You are on page 1of 10

cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335

available at www.sciencedirect.com

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Special issue: Research report

Believing in paranormal phenomena: Relations


to asymmetry of body and brain

Günter Schulter* and Ilona Papousek


Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria

article info abstract

Article history: The goal of this study was to investigate the possible relationship between established
Received 29 November 2006 measures of body and brain asymmetries and individual differences in paranormal beliefs.
Reviewed 20 March 2007 In addition to behavioural measures of cerebral laterality, measures of facial features and
Revised 28 August 2007 finger length were taken to calculate body asymmetry scores and indicators of fluctuating
Accepted 29 August 2007 asymmetry (average absolute differences between left and right body features). Both the
Published online 10 July 2008 direction and degree of laterality measures were used. In addition to that, quantitative
measures of inconsistency of cerebral lateralization were obtained. Results indicated
Keywords: that a stronger belief in paranormal phenomena was associated with fluctuating asymme-
Belief in paranormal phenomena try of finger length, and that this aspect of body asymmetry may be related to greater intra-
Hemispheric asymmetry individual variability in the degree of ‘atypical’ functional lateralization. This
Fluctuating asymmetry intraindividual variability index, in turn, significantly predicted strength of belief in the
Developmental instability paranormal. Belief in the paranormal was also higher in women than men and it was nega-
tively correlated with the education level. In sum, these findings suggest that a part of the
variance of strength of belief in paranormal phenomena can be explained by patterns of
functional hemispheric asymmetry that may be related to perturbations during fetal
development.
ª 2008 Elsevier Srl. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction tasks involving probabilistic judgments and statistical


thinking, and higher scores in tests of fantasy proneness
Even though educational curriculum in the West is based on and the propensity to find correspondences in distantly
rationality, superstitious and magical thinking is still wide- related material (see Wiseman and Watt, 2006 for review).
spread (Haraldsson, 1985). It seems likely, therefore, that Most often, belief in the paranormal has been investigated
believers in paranormal phenomena not only share a certain in connection with psychosis-proneness, using scales of
set of beliefs, but possess psychological attributes (i.e., ‘magical ideation’, which is commonly regarded a feature of
a personality or cognitive disposition), which make them schizotypal thinking style and includes belief in paranormal
prone to search for meaningful connections among random phenomena such as telepathy, clairvoyance, and precognition
events (Brugger et al., 1993b; Farias et al., 2005). In fact, (Eckblad and Chapman, 1983). However, superstitious and
believers have been consistently shown to have, for instance, magical thinking is too prevalent among normal adults to
lower syllogistic reasoning ability, lower performances in justify ‘pathologization’ of belief in the paranormal. Some

* Corresponding author. Department of Psychology, University of Graz, A-8010 Graz, Austria.


E-mail address: guenter.schulter@uni-graz.at (G. Schulter).
0010-9452/$ – see front matter ª 2008 Elsevier Srl. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.022
cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335 1327

facets of paranormal belief may even be associated with brain lateralization (see Papousek and Schulter, 2006 for
subjective psychological health and beneficial personality review). All tests were performance measures, in order to
characteristics like extraversion (Thalbourne and Haraldsson, avoid the multiple problems associated with self-report later-
1980; Goulding, 2005) or creativity (Folley and Park, 2005). ality questionnaires (Papousek and Schulter, 1999a). Predomi-
Whether such tendency to believe in the paranormal indi- nantly on the basis of the findings demonstrated by Brugger’s
cates a normal variant or a predisposition to schizophrenia, group we expected that measures indicative of lateralization
it seems obvious to look also for biological bases of the psycho- in the temporal lobes may show significant relationships to
logical traits that may be characteristic of believers. The most belief in paranormal phenomena. However, asymmetry in
systematic research program focused on finding neuropsycho- parietal and frontal (executive) functions may also be involved
logical explanations for paranormal beliefs is the laterality in psychological traits that are related to paranormal belief
model advanced by Brugger and colleagues. Brugger argues (e.g., Gruzelier, 2006; Taylor et al., 2002).
that overactivity of the right hemisphere is the crucial feature, There is some evidence relating paranormal beliefs,
supported by findings of a series of experiments that he and his magical ideation, schizotypy, or psychotic states to inconsis-
coworkers have conducted (e.g., Brugger et al., 1993b; Brugger tency of lateralization. Inconsistencies in lateralization of
and Graves, 1997; see Brugger and Taylor, 2003 for review). different functional systems have been shown to be related
However, to date it is not known which cortical regions are to higher magical ideation scores (Nicholls et al., 2005). In
actually involved in generation and maintenance of para- a study investigating inconsistencies of laterality within one
normal beliefs. The idea that each individual has a universal functional system (e.g., motor functions), the highest magical
‘left hemisphere’ or ‘right hemisphere dominance’ that can be ideation scores were observed in subjects with increased
deduced from a single aspect of hemispheric specialization is inconsistent lateralization (Mohr et al., 2003).
no longer tenable. Instead, the concept of modularity of hemi- Lack of consistency among aspects of functional lateraliza-
spheric functions has increasingly been accepted, that is, the tion, however, may just indicate weak cerebral lateralization
assumption that different processing modules can be lateral- in a global sense (e.g., Porac, 1997). In fact, there is also
ized differently and independently of each other (see Papousek evidence that weak lateralization (irrespective of direction)
and Schulter, 2006 for a summary of the respective evidence). may be related to belief in paranormal phenomena. Barnett
Similarly, attempts to reduce all functional asymmetries to and Corballis (2002) demonstrated a significant correlation
one fundamental dichotomy that globally characterizes the between magical ideation and the absolute score of the later-
competences of the hemispheres (e.g., analytic vs holistic, ality coefficient, indicating an association of increased domi-
rational vs emotional, etc.), that were also in keeping with these nance in either direction (left or right) with lower magical
historical views, had to be clearly abandoned, as well (Papousek ideation scores. However, only one aspect of functional later-
and Schulter, 2006). ality was assessed in this study, and the degree of handedness
Considering the well-known phenomenon that epileptic was assessed by questionnaires, which might not be ideal in
seizures with foci within the temporal lobes can elicit para- obtaining quantitative measures (Yeo et al., 1993).
normal-like experiences, temporal regions may be the most The second aim of the present study, therefore, was to
obvious region to examine. However, contributions of other examine, whether degree of lateralization in various perfor-
cortical regions must also be considered. For instance, regions mance measures of functional lateralization may be related
in the left temporal lobe and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to belief in paranormal phenomena. For this purpose, the
were found to be involved in belief-biased reasoning, whereas absolute values of each laterality measure were related to
relatively greater activation of the right lateral prefrontal cortex the extent of belief in paranormal phenomena. Our hypoth-
was associated with correct logical reasoning (Goel and Dolan, esis was that lower degrees of lateralization might be associ-
2003). In suggestibility, which is closely related to belief in para- ated with stronger beliefs.
normal phenomena (Raz et al., 2008, this issue), disconnection The examination of relationships with absolute laterality
of frontal functions from posterior and subcortical functions measures at its core has to take an alternative theoretical
seems to play a crucial role, leading to less critical evaluation concept of individual variation of lateralized functions into
and reality testing (Gruzelier, 2006). account: the model of developmental instability (DI). This
The first aim of the present study, therefore, was to test model postulates that the individual expression of hand prefer-
whether trait aspects of functional hemispheric asymmetry ence, but likewise of all other lateralized functions, may be seen
may be related to belief in paranormal phenomena, and if as the consequence of developmental instability. DI is
so, which. Using seven behavioural laterality tests we were conceived as the degree to which environmental sources of
able to examine several independent modules of brain lateral- stress (during pre- and perinatal development) result in devia-
ization (Papousek and Schulter, 1999a). Usage of multiple lat- tions from an individual’s ontogenetic program for morpholog-
erality tests allows us to determine which brain region may be ical, physiological and also neurodevelopmental characteristics
related to psychological traits associated with paranormal and design. Random deviations from symmetry of bilateral
belief and which aspect of laterality (hemispheric specializa- features, which are symmetrical at the population level, for
tion, individual lateral preference) may be most important. instance, may be reflecting such developmental influences.
In addition, aspects of laterality were quantitatively assessed Therefore, the impact of stressors and also an individual’s
on continuous scales, thus, measuring both direction and ability to cope with them may be indicated by such deviations
degree of lateralization. Evidence suggests that individual from symmetry which frequently are used as measures of ‘fluc-
differences in the degree of lateralization are stable across tuating asymmetry’ (FA; Van Valen, 1962; Moller and Swaddle,
time and that they provide useful additional information on 1997).
1328 cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335

Based on this theoretical concept, a model of individual mean. Such diversity may best be captured by the intraindi-
lateralization has been proposed postulating that individual vidual variance of the unsigned (absolute) z values of each lat-
variation of handedness, for instance, may be a result of DI: erality measure. Thus, we also examined whether fluctuating
population-typical manifestation of genetically determined asymmetry of the body may be related to intraindividual vari-
handedness may be reflected in moderate right-handedness, ability of functional brain lateralization. Similarly to the anal-
whereas DI produces deviations from the population mean yses of body asymmetries, a measure of intraindividual
of handedness (Yeo and Gangestad, 1993; Yeo et al., 1993). variability of directed asymmetries was analysed in addition
Consequently, both left-handers and extreme right-handers to the score based on absolute deviations. This directed vari-
should show evidence of increased fluctuating anatomic ability measure may tap the concept of inconsistency of later-
asymmetries indicating DI. Additionally, and most impor- alization described above.
tantly for theoretical concepts of brain lateralization, Yeo
et al. (1997) postulated that cerebral asymmetry of every later-
alized brain function may be affected by DI: increased devia- 2. Methods
tions from population-typical patterns of functional
hemispheric asymmetry in either direction may be associated 2.1. Participants
with an elevated incidence of FA. Using tests that typically
show functional asymmetries at the population level (e.g., A total of 136 participants (68 men, 68 women) recruited by
dichotic listening, line bisection, or chimeric faces task), advertisements in local newspapers were paid for participa-
they were able to demonstrate that measures of FA were unre- tion in the study. The age range was 19–51 years (M ¼ 32.7,
lated to directional asymmetries, but significantly associated SD ¼ 8.7). Left-handers (Ss with negative laterality quotients
with ‘atypical’ lateralization, i.e., individuals with higher FA in a handedness test; see next paragraph) and subjects with
scores showed greater deviations from the sample mean in left-handed parents or siblings were excluded from the study.
either direction. Beyond functional asymmetries, Thoma There is evidence that – with respect to brain lateralization –
et al. (2002) used imaging techniques (MRI) to determine left-handedness is not simply the mirror image of right-
several anatomical brain features (e.g., left and right hemi- handedness, i.e., there may not be a single linear continuum
sphere volumes, areas of left and right planum temporale, from strong left- to strong right-handedness, and that lateral-
size of corpus callosum) and demonstrated that ‘atypical’ ization of functions may be heterogenous within left-handers
brain asymmetry was significantly related to a composite FA (see Papousek and Schulter, 1999a, 2006). It seems useful,
measure relying on the relative sizes of several body features therefore, to investigate cortical lateralization of functions in
(e.g., ears, feet, etc.). right-handers and left-handers separately. No participant
The third research question of the present study, therefore, was using drugs or medication. 9 participants (4 women)
concerns the possible impact of developmental instability on had obligatory school education only (i.e., 9 years of educa-
belief in paranormal phenomena. Body FA was assessed by tion), 51 (25) obligatory school education plus professional
obtaining the following measures: (1) asymmetry of finger training, 60 (32) higher school education (12–13 years), and
length of both hands, and (2) two different aspects of facial 16 (7) a university degree (16 or more years of education). For
asymmetry (see Section 2). In addition to classical measures some analyses participants had to be dropped because of
of fluctuating asymmetry (i.e., average absolute differences missing data: one participant failed to complete the Belief in
between left and right body features), directed measures of Paranormal Phenomena Questionnaire, a total of 26 partici-
body asymmetry were also calculated (i.e., signed differences pants failed to complete at least one of the behavioural later-
between left and right body features). The direction of certain ality tests, and digit length data were not available from three
body asymmetries may also indicate prenatal aspects of brain participants.
development and functional asymmetry (e.g., Means and
Walters, 1982; Kimura, 1994). In contrast to the above delin- 2.2. Behavioural laterality tests
eated expectation of negative correlations between absolute
measures of functional laterality and paranormal belief, 2.2.1. Handedness
within the framework of the theory of developmental insta- Relative hand skill was assessed by a standardized paper and
bility, positive correlations between body FA, absolute pencil test (HDT; Steingrüber and Lienert, 1971; Papousek and
measures of functional laterality, and belief in paranormal Schulter, 1999a, 1999b) consisting of three different tasks
phenomena were predicted. which had to be performed with the left and the right hand
However, within this theoretical context, and bearing the as quickly as possible within a given time limit; i.e., to draw
modular approach in mind, it may also be expected that inde- a line within small boundaries, to mark a series of squares
pendently lateralized functional systems may be affected in and a series of circles with one dot each. As only right-handers
different ways and to different degrees, as environmental were included in the study, the laterality coefficient [(R  L)/
stressors are presumed to cause random errors in brain devel- (R þ L)  100] indicates the participants’ degree of right-
opment. Therefore, deviations from population-typical handedness (HAND).
patterns of functional hemispheric asymmetry as predicted
by the DI model may show up in some functional systems, 2.2.2. Line bisection
but not in others. Consequently, DI may rather affect the In a computerized version of the classical line bisection task
intraindividual variability of functional hemisphere asymme- 10 lines were presented one after another on a computer
tries concerning the degree of deviation from the population screen. Participants were instructed to move the cursor to
cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335 1329

the center of the line and to transect it by clicking on the were those in which the composite with the smile in the view-
mouse button (see Papousek and Schulter, 1999a for details er’s right (left) hemispace was judged happier. The partici-
of this test). The distances of the transection marks from the pants’ bimanual answers were registered by the computer
true center of the lines were registered by the computer program which also calculated the laterality coefficient by
program and averaged across trials (LINE). Positive values subtracting the number of left-biased responses from the
indicate that subjects on average bisected lines deviating to number of right-biased responses, divided by the total
the right end of the line, while negative values reflect transec- number of observations (CF). Positive values indicate predom-
tions to the left of the midpoint. inance of right-hemispace biases in the perception of
emotional facial expressions, negative values indicate
2.2.3. Ear dominance predominance of left-hemispace biases.
Ear dominance was measured following a method initially
described by Efron and Yund (1974). In this test pairs of ‘dich- 2.2.6. Conjugate lateral eye movements
otic chords’ were presented which were composed of two pure To assess the preferred direction of conjugate lateral eye
tones of different frequencies (1650 and 1850 Hz). The two movements, 21 synonym questions and 21 orienting questions
tones of a dichotic chord were presented simultaneously, were presented (see Papousek and Schulter, 1999a, for details
one to each ear. With such a small frequency difference and rational of this test). The first eye movement between
between the dichotic tones, subjects hear a single, fused question and response was recorded by the experimenter
sound image containing both frequencies, the perceived who faced the subject, using the numerals on a clock face to
mixture being dominated by the frequency of the tone pre- represent direction of eye movement, a method proposed by
sented to the preferred ear. The ear-frequency relationship Ehrlichman et al. (1974). Also following Ehrlichman et al.
was always reversed for the second chord of a pair. For each (1974), a quantitative measure of the preferred direction of
trial, participants were required to judge whether the overall lateral eye movements was calculated (CLEM), which corre-
pitch of the first chord was higher or lower than that of the sponds to the mean x (i.e., horizontal) coordinate of each clock
second (see Papousek and Schulter, 1999a, 2002 for further position weighted by the number of eye movements occurring
details of this test). The number of right minus left ear deci- at that position. Positive values indicate dominance of right-
sions divided by the total was calculated as an index of ear ward eye movements (‘rightmovers’), ‘leftmovers’ show nega-
dominance (EAR). Dominance of the right ear is indicated by tive values.
positive values, participants with dominance of the left ear
show negative values. 2.2.7. Dichotic listening
For the dichotic listening test material and procedures were
2.2.4. Ocular dominance used that were in accordance with the guidelines of Hugdahl
Ocular dominance was determined by an apparatus devel- (1995). The dichotic stimuli consisted of the CV syllables ba,
oped on the basis of a classical ‘alignment test’ (sighting domi- da, ga, pa, ta, and ka. Both the consonant- and the vowel-
nance; Ruggieri et al., 1980). This test is based on the segments of the syllables were temporally aligned between
phenomenon that it is impossible to foveate a target with channels. The participants marked their answers continu-
both eyes, predominantly the input from the foveating eye is ously on a scoring form. Data were scored for each participant
used, while the input from the other eye seems to be tempo- as the number of correctly reported syllables for the right and
rarily suppressed to avoid diplopia. The participants were left ear input, respectively. The laterality coefficient was
instructed to bring two luminous rods into line. The task calculated by subtracting the number of left ear syllables
was performed with both eyes first, then with only the left from the number of right ear syllables divided by the total of
eye (balanced right), with both eyes again, and finally with correctly reported syllables (DL).
the right (left) eye. The basic principle of the score of this Of all laterality measures absolute scores were also calcu-
test is that the deviation of the two rods produced by the lated. These scores indicate degree of lateralization irrespec-
dominant eye is about the same as when performing the tive of its direction (absHand,1 absLINE, absEAR, absEYE,
task with both eyes, whereas the performance of the non- absCF, absCLEM, absDL).
dominant eye differs from this score to a considerable extent
(see Papousek and Schulter, 1999a, for details of the apparatus 2.3. Finger length asymmetry
and scoring procedure). Positive values indicate dominance of
the right eye, whereas left-eyed subjects show negative values Participant’s hands were photocopied (Konica Minolta 7030).
(EYE). Second through fifth digit lengths were measured from these
photocopies using a vernier caliper measuring to the nearest
2.2.5. Chimeric faces test .05 mm. Digit lengths were measured from the basal crease
An adapted version of the chimeric faces test described by of the digit proximal to the palm to the tip of the digit. Asym-
Levy et al. (1983) was presented on a computer screen. In metry of each digit was calculated as the difference between
each of a total of 24 trials a pair of chimeric faces was dis- the left and right measurements divided by their sum
played, with half the face showing a smiling and half a neutral [(L  R)/(L þ R)]. Finally, the composite score ‘directed asymme-
expression. The two halves were photos of the same person, try’ of finger length (fDA) was calculated as the mean of the
and the two faces of a trial were exact mirror images. Partici-
pants were instructed to judge which face of the pair (top or 1
As only right-handers were included in the present study,
bottom) looked happier. Right-biased (left-biased) responses absHAND is identical to HAND.
1330 cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335

asymmetry scores of all four digits. The absolute value of this 1. Some people have the ability to predict the future.
score was used to indicate the ‘fluctuating asymmetry’ of finger 2. Some people are capable of transferring thoughts to
length (fFA). All digit lengths were measured independently by another person (telepathy).
two experimenters. The intraclass correlation between the 3. There are good spirits and also evil spirits that can influ-
composite scores of the two experimenters was ICC ¼ .79 ence our lives.
( p < .001). Scores of the two experimenters were averaged to 4. The positions of the planets at the time of birth (the horo-
obtain the final finger length asymmetry measures. scope) determine a person’s life.
5. There is life after death.
2.4. Facial asymmetry
Each item had to be rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
The procedure of measuring facial fluctuating asymmetry was from ‘I don’t believe that at all’ (0) to ‘I am firmly convinced of
adapted from the work of Shackelford and Larsen (1997). At that’ (4). The internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s
first, some measures were made for calibration of the computer alpha) of the scale in the present sample was .81. Means and
program, i.e., the distance between the pupils measured by standard deviations of the five items were: M ¼ 1.6, SD ¼ 1.2
a pupillometer and the size of the ears measured by a vernier (Item 1); M ¼ 2.3, SD ¼ 1.3 (Item 2); M ¼ 1.3, SD ¼ 1.3 (Item 3);
caliper. Two digital photos were taken of each participant, M ¼ 1.4, SD ¼ 1.2 (Item 4); M ¼ 2.3, SD ¼ 1.4 (Item 5). The total
one close-up and one showing the exact position of the ears score of the scale showed an approximately normal distribu-
with the help of mirrors positioned at the sides of the head. tion with a range of 0–20. Hence, the scale proved to differen-
The photos then were analysed using a computer program in tiate very well within the normal, nonclinical range.
which several points were marked manually for the following Correlations with the subscales of the Eysenck Personality
features: pupil center of left and right eyes, outer eye (outer Questionnaire (EPQ; Ruch, 1999) were r ¼ .02 (ns.), r ¼ .19
closure of each eye), inner eye (inner closure of each eye), outer ( p < .05), r ¼ .06 (ns.) for neuroticism, extraversion, and psy-
point of each nostril, cheekbone (most prominent outer point of choticism, respectively.
each cheekbone), left and right corners of the mouth, jaw (most
prominent outer point of left and right jaws), lowermost point 2.6. Procedure
of the chin, upper and lower ends of left and right ears. The
vertical axis was defined as the line perpendicular to and inter- Participants were tested on three separate occasions with two
secting at the calculated midpoint of the line anchored by the to four days between sessions. In the first session, after
left and right pupil centers. The horizontal axis was defined informed consent was obtained, the handedness test and
as tangent to the point of the chin. For each of the bilateral char- the questionnaires were applied (BPP, EPQ), hands were pho-
acteristics the distances from the vertical axis to the feature tocopied for the calculation of finger length asymmetry, and
were determined and averaged across all features on the left pupil distance and size of ears were measured and photos
(avL) and across all features on the right side (avR). The taken for calculation of facial asymmetry. In the second and
composite score ‘horizontal directed asymmetry’ (hDA) is defined in the third session the behavioural laterality tests were
as the difference between the mean distances on the left and applied twice (with the exception of the handedness test).2
the right side [hDA ¼ avL  avR]. In addition to that, the
composite score ‘horizontal fluctuating asymmetry’ was calcu-
lated as the absolute difference between the mean distances
3. Results
on the left and the right side [hFA ¼ abs(avL  avR)]. Analogous
to the horizontal asymmetry scores, two composite vertical
Test–retest correlations of the behavioural laterality scores in
asymmetry scores were calculated using the horizontal axis
the present sample were LINE: r ¼ .72 ( p < .001), EAR: r ¼ .80
as the reference axis (vDA and vFA, respectively). All reference
( p < .001), EYE: r ¼ .83 ( p < .001), CF: r ¼ .84 ( p < .001), CLEM:
points were independently marked by two experimenters.
r ¼ .60 ( p < .001), and DL: r ¼ .37 ( p < .001). Therefore, with
Scores based on the work of the two experimenters were highly
the exception of the dichotic listening score, retest correla-
correlated (intraclass correlations ICC ¼ .88, p < .001 and
tions of all behavioural laterality scores are of sufficient
ICC ¼ .90, p < .001 for the horizontal and the vertical score,
magnitude to proceed on the assumption that temporally
respectively). For all subsequent analyses scores of the two
stable individual differences substantially contribute to the
experimenters were averaged.
observed asymmetry scores. Due to its low temporal stability,
DL was excluded from all subsequent analyses. Of all other
2.5. Belief in paranormal phenomena questionnaire
laterality measures, scores of the two sessions were averaged.
(BPP)
With one exception all intercorrelations among the different
behavioural laterality scores were not significant (r ¼ .01 to
Items were in part compiled from other scales (Brugger, 1991;
r ¼ .17, ns.). The correlation between EAR and CF reached
Musch and Ehrenberg, 2002) and in part especially developed
significance (r ¼ .23, p < .05), the low size of the correlation
for the present study, aiming at a medium item difficulty in
argues against a substantial relationship between these two
a normal sample and restricting the item content to belief in
measures, however.
paranormal phenomena, i.e., excluding the ‘experience facet’
to obtain a homogeneous scale differentiating well among 2
Only data directly relevant to this paper are reported here.
individuals of a rather heterogeneous sample. The BPP scale With regard to other research questions, additional variables
comprised the following items: were obtained. These data will be reported elsewhere.
cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335 1331

In order to evaluate relationships between functional hemi-


Table 2 – Standard multiple regression analysis: BPP
sphere asymmetries and belief in paranormal phenomena score on absolute measures of functional hemisphere
a standard multiple regression analysis was performed, using asymmetry
HAND, LINE, EAR, EYE, CF, CLEM, and sex of subjects as the
r b sr p
predictor variables and the BPP score as the dependent variable.
The full model was significant, F(7,101) ¼ 2.67, p < .05, R2 ¼ .16. absHAND .10 .04 .04 .656
absLINE .11 .08 .07 .425
However, only sex contributed significantly to the variance of
absEAR .19 .17 .16 .082
belief in paranormal phenomena, women scoring higher on
absEYE .01 .01 .01 .896
the BPP questionnaire then men (see Table 1). An equivalent absCF .07 .02 .02 .846
analysis was performed with the absolute laterality scores abs- absCLEM .04 .07 .07 .436
HAND,1 absLINE, absEAR, absEYE, absCF, absCLEM, indicating Sex .28 .27 .26 .005
degree of lateralization irrespective of direction, and sex as
Note: r, Pearson correlation; b, standardized regression coefficient;
independent variables. Besides the significant sex effect, this sr, semipartial correlation.
analysis yielded no significant effect, F(7,101) ¼ 2.12, p < .05,
R2 ¼ .13 (see Table 2). It can also be concluded from the statis-
tical results that neither the signed nor the absolute laterality First, a 2  2 multivariate analysis of variance was run, with
measures share some common variance that might explain fFA (median split) and sex as independent, and intraindividual
some variance of BPP. variance of directed (var-dLAT) and absolute behavioural lat-
Standard multiple regression was also utilized to assess erality scores (var-aLAT) as the dependent variables. For
the relationship between body asymmetry and belief in para- each participant var-dLAT was calculated as the standard
normal phenomena. In this analysis, fDA, fFA, hDA, hFA, vDA, deviation of his or her (z-transformed) scores in the tests
vFA, and sex were entered as the independent variables. HAND, LINE, EAR, EYE, CF, and CLEM, reflecting intraindivid-
Results indicated that these variables explain a significant ual variability in direction and degree of behavioural laterality,
amount of variance of BPP, F(7,125) ¼ 4.39, p < .001, R2 ¼ .20. relative to the sample mean. Var-aLAT indicates intraindivid-
However, in addition to sex, only fFA contributed significantly ual variability in the degree of deviation from the sample
to the variance of BPP (see Table 3). A greater degree of fluctu- mean only, and was calculated as the standard deviation of
ating asymmetry of finger length was associated with a higher absHAND, absLINE, absEAR, absEYE, absCF, and absCLEM.
BPP score. Intercorrelations between body asymmetry The multivariate test of the analysis of variance revealed
measures were significant for horizontal and vertical DA a significant main effect of fFA, F(2,128) ¼ 3.28, p < .05. The
(r ¼ .26, p < .01) and for horizontal and vertical FA (r ¼ .45, other effects were not significant (main effect sex:
p < .001). Correlations between facial and finger FA were not F(2,128) ¼ .32, ns.; interaction: F(2,128) ¼ .45, ns.). Subse-
significant (r ¼ .01 and r ¼ .07 for horizontal and vertical quently performed separate univariate F tests showed that
facial FA, respectively). only var-aLAT contributed to the significant main effect of
The hypothesis that developmental instability (indicated fFA, F(1,129)¼6.27, p < .05, but not var-dLAT (F(1,129)¼1.35,
by fFA) may determine one or several indicators of functional ns.). Individuals with a higher index of developmental insta-
hemisphere asymmetry and, therefore, may influence the bility (fFA) showed more variability in the degree of functional
tendency to believe in paranormal phenomena, can be lateralization (M ¼ .58) than individuals with lower fFA
excluded by the non-significant results of the first analyses (M ¼ .50).
regressing BPP on the signed and absolute measures of func- A second multiple regression analysis was performed,
tional hemispheric asymmetry. In order to examine the alter- using var-aLAT and sex as predictors and the BPP score as
native hypothesis that developmental instability may be the dependent variable. Sex and var-aLAT together explained
associated with greater intraindividual variability of func- about 11 percent of the variance of BPP, F(2,132) ¼ 8.51,
tional hemisphere asymmetries which, in turn, may be p < .001, R2 ¼ .11. Both var-aLAT and sex showed unique
related to BPP, two further analyses were performed. contributions to the prediction of BPP (see Table 4). The results

Table 1 – Standard multiple regression analysis: BPP Table 3 – Standard multiple regression analysis: BPP
score on functional hemisphere asymmetries score on asymmetries of the body
r b sr p r b sr p

HAND .10 .01 .01 .894 fDA .12 .03 .03 .742
LINE .11 .11 .11 .246 fFA .36 .33 .32 .000
EAR .09 .16 .16 .090 hDA .00 .08 .07 .429
EYE .08 .15 .14 .131 hFA .02 .08 .06 .375
CF .08 .08 .07 .443 vDA .07 .06 .05 .502
CLEM .16 .13 .13 .173 vFA .16 .17 .14 .091
Sex .28 .32 .29 .002 Sex .24 .18 .17 .037

Note: r, Pearson correlation; b, standardized regression coefficient; Note: r, Pearson correlation; b, standardized regression coefficient;
sr, semipartial correlation. sr, semipartial correlation.
1332 cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335

magical thinking frequently seen in the normal population.


Table 4 – Standard multiple regression analysis: BPP
score on variability in degree of lateralization and sex ‘Difficult’ items (i.e., items in which affirmative answers reflect
very high expressions of the trait) may predominantly differen-
r b sr p
tiate between schizotypal and psychologically healthy individ-
var_aLAT .25 .24 .24 .005 uals. In opposition to that, predominantly the non-pathological
Sex .24 .23 .23 .005 facet of paranormal belief, that may be related to personality or
Note: r, Pearson correlation; b, standardized regression coefficient; cognitive dispositions also seen in normal healthy individuals,
sr, semipartial correlation. may be measured with instruments with relatively lower item
difficulty (i.e., differentiating within the normal range). In the
questionnaire used in the present study the weaker, non-
indicate that more intraindividual variability in the degree of pathological aspect clearly predominates.
lateralization of different functions is associated with a higher Most often, belief in the paranormal has been investigated
tendency to believe in paranormal phenomena. This effect in connection with schizophrenia or psychosis-proneness. In
proved to be independent from the portion of variance that fact, as hallucinations and delusions can be seen as the very
is explained by the sex of subjects. hallmarks of psychosis, paranormal experiences may be
To investigate whether BPP may also be influenced by the more typical of psychosis-proneness than paranormal belief.
educational level of the participants and, hence by social There is other evidence, too, that it may be sensible to differen-
factors a one-way analysis of variance was run with educa- tiate between paranormal belief and paranormal experiences.
tional level (obligatory school education/higher school educa- Interestingly, whereas paranormal belief seems to be associ-
tion/university degree) as independent and BPP score as the ated with lower reasoning performance, this obviously does
dependent variable. The analysis yielded a significant main not hold for frequency of paranormal experiences (Hergovich
effect of education, F(2,132) ¼ 4.68, p < .05. The mean BPP and Arendasy, 2005; Musch and Ehrenberg, 2002; Wierzbicki,
scores of the three educational groups were M ¼ 2.02, 1985). This may suggest that individuals with better reasoning
M ¼ 1.74, M ¼ 1.25, respectively, indicating decreasing belief ability may be better able to verify whether their experiences
in paranormal phenomena with higher educational levels. are sufficient justification for belief in paranormal phenomena
(Hergovich and Arendasy, 2005). In addition, paranormal belief
is highly correlated with suggestibility, whereas the frequency
4. Discussion of paranormal experiences is not (Atkinson, 1994; Wiseman
et al., 2003). And in contrast to paranormal experiences that
Contrary to expectations, the results of the present study are associated with epileptic seizures or stimulation of the
showed no direct relationships of measures of functional hemi- (right) temporal lobe, other functions that are related to belief
spheric asymmetry to belief in paranormal phenomena. On the in paranormal phenomena, for instance, belief-biased
basis of the studies of Brugger’s group (e.g., Brugger and Taylor, reasoning or suggestibility have been demonstrated to be asso-
2003), believers were expected to be more right-lateralized, ciated with other cortical activation patterns (Goel and Dolan,
especially in functions ascribed to temporal regions. This null 2003; Gruzelier, 2006). Certainly, the failure to replicate the
finding cannot be attributed to unspecificity or unreliability of findings of Brugger’s group could also be due to a statistical
the laterality measures: in accordance with the concept of type 2 error, especially because only small effect sizes have
modularity of hemisphere functions (Papousek and Schulter, to be expected when investigating correlations between func-
2006), intercorrelations among behavioural laterality scores tional brain lateralization and a complex trait-like feature like
once again confirm independence of different quantitative belief in the paranormal and, therefore, the relatively large
aspects of behavioural laterality which can be measured with sample (n ¼ 136) still might have been too small to detect
sufficient reliability. One explanation for the fact that previous such slight effects. The best way to evaluate this possibility
findings could not be replicated may be that, to date, studies would be replication of the present findings (Carver, 1993;
have only partly corresponded in the definition of the construct Greenwald et al., 1996; Nickerson, 2000).
under investigation. The most frequently used questionnaires The results of the present study do not support the propo-
contain both paranormal belief and frequency of paranormal sition that belief in paranormal phenomena may be associ-
experiences. It has not been clarified yet, whether one or the ated with weak functional lateralization. In none of the
other or both facets may be related to the right hemisphere. laterality measures did the degree of lateralization show
However, whereas the magical ideation scale (containing both a correlation to paranormal belief. In previous studies it was
‘belief’ and ‘experience’ items, and assessing also other schizo- suggested that a weak preference for either hand may be asso-
phrenia-related symptoms) were used in recent studies of ciated with magical ideation or schizotypy (Barnett and
Brugger’s group (e.g., Taylor et al., 2002; Mohr et al., 2003; Corballis, 2002; Chapman and Chapman, 1987; Claridge
Mohr et al., 2006), in earlier studies belief in paranormal et al., 1998). However, in contrast to previous studies, in the
phenomena was assessed by a rating scale on which partici- present study a proved performance measure of handedness
pants rated how much they believed in extrasensory percep- was used. Although magical ideation has been shown to be
tion (telepathy, clairvoyance; Brugger et al., 1993a, 1993b). higher in individuals with weak hand preference assessed
Another difference may concern the item difficulty of the by questionnaires, no relationship could be found with perfor-
used instruments. This is related to the question whether find- mance measures of handedness (Nicholls et al., 2005). There
ings hold for schizotypal (i.e., schizophrenia-prone) character- are a series of methodological problems with handedness
istics in a narrower sense only or also for superstitious and (and eyedness etc.) questionnaires, and the correlation
cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335 1333

between preference and performance measures is often weak Specifically, which brain features and processes may affect
(Papousek and Schulter, 1999a). cognition in general and belief in paranormal phenomena in
Our quantitative measure of inconsistency of functional particular during fetal growth? Thoma et al. (2005), studying
lateralization (intraindividual variance of directed laterality possible mechanisms underlying the negative association
scores) did not show correlations to belief in paranormal between developmental instability and intellectual abilities,
phenomena, either. Magical ideation had been demonstrated recently concluded that DI may compromise aspects of brain
to be related to inconsistent lateralization assessed by behav- organization resulting, for instance, in a less efficient design,
ioural measures in a previous study (Mohr et al., 2003). entailing less effective and slower information processing.
However, in the study of Mohr et al. (2003) inconsistency of Results of the present study suggest that possible mediating
lateralization was observed within one functional system mechanisms may be related to the lateralization of functions.
(whereas inconsistency among several functional systems In their model on determinants of individual variation in cere-
was assessed in the present study). Inconsistency of laterali- bral lateralization, Yeo and colleagues (Yeo and Gangestad,
zation within one functional system, in turn, may rather be 1993; Yeo et al., 1993, 1997) postulated a relationship of
related to weaker lateralization of this system than to increased deviations from population-typical patterns of
different lateralization of independent functional modules. functional hemispheric asymmetry in either direction with
Although on the basis of the existing evidence one could an elevated incidence of body FA. Moreover, increased body
expect belief in the paranormal to be related to degree of FA was found to be associated with greater deviations from
handedness or inconsistency of (motor) lateralization, typical anatomical asymmetries of brain structures (Thoma
a straightforward neuropsychological interpretation of the et al., 2002). In the present study, however, atypical lateraliza-
motor lateralization findings seems difficult insofar as there tion, indicated by the absolute z values of several laterality
is no evidence that motor lateralization or handedness may measures, did not predict degree of belief in paranormal
also be an indicator of lateralization of functions that may phenomena.
be relevant to belief in paranormal phenomena. However, Based on the assumption of independence of lateralization
both degree of handedness and belief in paranormal of different functions, and also on the assumption of random
phenomena might be manifestations of certain processes effects of environmental stressors on direction and extent of
occurring during brain development. Fluctuating asymmetry functional asymmetries, we assumed that DI may affect the
may be a more direct indicator of such processes. intraindividual variability of functional hemispheric asymme-
In fact, the main results of the present study relate to fluctu- tries. With the intraindividual variance of both the directed
ating asymmetry. Whereas no associations of directed body and absolute behavioural laterality scores as the dependent
asymmetry scores to BPP were observed, a significant relation- measures in a multivariate analysis of variance, we first were
ship between BPP and an index of fluctuating asymmetry was able to demonstrate a relationship of FA to the variance of the
demonstrated: a greater degree of FA of finger length was asso- absolute scores, but not to variability of directed asymmetry
ciated with a stronger belief in paranormal phenomena. Both measures. This result is only partially in line with the model
scores of facial FA, however, were not related to BPP. Although proposed by Yeo and colleagues (Yeo et al., 1997), which predicts
in several studies on fluctuating asymmetry the use of higher FA scores to be associated with higher absolute scores in
a composite measure of FA is recommended as a more valid all laterality measures, but not with greater intraindividual vari-
index of DI (Yeo et al., 1997; Gangestad and Thornhill, 1999), ability of laterality measures. However, taking into account
composite indices may sometimes disguise differential both the assumption that different hemispheric functions are
patterns of relationships of different aspects of body FA. independently lateralized (modularity approach of hemispheric
Although it was shown that fluctuating asymmetries of functions; Papousek and Schulter, 2006), and the assumption
different individual traits are correlated, such correlations that not all lateralized functions may be equally affected by
were usually very small (r ¼ .01 and r ¼ .07 in the present the random effects of environmental stress, increased intrain-
study; estimated mean values are about r ¼ .03; Gangestad dividual variability of (z-standardized) laterality measures
and Thornhill, 1999). Body asymmetries are regarded as random may even be a more plausible result: if some functions are
fluctuations, i.e., the underlying processes are believed to affected showing high scores (i.e., atypical lateralization) and
randomly affect different features of the body. However, as others are not affected, with near-zero values indicating near-
body and brain development proceed according to a certain average, i.e., typical lateralization, an increase in the overall
time schedule, at certain points in fetal growth stress may result intraindividual variance of functional hemispheric asymme-
in deviations of certain features of body and brain that covary to tries has to be expected.
some extent. Such covariations may possibly underlie the spec- More central to the aim of the present study, we were also
ificity of finger FA with respect to its relation to BPP. That able to show that high variability of absolute laterality
different components of DI may be specifically related to partic- measures predicted stronger belief in paranormal phenomena
ular aspects of cerebral lateralization was already proposed by in a multiple regression analysis. Therefore, higher inconsis-
Yeo et al. (1997). However, confirmation from DI studies that tency in degree of lateralization of different functions may
were designed for that particular purpose would be needed for be caused by one brain mechanism mediating the relationship
such an interpretation of the present findings. between developmental instability and belief in paranormal
As was stressed in a recent review on FA and DI (Dongen, phenomena. Nevertheless, the important question why
2006), a question of even greater significance concerns the inconsistency of atypical lateralization may be related to
mechanisms underlying DI, and how these respective mecha- stronger belief in paranormal phenomena is left unanswered
nisms and processes may map to the observable phenotype. and must remain the subject of further research.
1334 cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335

A similar amount of variance of BPP as was explained by Eckblad M and Chapman LJ. Magical ideation as an indicator of
intraindividual variability in atypical lateralization was schizotypy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51:
explained by sex. Independently of each other, each of these 215–255, 1983.
Efron R and Yund EW. Dichotic competition of simultaneous tone
two variables predicted about 5 percent of the variance in
bursts of different frequency – I. Dissociation of pitch from
BPP. Higher scores of magical ideation or related constructs lateralization and loudness. Neuropsychologia, 12: 249–256,
like self-transcendence, esoteric beliefs, and religiosity in 1974.
women than men were observed in several other studies, too Ehrlichman H, Weiner SL, and Baker AH. Effects of verbal and
(Farias et al., 2005; Goulding, 2004; Hood et al., 1996; Kaasinen spatial questions on initial gaze shifts. Neuropsychologia, 12:
et al., 2005; Kirk et al., 1999; Nicholls et al., 2005), although not 265–277, 1974.
Farias M, Claridge G, and Lalljee M. Personality and cognitive
all investigators found a sex difference (e.g., Goulding, 2005;
predictors of new age practices and beliefs. Personality and
Mohr et al., 2001; Mohr et al., 2003). As there also seem to be
Individual Differences, 39: 979–989, 2005.
quite strong cultural influences in belief in the paranormal Farmer A, Mahmood A, Redman K, Harris T, Sadler S, and
and related constructs (Farmer et al., 2003), it seems likely McGuffin P. A sib-pair study of the temperament and
that this finding may reflect a non-biological factor that, like character inventory scales in major depression. Archives of
education, influences belief in paranormal phenomena beyond General Psychiatry, 60: 490–496, 2003.
the effects of biologically substantiated psychological traits. Folley BS and Park S. Verbal creativity and schizotypal personality
in relation to prefrontal hemispheric laterality: a behavioral
and near-infrared optical imaging study. Schizophrenia
Research, 80: 271–282, 2005.
Gangestad SW and Thornhill R. Individual differences in
Acknowledgements
developmental precision and fluctuating asymmetry: a model
and its implications. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 12: 402–416,
This research was supported by the Austrian Science Fund 1999.
(FWF Project Nr. 13619). We thank Dr. Sohee Park, Vanderbilt Goel V and Dolan RJ. Explaining modulation of reasoning by
University, Nashville, TN, for helpful discussions and editorial belief. Cognition, 87: B11–B22, 2003.
assistance. Goulding A. Schizotypy models in relation to subjective health
and paranormal beliefs and experiences. Personality and
Individual Differences, 37: 157–167, 2004.
Goulding A. Healthy schizotypy in a population of paranormal
references believers and experients. Personality and Individual Differences,
38: 1069–1083, 2005.
Greenwald AG, Gonzales R, Harris RJ, and Guthrie D. Effect sizes
Atkinson RP. Relationships of hypnotic susceptibility to and p values: what should be reported and what should be
paranormal beliefs and claimed experiences: implications for replicated. Psychophysiology, 33: 175–183, 1996.
hypnotic absorption. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 37: Gruzelier JH. Frontal functions, connectivity and neural efficiency
34–40, 1994. underpinning hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility.
Barnett KJ and Corballis MC. Ambidexterity and magical ideation. Contemporary Hypnosis, 23: 15–32, 2006.
Laterality, 7: 75–84, 2002. Haraldsson E. Representative national surveys of psychic
Brugger P. ASW: Außersinnliche Wahrnehmung oder Ausdruck phenomena: Iceland, Great Britain, Sweden, USA and Gallup’s
subjektiver Wahrscheinlichkeit?. [Extra-sensory perception or multinational survey. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research,
effect of subjective probability?] Zeitschrift für Parapsychologie 53: 145–158, 1985.
und Grenzgebiete der Psychologie, 33: 76–102, 1991. Hergovich A and Arendasy M. Critical thinking ability and belief
Brugger P, Gamma A, Muri R, Schäfer M, and Taylor KI. Functional in the paranormal. Personality and Individual Differences, 38:
hemispheric asymmetry and belief in ESP: towards 1805–1812, 2005.
a ‘‘neuropsychology of belief’’. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77: Hood R, Spilka B, Hunsberger B, and Gorsuch R. The Psychology of
1299–1308, 1993a. Religion. An Empirical Approach. New York, London: The
Brugger P and Graves RE. Right hemispatial inattention and Guilford Press, 1996.
magical ideation. European Archive of Psychiatry and Clinical Hugdahl K. Dichotic listening: Probing temporal lobe functional
Neuroscience, 247: 55–57, 1997. integrity. In Davidson RJ, and Hugdahl K (Eds), Brain
Brugger P, Regard M, Landis T, Cook N, Krebs D, and Asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995: 123–156.
Niederberger J. Meaningful patterns in visual noise: effects of Kaasinen V, Maguire RP, Kurki T, Brock A, and Rinne JO. Mapping
lateral stimulation and the observer’s belief in ESP. brain structure and personality in late adulthood. NeuroImage,
Psychopathology, 26: 261–265, 1993b. 24: 315–322, 2005.
Brugger P and Taylor KI. Extrasensory perception or effect of Kimura D. Body asymmetry and intellectual pattern. Personality
subjective probability. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10: and Individual Differences, 17: 53–60, 1994.
221–246, 2003. Kirk KM, Eaves LJ, and Martin NG. Self-transcendence as
Carver RP. The case against statistical significance testing, a measure of spirituality in a sample of older Australian twins.
revisited. Journal of Experimental Education, 61: 287–292, 1993. Twin Research, 2: 81–87, 1999.
Chapman JP and Chapman LJ. Handedness of hypothetically Levy J, Heller W, Banich MT, and Burton LA. Asymmetry of
psychosis-prone subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96: perception in free viewing of chimeric faces. Brain and
89–93, 1987. Cognition, 2: 404–419, 1983.
Claridge G, Clark K, Davis C, and Mason O. Schizophrenia risk and Means LW and Walters RE. Sex, handedness and asymmetry of
handedness: a mixed picture. Laterality, 3: 209–220, 1998. hand and foot length. Neuropsychologia, 20: 715–719, 1982.
Dongen SV. Fluctuating asymmetry and developmental Mohr C, Graves RE, Gianotti LRR, Pizzagalli D, and Brugger P.
instability in evolutionary biology: past, present and future. Loose but normal: a semantic association study. Journal of
Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 19: 1727–1743, 2006. Psycholinguistic Research, 30: 475–483, 2001.
cortex 44 (2008) 1326–1335 1335

Mohr C, Landis T, and Brugger P. Lateralized semantic priming: bzw. EPQ-RK. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische
modulation by levodopa, semantic distance, and participants’ Psychologie, 20: 1–24, 1999.
magical beliefs. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2: 71–84, Ruggieri V, Cei A, Ceridono D, and Bergerone C. Dimensional
2006. approach to the study of sighting dominance. Perceptual and
Mohr C, Thut G, Landis T, and Brugger P. Hands, arms, and minds: Motor Skills, 51: 247–251, 1980.
interactions between posture and thought. Journal of Clinical Shackelford TK and Larsen RJ. Facial asymmetry as an indicator
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25: 1000–1010, 2003. of psychological, emotional, and physiological distress. Journal
Moller AP and Swaddle JP. Asymmetry, Developmental Stability and of Personality and Social Psychology, 72: 456–466, 1997.
Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1997. Steingrüber H and Lienert G. Hand-Dominanz-Test. Göttingen:
Musch J and Ehrenberg K. Probability misjudgment, cognitive Hogrefe, 1971.
ability, and belief in the paranormal. British Journal of Taylor KI, Zach P, and Brugger P. Why is magical ideation related
Psychology, 93: 169–177, 2002. to leftward deviation on an implicit line bisection task. Cortex,
Nicholls ME, Orr CA, and Lindell AK. Magical ideation and its 38: 247–252, 2002.
relation to lateral preference. Laterality, 10: 503–515, 2005. Thalbourne MA and Haraldsson E. Personality characteristics
Nickerson R. Null hypothesis significance testing: a review of of sheep and goats. Personality and Individual Differences, 1:
an old and continuing controversy. Psychological Methods, 5: 180–185, 1980.
241–301, 2000. Thoma RJ, Yeo RA, Gangestad SW, Halgren E, Sanchez NM, and
Papousek I and Schulter G. Quantitative assessment of five Lewine JD. Cortical volume and developmental instability are
behavioural laterality measures: distribution of scores and independent predictors of general intellectual ability.
intercorrelations among right-handers. Laterality, 4: 345–362, Intelligence, 33: 27–38, 2005.
1999a. Thoma RJ, Yeo RA, Gangestad SW, Lewine JD, and Davis JT. Fluctuating
Papousek I and Schulter G. EEG correlates of behavioural asymmetry and the human brain. Laterality, 7: 45–58, 2002.
laterality: right-handedness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89: Van Valen L. A study of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution, 16:
403–411, 1999b. 125–142, 1962.
Papousek I and Schulter G. EEG correlates of behavioural Wierzbicki M. Reasoning errors and belief in the paranormal.
laterality: ear dominance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94: Journal of Social Psychology, 125: 489–494, 1985.
355–362, 2002. Wiseman R, Greening E, and Smith M. Belief in the paranormal
Papousek I and Schulter G. Individual differences in functional and suggestion in the seance room. British Journal of Psychology,
asymmetries of the cortical hemispheres. Revival of laterality 94: 285–297, 2003.
research in emotion and psychopathology. Cognition, Brain, Wiseman R and Watt C. Belief in psychic ability and the
Behavior, 10: 269–329, 2006. misattribution hypothesis: a qualitative review. British Journal
Porac C. Eye preference patterns among left-handed adults. of Psychology, 97: 323–338, 2006.
Laterality, 2: 305–316, 1997. Yeo RA and Gangestad SW. Developmental origins of variation in
Raz A, Hines T, Fossella J, and Castro D. Paranormal experience human hand preference. Genetica, 89: 281–296, 1993.
and the COMT dopaminergic gene: a preliminary attempt to Yeo RA, Gangestad SW, and Daniel WF. Hand preference and
associate phenotype with genotype using an underlying brain developmental instability. Psychobiology, 21: 161–168, 1993.
theory. Cortex, 44: 1336–1341, 2008. Yeo RA, Gangestad SW, Thoma R, Shaw P, and Repa K.
Ruch W. Die revidierte Fassung des Eysenck Personality Developmental instability and cerebral lateralization.
Questionnaire und die Konstruktion des deutschen EPQ-R Neuropsychology, 11: 552–561, 1997.

You might also like