes J: Self-determination and 37 Milak MS, Aniskin DB, Eisenberg DP,
Ther- apy’ (PSG) (in German). Psychother
empowerment: a feminist standpoint Prikho- jan A, Cohen LJ, Yard SS, Galynker
Psycho- som Med Psychol 2003;53:302–309.
analysis of talk about disability. Am J II: The negative syndrome as a dimension:
7 Freyberger H, Kunsebeck HW, Lempa W,
Community Psy- chol 2000;28:671–695. factor analyses of PANSS in major depressive
Wellmann W, Avenarius HJ: Psychothera-
24 Seligman ME: Positive psychology, positive disor- der and organic brain disease compared
peutic interventions in alexithymic patients,
prevention and positive therapy; in Snyder with negative syndrome structures found in
with special regard to ulcerative colitis and
CR, Lopez SJ (eds): Handbook of Positive the schizophrenia literature. Cogn Behav
Crohn patients. Psychother Psychosom
Psy- chology. New York, Oxford University Neurol 2007;20:113–120.
1985; 44:72–81.
Press, 2002, pp 3–9. 38 van Os J: ‘Salience syndrome’ replaces
8 Henderson S: The neglect of volition. Br J
25 Gold C, Rolvsjord R, Aaro LE, Aarre T, ‘schizo- phrenia’ in DSM-V and ICD-11:
Psy- chiatry 2005;186:273–274.
Tjemsland L, Stige B: Resource-oriented psychiatry’s evidence-based entry into the
9 Hasnain M, Victor W, Vieweg R: Do we truly
mu- sic therapy for psychiatric patients with 21st century? Acta Psychiatr Scand
appreciate how difficult it is for patients with
low therapy motivation: protocol for a ran- 2009;120:363–372.
schizophrenia to adapt a healthy lifestyle?
domised controlled trial (NCT00137189). 39 Procter S: Reparative musicing: thinking on
Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011;123:409–410.
BMC Psychiatry 2005;5:39. the usefulness of social capital theory within
10 Swift JK, Callahan JL: Decreasing
26 Hannibal N: Beskrivelse av patientpopula- music therapy. Nord J Music Ther 2011;20:
treatment dropout by addressing
tionen i klinisk musikterapi på fem psyki- 242–262.
expectations for treat- ment length.
atriske institutioner i danmark i perioden 40 Bentall RP: Madness Explained: Psychosis
Psychother Res 2011;21:193– 200.
au- gust 2003–july 2004; in Ochsner Ridder and Human Nature. London, Penguin, 2004.
11 Barnicot K, Katsakou C, Marougka S, Priebe
HM, Nygaard Pedersen I, Hannibal N (eds): 41 Andreasen NC: Scale for the assessment of
S: Treatment completion in psychotherapy
Musikterapi i Psykiatrien. Aalborg, positive symptoms (SAPS) and scale for the
for borderline personality disorder: a system-
Musikter- apiklinikken, Aalborg assessment of negative symptoms (SANS); in
atic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr
Psykiatriske Sygehus, Aalborg Universitet, American Psychiatric Association (ed): Hand-
Scand 2011;123:327–338.
2005, pp 64–75. book of Psychiatric Measures. Washington,
12 Mössler K, Chen XJ, Heldal TO, Gold C: Mu-
27 Hanser SB, Thompson LW: Effects of a music American Psychiatric Association, 2000, pp
sic therapy for schizophrenia and schizophre-
therapy strategy on depressed older adults. J 498–501.
nia-like disorders. Cochrane Database Syst
Gerontol 1994;49:265–269. 42 Ulrich G, Houtmans T, Gold C: The addition-
Rev 2011:CD004025.
28 Meschede HG, Bender W, Pfeiffer H: Music al therapeutic effect of group music therapy
13 Erkkilä J, Punkanen M, Fachner J, Ala-Ruona
therapy with psychiatric problem patients (in for schizophrenic patients: a randomized
E, Pöntiö I, Tervaniemi M, Vanhala M, Gold
German). Psychother Psychosom Med Psy- study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007;116:362–
C: Individual music therapy for depression:
chol 1983;33:101–106. 370.
randomised controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry
29 Rolvsjord R: Sophie learns to play her 43 Tang W, Yao X, Zheng Z: Rehabilitative
2011;199:132–139.
songs of tears: a case study exploring the effect of music therapy for residual
14 Gold C, Solli HP, Krüger V, Lie SA: Dose-re-
dialectics be- tween didactic and schizophrenia: a one-month randomised
sponse relationship in music therapy for peo-
psychotherapeutic music therapy practices. controlled trial in Shanghai. Br J Psychiatry
ple with serious mental disorders: systematic
Nord J Music Ther 2001;10: 77–85. 1994; 165(suppl 24):38–44.
review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev
30 Solli HP: ‘Shut up and play!’ 44 Yang WY, Li Z, Weng YZ, Zhang HY, Ma
2009;29:193–207.
Improvisational use of popular music for a B: Psychosocial rehabilitation effects of
15 Maratos A, Gold C, Wang X, Crawford M:
man with schizo- phrenia. Nord J Music music therapy in chronic schizophrenia.
Music therapy for depression. Cochrane Da-
Ther 2008;17:67–77. Hong Kong J Psychiatry 1998;8:38–40.
tabase Syst Rev 2008:CD004517.
31 Thorpe KE, Zwarenstein M, Oxman AD, 45 Zabora J, BrintzenhofeSzoc K, Jacobsen P,
16 Albornoz Y: The effects of group
Tre- week S, Furberg CD, Altman DG, Curbow B, Piantadosi S, Hooker C, Owens A,
improvisa- tional music therapy on
Tunis S, Bergel E, Harvey I, Magid DJ, Derogatis L: A new psychosocial screening
depression in adoles- cents and adults with
Chalkidou K: A pragmatic-explanatory in- strument for use with cancer patients. Psy-
substance abuse: a ran- domized controlled
continuum indicator summary (PRECIS): a chosomatics 2001;42:241–246.
trial. Nord J Music Ther 2011;20:208–224.
tool to help trial de- signers. J Clin 46 Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Endicott J: Global As-
17 Rolvsjord R, Gold C, Stige B: Research
Epidemiol 2009;62:464–475. sessment Scale (GAS), Global Assessment of
rigour and therapeutic flexibility: rationale for
32 Waltz J, Addis ME, Koerner K, Jacobson Functioning (GAF) Scale, Social and Occupa-
a ther- apy manual developed for a
NS: Testing the integrity of a psychotherapy tional Functioning Assessment Scale
randomised con- trolled trial. Nord J Music
pro- tocol: assessment of adherence and (SOFAS); in American Psychiatric
Ther 2005;14:15–32.
compe- tence. J Consult Clin Psychol Association (ed): Handbook of Psychiatric
18 Rolvsjord R: Resource-Oriented Music
1993;61:620– 630. Measures. Washing- ton, American
Ther- apy in Mental Health Care. Gilsum,
33 Bottlender R, Sato T, Groll C, Jager M, Psychiatric Association, 2000.
Barcelona Publishers, 2010.
Kunze I, Moller HJ: Negative symptoms in 47 Guy W: Clinical Global Impressions (CGI)
19 Bohart AC: The client is the most important
de- pressed and schizophrenic patients: how Scale; in American Psychiatric Association
common factor: clients’ self-healing capaci-
do they differ? J Clin Psychiatry (ed): Handbook of Psychiatric Measures.
ties and psychotherapy. J Psychother Integr
2003;64:954– 958. Washing- ton, American Psychiatric
2000;10:127–149.
34 Gerbaldo H, Fickinger MP, Wetzel H, Association, 2000.
20 Frank JD, Frank JB: Persuasion and
Helisch A, Philipp M, Benkert O: Primary 48 Gold C, Rolvsjord R, Mössler K, Stige B:
Healing. A Comparative Study of
enduring negative symptoms in Reli- ability and validity of a scale to measure
Psychotherapy. Bal- timore, Hopkins, 1991.
schizophrenia and ma- jor depression. J inter- est in music among clients in mental
McMaster University Downloaded by:

21 Wampold BE: The Great Psychotherapy


Psychiatr Res 1995;29:297– 302. health care. Psychol Music, 2012 (doi:
De- bate: Models, Methods and Findings.
35 Gerbaldo H, Philipp M: The deficit 10.1177/ 0305735612441739).
Mah- wah, Erlbaum, 2001.
syndrome in schizophrenic and 49 University of Rhode Island: University of
nonschizophrenic pa- tients: preliminary Rhode Island Change Assessment Scale (URI-
studies. Psychopathology 1995;28:55–63. CA): psychotherapy version, 2004. Http://
www.Umbc.Edu/psyc/habits/urica%20
psychotherapy.Htm (retrieved 19 July 2004).

330 Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 Gold et al.


DOI: 10.1159/000348452
50 University of Rhode Island: URICA: Instru-
62 Berger VW: The reverse propensity score to 75 Gomes BC, Abreu LN, Brietzke E, Caetano
ment Information, 2004.
detect selection bias and correct for baseline SC, Kleinman A, Nery FG, Lafer B: A ran-
Http://www.Umbc.
imbalances. Stat Med 2005;24:2777–2787. domized controlled trial of cognitive behav-
Edu/psyc/habits/urica.Html (retrieved 19
63 Crawford MJ, Killaspy H, Barnes TR, Barrett ioral group therapy for bipolar disorder. Psy-
July 2004).
B, Byford S, Clayton K, Dinsmore J, Floyd S, chother Psychosom 2011;80:144–150.
51 Dozois DJ, Westra HA, Collins KA, Fung TS,
Hoad- ley A, Johnson T, Kalaitzaki E, King 76 Bonsack C, Manetti SG, Favrod J, Montagrin
Garry JK: Stages of change in anxiety:
M, Leurent B, Maratos A, O’Neill FA, Osborn Y, Besson J, Bovet P, Conus P: Motivational
psycho- metric properties of the University of
DP, Patterson S, Soteriou T, Tyrer P, Waller D: inter- vention to reduce cannabis use in young
Rhode Island Change Assessment (URICA)
Group art ther- apy as an adjunctive treatment people with psychosis: a randomized
Scale. Behav Res Ther 2004;42:711–729.
for people with schizophrenia: multicentre controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom
52 Hasler G, Klaghofer R, Buddeberg C: The
pragmatic ran- domised trial. BMJ 2011;80:287–297.
University of Rhode Island Change Assess-
2012;344:e846. 77 Bos EH, van Wel EB, Appelo MT, Verbraak
ment Scale (URICA). Psychother Psychosom
64 Gold C, Erkkilä J, Crawford MJ: Shifting ef- MJ: Effectiveness of systems training for
Med Psychol 2003;53:406–411.
fects in three randomised controlled trials of emo- tional predictability and problem solving
53 Littell JH, Girvin H: Stages of change. A cri-
a complex intervention. Acta Psychiatr Scand (STEPPS) for borderline personality prob-
tique. Behav Modif 2002;26:223–273.
2012;126:307–314. lems in a ‘real-world’ sample: moderation by
54 Røysamb E, Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M:
65 Zeger SL, Liang KY: Longitudinal data diagnosis or severity? Psychother Psychosom
Nor- wegian version of the General
analy- sis for discrete and continuous 2011;80:173–181.
Perceived Self- Efficacy Scale, 1998.
outcomes. Biometrics 1986;42:121–130. 78 Gattino G, Riesgo R, Longo D, Leite J,
Http://userpage.Fu-ber-
66 Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the be- Faccini L: Effects of relational music therapy
lin.De/∼health/norway.Htm (retrieved 19 havioral sciences, ed 2. Hillsdale, Erlbaum, on com- munication of children with autism: a
July 2004).
1988. ran- domized controlled study. Nord J Music
55 Rosenberg M: Society and the Adolescent
67 Gold C: The use of effect sizes in music Ther 2011;20:142–154.
Self-Image, ed rev. Middletown, Wesleyan
ther- apy research. Music Ther Perspect 79 Roth A, Fonagy P: What Works for Whom? A
University Press, 1989.
2004;22: 91–95. Critical Review of Psychotherapy Research,
56 Ware JE: SF-36 Health Survey (SF-36); in
68 Mössler K, Assmus J, Heldal TO, Fuchs K, ed 2. New York, Guilford Press, 2005.
American Psychiatric Association (ed): Hand-
Gold C: Music therapeutic techniques as pre- 80 Smeijsters H: Analogy and metaphor in
book of Psychiatric Measures. Washington,
dictors of change in mental health care. Art music therapy: theory and practice. Nord J
American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
Psychother 2012;39:333–341. Music Ther 2012;21:227–249.
57 Barkham M, Hardy GE, Startup M: The IIP-
69 Mössler K, Fuchs K, Heldal TO, Karterud I- 81 Baker FA, Ballantyne J: ‘You’ve got to
32: a short version of the Inventory of
M, Kenner J, Næsheim S, Gold C: The clini- accen- tuate the positive’: Group
Interperson- al Problems. Br J Clin Psychol
cal application and relevance of resource-ori- songwriting to pro- mote a life of
1996;35:21–35.
ented principles in music therapy within an enjoyment, engagement and meaning in
58 Endicott J, Nee J, Harrison W, Blumenthal R:
international multicentre study in psychiatry. aging Australians. Nord J Music Ther
Quality of Life Enjoyment and Satisfaction
Br J Music Ther 2011;25:72–91. 2012;22:7–24.
Questionnaire: a new measure. Psychophar-
70 Arfken CL, Balon R: Declining 82 National Collaborating Centre for Mental
macol Bull 1993;29:321–326.
participation in research studies. Psychother Health: Schizophrenia: Core Interventions
59 Gold C, Heldal TO, Dahle T, Wigram T: Mu-
Psychosom 2011;80:325–328. in the Treatment and Management of
sic therapy for schizophrenia or schizophre-
71 Tomba E: Nowhere patients. Psychother Psy- Schizo- phrenia in Primary and Secondary
nia-like illnesses. Cochrane Database Syst
chosom 2012;81:69–72. Care (up- date). London, National Institute
Rev 2005:CD004025.
72 Stern D: The issue of vitality. Nord J Music for Health and Clinical Excellence, 2009.
60 Venter A, Maxwell SE, Bolig E: Power in
Ther 2010;19:88–102. http://guida nce.nice.org.uk/CG82, 2009.
ran- domized group comparisons: the value
73 Gold C, Erkkilä J, Wampold B: Designing 83 Hawe P, Shiell A, Riley T: Complex interven-
of adding a single intermediate time point to
prag- matic and meaningful trials of tions: how ‘out of control’ can a randomised
a traditional pretest-posttest design. Psychol
psychosocial in- terventions. Br J Psychiatry trial be? BMJ 2004;328:1561–1563.
Methods 2002;7:194–209.
2011, e-Letter. 84 Geretsegger M, Holck U, Gold C:
61 Ukoumunne OC, Gulliford MC, Chinn S,
74 Dorrepaal E, Thomaes K, Smit JH, van Randomised controlled trial of
Sterne JAC, Burney PGJ: Methods for
Balkorn AJLM, Veltman DJ, Hoogendoorn improvisational music therapy’s
evalu- ating area-wide and organisation-
AW, Draijer N: Stabilizing group treatment effectiveness for children with au- tism
based in- terventions in health and health
for complex posttraumatic stress disorder spectrum disorders (TIME-A): study
care: a sys- tematic review. Health Technol
re- lated to child abuse based on protocol. BMC Pediatr, 2012 (doi 10.1186/
Assess 1999; 3:92.
psychoeduca- tion and cognitive 1471-2431-12-2).
behavioural therapy: a multisite randomized
controlled trial. Psy- chother Psychosom
2012;81:217–225.
McMaster University Downloaded by:
Music Therapy for Clients with Low Psychother Psychosom 2013;82:319–331 331
Motivation DOI: 10.1159/000348452

McMaster University Downloaded by:

You might also like