You are on page 1of 8

European Journal of Sport Science

ISSN: 1746-1391 (Print) 1536-7290 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tejs20

Foul or dive? Motor contributions to judging


ambiguous foul situations in football

Peter G. Renden , Sander Kerstens , Raôul R. D. Oudejans & Rouwen Cañal-


Bruland

To cite this article: Peter G. Renden , Sander Kerstens , Raôul R. D. Oudejans & Rouwen
Cañal-Bruland (2014) Foul or dive? Motor contributions to judging ambiguous foul
situations in football, European Journal of Sport Science, 14:sup1, S221-S227, DOI:
10.1080/17461391.2012.683813

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2012.683813

Published online: 31 May 2012.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 465

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 8 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tejs20

Download by: [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] Date: 23 February 2016, At: 02:29
European Journal of Sport Science, 2014
Vol. 14, No. S1, S221S227, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2012.683813

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Foul or dive? Motor contributions to judging ambiguous foul situations


in football

PETER G. RENDEN, SANDER KERSTENS, RAÔUL R. D. OUDEJANS, & ROUWEN


CAÑAL-BRULAND

Research Institute MOVE, Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

Abstract
Football (soccer) referees frequently face situations in which they have to distinguish dives and fouls. Yet, little is known
about the contributing factors that characterise the ability to judge these ambiguous situations correctly. To this end, in the
current article we tested the hypothesis that motor experience of observers contributes to the visual identification of
deceptive actions. Thereto, we asked skilled football referees, skilled football players, wheelchair bounded football fans (thus
with limited motor experience) and novices to judge whether potential tackle situations in football were either fouls or dives.
Results revealed that the referees (accuracy 72.2%, s6.2) and players (accuracy 72.0%, s 6.4) were better at
discriminating fouls and dives than the fans (accuracy 61.1%, s 7.2) and the novices (accuracy 57.4%, s7.0)
(P B 0.001). The results seem to point to an added value of motor experience in detecting deceptive movements.

Keywords: Deception, dives, fouls, motor experience, referees

Introduction That observers are in fact able to detect deceptive


intentions from bodily information was first reported
Taking a dive has become a commonly used strategy
by Runeson and Frykholm (1983). They demon-
by football players in the attempt to receive a free
strated that naive observers were able to distinguish
kick or a penalty. According to the football rules
actors lifting a heavy or a light box and even whether
however, this strategy is an illegal action and often
actors faked or did not fake the weight of the lifted
leads to much discussion between players, referees boxes. The observers’ decisions were solely based on
and spectators. In an attempt to reduce the number the bodily (kinematic) information of the actors, and
of dives taken, any simulating action intended to the results thus indicated that bodily movements
deceive the referee is now labelled as unsporting convey sufficient (reliable) information about decep-
behaviour, which is sanctioned by a yellow card. As a tive actions.
result, the referee faces the difficult task to success- This is also the case in football in which important
fully distinguish dives from fouls. This task is not just information is portrayed in the kinematics of the
demanding but also very important as wrong deci- falling player. Recently, Morris and Lewis (2010)
sions can have crucial effects on the outcome of the identified recognisable movement characteristics
game. Therefore it is of particular relevance to that can be observed when a player is diving. For
understand which factors contribute to the deci- example, when a player is tackled, the body bends
sion-making process when distinguishing dives from forward over the legs, the head stays forward and the
fouls. In this article, we aim to test the hypothesis arms prepare for impact. In contrast, when a player
that motor experience in football contributes to the is diving, the chest is more upright, the head is
ability to recognise dives in potential tackle situa- turned back and the arms are raised in the air. The
tions. authors denominate this specific diving movement as

Correspondence: Peter Renden, Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, VU University Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 9,
Amsterdam 1081 BT, The Netherlands. E-mail: p.g.renden@vu.nl

# 2013 European College of Sport Science


S222 P. Renden et al.

the ‘archers bow’. Although more ambiguous dive Haggard, 2005; Calvo-Merino, Grèzes, Glaser, Pas-
movements have been observed as well (Morris & singham, & Haggard, 2006). As such, higher activa-
Lewis, 2010), kinematic patterns conveyed in the tions of the motor areas pertaining to the mirror
movement of the falling player indeed seem to neuron system show positive correlations with better
provide key information about the deceptive or recognition and outcome prediction of other peo-
non-deceptive nature of falling. ple’s movements.
In the field of sports, Jackson, Warren and These findings lead to the assumption that motor
Abernethy (2006) were the first to examine the experience may also enhance the ability to detect
influence of expertise on detecting deceptive move- deceptive movements. However, studies that exam-
ments. They asked experienced and inexperienced ined deception detection could thus far not confirm
rugby players to detect deceptive intentions of this hypothesis. For instance, in an attempt to
approaching rugby players during one on one tackle dissociate motor and perceptual experience, Cañal-
situations. The results revealed that the experienced Bruland and Schmidt (2009) asked handball field
players were better able to detect the deceptive players, goal-keepers and novices to detect the
intentions of the approaching rugby player than their deceptive intentions of a player taking penalty shots.
less experienced counterparts. Jackson et al. argued Although it was expected that the motor experience
that the experienced players, when compared to the of the field players would lead to better judgements
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

inexperienced players, had faced more deceptive than those of the goalkeepers, it appeared that field
actions during training and matches, which im- players and goalkeepers performed equally well,
proved their perceptual expertise. In football refer- while both groups outperformed the inexperienced
eeing, this could mean that the more referees train control group. This would imply that the motor
and improve their visual expertise (e.g. by means of experience of the field players did not contribute to
refereeing many matches or by video training, see deception detection. However, although handball
Plessner, Schweizer, Brand, & O’Hare, 2009; goalkeepers do not generally take penalty shots,
Schweizer, Plessner, Kahlert, & Brand, 2011), the they do perform many throws during matches and
better they may become in distinguishing dives from training. Thus, the amount of motor experience of
fouls. the keepers may well have influenced the results of
However, Cañal-Bruland, van der Kamp, and van this experiment.
Kesteren (2010) recently argued that the skilled In a similar study, Sebanz and Shiffrar (2009)
rugby players in the Jackson et al. study had probably presented videos and static pictures of fake and real
not only faced, but also performed more deceptive basketball passes to basketball players and control
actions, suggesting that the higher degree of motor participants. They found that experts outperformed
experience could also have contributed to better novices only when videos were presented, that is,
accuracy scores. The authors supported their argu- when dynamic information was available. The find-
ment by referring to findings from a recent study by ing that experts only outperformed novices using
Aglioti, Cesari, Romani, and Urgesi (2008). Aglioti dynamic information, led the authors to suggest that
et al. (2008) found that basketball players, who the experts may have tapped into their own action
possessed extensive motor experience, were superior repertoire. However, the experimental design was
in judging the outcome of basketball free throws (i.e. not build to rule out the possibility that experts also
hit or miss) when compared to the visually equally made use of their extensive visual experience. That
experienced coaches and journalists, and to inexper- is, Sebanz and Shiffrar (2009) did not include a
ienced laymen. Because of the assumption that the control group that had equal visual experience (as
players, coaches and journalists had comparable compared to the experts), but no motor experience.
visual experience, these results suggest that motor Taken together, several studies seem to suggest
experience has additional value on top of visual that motor experience contributes to recognising
experience (for more behavioural evidence, see e.g. and/or predicting the movements of observed others
Casile & Giese, 2006; Hecht, Vogt, & Prinz, 2001). (cf. Aglioti et al., 2008; Calvo-Merino et al., 2005,
Further support for this notion comes from studies 2006). Yet, due to methodological limitations, espe-
using functional magnetic resonance imaging and cially in testing appropriate groups with differences
transcranial magnetic stimulation. These studies in motor and visual experience, strong evidence for
show that certain motor areas in the brain, known this hypothesis in the realm of deception detection
as the ‘mirror neuron system’ (e.g. Rizzolatti & remains to be provided (Cañal-Bruland & Schmidt,
Craighero, 2004), are more activated when obser- 2009; Cañal-Bruland et al., 2010; Sebanz & Shiffrar,
ving actions that are part of one’s own action 2009). Therefore, the aim of the present study was to
repertoire than when watching actions that are not examine if motor experience enhances the ability to
part of one’s own motor repertoire (cf. Aglioti et al., detect deceptive movements when compared to mere
2008; Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham, & visual experience.
Judging foul situations in football S223

Table I. Mean data (9s) for motor and visual experience in years and hours per week for the football referees, football players, the
wheelchair bounded (WB) fans and the novices

Motor experience Visual experience

Years Hours per weeka Years Hours per weekb

Football referees 11.7794.6 5.0291.58 15.4593.8 9.0693.6


Football players 20.2795.7 6.5092.11 20.2995.7 9.0692.6
WB fans 3.5894.9 2.8393.69 36.00910.5 7.0794.6
Novices 0.9492.8 0.3190.93 1.2493.6 90.0
a
For the referees, fans and novices: on average 5, 24 and 18 years ago (see Method section).
b
Average visual experience at the time they participated in the study.

To this end, we invited skilled football referees, referees, would outperform the fans and the novices
skilled football players, wheelchair bounded football as these groups had no football motor experience.
fans and a group of novices to judge deceptive and Because of their visual experience, we expected the
non-deceptive tackle situations in football in a video- fans to outperform the novices. These expectations
based test. The referees were considered to be should minimally result in higher accuracy scores for
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

experts in this task with task-specific visual experi- the players and the referees when compared to the
ence in judging tackle situations, general visual fans and the novices as well as for the fans when
experience in watching football, and football motor compared to the novices.
experience in the past (which could still be of added
value). The players had football motor experience, as
Method
well as general visual experience in watching foot-
ball. Notably, when the referees and players gained Participants
their motor experience, they simultaneously gained
A total of 78 participants (66 men, 12 women) took
perceptual experience (see also Cañal-Bruland,
part in the study; 31 skilled football referees (mean
Mooren, & Savelsbergh, 2011). Therefore, it is
age 25.35 years, s3.73, 30 male), 17 skilled
reasonable to argue that both the referees and players
football players (mean age 27.29 years; s 6.18, 17
had perceptual-motor experience in football. Yet,
male), 12 wheelchair bounded football fans (mean
what distinguishes these two groups from the other age 45.33 years, s10.63, 10 male) and 18 novices
two groups is particularly their motor experience in (mean age 30.72 years, s12.52, 9 male) partici-
football. That is, compared to the fans and the pated voluntarily.1
novices the referees and players had more motor An overview of the experiences of the groups is
experience. Therefore, in the remainder of the article presented in Table I. The referees were selected by
we refer to referees and players as motor experi- the Dutch football association (KNVB) for the
enced. The wheelchair bounded fans had only national talent pool for referees and they performed
general visual experience in watching football. Be- in the top amateur league of The Netherlands. The
cause of their disability they had very limited motor mean experience as a referee was 6.82 years
experience related to walking and running. The (s2.12), refereeing on average one match a week.
novices had neither motor nor visual experience On average, they had ceased to play football more
with football. The video based test consisted of than five years before the experiment (mean 5.83
watching and judging video clips of tackle situations, years, s3.2). Their mean playing experience was
in which either a foul or a dive was presented. 11.77 years (s 4.6). Referees reported to have
We expected that given their task-specific visual played football on average 5.02 hours per week
experience referees would be best at discriminating (s1.58). The referees reported an average of 9.06
fouls and dives (e.g. Catteeuw, Helsen, Gilis, & (s3.55) hours of visual experience with football per
Wagemans, 2009). In line with the assumption that week. This included the time watching football
motor experience is of added value on top of visual matches (live or on television) as well as refereeing
experience, we expected that the players, just as the football matches.

1
Originally we aimed for 18 participants per group. For the group of skilled players we achieved this number with the exception of one
player who was not available for testing due to being ill. As concerns the referees we depended on the Dutch Football Association. The
association made it possible that we measured all referees (n31) on one of their training days. After approaching over 50 wheelchair
bounded fans, in the end 12 qualified participants were willing to participate within the time span of the study. Statistically we checked
whether corrections were needed, but Levene’s test of Equality of Error Variance was not significant indicating that homogeneity of variance
was not violated and that corrections were not necessary.
S224 P. Renden et al.

All players were members of one team performing experiment if both referees independently gave the
at the same level of competition as the referees. The same judgement. In total, 60 situations were in-
league can be compared to the Second Division in cluded containing 30 fouls and 30 dives (6 practice
England. These players can therefore be considered clips and 54 experimental clips). The situations were
highly skilled football players. Their mean playing cut into brief video clips that stopped at the moment
experience was 20.27 years (s 5.65). They reported the whole trunk of the falling player had contact with
to play football on average 6.5 hours per week the ground. Replays or parts thereof, were not
(s2.11). On average, the players reported 9.06 included. Furthermore, reactions of referees were
hours (s 2.63) of visual experience with football per not visible in the clips. Clips were always cut-off
week. This included the time watching football before any reaction of the referee occurred. The
matches (live or on television), football practice average length of the clips was 3.7 seconds. The clips
(fitness training was excluded) and playing matches. were presented in a random order.
Five fans had played football before they were
bounded to a wheelchair,2 but all had ceased playing
more than 12 years ago (mean 24.6 years, s14.99). Procedure
The mean playing experience of their group was 2.83
hours a week (s3.69). The fans reported 7.07 The participants were tested individually and 54
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

hours of visual experience a week (s4.60). clips (27 fouls and 27 dives) were presented on a
Two of the novices had also played football in the laptop (Packard Bell EasyNote) with a 15.4 inch
past. They stopped playing, respectively, 7 and 30 screen. The distance between the screen and the
years ago. The mean playing experience of the participant’s head was approximately 50 centimetres.
novices was 0.31 hours a week (s0.93). None of No information was provided about the number of
the novices watched football regularly. fouls and dives that would be presented. After each
Participants provided written informed consent clip, the participants had three seconds to indicate
prior to participation, and the experiment was whether the player’s fall was caused by a foul or a
approved by the Ethics committee of the Research dive (for the use of a three seconds time window see,
Institute. e.g. Cañal-Bruland et al., 2011). Participants gave
their answer by clicking a button on the keyboard.
Before the 54 experimental clips were presented, the
Materials participants judged 6 practice clips to become
The experimenters selected potential tackle situa- familiar with the experimental procedure and they
tions from broadcasted television images of the were informed about their results. The entire experi-
World Cup 2006 in Germany. The potential tackle ment lasted approximately 15 minutes.
situations were selected if the experimenters believed It was not possible to test all the referees indivi-
it showed a foul or a dive. In the context of dually, because of the time schedule that was
association football, a foul is defined as an unfair provided by the KNVB. Of the 31 referees 24
tackle of the opponent. A dive is defined as a watched the clips on a wide screen (2 3 meters)
voluntary fall to feint that a foul was committed in in one room, and wrote their judgements on an
an attempt to receive a penalty or a free kick (FIFA, answer sheet. These participants were divided in
2011). Because the determination of a foul or dive is three groups of eight and they were positioned
based on the interpretation of the referee, after initial approximately three meters from the screen. To
scene selection two experienced referees were se- find out whether the different test circumstances
lected to form an expert panel. Both referees were had an effect on the results, we compared the 24
accredited by the Dutch national football associa- referees tested in groups with the 7 referees who were
tion. The referees watched all selected situations tested individually. A 2 (foul, dive) 4 (individually
including replays from different angles as often as tested, group one, group two and group three)
they needed, and judged whether the player’s fall was ANOVA revealed no significant main effect for
caused by a foul of the opponent or a dive of the group, F3,27 0.921, P 0.444. Therefore, we in-
concerning player. A situation was included in the cluded all referees in the study.

2
We had no reason to believe that the fans’ visual-cognitive skills were impaired by their injury. Seven participants of this group were active
as wheelchair basketball players. Therefore, we assumed that they had no problems with their visual-cognitive skills. We visited the other five
participants in their home and we noticed no signs of visual or cognitive impairments during our contact with them or when they filled in the
accompanying questionnaire asking for their experiences with football. One potential participant was excluded because we did have doubts
about his visual-cognitive skills and whether he fully understood the task.
Judging foul situations in football S225

Data analysis that response bias had an influence on the perceptual


judgements of the four groups.
We performed a mixed-design ANOVA on the mean
accuracy scores (  number of correct decisions)
with condition (foul, dive) as the within-subjects Discussion
factor and group (players, referees, fans and novices)
as the between-subjects factor. In addition, to The aim of the present study was to examine if sport
examine whether a response bias for either ‘foul’ or specific motor experience enhances the ability to
‘dive’ judgements may have influenced the accuracy detect deceptive movements in ‘foul vs. dive’ judge-
scores, we performed a one-way ANOVA on the ments in football. To this end, referees, players,
absolute percentage scores of ‘dive’ judgements with wheelchair bounded fans and a group of novices
group as the between-subjects factor. Furthermore, judged video clips of tackle situations in football.
we tested the absolute percentage scores of the ‘dive’ The most direct examination of the role of motor
experience on foul vs. dive judgements concerned
judgements of each group separately against the
the comparison between the players and wheelchair
value of 50%, which is the value for an unbiased
bounded fans, as only the players possessed signifi-
response.3 When appropriate, we performed post-
cant motor experience while both groups had similar
hoc comparisons using Bonferroni correction. The
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

amounts of visual experience and no experience or


uncorrected alpha level for significance was set at
formal training as referees. Regarding this compar-
0.05. Effect sizes were calculated as partial eta- ison, results showed that players were significantly
squared values (hp 2). more accurate than wheelchair bounded fans in
judging fouls and dives, thereby providing support
for the hypothesis that motor experience may have
Results beneficial effects on decision-making processes in
The mean accuracy scores for each group are refereeing.
presented in Figure 1. The 2 (foul, dive)4 Furthermore, results showed that both referees
(referees, players, fans, novices) ANOVA on accu- and players performed significantly better than the
racy scores revealed a significant main effect for fans and novices as regards judging fouls and dives.
group, F3,74 25.40, P B0.001, h2p 0.507.4 Post- Notably, the players were just as good as the referees
hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that the players in discriminating between dives and fouls. Fans and
(mean 72.0%) were more accurate in their judge- novices also showed no differences. We argue that
ments than the fans (mean 61.1%), P B 0.001, 95% because the players (with motor experience) were as
CI [4%, 18%] and novices (mean 57.4%), accurate as the referees and more accurate than the
wheelchair bounded fans (note that these three
P B 0.001, 95% CI [9%, 21%]. Likewise, the
groups were comparable with respect to general
referees (mean 72.2%) did better than the fans,
visual football experience), it seems that motor
P B 0.001, 95% CI [17%, 49%] and novices,
experience of the players enhanced their ability to
P B 0.001, 95% CI [10%, 20%]. There were no
detect deceptive movements. That wheelchair
significant differences in accuracy scores between the
bounded fans were as ‘good’ as the novices and
players and referees (P0.998) nor between the fans worse than the players may indicate that mere
and the novices (P0.799). There was no signifi- general visual experience in watching football has
cant main effect for condition (foul, dive), no added value for detecting deceptive movements.
F1,74 0.21, P 0.885, nor a significant interaction In comparison to earlier studies (Aglioti et al.,
between group and condition, F3,74 1.883, 2008; Cañal-Bruland & Schmidt, 2009; Sebanz &
P 0.140. Shiffrar, 2009), we tried to make a clearer distinction
For the analysis of response bias, the results between participants with both visual and motor
showed no significant difference among groups, experience (players) and participants with merely
F3,74 1.823, P 0.150. Furthermore, one-sample visual experience (wheelchair bounded fans). Note
t-tests revealed no significant biases for the players that Aglioti et al. (2008) showed that their expert
(mean 52.0%), t16 1.031, the referees (mean watchers (coaches and journalists) were more accu-
52.4%), t30 1.255, the fans (mean 44.7%), rate in predicting the outcome of basketball shots
t11   1.226, and the novices (mean 52.7%), than novices. As argued by Cañal-Bruland et al.
t17 1.262, all P 0.05. These results rule out (2010) this could be due to the fact that basketball

3
Note that analyses on the percentage of foul judgements are redundant with analyses on the percentage of dive judgements.
4
To rule out that the variable ‘age’ influenced the accuracy scores, we included age as a covariate. The 2 (foul, dive)4 (referees, players,
fans, novices) ANCOVA on accuracy scores with age as covariate revealed no significant effects of age.
S226 P. Renden et al.

Figure 1. Mean accuracy scores (in %) for the football referees, football players, the wheelchair bounded (WB) fans and the novices.
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

Vertical bars indicate the Standard Deviation (s).

coaches, who joint the basketball journalists in the fouls and dives remains to be determined. The
expert watchers group, had gained considerable question is whether the referees would have per-
motor experience during their active careers as formed the same if they had not gained any motor
players. Possibly, past motor experience (on average experience in football. In this experiment, the fans
coaches had stopped playing basketball 9.4 years had a comparable amount of visual experience but
earlier) is still of added value for predicting shot very limited motor experience and they did neither
outcome. Unfortunately, the results of the coaches perform better than the novices nor did they perform
and journalists were not separated and thus the as good as the referees. Thus although the referees’
influence of the coaches’ previous motor experience task-specific experience may have had added value, it
could not be determined. Similarly, the distinction may well be that their past motor experience was an
regarding motor and visual experience among the important contributor to their accuracy scores as
groups in the studies of Cañal-Bruland and Schmidt well. Future research is needed to gain more insight
(2009) and Sebanz and Shiffrar (2009) did not allow into the relationship between after-effects of previous
unambiguous conclusions regarding the contribution motor experience and the ability to detect deceptive
of motor experience in detecting deceptive move- movements.
ments. Although we argue that the presence or absence of
In the current study, the referees were considered motor experience seems to have influenced the
the experts given that they had past motor experi- accuracy scores in this experiment, some possible
ence, general visual experience in watching football, limitations need to be taken into account. Firstly, a
but most important task-specific visual experience. possible limitation is that the experimental task is not
Referees were the only group that had extensive task- representative for the whole field of decisions a
specific experience in actually judging and providing referee has to make. For instance, football referees
a formal decision regarding fouls and dives. A have to judge offside situations, ensure that players
demonstration of the importance of the influence adhere to the rules (e.g. penalise hand play), penalise
of task-specific experience has been recently given by coaches for unsporting behaviours etc. Thus, gen-
Dicks, Button, and Davids (2010). They showed eralising our findings on foul and dive judgements to
that gaze and movement behaviour of football goal- other decisions referees make during games needs to
keepers during penalty shots were influenced by the be done with caution. In a similar vein, it is unlikely
type of experimental task. The goalkeepers made that potential tackle situations in regular matches do
more saves under a natural (in situ) experimental have a 50% chance of being a foul or a dive.
condition when compared to joystick and verbal Therefore, the 50/50 distribution of fouls and dives
response conditions. The more representative an in the experiment may have differed from the
experimental task is (the closer to the task they are distribution with which referees are normally con-
used to perform in the field), the higher the chance fronted in actual matches. As a result, it is possible
that participants can draw from their specific experi- that in comparison with the other groups, particu-
ences and also show representative performances. larly the players, referees may have had a disadvan-
To what degree the past motor experience of the tage in judging the clips, as they may have expected a
referees contributed to their ability to discriminate different distribution between fouls and dives. This
Judging foul situations in football S227

may provide an additional explanation for why References


referees were not more accurate than players. Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008).
Secondly, the visual experience of the referees and Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball
players was partly gained on the football field. The players. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 11091116.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., &
fans only gained their visual experience as spectators.
Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor
It may be possible that visual experience gained on skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15,
the football field (especially given the specific view- 12431249.
ing perspective [immersed in play]) was of added Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., &
Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and
value compared to visual experience gained as a
motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16,
spectator or from watching television. 19051910.
Thirdly, although we ensured a clearer distinction Cañal-Bruland, R., Mooren, M., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2011).
between groups, there is still some minimal overlap Differentiting experts’ anticipatory skills in beach volleyball.
in experiences. MacMahon, Starkes, and Deakin Research Quaterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 667674.
Cañal-Bruland, R., & Schmidt, M. (2009). Response bias in
(2009) recently argued that in experiments that judging deceptive movements. Acta Psychologica, 130,
examine the contribution of different experiences, 235240.
experiences may also overlap between experimental Cañal-Bruland, R., van der Kamp, J., & van Kesteren, J. (2010).
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 02:29 23 February 2016

groups due to role transitions (e.g. players become An examination of motor and perceptual contributions to the
recognition of deception from others’ actions. Human Move-
referees). As for the referees, we did discuss their ment Science, 29, 94102.
previous football experience. As for the other groups Casile, A., & Giese, M.A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training
there was a small overlap in experiences as well. influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16,
Some of the fans gained little football motor 6974.
Catteeuw, P., Helsen, W., Gilis, B., & Wagemans, J. (2009).
experience before their injury and some of the Decision-making skills, role specifity, and deliberate practice in
players did a little refereeing in youth matches assocation football refereeing. Journal of Sport Sciences, 27,
(players at the age of 610 years). However, as 11251136.
mentioned before and most importantly, compared Dicks, M., Button, C., & Davids, K. (2010). Examination of gaze
behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints
to previous studies (Aglioti et al., 2008; Cañal- reveals differences in information pickup for perception and
Bruland & Schmidt, 2009; Sebanz & Shiffrar, action. Attention, Perception, Psychophysics, 72, 706720.
2009) the overlap in experiences of the groups tested FIFA. (2011). Laws of the game 2010/2011. Retrieved March 29,
in the current study is present yet minimal. In 2011, from http://www.fifa.com/mm/document/affederation/
generic/81/42/36/lawsofthegame_2010_11_e.pdf
addition, MacMahon et al. further argued that task Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor learning enhances
characteristics that are for instance assigned to perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer.
refereeing a match may play a central role when Psychological Research, 65, 314.
Jackson, R. C., Warren, S., & Abernethy, B. (2006). Anticipation
playing. For example, players constantly have to
skill and susceptibility to deceptive movement. Acta
anticipate what is likely to occur next. As such, they Psychologica, 123, 355371.
may also have to make anticipatory judgements as to MacMahon, C., Starkes, J. L., & Deakin, J. (2009). Differences in
whether a potential tackle situation will be judged as processing of game information in basketball players, coaches
a foul or not. Therefore, it may be possible that the and referees. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40,
403423.
players in our experiment had gained task-specific Morris, P. H., & Lewis, D. (2010). Tackling diving: The
experience in judging tackle situations (typically perception of deceptive intentions in association football
assigned to referees). (soccer). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34, 113.
To summarise, our results seem to suggest that Plessner, H., Schweizer, G., Brand, R., & O’Hare, D. (2009). A
multiple-cue learning approach as the basis for understanding
sport specific motor experience contributes to de- and improving soccer referees’ decision making. Progress in
tecting deceptive movements, and, more specifically, Brain Research, 174, 151158.
to discriminating fouls and dives in football. How- Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron
ever, it remains to be determined whether recent system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169192.
Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of
motor experience is of more value compared to dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action
motor experience gained in the past. If this is the perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive
case, practical implications of this finding may be intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112,
that athletes but also referees may improve their 585615.
Schweizer, G., Plessner, H., Kahlert, D., & Brand, R. (2011). A
ability to detect a certain deceptive movement by video-based training method for improving soccer referees’
improving their own experience of performing that intuitive decision-making skills. Journal of Applied Sport
movement. For example, referees might be able to Psychology, 23, 429442.
improve their judging performance by training dives Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a
bluffing body: The role of expertise. Psychonomic Bulletin &
and fouls in practice. Review, 16, 170175.

You might also like