You are on page 1of 9

Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Children and Youth Services Review


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/childyouth

Pre-college STEM camps as developmental context: Mediational relations T


between gender, career decidedness, socioemotional development, and
engagement

Ryan J. Gagnona, , Alexandra Sandovalb
a
Clemson University, 276A Lehotsky Hall, Clemson, SC 29634, United States
b
Clemson University, 263 Lehotsky Hall, Clemson, SC 29634, United States

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: One potential mechanism to mitigate the underrepresentation of females within STEM is the use of Out-of-
STEM School-Time programs and camps with supportive staff, instructors, and parallel orientation towards socio-
Adolescence emotional development. This study examines factors which may mediate the relation between gender and STEM
Career decidedness career aspiration within a pre-collegiate week-long STEM camp experience among a sample of 365 adolescent
Gender
campers. Campers were an average of 15.58 years old, primarily white (69.8%), and male (51.8%). The results of
Camp
Pre collegiate
the mediational structural equation model (SEM) suggested socioemotional development did not mediate the
relation between gender and career decidedness, and there was no statistical difference between male and fe-
male STEM career decidedness. The lack of effect could be a result of the systematic and ongoing emphasis
towards the development of STEM skills and aspiration within minority and underrepresented groups, specifi-
cally females.

1. Introduction 1.1. The gender gap within STEM

An aggregate of research examining the career fields of science, The gender gap within some aspects of the STEM career field is
technology, engineering, and math (STEM), indicates a longstanding potentially declining; in some areas, girls and young women are per-
marginalization and absence of women from the field (Hughes, forming equivalent to or better than their male counterparts in STEM
Nzekew, & Molyneaux, 2013; Wang & Degol, 2017). While the causes competencies (Klahr, Triona, & Williams, 2007). Indeed, Stoet and
and consequences of the lack of representation of these groups within Geary (2018) illustrated adolescent females tended to perform as good
STEM are seemingly well documented (Dasgupta & Stout, 2014; Wang or better than their male counterparts in science, mathematics, and
& Degol, 2013), exploration to establish strategies to address these reading competencies. However, the overall representation of women
deficiencies remain ongoing. Specifically, one potential mechanism is within actual STEM remains limited compared to men (Eccles, 2011).
the use of informal science programs and semi-structured STEM courses For instance, about 50% of all science and engineering (S&E) degrees
to encourage the continuous participation of unrepresented groups have been awarded to women since the late 1990s, but approximately
across STEM career paths, with a specific focus on youth STEM pro- 64% of scientists or engineers working in the for-profit S&E sector are
grams to encourage them in their future careers (Beier et al., 2018; male (National Science Foundation, 2017). While there are other po-
Holmes, Redmond, Thomas, & High, 2012). However, the factors within tential outlets for STEM employment (e.g., education, government), the
these programs that promote STEM career decidedness are less under- for-profit sector is the largest STEM employer, “regardless of gender,
stood (Beier et al., 2018; Liben & Coyle, 2014). Consequently, the race, ethnicity, or disability status” (p. 12, National Science
present study examines factors which may enhance STEM career de- Foundation, 2017). The lack of balance between degrees awarded (50%
cidedness through the attendance of pre-collegiate STEM programs. male: 50% female) and those actually employed in their degreed field
Below research exploring the gender gap within STEM careers is ex- (64% male: 43% female) represents a challenge within STEM, pointing
plored, literature examining potential contributors to STEM program to a potential issue with pipeline for young women and girls interested
success are shared, and the current study is presented. in a STEM career (Stoet & Geary, 2018). Put differently, it appears the


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: rjgagno@clemson.edu (R.J. Gagnon), asandov@clemson.edu (A. Sandoval).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104584
Received 25 May 2019; Received in revised form 27 October 2019; Accepted 28 October 2019
Available online 20 November 2019
0190-7409/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

early competencies and requisite skills sets are present in both adoles- appreciation, and career decidedness among adolescent campers. In a
cent females and males, but females tend to engage in STEM careers at broader study of OST programs, Tiffany, Exner-Cortens, and Eckenrode
lower rates and in many cases forgo participating in a STEM centered (2012) illustrated similar findings, where the presence of caring and
college major, representing a potential “exodus of talent among girls supportive staff tended to enhance programmatic quality. Explicating
and women who could otherwise become the next generation of sci- on the unique contexts within STEM programs, some research also
entists, engineers, and creators of technology” (Dasgupta & Stout, 2014, suggests the intentional exposure of youth to active scientists, may
p. 22). narrow the gender gap within STEM, where female participants become
more likely to shift to a STEM career path after their participation in
1.2. Out-of-school-time programming to enhance STEM career decidedness these programs, due in part to role modelling behaviors demonstrated
by STEM instructors and scientists (Kang et al., 2019; Mohr-Schroeder
The motivation to pursue (or not pursue) a STEM career may begin et al., 2014; Mostache, Matloff-Nieves, Kekelis, & Lawner, 2013).
in adolescence (Lubinski, 2010), where a variety of macro- and micro- Importantly, simple attendance of a STEM program alone is likely
level factors can inhibit later gender equity across STEM fields (Sinclair, insufficient to improve an adolescents STEM career decidedness levels,
Nilsson, & Cederskar, 2019; Stoet & Geary, 2018). More specifically, rather quality of participation and engagement with a particular program
evidence suggests gender disparities within STEM appear early in or setting tend to also represent necessary conditions for outcome
adolescence, illustrating the need for “intervention” at phases earlier achievement (Roth, Malone, & Brooks-Gunn, 2010; Simpkins, Little, &
than emerging adulthood (e.g., 18–24 years old) where STEM equity Weiss, 2004). Further, there is a breadth of literature linking caring and
programs are more frequent (Kang, Hense, Scheersoi, & Keinonen, supportive staff behaviors with greater rates of participant engagement
2019). For instance, out-of-school-time STEM programs that target and corresponding achievement of desirable program outcomes in the
early to middle adolescents have been associated with the development broader sphere of adolescent program research (Simpkins, 2015;
of STEM competencies and corresponding confidence in one’s “fit” or Tiffany, Exner-Cortens, & Eckenrode, 2013) and to a lesser degree
desire towards a STEM career, in both mixed gender settings and female within STEM centered programs (Beier et al., 2018; Kramer et al., 2016;
only settings (Holmes et al., 2012; Hughes et al., 2013). In a review of Leon et al., 2015). The links between supportive staff behaviors, camper
research examining the gender gap within the STEM field, Wang and engagement, and camper outcomes illustrate how quality STEM
Degol (2017) noted career paths and preferences are often established summer programs (e.g., science camps) can lead to greater engagement
in adolescence, and gender frequently plays a meaningful role in this with STEM coursework, greater aspirations towards STEM college ma-
decision-making process. Programs that cultivate aspiration towards jors, career interests, and improved self-confidence among males and
STEM-centered learning in adolescence could lead to meaningful de- females (Dieker, Grillo, & Ramlakhan, 2012; Fields, 2009; Kong et al.,
creases in the gender gap within STEM (Wegemer & Eccles, 2019). 2014).
Both within-school and out-of-school-time (OST) STEM programs
serving adolescents are generally orientated to enhance engagement 1.3. Socioemotional skill development within OST STEM programs
and motivation in STEM centered learning (Kong, Dabney, & Tai,
2014). In many cases these programs are designed to foster youth Although underexamined within the context of STEM program re-
motivations to engage in advanced STEM learning (i.e., college major search, the development of socioemotional skills and self-determinate
selection) and careers (Dasgupta & Stout, 2014; Jungert, Hubbart, behaviors (e.g., autonomy, relatedness, competence) in parallel with
Dedic, & Rosenfield, 2019), especially for cohorts historically under- STEM career decidedness represents an additional conduit to under-
represented in STEM (e.g., some ethnic/racial minorities, persons with stand how to best serve and cultivate the next generation of STEM
disabilities, females) (Eccles & Wang, 2016; Gasiewski, Eagan, Garcia, professionals (Beier et al., 2018; Jungert et al., 2019; Kramer et al.,
Hurtado, & Chang, 2012). For example, an emerging body of evidence 2016; Wilson et al., 2015). Indeed, some research suggests programs
suggests participation in these STEM centered experiences may enhance that also target and foster socioemotional skills are more likely to en-
female motivation to engage in further STEM education, conceptualized hance STEM career decidedness and motivations to engage in current
by an improvement or shift in decidedness on a STEM centered career and future STEM education (Falk et al., 2016; Guay, Senecal, Gauthier,
(Mohr-Schroeder et al., 2014; Wang & Degol, 2017). However, research & Fernet, 2003; Leon et al., 2015). For instance, programs emphasizing
in this area is relatively scant and based primarily upon college student the development and/or enhancement of friendship skills, self-esteem,
(i.e., 18–24 years old) samples rather than adolescents, illustrating a and communication abilities can facilitate a sense of belonging to a
gap in our understanding of how these programs may decrease gender community interested in STEM and thus enhance motivation to more
effects on STEM career decidedness at earlier phases (Kang et al., 2019). readily involve oneself in later STEM-centric opportunities (Eccles,
In seeming parallel with the developing research exploring con- 2011; Sinclair et al., 2019). This engagement and satisfaction with
tributors to the gender gap within STEM, there is an escalation in re- course materials and activities has been associated with greater rates of
search regarding the role of OST STEM programs on addressing this aspiration towards STEM and academic performance (Vedder-Weiss &
gap. More specifically, research examining OST STEM programs has Fortus, 2011; Wilson et al., 2015). For instance, in an examination of
shifted focus from solely on STEM program attendance and outcomes, the relationship between gender and motivation within STEM, Solanki
to an additional focus towards understanding the qualities of STEM and Xu (2018) demonstrated when female undergraduate students de-
programs which best promote continuous STEM-centered learning and velop greater levels of motivation, they are generally more engaged
career development (Kong et al., 2014; Wang & Degol, 2013). This shift with thier STEM course work.
towards a more complete story of the “how and what” of OST STEM Pre-collegiate STEM summer camps may offer one potential context
programs and camps has increased understanding of mechanisms which to develop these socioemotional and motivational skills in parallel with
may magnify desirable program effects. For example, when a re- STEM career aspirations and career decidedness (Bevan, Petric, &
sponsive and supportive instructor is present within a STEM course, Wilkinson, 2014; Kang et al., 2019). Indeed, research examining career
students tend to become more motivated and perform at higher effort path development programs among adolescents suggests levels of ca-
levels in proceeding STEM contexts and courses (Kramer et al., 2018; reer decidedness are often intertwined with positive youth development
Leon, Nunez, & Liew, 2015). Similarly, in the context of summer STEM and wellbeing (Hirschi, 2009; Lubinski and Persson-Benbow, 2006). At
experiences, camp counselors can play a unique role in the improve- a broader level, research examining camp as a context for positive
ment of aspirations towards a STEM career (i.e., career decidedness). youth development suggests engaged participation can improve youth
This focus towards motivations was demonstrated by Fields (2009) who communication skills, attitude, self-regulation, exploration and curi-
indicated caring and supportive counselors enhanced STEM learning, osity, cooperation skills, and personal responsibility (Garst & Gagnon,

2
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

2016; Nugent, Barker, Grandgennett, & Adamchuk, 2010; Sibthorp, 3. Method


Bialeschki, Morgan, & Browne, 2013), and the development of these
skills within quality STEM programming may lead to greater aspiration 3.1. Participants
towards a STEM career and thus career decidedness (London, Pastor,
Servon, Rosner, & Wallace, 2010; Pace-Marshall, McGee, McLaren, & The 365 study participants were an average of 15.58 years old
Veal, 2011), regardless of participant gender (Myers, Jahn, Gailliard, & (SD = 1.12 years) and primarily identified as male (n = 189, 51.8%);
Stoltzfus, 2011). with 47.1% (n = 172) identifying as female, and one respondent
identifying as non-binary. Participants primarily identified as white
(n = 254, 69.8%), with the remainder identifying as either African-
2. The present study American (n = 68, 18.7%), Asian origin (n = 19; 5.2%), Hispanic or
Latino origin (n = 12, 3.3%), or multiple race (n = 11, 3%). Post-hoc
Given the ongoing and systematic allocation of resources towards power analyses to determine the minimum necessary sample for testing
enhancing representation of females in STEM careers, the examination the study measurement model and hypotheses indicate the sample of
of potential factors which may enhance or explain a female deciding on 365 respondents was sufficient for testing of the measurement and
a STEM career remains critical (Krishnamurthi, Bevan, Rinehart, & structural models [(average between factor r = 0.01, α = 0.05,
Coulon, 2013; National Science Foundation, 2017). As such, this study λ = 39.22) (R2 ≥ 0.08, p ≤ 0.05, λ = 30.78, α = 0.05, 10 predictor
examines contextual factors which may explain the relations between variables)] (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003).
gender and career decidedness resulting from a pre-collegiate STEM
camp experience. Specifically, the mediational influences of camp
counselor behaviors, STEM course instructor behaviors, camp structure 3.2. Procedures
engagement, and five dimensions of youth reported socioemotional skill
development (YRSESD) were examined, to determine how they may As part of a larger study exploring the potential benefits of re-
account for the relationship between gender, career decidedness and sidential summer camps and out-of-school-time experiences for ado-
career undecidedness (see also Fig. 1). It is hypothesized that suppor- lescents, the study data were collected from adolescents attending a
tive course instructor behaviors (H1), supportive camp counselor be- pre-collegiate STEM camp program. Specifically, a purposive sampling
haviors (H2), camp structure engagement (H3), and YRSESD (H4) will technique was utilized, where the population of interest was asked to
mediate the relationship between camper gender and STEM career participate in the study (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). At the end
decidedness. Further, it is hypothesized that supportive course in- of their one-week experience, campers at the study site were asked to
structor behaviors (H5), supportive camp counselor behaviors (H6), respond to a paper questionnaire provided by a member of the camp
camp structure engagement (H7), and YRSESD (H8) will mediate the administration team, who was trained in the constructs underpinning
relationship between camper gender and STEM career undecidedness. the items within the questionnaire and how to address common ques-
tions which may arise. Of a possible 424 respondents, 390 opted to
participate in the study, indicating a 91.98% response rate. As noted in
greater detail in the data processing and results sections below, 25

Fig. 1. Conceptual Model of Potential Mediational Processes. Paths reduced for parsimony of presentation.

3
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

respondents were removed from the data set after data processing, higher scores indicate better understanding of what career path the
leading to a final overall sample of 365 (post data processing response respondent would like to pursue (i.e., career decidedness) or greater
rate = 86.08%). confusion about a potential career path (i.e., career undecidedness).
The camp is marketed as a pre-collegiate STEM experience, where The scale exhibited acceptable levels of internal consistency in past
campers (e.g., aged 13–17) participate in an experiential STEM course, research (α = 0.86) and the present study (α = 0.771; 0.637).
with active college professors. Campers engage with professors in au-
thentic experiments and coding sessions tailored to their interests, fa- 3.3.3. Engagement and support
cilitating closer relationships between camper and professor; im- To measure engagement, support, and involvement with the camp,
portantly, courses are designed with low instructor to camper ratios three factors were assessed, based in part upon the Tiffany-Eckenrode
(i.e., 1:12–15), to reflect this aim. Each week long experience has Program Participation Scale (TEPPS) (Tiffany et al., 2012). Specifically,
specific tracks for a camper, including: bioengineering (i.e., cell culture, the personal development subscale from the TEPPS was adapted for the
bioinstrumentation), veterinary medicine (livestock anatomy, parasite present study to assess two potential mechanisms of personal devel-
identification), civil engineering (i.e., structural design, building failure opment: supportive camp counselor behaviors (e.g., My counselor took a
testing), autonomous vehicle engineering (i.e., robotics development personal interest in me, 5-items) supportive STEM course instructor be-
and coding), audio engineering (developing and testing music record- haviors (e.g., My instructor took personal interest in me; 5-items). In past
ings), and built environment design (i.e., energy efficient building de- research (α = 0.82) and the present study (α = 0.849; 0.836) the
velopment, computer animated design). Across the one-week experi- subscales have exhibited acceptable levels of internal consistency. In
ence, campers stay on-site in campus residence halls, with current addition to these measures, three generic items historically utilized at
university students acting as counselors to guide and mentor campers as the program site to assess participant engagement with camp activities
they potentially progress towards college and a STEM career. and structure were employed, (e.g., The camp activities were fun; 3-
To assess how/if campers were oriented towards a STEM career, items), which also demonstrated an acceptable level of internal con-
campers were asked to identify their desired college major, following sistency within the present study (α = 0.814). All items were measured
the format of Wegemer and Eccles (2019). Of total respondents, 35 on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) Likert style scale, where
indicated “don’t know” or left the question blank. Of valid responses, higher scores indicate greater rates of satisfaction and comfort.
305 indicated a STEM-centered major (e.g., engineering, biochemistry,
zoology, astrophysics, computer science) and 25 indicated either hu- 3.4. Data processing and analyses
manities (e.g., history, creative writing, journalism, political science) or
a business centered major (e.g., entrepreneurship, sports management). Prior to testing of the measurement model and examination of the
study hypotheses, the data were screened for outliers and normality
3.3. Measures within EQS 6.3 software. First, the data were examined for multivariate
kurtosis (i.e., multivariate kurtosis estimate 500 greater than other
3.3.1. Youth reported socioemotional skill development cases in data set; Byrne, 2006). The results of this examination in-
To assess potential changes in socioemotional skills resulting from dicated 15 cases were contributing to nonnormality within the data set
the camp experience, a modified version of the Parental Perceptions of and were correspondingly removed from additional analyses, leading to
Developmental Outcomes (PPDO) scale was utilized (Gagnon & Garst, a sample of 375. Second, the data were explored for normality; the
2019; Garst & Gagnon, 2016). Specifically, the items comprising the results of which indicated the data were non-normal in their dis-
PPDO were adapted from a parental perspective (e.g., As a result of tributive properties (e.g., normalized estimate = 54.606). As such,
camp…my child participates in new learning experiences) to a child per- robust estimation techniques were utilized to account for skewness in
spective (e.g., As a result of my camp experience…I participate in new the data set in proceeding analyses and in identification of fit indices
learning experiences). Within the present study the YRSESD (Youth Re- [e.g., Satorra-Bentler chi-square (S/Bχ2); Bentler, 2006]. Next the data
ported Socioemotional Skill Development) is comprised of five factors were examined for missing data issues. Due to evidence of non-nor-
and 21-items for the present investigation: (1) communication skills mality, a robust version of Little (1988) test of MCAR (Missing Com-
(e.g., I participate more in discussions; 4-items), (2) responsibility (e.g., I pletely at Random) was utilized to examine the data for systematic
take responsibility for my own actions; 5-items), (3) self-regulation (e.g., I causes of missingness with a p > .05 criterion (Bentler, 2006). The
manage disappointment well; 4-items), (4) attitude (e.g., I am more results of this analysis, as part of testing of the measurement model, the
hopeful about the future; 4-items), and (5) exploration (e.g., I seek chal- MCAR criterion was not achieved [mean & variance adjusted
lenges beyond my comfort zone; 4-items). Respondents were asked to χ2(1 3 0) = 221.303, p < .001], suggesting the data were not MCAR.
reflect on how their experience at the camp changed them rating the However, given the low rate of missing data (95.73% of cases had no
items on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) Likert format, where missing data; no variable/item had more than 0.53% missing values), a
higher scores indicate improvement in the corresponding factor. The Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) technique was utilized to
YRSESD scale has demonstrated acceptable levels of internal con- simulate missing values in measurement model testing and an Ex-
sistency across the five factors in past research (α = 0.855–0.938) and pectation Maximization (EM) technique was utilized to impute missing
in the present study (α = 0.884–0.918). values in the structural models (Bentler, 2006; Byrne, 2006).
After data processing, the analyses in the present study consisted of
3.3.2. Career (Un)Decidedness two phases, first a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to
To assess aspirations towards a STEM career, a modified version of examine the measurement properties of the scale, and two structural
the goal decidedness/commitment scale (Hirschi, 2009) was adapted to equation models (SEM) were utilized to test the study hypotheses. To
reflect two potential dimensions. First, career decidedness assessed the assess if acceptable model fit was present within the measurement and
degree to which adolescent beliefs about a potential career path match structural models, the Comparative Fit Indices (CFI) and Non-Normed
with their academic interests (e.g., I see how my interests can become a Fit Indices (N-NFI) were utilized, where levels closer to 1.0 indicate
career). Second, career undecidedness, assessed how adolescents’ con- “better” model fit (e.g., CFI = 0.900). Additionally, the Root Mean
fidence about a potential career may have changed or declined (e.g., I Squared Error Approximation (RMSEA) and its 90% confidence inter-
am confused about a future career). When responding to these items, vals were utilized to assess model parsimony (e.g., unnecessary paths),
campers were asked to reflect on how their experience at camp changed where levels closer to zero indicate better RMSEA scores [e.g.,
their desired career path (i.e., college major). The items were measured RMSEA = 0.060 (90%, CI 0.049 to 0.069)] (Byrne, 2006). As part of
on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) Likert style scale, where the CFA, a combination of goodness-of-fit indices were utilized.

4
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

Specifically, Cronbach’s Alphas (α) were examined to determine the demonstrated in past studies utilizing these subscales, with differential
internal consistency of each factor, with levels above 0.600 typically findings and effect sizes across factors within the same overall struc-
indicating acceptability. Further, the factor loadings (λ), for each item tural models, supporting their unshared/unique variance and suitability
were assessed, with loadings greater than 0.500 typically suggesting for SEM (e.g., Gagnon & Garst, 2019).
acceptability (Brown, 2015). Finally, to assess that each factor was Given the evidence of acceptable measurement properties, two
accounting for more unique variance than error, Average Variance models were produced to examine the study hypotheses: (1) a partially
Extracted (AVE) scores were utilized, where values greater than 0.500 mediated model, with the direct path from the IV (gender) to the DVs
generally suggest acceptability (Kline, 2016). The discriminant validity (career decidedness and undecidedness) included, and (2) a fully
of the scale was assessed utilizing between factor correlations levels, mediated model, with the direct path from gender (IV) to the career
where levels greater than 0.800 suggest the factors may be sharing decidedness (DV) and undecidedness removed (DV). As part of the
more variance than they are uniquely accounting for (Byrne, 2006). To examination of the SEM model fit, the LaGrange Multiplier (LM) fit
further substantiate discriminant validity, √AVE was also utilized, indices were examined (Byrne, 2006). The LM test statistics suggested
where the √AVE should be greater than the between factor correlation an item within the self-regulation factor was sharing error variance
levels of each corresponding factor (i.e., √AVE > r). Importantly, with an item in the attitude factor (denoted by *** in Table 1), in ad-
measures of acceptable fit and validity were also conditioned upon past dition to two items within the supportive camp counselor behaviors
studies utilizing the factors and items within the current study, and fit factor (denoted by * in Table 1), and two items within the responsibility
indices were not “rule-bound” by arbitrary cutoff levels (e.g., “Golden factor (denoted by ** by in Table 1). As such the covariance matrices
Rules,” see also Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004). and LM respecification indices were explored, which did not provide
To test the study hypotheses, two mediational models were ex- sufficient evidence to re specify or remove the items without engaging
amined. First, a partially mediated model with the direct paths from the in data-driven (rather than theory-driven) modelling. Thus, given the
study independent variable (IV), camper gender, to the dependent relative strength of factor loadings and acceptable levels of model fit,
variables (DV), career decidedness and undecidedness. Second, a fully the items were retained in their prespecified factors with their errors
mediated model with no direct paths between the IV (camper gender) covaried.
and DVs (career decidedness and career undecidedness). In both Both the partially mediated [S/Bχ2(7 4 1) = 1368.092, p ≤ 0.001,
models, camper gender was dummy coded (Male = 0; Female = 1) for N-NFI = 0.905, CFI = 0.914, RMSEA = 0.048 (90%, CI 0.044 to
analysis of direct and mediational effects, where when a score (re- 0.052)] and fully mediated [S/Bχ2(7 4 3) = 1369.646, p ≤ 0.001, N-
gression coefficient, β) is higher, this higher β indicates a positive effect NFI = 0.905, CFI = 0.914, RMSEA = 0.048 (90%, CI 0.044 to 0.052)]
of female gender on the outcome of interest. To determine if media- models exhibited no substantive differences between their respective fit
tional effects were possible, the S/Bχ2 levels for the partially and fully indices. As noted earlier, the factors comprising the five socioemotional
mediated models were tested utilizing the S/Bχ2 difference test, where factors exhibited high between factor correlations (e.g., r < 0.700) in
a nonsignificant result (p > .05) in combination with a nonsignificant the CFA. To mitigate the high between factor correlations the dis-
path from the IV to DVs indicates the potential for mediation (Byrne, turbance terms across the five factors were covaried for both the partial
2006). Further, to test the significance and magnitude of potential and full mediation models. Within the partially mediated model, gender
mediational effects (i.e., indirect effects), the Sobel test was utilized had no significant direct effect on career decidedness (β = −0.055,
(Hayes, 2013). SE = 0.062, p = .284) or career undecidedness (β = 0.040,
SE = 0.052, p = .532). Further, the results of the S/Bχ2 difference test
4. Results indicated no statistically significant difference between the partially
and fully mediated models [ΔS/Bχ2(2) = 1.385, p = .501]. Taken to-
As part of the measurement analysis, the data were again screened gether, the results of these analyses indicated the possibility of med-
for highly non-normal cases, which indicated an additional ten outliers iation, and thus appropriateness of testing for mediational effects. As
within the data set and were removed from additional analyses, leading such the hypotheses (H1-H8) were tested utilizing the Sobel test in the
to a final sample of 365. Correspondingly, the CFA results indicated fully mediated model to determine if significant mediation was present.
acceptable model fit: [S/Bχ2(6 9 5) = 1185.227, p ≤ 0.001, N- As illustrated in Table 3, the Sobel test results indicate no significant
NFI = 0.926, CFI = 0.934, RMSEA = 0.043 (90%, CI 0.039 to 0.047)]. mediational (i.e., indirect) effects were present within the model, de-
The scale exhibited acceptable convergent validity as illustrated in monstrating a lack of support for the study hypotheses (H1 – H8).
Table 1 with all factor loadings greater than 0.500, all internal con- However, while tertiary to the study purpose, there were significant
sistency levels greater than 0.700, and all AVE levels greater than direct effects of gender on exploration score (β = 0.106, SE = 0.078,
0.500, with the exception of the career undecidedness factor. While this p = .049), suggesting females in the sample were more curious about
factor exhibited acceptable internal consistency, one-item (I want to new experiences than their male peers as a result of the STEM camp.
study different subjects) indicated a poor loading (λ = 0.452). Inspection Further, the results indicated supportive camp counselor behaviors
of the covariance matrices and LaGrange Multiplier (LM) test results to (β = 0.182, SE = 0.050, p = 0.002), supportive course instructor be-
realign or remove this factor did not indicate meaningful improvement haviors (β = 0.338, SE = 0.055, p ≤ 0.001), and camp structure en-
to model fit (Byrne, 2006). As such both the item and corresponding gagement (β = 0.161, SE = 0.052, p = .006), all positively influenced
factor was retained. Additionally, the evidence presented in Table 2 career decidedness score. Additionally, attitude both positively influ-
demonstrates the discriminant validity of the scale, where the majority enced career decidedness score (β = 0.433, SE = 0.122, p = .009), and
of √AVE levels were greater than corresponding between factor corre- negatively influenced career undecidedness score (β = −0.464,
lations, with the exception of the exploration and attitude factors. SE = 0.217, p = .033).
Specifically, their √AVE levels (0.814; 0.835) were slightly lower than
the exploration/attitude correlation levels (r = 0.846) and self-reg- 5. Discussion
ulation/attitude levels (r = 0.829).
However, these differences were not meaningful enough to suggest This study examined contextual factors which may explain the re-
the need for model modification (e.g., √AVE minus r < 0.100), so the lation between gender and career decidedness resulting from a pre-
model was retained (Byrne, 2006; Farrell, 2010). Furthermore, six of collegiate STEM camp experience. Given the heavy dedication of re-
the ten correlations comprising the socioemotional measures (i.e., F6 to search dollars and resources towards developing pathways for groups
F10 in Table 2) exhibited higher than desirable correlation levels (e.g., underrepresented in STEM, understanding how pathways such as
r ≥ 0.800). However, similar high shared variance has been summer camp experiences enhance aspiration and representation of

5
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

Table 1
Descriptive and Confirmatory Statistics.
Factor/Item M◊ (SD) λ α AVE

Career Decidedness 0.771 0.538


I better understand what I want to do for a job. 3.962 (0.858) 0.775
I better understand of what I can do in the future. 4.144 (0.821) 0.810
I see how my interests can become a career. 4.204 (0.770) 0.599

Career Undecidedness 0.637 0.402


I want to change my career. 2.203 (0.985) 0.509
I am confused about a future career. 2.314 (1.117) 0.862
I want to study different subjects. 3.067 (1.122) 0.452

Supportive Camp Counselor Behaviors 0.849 0.538


My counselor shared their knowledge of college. * 4.265 (0.811) 0.694
My counselor provided guidance about college. * 4.075 (0.896) 0.678
My counselor was enthusiastic. 4.572 (0.624) 0.609
My counselor took personal interest in me. 4.174 (0.873) 0.830
My counselor valued my opinion. 4.242 (0.801) 0.831

Camp Structure Engagement 0.814 0.597


The daily camp structure was appropriate. 4.047 (0.924) 0.696
The program provided a variety of activities. 4.248 (0.826) 0.825
The camp activities were fun. 4.171 (0.834) 0.792

Supportive Instructor Behaviors 0.836 0.511


The instructor was prepared. 4.429 (0.777) 0.708
My instructor provided guidance about college. 4.160 (0.883) 0.826
My instructor took personal interest in me. 3.885 (1.067) 0.716
The instructor allowed me to gain new knowledge. 4.426 (0.861) 0.717
The courses were challenging. 3.626 (1.106) 0.589

Communication 0.903 0.702


I participate more in discussions. 3.568 (0.868) 0.810
I communicate better with others. 3.661 (0.874) 0.876
I better share thoughts and ideas verbally. 3.599 (0.908) 0.888
I ask questions. 3.616 (0.920) 0.774

Responsibility 0.918 0.693


I take responsibility for my own actions. ** 3.892 (0.901) 0.845
I take care of my own things. ** 4.027 (0.902) 0.789
I share in work responsibilities. 3.877 (0.884) 0.813
I follow through when asked to do something. 4.057 (0.878) 0.854
I better follow directions. 3.845 (0.926) 0.861

Self-Regulation 0.884 0.662


I better handle success and failure. 3.755 (0.947) 0.849
I manage disappointment well. 3.624 (0.956) 0.863
I deal effectively with conflict. 3.713 (0.946) 0.861
I don’t get frustrated easily. *** 3.473 (1.032) 0.667

Attitude 0.915 0.731


I have a good mental attitude. *** 3.869 (0.920) 0.871
I have a generally “positive” view on life. 3.948 (0.937) 0.894
I am more hopeful about the future. 4.080 (0.930) 0.816
I show a positive attitude when around others. 3.974 (0.912) 0.837

Exploration 0.901 0.698


I participate in new learning experiences. 4.109 (0.848) 0.854
I am curious about new topics and subjects. 4.226 (0.810) 0.762
I seek challenges beyond my comfort zone. 4.010 (0.898) 0.850
I am willing to try new experiences. 4.237 (0.820) 0.872

Note: ◊ Means (M) are based upon Expectation Maximization (EM) Values; λ: standardized coefficient (factor loading); AVE: Average Variance Extracted; α:
Cronbach’s alpha; *, **, ***: indicates covariance between corresponding items.

such groups remains an important area of inquiry (Krishnamurthi et al., undecidedness (H5). Similarly, there was no significant mediational
2013). Within the present study, the mediational analyses indicated a effect of supportive camp counselor behaviors on the relationship be-
lack of support for the eight study hypotheses. Importantly, as de- tween career decidedness (H2), career undecidedness (H6), and camper
monstrated in the partially mediated model, there was no significant gender.
direct effect of camper gender on career decidedness or career un- While some in-school (Leon et al., 2015; Solanki & Xu, 2018) and
decidedness score. The lack of direct effects in this model does indicate out-of-school (Fields, 2009) research suggests instructor and camp
mediational processes could be in place if there is support for differ- counselor behaviors and traits should enhance the effect of STEM
entiation in STEM career decidedness (or undecidedness) due to camper programs on career decidedness, this was not supported in the current
gender. However, in the current study, there was no evidence of in- study. The lack of differentiation seems to reflect literature suggesting
direct (i.e., mediational) effects utilizing the eight potential mediator negligible to nonexistent differences in STEM career decidedness, per-
factors. Specifically, the results indicated a lack of significant media- sistence, or aspiration due to participant gender regardless of instructor
tional effect of supportive course instructor behaviors on the relation- behaviors (Chow & Salmela-Aro, 2011; Falk et al., 2016; Jungert et al.,
ship between camper gender, career decidedness (H1), and career 2019; Price, 2010; Solanki & Xu, 2018). There was also a lack of support

6
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

Table 2
Between Factor Correlations.
Factor F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 F7 F8 F9 F10

F1. Career Decidedness 0.733


F2. Career Undecidedness −0.509 0.634
F3. S.C.C.B. 0.449 −0.189 0.733
F4. C.S.E. 0.463 −0.188 0.440 0.772
F5. S.C.I.B. 0.581 −0.231 0.363 0.353 0.715
F6. Communication 0.523 −0.237 0.497 0.475 0.437 0.838
F7. Responsibility 0.566 −0.288 0.392 0.461 0.448 0.822 0.832
F8. Self-Regulation 0.554 −0.232 0.417 0.456 0.426 0.776 0.831 0.814
F9. Attitude 0.620 −0.308 0.376 0.462 0.438 0.786 0.852 0.829 0.855
F10. Exploration 0.606 −0.235 0.386 0.439 0.486 0.720 0.804 0.740 0.846 0.835

Note: All factor correlations p ≤ 0.05; Bold indicates √AVE; S.C.C.B. = Supportive Camp Counselor Behaviors; C.S.E. = Camp Structure Engagement;
S.C.I.B. = Supportive Course Instructor Behaviors.

Table 3 for the mediating effect of camp structure engagement on the re-
Strength and Significance of Direct and Indirect Effects and Sobel Tests. lationship between camper gender, career decidedness (H3), and career
β SE p-value
undecidedness (H7). While some research indicates the relationship
between gender and career decidedness may be explained in part by
Gender to Mediating Variable participant engagement (Vedder-Weiss & Fortus, 2011; Wilson et al.,
Gender → S.C.C.B. 0.075 0.077 0.178 2015), this effect was not demonstrated within the present study. In-
Gender → C.S.E. 0.006 0.077 0.910
Gender → S.C.I.B. 0.102 0.081 0.066
deed, similar results have been illustrated in studies of college students,
Gender → Communication 0.031 0.087 0.573 where the relationship between gender and STEM aspiration was not
Gender → Responsibility 0.032 0.087 0.554 influenced by engagement with the course activities or with the in-
Gender → Self-Regulation 0.037 0.089 0.490 structors themselves (Gasiewski et al., 2012).
Gender → Attitude 0.003 0.088 0.960
The lack of support for hypotheses H4 and H8 suggested these so-
Gender → Exploration 0.106 0.078 0.049
cioemotional skills (i.e., communication, responsibility, self-regulation,
Mediating Variable to Career Decidedness
attitude, and exploration) did not account for the relationship between
S.C.C.B. → C.D. 0.182 0.050 0.001*
C.S.E. → C.D. 0.161 0.052 0.006
gender, career decidedness (H4), and career undecidedness (H8). The
S.C.I.B. → C.D. 0.338 0.055 0.001* development of these skills was a desired outcome of the organization
Communication → C.D. −0.193 0.083 0.079 facilitating the camp in addition to the fostering of STEM career de-
Responsibility → C.D. 0.019 0.120 0.904 cidedness. Examination of the distributions of the item means within
Self-Regulation → C.D. 0.066 0.089 0.585
the five factors suggests greater variation across these items than those
Attitude → C.D. 0.433 0.122 0.009
Exploration → C.D. 0.138 0.108 0.287 comprising the career decidedness factor. Given the high between
factor correlations across these factors, the lack of an effect across all
Sobel Test (Mediating Effect on Career Decidedness)
Gender → S.C.C.B. → C.D. 0.014● 0.015 0.346 five factors was somewhat unsurprising, despite evidence of the sub-
Gender → C.S.E. → C.D. 0.001● 0.012 0.937 scales producing differential effect sizes in previous studies (Gagnon &
Gender → S.C.I.B. → C.D. 0.034● 0.028 0.217 Garst, 2019). However, some research does suggest this lack of effect
Gender → Communication → C.D. −0.006● 0.017 0.724 may be increasingly normative within adolescent samples. For instance,
Gender → Responsibility → C.D. 0.001● 0.004 0.884
the work of Beier et al. (2018) and Falk et al. (2016) both suggested
Gender → Self-Regulation → C.D. 0.002● 0.007 0.716
Gender → Attitude → C.D. 0.001● 0.038 0.972 STEM interest scores did not meaningfully differentiate due to gender
Gender → Exploration → C.D. 0.015● 0.016 0.351 or self-concept in their research.
Mediating Variable to Career Undecidedness There are likely several complementary explanations for the lack of
S.C.C.B. → C.U. −0.057 0.080 0.401 mediational or direct effects evidenced in the present study. First, and
C.S.E. → C.U. −0.039 0.088 0.589 perhaps the most hopeful explanation, the lack of effect could be a
S.C.I.B. → C.U. −0.085 0.077 0.212 result of the systematic and ongoing emphasis towards the development
Communication → C.U. 0.161 0.154 0.283
Responsibility → C.U. −0.250 0.200 0.204
of STEM skills and aspiration within female adolescents (Krishnamurthi
Self-Regulation → C.U. 0.134 0.162 0.409 et al., 2013). Put differently, the programs, resources, and overarching
Attitude → C.U. −0.464 0.217 0.033 cultural shift towards alleviating longstanding suppression of females
Exploration → C.U. 0.258 0.163 0.071 from the STEM professions could be working. Indeed, evidence pre-
Sobel Test (Mediating Effect on Career Undecidedness) sented through the National Science Foundation and others suggests,
Gender → S.C.C.B. → C.U. −0.004● 0.007 0.565 females continue to be represented at greater rates in terms of aspira-
Gender → C.S.E. → C.U. 0.001● 0.003 0.938
tion towards STEM, STEM college major selection, and STEM careers
Gender → S.C.I.B. → C.U. −0.009● 0.010 0.406
Gender → Communication → C.U. 0.005● 0.015 0.736
(National Science Foundation, 2017; Wai, Lubinski, Benbow, & Steiger,
Gender → Responsibility → C.U. −0.008● 0.023 0.724 2010). Second, it is likely the sample examined in the current study
Gender → Self-Regulation → C.U. 0.005● 0.013 0.710 itself was biased. As noted earlier, the participants opted into a STEM
Gender → Attitude → C.U. −0.001● 0.041 0.972 summer camp experience, this may imply the participants were already
Gender → Exploration → C.U. 0.027● 0.027 0.302
“decided” on a STEM career; indeed, the means within career decid-
Note: S.C.C.B. = Supportive Camp Counselor Behaviors; C.S.E. = Camp edness factor suggest high scores in this area (i.e., ceiling effects). As
Structure Engagement; S.C.I.B. = Supportive Course Instructor Behaviors; suggested in research exploring afterschool and summer-based STEM
C.D. = Career Decidedness; C.U. = Career Undecidedness; β = regression experiences, children and adolescents opting into STEM experiences
coefficient; SE = Standard Error; ● = Mediating effect; * = p ≤ 0.001. beyond their regular coursework may already be motivated to engage
in these experiences, and thus able to overcome constraints that would
stymie their less motivated or less equipped peers (Pace-Marshall et al.,
2011). Third, it is also probable there are unmeasured mediators

7
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

explaining the relationship between gender and career decidedness. submission has not been published previously and is not under review
Indeed, some research suggests persistence may account for the re- for publication elsewhere. This submission has been approved by all
lationship between gender and decidedness, where females with greater authors and that, if accepted, it will not be published elsewhere in the
rates of intrinsic motivation tended to persist towards a STEM college same form, in English or in any other language, including electronically
major (Griffith, 2010). without the written consent of the copyright-holder.

5.1. Limitations Appendix A. Supplementary material

Beyond the limitations already described, a few warrant deeper Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://
explanation. The high between factor correlations evidenced in Table 2 doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104584.
among the five socioemotional factors (YRSESD) illustrates potential
multicollinearity, and thus calls into question the discriminant validity References
of these factors (Kline, 2016). Further, the high-correlations point to
another potential issue across the measures, where a 2nd order factor Beier, M. E., Kim, M. H., Saterbak, A., Leautaud, V., Bishnoi, S., & Giberto, J. M. (2018).
may be acting as a common-cause of the socioemotional factors (Byrne, The effect of authentic project-based learning on attitudes and career aspirations in
STEM. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 56(1), 3–23. https://doi.org/10.1002/
2006). Similar issues with poor variation across socioemotional mea- tea.21465.
sures have been demonstrated within camp research (e.g., Garst & Bentler, P. M. (2006). EQS 6 Structural Equations Program Manual. Encino, CA:
Gagnon, 2016; Sibthorp et al., 2013), and the theoretically appropriate Multivariate Software Inc.
Bevan, B., Petich, M., & Wilkinson, K. (2014). Tinkering is serious play. Educational
use of 2nd order factor models has been found to introduce parsimony Leadership: Journal of the Department of Supervision and Curriculum Development, 72(4),
to models (i.e., reduction of SEM parameters and paths), especially 28–33.
when high correlations are present (Gagnon, Garst, & Townsend, 2019). Byrne, B. M. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications and
programming (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
In addition to the collinearity issues within the model, some items in-
Chow, A., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2011). Task-values across subject domains: A gender
dicated ceiling effects which could harm the inferences that could be comparison using a person-centered approach. International Journal of Behavioral
made about the relations and effects within the models (Bandalos, Development, 35(3), 202–209. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025411398184.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/cor-
2018). In future research, the 10-factors comprising the measure in the
relation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
current study should be explored with greater ranges (e.g., 1 to 7 versus College Board (2018). Number of females and underrepresented students taking AP computer
1 to 5), to potentially alleviate this issue. science courses spikes again. New York: The College Board.
The present study utilized a cross-sectional design, which seriously Dasgupta, N., & Stout, J. G. (2014). Girls and women in science, technology, engineering,
and mathematics: STEMing the tide and broadening participation in STEM careers.
limits the conclusions which can be drawn from the findings. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.
Longitudinal data could potentially provide support for the study 1177/2372732214549471.
findings, but also could illustrate if shifts (or lack thereof) in career Dieker, L., Grillo, K., & Ramlakhan, N. (2012). The use of virtual and simulated teaching
and learning environments: Inviting gifted students into science, technology, en-
decidedness scores are moderated by STEM camper gender. More spe- gineering, and mathematics careers (STEM) through summer partnerships. Gifted
cifically, it is possible male campers had different slopes (i.e., shifts) Education International, 28(1), 96–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/
than their female counterparts, and an exploration of this shift con- 0261429411427647.
Eccles, J. (2011). Gendered educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles
textualized by other potential factors (e.g., prior STEM education ex- et al. model of achievement-related choices. International Journal of Behavioral
periences, socioemotional skill shifts, counselor/instructor behaviors) Development, 35(3), 195–201. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025411398185.
could demonstrate which unique combination(s) of camper traits and/ Eccles, J. S., & Wang, M. (2016). What motivates females and males to pursue careers in
mathematics and science? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(2),
or contextually dependent factors best promote STEM career decided- 100–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415616201.
ness in both female and male campers. Importantly, there is also a likely Falk, J. H., Staus, N., Dierking, L. D., Penuel, W., Wyld, J., & Bailey, D. (2016).
selection bias present within the study sample; specifically, all campers Understanding youth STEM interest pathways within a single community: The
Synergies project. International Journal of Science Education, Part B, 6(4), 369–384.
opted in to a STEM centered experience. Thus, the effects (and/or lack
https://doi.org/10.1080/21548455.2015.1093670.
thereof) demonstrated in the present study may fail to replicate within a Farrell, A. M. (2010). Insufficient discriminant validity: A comment on Bove, Pervan,
comparison sample not opting in to a STEM experience. Ideally, a study Beatty, and Shiu (2009). Journal of Business Research, 63, 324–327. https://doi.org/
with a randomized design assigning campers to either a STEM or non- 10.1016/j.jbusres.2009.05.003.
Fields, D. A. (2009). What do students gain from a week at science camp? Youth per-
STEM experience may demonstrate if STEM career decidedness is due to ceptions and the design of an immersive, research-oriented astronomy camp.
the context or factors exclusive of the camp. International Journal of Science Education, 31(2), 151–171. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09500690701648291.
Gagnon, R. J., & Garst, B. A. (2019). Research and practice: An applied examination of the
6. Conclusion parental perceptions of developmental outcomes scale. Journal of Outdoor Recreation,
Education, and Leadership, 11(1), 3–20. https://doi.org/10.18666/JOREL-2019-V11-
It appears that for this study, with this sample, at this site, gender I1-9070.
Gagnon, R. J., Garst, B. A., & Townsend, J. A. (2019). Tough decisions in medical spe-
did not play a meaningful role in STEM career decidedness or un- cialty camps: Relationships between camp dosage, outcomes, and camper attendance.
decidedness. Importantly, with larger shifts in STEM education, the lack Social Sciences and Medicine, 221, 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.
of effects may be explained by the developing trend of orientation to- 12.014.
Garst, B. A., & Gagnon, R. J. (2016). A structural model of camp director practices and
wards enhancing equity within the STEM field (College Board, 2018). outcomes: Does intention toward program outcomes matter? Journal of Park and
Indeed, the increasing research and resource allocation to address these Recreation Administration, 34(4), 51–65. https://doi.org/10.18666/JPRA-2016-V34-
concerns, suggests an ongoing a systematic push of women, women of I4-7293.
Gasiewski, J. A., Eagan, M. K., Garcia, G. A., Hurtado, S., & Chang, M. J. (2012). From
color, and many minority youth cohorts towards the STEM field (Eccles
gatekeeping to engagement: A multicontextual, mixed method study of student
& Wang, 2016; Johnston-Nicholson, Collins, & Holmer, 2004; National academic engagement in introductory stem courses. Research in Higher Education, 53,
Science Foundation, 2017). However, the lack of representation of 229–261. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11162-011-9247-y.
people of color and those with disabilities within the STEM career field Griffith, A. L. (2010). Persistence of women and minorities in STEM field majors: Is it the
school that matters? Economics of Education Review, 29, 911–922. https://doi.org/10.
remains problematic, and potentially stifles the next generation of sci- 1016/j.econedurev.2010.06.010.
entists and problem solvers. Guay, F., Senecal, C., Gauthier, L., & Fernet, C. (2003). Predicting career indecision: A
self- determination theory perspective. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50(2),
165–177. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.50.2.165.
Declaration of Competing Interest Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis:
A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
None of the authors has a conflict of interest to report. This Hirschi, A. (2009). Career adaptability development in adolescence: Multiple predictors

8
R.J. Gagnon and A. Sandoval Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104584

and effect on sense of power and life satisfaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 74, Socialization (VAS): A communicative model of adolescents’ interests in STEM.
145–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2009.01.002. Management Communication Quarterly, 25(1), 87–120. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Holmes, S., Redmond, A., Thomas, J., & High, K. (2012). Girls helping girls: Assessing the 0893318910377068.
influence of college student mentors in an afterschool engineering program. National Science Foundation (2017). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in
Mentoring & Tutoring: Partnership in Learning, 20(1), 137–150. https://doi.org/10. science and engineering: 2017. Arlington, VA.: National Science Foundation.
1080/13611267.2012.645604. Nugent, G., Barker, B., Grandgenett, N., & Adamchuk, V. I. (2010). Impact of robotics and
Hughes, R. M., Nzekew, B., & Molyneaux, K. J. (2013). The single sex debate for girls in geospatial technology interventions on youth stem learning and attitudes. Journal of
science: A comparison between two informal science programs on middle school Research on Technology in Education, 42(4), 391–408. https://doi.org/10.1080/
students’ STEM identity formation. Research in Science Education, 43(5), 1979–2007. 15391523.2010.10782557.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-012-9345-7. Pace-Marshall, S., McGee, G. W. M., McLaren, E., & Veal, C. C. (2011). Discovering and
Johnston-Nicholson, H., Collins, C., & Holmer, H. (2004). Youth as people: The protective developing diverse STEM talent: Enabling academically talented urban youth to
aspects of youth development in after-school settings. The ANNALS of the American flourish. Gifted Child Today, 34(1), 16–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Academy of Political and Social Science, 591(1), 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 107621751103400107.
0002716203260081. Price, J. (2010). The effect of instructor race and gender on student persistence in STEM
Jungert, T., Hubbart, K., Dedic, H., & Rosenfield, S. (2019). Systemizing and the gender fields. Economics of Education Review, 901–910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
gap: Examining academic achievement and perseverance in STEM. European Journal econedurev.2010.07.009.
of Psychology of Education, 34(2), 479–500 10.1007%2Fs10212-018-0390-0. Roth, J. L., Malone, L. M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2010). Does the amount of participation in
Kang, J., Hense, J., Scheersoi, A., & Keinonen, T. (2019). Gender study on the relation- afterschool programs relate to developmental outcomes? A review of the literature.
ships between science interest and future career perspectives. International Journal of American Journal of Community Psychology, 45(3–4), 310–324. https://doi.org/10.
Science Education, 41(1), 80–101. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2018. 1007/s10464- 010-9303-3.
1534021. Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental
Klahr, D., Triona, L. M., & Williams, C. (2007). Hands on what? The relative effectiveness designs for causal inference. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.
of physical versus virtual materials in an engineering design project by middle school Sibthorp, J., Bialeschki, M. D., Morgan, C., & Browne, L. (2013). Validating, norming, and
children. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(1), 183–203. https://doi.org/10. utility of a youth outcomes battery for recreation programs and camps. Journal of
1002/tea.20152. Leisure Research, 45(4), 514–536. https://doi.org/10.18666/jlr-2013-v45-i4-3897.
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). New Simpkins, S. D. (2015). When and how does participating in an organized after-school
York, NY: Guilford. activity matter? Applied Developmental Science, 19(3), 121–126. https://doi.org/10.
Kong, X., Dabney, K. P., & Tai, R. H. (2014). The association between science summer 1080/10888691.2015.1056344.
camps and career interest in science and engineering. International Journal of Science Simpkins, S. D., Little, P. M., & Weiss, H. B. (2004). Understanding and measuring at-
Education, Part B, 4(1), 54–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/21548455.2012.760856. tendance in out-of-school programs. Issues and Opportunities in Out-of-School Time
Kramer, N. C., Kracora, B., Lucas, G., Dehghani, M., Ruther, G., & Gratch, J. (2016). Evaluation, 7, 1–12.
Closing the gender gap in STEM with friendly male instructors? On the effects of Sinclair, S., Nilsson, A., & Cederskar, E. (2019). Explaining gender-typed educational
rapport behavior and gender of a virtual agent in an instructional interaction. choice in adolescence: The role of social identity, self-concept, goals, grades, and
Computers and Education, 99, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2016.04. interests. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 101(A), 54–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
002. jvb.2018.11.007.
Krishnamurthi, A., Bevan, B., Rinehart, J., & Coulon, V. (2013). What after-school STEM Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2018). The gender-equality paradox in science, technology,
does best: How stakeholders describe youth learning outcomes. After-school Matters, engineering, and mathematics education. Psychological Science, 29(4), 581–593.
42–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617741719.
Leon, J., Nunez, J. L., & Liew, J. (2015). Self-determination and STEM education: Effects Solanki, S. M., & Xu, D. (2018). Looking beyond academic performance: The influence of
of autonomy, motivation, and self-regulated learning on high school math achieve- instructor gender on student motivation in STEM fields. American Educational
ment. Learning and Individual Differences, 43, 156–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Research Journal, 55(4), 801–835. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831218759034.
lindif.2015.08.017. Tiffany, J. S., Exner-Cortens, D., & Eckenrode, J. (2012). A new measure for assessing
Liben, L. S., & Coyle, E. F. (2014). Chapter three-developmental interventions to address youth program participation. Journal of Community Psychology, 40(3), 277–291.
the STEM gender gap: Exploring intended and unintended consequences. Advances in https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20508.
Child Development and Behavior, 47, 77–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2014. Tiffany, J. S., Exner-Cortens, D., & Eckenrode, J. (2013). Longitudinal associations be-
06.001. tween HIV risk reduction and out-of-school time program participation. Journal of
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with Adolescent Health, 52, 795–797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.01.011.
missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. Vedder-Weiss, D., & Fortus, D. (2011). Adolescents’ declining motivation to learn science:
London, R. A., Pastor, M., Servon, L. J., Rosner, R., & Wallace, A. (2010). The role of Inevitable or not? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(2), 199–216. https://doi.
community technology centers in promoting youth development. Youth and Society, org/10.1002/tea.20398.
42(2), 199–228. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X09351278. Wai, J., Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Steiger, J. H. (2010). Accomplishment in science,
Lubinski, D., & Persson-Benbow, C. (2006). Study of mathematically precocious youth technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) and its relation to STEM educa-
after 35 years uncovering antecedents for the development of math-science expertise. tion dose: A 25-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4),
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(4), 316–345. 860–871. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019454.
Lubinski, D. (2010). Spatial ability and STEM: A sleeping giant for talent identification Wang, M., & Degol, J. (2013). Motivational pathways to STEM career choices: Using
and development. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(4), 344–351. https://doi. expectancy–value perspective to understand individual and gender differences in
org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.022. STEM fields. Developmental Review, 33, 304–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2013.
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on 08.001.
hypothesis- testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in Wang, M. T., & Degol, J. L. (2017). Gender gap in science, technology, engineering, and
overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A mathematics (stem): Current knowledge, implications for practice, policy, and future
Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1207/ directions. Educational Psychology Review, 29(1), 119–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s15328007sem1103_2. s10648-015- 9355-x.
Mohr-Schroeder, M. J., Jackson, C., Miller, M., Walcott, B., Little, D. L., Speler, L., et al. Wegemer, C. M., & Eccles, J. S. (2019). Gendered STEM career choices: Altruistic values,
(2014). Developing middle school students' interests in STEM via summer learning beliefs, and identity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 110(A), 28–42. https://doi.org/
experiences: See blue STEM camp. School Science and Mathematics, 114(6), 291–301. 10.1016/j.jvb.2018.10.020.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ssm.12079. Wilson, D., Jones, D., Bocell, F., Crawford, J., Kim, M., Veilleux, N., et al. (2015).
Mostache, H. S., Matloff-Nieves, S., Kekelis, L., & Lawner, E. K. (2013). Effective STEM Belonging and academic engagement among undergraduate STEM students: A multi-
programs for adolescent girls: Three approaches and many lessons learned. institutional study. Research in Higher Education, 56, 750–776. https://doi.org/10.
Afterschool Matters, 17–25. 1007/s11162-015-9367-x.
Myers, K. K., Jahn, J. L. S., Gailliard, B. M., & Stoltzfus, K. (2010). Vocational Anticipatory

You might also like