You are on page 1of 11

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103


www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

A cognitive dissonance interpretation of consistencies and


inconsistencies in environmentally responsible behavior
John Th^gersen*
Department of Marketing, Aarhus School of Business, Haslegaardsvej 10, 8210 Aarhus V, Denmark
Received 4 September 2002; received in revised form 7 April 2003; accepted 17 June 2003

Abstract

The issue of consistency and inconsistency in environmentally responsible behavior (ERB), as reflected in the correlations between
different ERBs, is discussed in the light of social-psychological theories suggesting that most people desire to behave consistently. It
is argued that except in cases where different ERBs are substitutes or at least one of them is totally determined by idiosyncratic
conditions the desire to behave consistently should lead to ERBs being positively correlated. However, the correlation may be
attenuated by the influence of idiosyncratic conditions (considered ‘‘noise’’ in this context) and measurement error and it is
moderated by perceived dissimilarity between the behaviors and by the (moral) importance of behaving in a responsible way towards
the environment. These propositions are tested (and confirmed) by means of a mall-intercept survey of ordinary Danish shoppers.
Implications for the promotion of a generalized ERB pattern are discussed.
r 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction instead made on an activity-to-activity basis’’ (Pickett


et al., 1993).
In the (relatively short) history of research into The two latter references also illustrate the main
environmentally responsible behavior (ERB) one of reasons why researchers have taken interest in the
the persistent, yet still not completely settled, questions correlation between different ERB. Positive correlations
has been whether different ERBs are—or should be between different ERBs should be expected if they share
expected to be—correlated (e.g. Gray, 1985; Stern & motivational roots (‘‘a general conservation stance’’)
Oskamp, 1987; Pickett, Kangun, & Grove, 1993; Berger, and a lack of correlation is taken to indicate that each
1997; Diekmann & Preisendorfer, . 1998; Th^gersen, ERB is considered in isolation and that behavior is
1999b). The international literature contains studies completely determined by the specificities of each
that report positive correlations between people’s behavior and situation. Which of these possibilities is
propensities to behave in an environmentally friendly true has important implications for policy aimed at
way across domains (e.g. Berger, 1997) as well as studies promoting ERB, of course.
that fail to find such correlations (e.g. Stern & Oskamp, Positive correlations between different ERBs should
1987; De Kruijk & van Raaij, 1991). In particular, the also be expected if ‘‘heightened pro-ecological behavior’’
early studies found low correlations between ERBs. is transferred from ‘‘the target behavior to other
Hence, in the mid-1980s a thorough reviewer of the behaviors’’ or, as others have expressed it, if there is a
literature had to conclude that ‘‘there is very little tendency for ERB to ‘‘spill over’’ into other behavioral
evidence of transfer of heightened pro-ecological beha- domains (Frey, 1993; Th^gersen, 1999b). Spillover of
vior from the target behavior to other behaviors’’ (Gray, ERB would mean that the effects of interventions
1985, p. 188). Based on the same evidence others promoting specific ERBs are multiplied for the benefit
inferred that ‘‘environmentally beneficial choicesyare of a more sustainable society overall. Hence, the
not reflective of a general conservation stance, but are practical importance of this issue should not be under-
estimated either. The two reasons for expecting different
ERBs to be correlated are hardly independent. Most
*Corresponding author. Tel.: +45-8948-6440; fax: +45-8615-3988. likely, a tendency for ERB to spill over depends on ERB
E-mail address: john.thogersen@asb.dk (J. Th^gersen). in different areas having shared motivational roots.

0272-4944/$ - see front matter r 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00039-2
ARTICLE IN PRESS
94 J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103

Even those most strongly convinced that, in general, hold inconsistent attitudes and/or perceptions or to
ERBs should be positively correlated will hardly claim behave inconsistently (Festinger, 1957). Therefore, the
this to be universally true. An obvious exception is desire to avoid cognitive dissonance is assumed to create
ERBs that are substitutes,1 such as traveling by bicycle a drive to behave consistently (and to hold consistent
and by public transportation. Neither can it be ignored beliefs about oneself in general). Experimental studies
that ERB is determined by personal and contextual have demonstrated that cognitive dissonance can
constraints, in addition to the individual’s motivation to produce environment-friendly adjustments in behavior
perform the behavior (e.g. Guagnano, Stern, & Dietz, (e.g. Kantola, Syme, & Campbell, 1984; Dickerson,
.
1995; Olander & Th^gersen, 1995). Constraints are Thibodeau, & Aronson, 1992; Aitken, McMahon,
typically behavior and sometimes even situation specific. Wearing, & Finlayson, 1994). In these cases,
Hence, it is also obvious that the correlation between dissonance was produced by increasing the salience
any two ERBs will be attenuated to the degree that of the discrepancy between a person’s behavior
either of them is influenced by idiosyncratic constraints. and his or her norms for environmentally proper
In the context of the present discussion, attenuation due behavior, i.e. by inducing a sense of ‘‘hypocrisy’’
to the influence of idiosyncratic constraints is best (Dickerson et al., 1992). However, whether this
considered noise, which may obscure possible common drive has the power to spontaneously align different
sources of motivation or the propensity for ERB to spill ERBs is a matter not yet investigated empirically, as far
over into other behaviors. as I know.
Another common source of attenuation of correlations If two volitional behaviors are believed to lead to the
between ERBs is the use of fallible measures (typically same super-ordinate goal, a desire to avoid cognitive
self-reports). Correlations based on fallible measures dissonance should create a drive to perform either both
usually underestimate the ‘‘true’’ correlations due to or neither of them.2 Obviously, what matters is how the
uncorrelated errors of measurement (Bagozzi, 1994; actors themselves, not some outside observer, perceive
Mackenzie, 2001). When attenuation due to measure- the two behaviors. If actors fail to see that the two
ment error is combined with attenuation due to the behaviors are linked to a common goal, they may not
influence of idiosyncratic constraints, theoretically mean- experience the slightest inconsistency when performing
ingful correlations between behaviors may easily dis- one and not the other.
appear, especially when the sample size is small. In addition, the individual may perceive that the two
Hence, in order to achieve a satisfactory answer to the behaviors differ in the level of freedom to choose.
question whether ERBs tend to be—and should be Experimental research documents that cognitive dis-
expected to be—positively correlated it is necessary to sonance is not produced if inconsistency can be
explicitly formulate the limiting conditions for this attributed to external forces (e.g. Festinger & Carlsmith,
proposition. Important limiting conditions have been 1959; Collins & Hoyt, 1972). Specifically, environmen-
suggested above: the ERBs should not be substitutes, not tally relevant behaviors may differ in many ways, for
totally determined by idiosyncratic constraints, and instance with regard to the specific physical acts
measurement error should be accounted for. In addition, performed, where and when they are performed, the
the motivational forces assumed to produce a positive amount of effort and/or resources expended, and/or the
correlation between ERBs should be considered. specific outcomes obtained. Whether or not these
The expectation that different ERBs should be differences translate into different degrees of external
positively correlated is based on (or receive theoretical determination and, hence, are legitimate reasons for
backing from) social-psychological theories proposing behaving differently (viewed from the perspective of a
that most people have a strong desire to be consistent in super-ordinate goal) is to a high degree a subjective
their attitudes, beliefs, words, and behaviors (Feldman, question. Hence, the amount of dissonance produced by
1966; Cialdini, 1989; Bator & Cialdini, 2000; for an acting inconsistently not only depends on the indivi-
extensive review, see Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). One of the dual’s perception of the behaviors’ relationship to the
most prominent among these theories is Festinger’s super-ordinate goal, but also on the level of his/her
(1957) theory of cognitive dissonance, which claims that perceived volitional control over the behavior (i.e.
we have a need to avoid inconsistencies in our beliefs, whether the reason for performing a behavior is
attitudes, and behaviors. attributed to intrinsic motivation or external forces)
Backed by experimental evidence (e.g. Zanna & (cf. Festinger, 1957).
Cooper, 1974; Zanna, Higgins, & Taves, 1976; Elliot & Hence, the following hypotheses about correlations
Devine, 1994), cognitive dissonance theory suggests that between ERBs can be derived from cognitive dissonance
it is emotionally disturbing and, hence, unpleasant to

1 2
The opposite situation, that a pair of ERBs are complements, is of Whether it will be both or neither probably depends on the
course also possible, in which case the finding of a correlation is trivial. importance of the goal to the individual.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103 95

theory: H2: The influence of perceived similarity on the


H1.1: Volitional ERBs are positively correlated because correlation between ERBs (cf. H1.2) depends on how
people strive to be consistent in their performance or morally important it is for the person to behave in an
nonperformance of behaviors that are connected by a environmentally friendly way because moral importance
common association to a super-ordinate goal, such as influences the amount of dissonance produced by acting
environmental protection. inconsistently in the environmental field.
However, due to lack of clarity of the connection Notice that what is proposed here is an interaction
between behaviors and super-ordinate goals, as well as between moral norms and similarity perceptions in
different degrees of external determination, the indivi- determining the correlation between pairs of ERBs. The
dual does not necessarily perceive it to be inconsistent to effect of similarity perceptions on the correlation
behave in a way that furthers a super-ordinate goal in between behaviors should be stronger among indivi-
one behavioral domain while behaving in a way that duals with strong than among those with weak moral
does not, or perhaps even goes in the opposite direction, norms, and the effect of moral norms on the correlation
in another domain. Hence: should be stronger for behaviors that are perceived as
H1.2: The correlation between ERBs depends on how relatively similar. In fact, direct effects of the two
similar or dissimilar they are perceived to be in terms of variables should only be expected if most people in the
relationship to super-ordinate goal and external determi- analysed population share norms for the behaviors of
nation because individuals will strive harder to perform some importance (allowing for a direct effect of
two such activities consistently the more similar they similarity perceptions) and/or if most people perceive
perceive them to be in these respects. at least some similarity between the behaviors (allowing
Not all inconsistencies are assumed to be equally for a direct effect of moral norms).
disturbing and, hence, equally likely to produce
cognitive dissonance. Festinger (1957) was not very
precise in specifying possible sources of variation in the 2. Method
amount of dissonance produced by inconsistency, but
later research has attempted to strengthen the theory on The hypotheses are tested by means of a survey of 309
this point. The most influential of these attempts is ordinary consumers, contacted in two shopping malls in
Aronson’s (1997; Aronson & Carlsmith, 1962) Aarhus, Denmark, in June 1998. No log was made of
suggestion that the feeling of cognitive dissonance the number of individuals refuting to participate, but
depends on whether important elements of ones self- since they refused without knowing the subject of the
concept (i.e. being competent or possessing high study3 the nonresponse is not likely to cause biases in
moral standards) are being threatened. Specifically, the the hypothesis tests. Of course, the sampling procedure
desire to perceive oneself as a moral person is regarded a does not assure a sample, which is representative of a
key concern in the self-concept explanation of well-defined population, but it did provide information
dissonance (Dickerson et al., 1992; Thibodeau & from a broad selection of ordinary consumers (see the
Aronson, 1992). sample’s demographic profile in Table 1), which is
It has repeatedly been demonstrated that moral sufficient for the present purpose.
standards (or norms) play an important role in guiding Among other things, the survey instrument contained
ERBs (e.g. Heberlein, 1972; Black, Stern, & Elworth, questions about the similarity or dissimilarity of pairs
1985; Stern, Dietz, & Black, 1986; Hopper & Nielsen, that can be constructed from a set of 15 environmentally
1991; Th^gersen, 1999a). These empirical results in relevant behaviors as well as about the performance and
combination with the self-concept revision of dissonance moral norms regarding the 15 behaviors, in that order.
theory suggest that the importance of consistency in the Behavior with respect to the 15 activities (see Table 2)
environmental field, and, hence, the amount of dis- was measured by asking the respondent how often he/
sonance produced by behaving inconsistently, depends she carried out the activity in question,4 using a scale
on the person’s moral standards for ERBs. Behavioral with five verbally defined steps: ‘‘never’’, ‘‘rarely’’, ‘‘half
inconsistency threatens the individual’s self-perception the times’’, ‘‘often’’, and ‘‘always/every time.’’
as a morally reliable person only if he or she holds moral
norms of some strength for this type of behavior. Hence,
for individuals who have no or only weak moral norms 3
Passers by in the shopping mall who seemed to be at least 18 years
for environmentally relevant behaviors it matters little if old were asked to participate in ‘‘a survey about Danes’ consumption
they perceive their behavior in this domain to be habits.’’
4
The environmental significance of the analysed behaviors was
inconsistent. However, for individuals with strong moral
secured by using a study on the environmental impact of consumption,
norms the desire to avoid cognitive dissonance creates a commissioned by the Danish Environment and Consumer Agencies
drive to behave consistently. Specifically we may (Forbrugerstyrelsen og Milj^styrelsen, 1996), as the basis for selecting
hypothesize that: the behaviors.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
96 J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103

Table 1 all activity pairs.5 Thus, the respondents were divided


Sample’s demographic profile, N ¼ 309 into three groups, which were given different versions of
Percenta the questionnaire; in each group, only 7 of the activities
were compared. Three activities were however common
Female 53
to all groups: (buying organic milk, source-separating
Male 47
Less than high school 27 glass and paper, and taking the bus to work and
High school 19 shopping), and for the pairwise similarity ratings of
Bachelor or equivalent 25 these, no significant differences were found between the
Post graduate degree 29 various groups. The three versions of the questionnaire
Single 60
also contained identical questions about the perfor-
Living with a partner 40
Have children 54 mance of the behaviors and about demographics. There
No children 46 are no differences in demographic characteristics or
Own car(s) 43 reported behavior among respondents filling out differ-
No car 57 ent versions of the questionnaire. Hence, in the analyses
Live in apartment 43
to be reported here, the ratings from all three
Live in a house 57
Mean age, years 35 respondent groups are pooled.
a
An index of the strength of personal moral norms for
Except for mean age.
behaving in a responsible way towards the environment
is constructed by summing the answers on 15 nine-point
semantic differential scales with the end points ‘‘right’’–
Table 2 shows that it is common in this sample of ‘‘wrong.’’ The scales referred to each of the behaviors
Danish consumers to behave in an environmentally that were assessed for similarity and respondents were
friendly way, at least according to themselves. However, only asked to evaluate the behaviors that they had
the various kinds of environmentally friendly behavior assessed for similarity.6 The scale reliability coefficient r
also differ greatly in popularity as indicated by means (Raykov, 2001) for the index is 0.89.7 In the following
varying from below 2 to over 4 on the five-point scale. analyses the index is dichotomized at its mean.
The measures of perceived similiarity were obtained by
asking the respondents to make pairwise comparisons of
the various activities and for each pair to report the
3. Data analysis
degree of similarity on a scale from 0 to 10, where 0 was
defined as ‘‘the two activities have absolutely nothing in
First, the hypotheses are tested by means of simple
common’’ and 10 as ‘‘the two activities have very much
regression analyses using SPSS10. Next, the hypothesis
in common.’’ It was not specified in which respect the
tests are repeated on a subset of the data by means of
activities might be perceived as similar, if at all. In this
multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using
way we diminished the risk of producing demand effects.
.
Lisrel 8.53 (Joreskog, .
1993; Joreskog, .
Sorbom, Toit, &
We also judged that specifying dimensions for assessing
Toit, 1999; du Toit, du Toit, & Hawkins, 2001). The
similarity was not necessary for the present purpose.
main advantage of this procedure is that it allows one to
Remember that we assume that similarity judgments in
explicitly account for measurement error,8 which other-
cases like this have motivational properties. If this is
wise substantially attenuates the analysed correlations
true, it will show up in the data analysis whether or not
(Mackenzie, 2001). Measures of how well the implied
subjects used the type of criteria for assessing the
variance–covariance matrix from the parameter esti-
similarity between pairs of behaviors that we assumed
mates reflects the observed sample variance–covariance
they would. Further, there is strong evidence suggesting
that when making similarity judgments, people actively 5
If the similarity of each of the 15 activities with each of the other 14
search for the ways in which stimuli are similar (e.g. were to be evaluated, the respondents would have to evaluate 104
.
Sjoberg & Thorslund, 1979) and that perceptions pairs.
6
about consequences play a key role in their general- Notice that this means that most of the items used to construct the
izations about objects (e.g. Shepard, 1987). Hence, index differ between respondents that have filled out different versions
of the questionnaire. Analysis of variance was used to control whether
when the compared objects are all behaviors that the mean of the index differs between the three groups, which it does
have been associated with environmental consequences not (F ¼ 0:08; p ¼ 0:92). On this basis it is inferred that the index
(i.e. the avoidance of negative environmental impacts) in measures the same overall concern irrespective of the specific behaviors
the public debate it seems highly likely that this referred to in the items used to represent it.
7
Full Information Maximum Likelihood (see later) was used to
dimension will be prominent in subjects’ similarity
produce the input for the calculation of the scale reliability coefficient.
judgments. 8
Strictly spoken, what is accounted for is unique variance in the
As the total number of activities included in the study manifest variables, i.e. variance not reflecting the hypothesized latent
was 15, it was not possible to have every respondent rate category, but something else, including measurement error.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103 97

Table 2
Fifteen analysed behaviors, N ¼ 309

How often ya Mean s.d. # Missing

Is the milk you buy organic? 2.784 1.536 13


Are the frozen peas you buy organic? 1.945 1.139 38
Is the minced beef you buy organic? 1.911 1.053 18
Do you take the bus or train to work, shopping, and the like? 2.440 1.425 2
Do you bike to work, shopping, and the like? 3.154 1.515 4
Do you keep the temperature in the living room below 21 degree Celsius in the winter? 3.655 1.228 5
Do you turn off all light when you leave a room (as last person)? 4.314 0.795 0
Do you turn off the water when brushing your teeth? 3.929 1.324 1
Are the light bulbs you buy energy saving? 2.525 1.308 4
Is the paint you buy based on water or linen oil? 3.828 1.147 71
Is the shampoo you buy of an environmentally approved brand? 2.932 1.324 31
Do you deliver empty packaging glass and newspaper to recycling? 4.294 1.080 3
Is your green kitchen waste composted in the garden or at a municipal facility? 2.222 1.604 7
Do you deliver paint residues and similar waste to the ‘‘environment car’’ or a paint shop? 3.221 1.610 51
Do you wash your ‘‘boiling wash’’ at 60 C instead of 90 C? 3.904 1.386 16
a
At a five-point scale with the end points ‘‘never’’ (1) and ‘‘always/every time’’ (5).

matrix can be used to determine whether the hypothe- Correlation


sized model gives an acceptable representation of the 0.5
analysed data. Nonresponse (full as well as item) reduces
statistical power and may lead to biased parameter 0.4

estimates. Recent years’ extensive research into ways of 0.3


dealing with missing data strongly suggests that Full
Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) currently is 0.2
the most efficient method (at least missingness due to
item-nonresponse), not only because it obviously mini- 0.1
mizes the loss of information and, hence, statistical
0
power, but also because it leads to the most unbiased
parameter estimates (Arbuckle, 1996), and this even in -0.1
the case of non-normal data (Enders, 2001). 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5 7 7.5
Similarity
C = -0.33 + 0.086 • S; r2 = 0.414

4. Results Fig. 1. Relationship between similarity assessment and pairwise


correlation between environmentally relevant behaviors, regression
4.1. Regressing correlations between behaviors on analysis, N ¼ 57:
similarity assessment

Hypothesis 1.1 states that, in general, voluntary ERBs important reason is that there is no correction for
are positively correlated and Hypothesis 1.2 that the measurement error, which attenuates the true correla-
correlation between ERBs depends on how similar or tions. The importance of this methodological short-
dissimilar they are perceived to be. These propositions coming will become obvious from the CFA in the next
can be tested by inspecting pairwise correlations section. Considering the downward biased correla-
between the behaviors listed in Table 2 and by regressing tions—and error in the measurement of similarity as
the pairwise correlations on the similarity ratings of the well—a remarkably strong relationship is found between
same pairs of behavior. The regression plot is shown in the similarity assessments and the pairwise correlations,
Fig. 1. however, perceived similarity explaining 41.4% of the
Notice first that the pairwise correlations vary from variation in pairwise correlations. Hence, Hypothesis
below 0 to about 0.45. The mean correlation is only 1.2 is strongly supported by the data.
0.123. At best, this can be interpreted as weak support As mentioned above, the similarity measure is devoid
for Hypothesis 1.1. The low correlations may partly of directions as to the dimension(s) that respondents
have substantive reasons: due to different unique might use to compare the behaviors. This procedure
constraints and other sources of ‘‘inconsistency’’ some assures that we measure respondents’ subjective percep-
ERBs just do not go together. However, another tion of similarities and differences rather than how
ARTICLE IN PRESS
98 J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103

similarities and differences are assessed by some external Three behavior items concern the purchase of different
observer. On the other hand there is a risk that the kinds of organic food products, assumed to reflect the
instrument does not really captures what we want to behavior category ‘‘buying organic food,’’ and three
measure, that is, does not capture perceptions about items concern the performance of different kinds of
similarities and differences that are relevant for judging waste source-separation, assumed to reflect the behavior
whether it is inconsistent to behave differently in the two category ‘‘recycling.’’ Since these behaviors are per-
areas. The regression analysis strongly supports the formed in different settings (the shop and at home),
validity of the similarity measure. target different environmental problems (water pollu-
In order to analyse whether the moral importance of tion and the waste problem), and have different personal
behaving in an environment-friendly way influences the consequences as well (organic buying costs more and
relationship between the similarity assessment and the may produce health benefits, source-separation takes
correlation between pairs of behavior, as suggested by time and effort, composting produce compost for the
Hypothesis 2, pairwise correlations and average simi- garden) it is by no means trivial that they should be
larity ratings have been calculated for two groups, correlated. In a North-American context, environment-
differentiated by moral norms for ERB (the sample split friendly purchasing has been classified as medium and
at the mean of the moral norms index).9 As expected, recycling as low in difficulty (i.e. low dependence on
the mean correlation between pairs of behaviors is external conditions) (Green-Demers, Pelletier, & Me-
higher in the high (0.139) than in the low (0.072) moral nard, 1997). This is equally true in a Danish context. In
norm group. Also, as predicted, perceived similarity Denmark there is a fairly good supply of organic
explains a higher proportion of the variation in the products in ordinary super-markets and Danish munici-
pairwise correlation in the high (0.291)10 than in the low palities run collection schemes for paper, glass, and for
(0.187) moral norm group, thus indicating that the effect hazardous waste. However, even if these behaviors are
of perceived similarity on behavioral consistency de- in general highly volitional, individuals may perceive
pends on the moral importance of the behaviors. The that external restrictions differ quite substantively
fact that it has predictive validity also supports the between specific behaviors in each category. This means
construct validity of the moral norm index as an that the same level of motivation to buy organic food or
instrument to group respondents with regard to the to recycle may be reflected in different levels of
moral importance of ERBs. performance of specific behaviors in the category. With
regard to buying organic food, the most important
4.2. Confirmatory factor analysis external restriction is the supply of the specific type of
food, which varies between products and super-markets.
As mentioned above, an important advantage of CFA With regard to recycling opportunities, Danish munici-
is that it allows the researcher to account for measure- palities offer collection systems for recyclable glass and
ment error when the data set contains more than one paper and often also for hazardous waste, but the
indicator (manifest variables) for an analysed phenom- service level provided by the system is not as high for
enon (latent variable). For this to be the case here we the latter as for the former. In the case of composting, the
need to change focus from the level of specific behaviors kind of home the person inhabits also plays a role.
to broader behavior categories. I use the term behavior To test whether it is reasonable to assume that the
category, as suggested by Verhallen and Pieters (1984), analysed consumers entertain behavior concepts for
to refer to a set of single acts that are similar in at least buying organic food and recycling and, if it is, whether
one of the specification elements: action, target, context, there is a positive correlation between performing the
and time (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977, 1980). two behavior categories, a CFA is performed on the six
For the present analysis, at least two behavior items assumed to reflect the two behavior categories (see
categories are needed and preferably some that are Fig. 2). A simple structure factor pattern is assumed, in
neither too strongly determined by idiosyncratic condi- addition to the usual assumptions, for instance about
tions nor so similar that finding correlations between the unique (or error) variances of the individual items
them would be trivial. When designing the study, some being uncorrelated with the latent variables and
of the behavior items were specifically selected so as to mutually uncorrelated.
reflect two behavior categories that fulfill these criteria. When using the FIML approach, LISREL supplies
two test variables that can be used to assess the overall
9
In order to conserve space the regression output from these adequacy of the statistical model. w2 is a measure of the
calculations is not shown, but it can be obtained from the author. overall fit of the model to the data. It is a badness-of-fit
10
The smaller sample sizes available for calculating the pairwise
measure in the sense that a small w2 corresponds to a
correlations that serve as the dependent variable means larger
measurement error in the individual correlations, which is the most good fit and a large w2 to a bad fit. Statistically, it is
likely reason why the explained variance is lower than when the total based on the assumption that the model holds exactly in
sample is analysed, even in the high moral norm group. the population, which is rarely realistic in the behavioral
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103 99

be desired. The factor loadings are all significant,


Organic milk
0.69 0.55
reflecting the tendency of specific behaviors in a
behavior category to vary in concert, though. This also
0.81 means that the composite reliability index may be
Organic peas Buying organic interpreted as a summary measure of the degree to
0.35
0.79
which consumers behave consistently within these
behavior categories. Hence, the low composite reliability
Organic beef
for recycling is an indication that there are serious
0.38 idiosyncratic restrictions on (some of) the behaviors in
this behavior category. That this is an important part of
0.64
the explanation for the overall fit of the model not being
Glass & paper too good as well is illustrated by the fact that if the error
0.86 0.37 terms of the two recycling behaviors depending on a
collection service from the municipality (recyclable
0.56 materials and hazardous waste) are allowed to correlate,
Compost Recycling
0.68 RMSEA drops to a satisfactory 0.08 and w2 to 20.77
0.52 (7 df.).12 However, because this error correlation is of
neither theoretical nor substantive interest it is not
Hazardous included in the following calculations.
0.73
Turning now again to the hypotheses tests, Hypoth-
Fig. 2. CFA, buying organic food and recycling. Completely esis 1.1 proposes that, in general, ERBs are positively
standardized solution. N ¼ 309: Note: w2 (8 df.)=33.82, po0:001;
correlated. In the general introduction and in the
RMSEA=0.10, p ¼ 0:007:
discussion of the simple pairwise correlations above it
was further proposed that measurement error might be
one of the reasons for not finding theoretically expected
sciences. A consequence of this assumption is that a correlations. The CFA reported in Fig. 2 confirms both,
model which holds approximately in the population will showing that when using a method that corrects for
.
be rejected if the sample is large (Joreskog .
& Sorbom, measurement error a strong positive correlation appears
1996). As the name indicates, the Root Mean Square between the analysed ERBs (f ¼ 0:64), much stronger
Error of Approximation (RMSEA) take account of the than anyone found when analysing the pairwise
error of approximation in the population and the correlations above. Specifically, buying organic food
precision of the fit measure itself. In order to correct and source-separating waste for the sake of recycling
for the decrease in discrepancy when parameters are and careful treatment seem to be strongly and positively
added in the model, the RMSEA is a measure of correlated activities in the analysed setting.13
discrepancy per degree of freedom. A value of 0.05 of If the sample is divided into groups according to
RMSEA is generally assumed to indicate a close fit and individuals’ similarity ratings, the hypothesis that the
values up to 0.08 a reasonable fit (Browne & Cudeck, correlation between ERBs depends on how similar or
1993). dissimilar they are perceived to be can be tested by
The test statistics indicate that the proposed factor means of multigroup CFA. The similarity rating of
structure produces a somewhat less than acceptable fit ‘‘buying organic milk’’ and ‘‘delivering empty packaging
to the data. This means that one or more of the glass and used newspapers to recycling’’ is used for this
assumptions made—about a simple structure factor purpose.14 Because of sample-size concerns the sample is
pattern and about zero error covariances—does not only divided in two groups, below and above the mean
hold for this data set. The individual factor loadings of the similarity rating. The correlation between the two
surpass conventionally accepted thresholds, although behaviors in groups differentiated by similarity rating is
some of them are clearly at the low end. Based on the shown in Table 3, the rightmost column.
factor loadings and item error variances one can
calculate a composite reliability index, r; for each of 12
The LIREL output from the calculation can be obtained from the
the latent variables. As indicated by the factor loadings, author.
the r of buying organic is a satisfactory 0.73, while that 13
On the other hand, the correlation between latent constructs is
of recycling is a rather low 0.49. Hence, the ‘‘construct significantly different from 1, which is good measurement-wise,
validity’’11 of the latter behavior category leaves a lot to because it shows that the proposed behavior categories possess
acceptable discriminant validity.
14
Hence, the split is based on the similarity rating of two more
11
Put in quotation marks because the behavioral items reflect more specific behaviors, one from each of the two behavior categories. This
than conceptions, which means that r is not a measure of construct is the only pair of behaviors from the two categories that were rated for
validity in a strict sense. similarity by all respondents.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
100 J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103

Table 3 1.0
Correlations between the behavioral categories ‘‘buying organic’’ and 0.9
Strong norms
‘‘recycling’’ in groups differentiated by similarity ratings and the moral 0.8
importance of environmentally relevant behavior, N ¼ 309 0.7

Correlation
0.6
Similarity Moral norm 0.5
Weak norms
0.4
High Low All
0.3
High 1.00 0.54 0.82 0.2
Low 0.38 0.50 0.43 0.1
All 0.75 0.47 0.64 0.0
High similarity Low similarity

Fig. 3. Effects of similarity rating and the personal moral importance


As predicted in Hypothesis 1.2, the correlation of environmentally relevant behavior on the correlation between the
between behavioral categories is stronger in the high behavior categories ‘‘buying organic food’’ and recycling, N ¼ 309:
(0.82) than in the low similarity group (0.43).15 The
difference is statistically significant as shown by a
significant increase in w2 (5.73, 1 df., po0:05) if the The correlation between the two behaviors in groups
two correlations are restricted to be equal.16 differentiated by similarity rating and by moral norms is
It was argued above that a direct effect of moral shown by the four values to the upper left of Table 3.
norms on the correlation between the analysed behavior The now familiar pattern is repeated in the high moral
categories should only be expected if respondents norm case, and the correlation reaches the ceiling value
perceive them as ‘‘sufficiently similar.’’ This prerequisite 1.00 among those high on both similarity rating and
seems to be fulfilled in the present case. Only 9.4% of moral norms. Hence, as predicted individuals that both
the sample express that ‘‘the two activities have perceive the two behaviors as similar and hold strong
absolutely nothing in common.’’ Hence, most respon- moral norms for this type of behavior report the most
dents perceive at least some similarity between the consistent behavior. Further, the group of individuals
behaviors.17 holding strong moral norms for this type of behavior,
Whether there is a direct effect of moral norms on the but perceiving the two behaviors as dissimilar, reports
correlation between behaviors can be tested in the same the least consistent behavior of all. If the correlation
way as the direct effect of similarity perceptions, by between the two behavior categories is restricted to be
using the moral norm index as the grouping variable. equal in these two groups, it results in a significant
Again, sample-size concerns dictate that the sample is (po0:05) increase in w2 (8.27, 1 df.). On the other hand,
only divided in two groups, below and above the mean for individuals holding weak moral norms for this type
of the index. The resulting correlations are shown in of behavior, the degree of behavioral consistency (i.e.
Table 3, the last row. As expected, the correlation the correlation) is independent of how similar the two
between behavior categories is stronger in the high behavior categories are perceived to be. If the correla-
(0.75) than in the low moral norm group (0.47). If the tion is restricted to be equal in these two groups, it
two correlations are restricted to be equal it results in a results in an insignificant increase in w2 (0.02, 1 df.).19
marginally significant increase in w2 (2.95, 1 df., Hence, Hypothesis 2, that the influence of perceived
po0:10).18 similarity on the correlation between ERBs depends on
The interaction effect proposed by Hypothesis 2 can how morally important it is for the person to behave in
be identified by means of multigroup CFA if both an environmentally friendly way, is confirmed by the
variables are used as grouping variables simultaneously. analysis, as illustrated clearly by Fig. 3.

15
The full Lisrel output for this and the following calculations can
be acquired from the author. 5. Summary and implications
16
This result also supports the construct validity of the similarity
measure. This paper deals with the issue of consistency and
17
As also mentioned above, the hypothesized direct effect of inconsistency in ERB as reflected in the correlations
similarity perceptions on the correlation between the analysed
behavior categories is likewise based on the implicit assumption that
between different ERBs. Social-psychological theories,
most people in the sample share norms of some importance supporting including cognitive dissonance theory, suggest that most
environmentally friendly behaviors, which indeed is true in this case. people desire to behave consistently. This desire should
On the moral norm index, where a low score reflects strong norms for
ERB, only 5.5% scored on the midpoint or above (indicating the
19
absence of norms supporting ERB). It may seem surprising that these correlations are higher than in
18
This result also supports the construct validity of the moral norm the high moral norm-low similarity group. However, the latter is not
index as an instrument to group respondents with regard to the moral significantly different from any of the former, Dw2 ¼ 0:28 and 0.22,
importance of ERBs. respectively (1 df.).
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103 101

lead to ERBs being positively correlated, except in cases At the Earth Summit in Rio in 1992, governments of
where different ERBs are substitutes or at least one of most countries of the world agreed that ‘‘altering
them is totally determined by idiosyncratic conditions. consumption patterns is one of humanity’s greatest
However, it is argued here that correlations may be challenges in the quest for environmentally sound and
attenuated due to the influence of idiosyncratic sustainable development’’ (Sitarz, 1994, p. 39). How-
conditions (considered ‘‘noise’’ in this context) and ever, perhaps based on the understanding that ‘‘envir-
measurement error and that the influence of the desire to onmentally beneficial choicesyareymade on an
behave consistently depends on the perceived similarity activity-to-activity basis’’ (Pickett et al., 1993), most
of the behaviors and on the perceived (moral) impor- research and most attempts to change consumer
tance of behaving in a responsible way towards the behavior in an environmentally responsible direction
environment. have focused on isolated habits or groups of habits (like
The hypotheses are tested by means of mall-intercept participation in a recycling program, lowering the
survey data from the Danish town Aarhus. Hence, an thermostat, or saving water) rather than on achieving
important limitation of the study is that behaviors are sweeping changes into lifestyles. This situation has
registered by means of self-report. On the other hand, crippled the ability of the behavioral sciences to provide
the fact that the respondents are ordinary consumers appropriate recommendations for achieving such
contacted, relatively briefly, in their ordinary shopping changes and could be one of the reasons why the
environment strengthens the ecological validity of the evolution away from unsustainable patterns of con-
findings. sumption and production is disturbingly slow.20
At the item level, the study finds rather low This study provides evidence to suggest that the
correlations between pairs of ERBs. However, the evolution towards a more (environmentally) sustainable
CFA indicates that item-level correlations are strongly society may be facilitated by a tendency of ERB to
attenuated due to measurement error. Hence, this study generalize, which is produced by an innate human desire
renders probable that the use of fallible measures is one to be consistent. A number of prerequisites need to be in
of the reasons why low correlations between ERBs are place for this outcome to be achieved, however. First
often reported. When correcting for measurement error, and foremost, there have to be areas where the person
a significant correlation of substantial magnitude knowingly and willingly behaves in an environmentally
appears (at least between recycling and buying organic responsible way. Hence, this mechanism cannot func-
food). Further, the regression analyses and the multi- tion until a process leading to sustainability has already
group CFAs confirm that the correlation between ERBs started in some areas. For instance, the establishment of
depends both on the perceived similarity of the two ambitious recycling programs, in Denmark and many
behaviors and on the moral importance of acting in an other countries, may have the important derived effect
environmentally responsible way. Support for the that it initiates an evolution towards sustainability (see
hypothesis that the influence of the former depends on also Berger, 1997; Th^gersen, 1999b). Further, for
the latter is also obtained. All these results support the transfer or spillover of ERB to occur there has to be
social-psychological proposition that the desire to avoid other areas where there are obvious opportunities for
inconsistency is indeed an important factor shaping acting in an environmentally responsible way. Cognitive
behavioral patterns in the environmental field. The dissonance may be unpleasant, but the unpleasantness
interaction between moral norms and similarity percep- of the sacrifices needed in order to behave in an
tions in determining the level of behavioral consistency environmentally responsible way may easily be worse,
strongly supports the proposition that the desire to in which case most people adopt other than behavioral
avoid cognitive dissonance is driving this process. means to resolve the dissonance or simply choose to live
Hence, this analysis also points to important reasons with their perceived behavioral inconsistency (Festinger,
why people often do not act consistently across 1957; Abelson, 1968). Hence, in a sustainability per-
behaviors, at least not in the eyes of an outside observer. spective the most important policies are those that
Most basically, seemingly inconsistent behavior patterns .
facilitate the actual performance of ERBs (Olander &
may be caused by the actors failing to perceive the Th^gersen, 1995; Th^gersen, 1997).
relevant similarity between the behaviors warranting a This study shows that a third prerequisite that needs
similar behavioral response. Hence, they themselves see to be in place before the human desire to be consistent
no inconsistency. In addition, inconsistency in a specific leads to spillover of ERB is that individuals are aware
behavioral domain, such as the domain of environment- of relevant similarities between environmentally signifi-
friendly behaviors, is less threatening to the self-concept cant behaviors. Basically, this means that consumers/
of individuals who do not find these behaviors morally citizens needs to be educated about the environmental
important. Because such people can more easily endure
this particular type of inconsistency they are more likely 20
For instance, the UN Secretary-General: Implementing Agenda 21
to act inconsistently. (December 19, 2001, section 5).
ARTICLE IN PRESS
102 J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103

significance of our everyday behaviors. To perform this Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting
task it is not only or primarily the advertising sector that social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
is called for, but the whole education system as well as Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of
relevant content in the general media (Olander . &
incomplete data. In G. A. Marcoulides, & R. E. Schumacker
(Eds.), Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techni-
Th^gersen, 1995; Th^gersen, 2000). ques. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Any promotion strategy should be based on a clear Aronson, E. (1997). The theory of cognitive dissonance: The evolution
understanding that what matters is the individual’s and vicissitudes of an idea. In C. McGarty, & A. Haslam (Eds.),
subjective perception of similarities and differences, The message of social psychology (pp. 20–35). Oxford: Blackwell.
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). Performance expectancy as a
rather than the objective reality. This means that a
determinant of actual performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social
defensive distortion of reality by subjectively emphasiz- Psychology, 65, 178–182.
ing the differences and de-emphasizing the similarity Bagozzi, R. P. (1994). Structural equation models in marketing
between environmentally relevant behaviors may be an research: Basic principles. In R. P. Bagozzi (Ed.), Principles of
effective means to dampen the discomfort of cognitive marketing research (pp. 317–385). Cambridge: Blackwell Publishers.
dissonance. In such cases, information containing the Bator, R. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (2000). New ways to promote
proenvironmental behavior: The application of persuasion theory
opposite message will most likely be resisted, if it is not to the development of effective proenvironmental public service
accompanied by changes in reality to be more facilitat- announcements. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 527–541.
ing to ERB. Berger, I. E. (1997). The demographics of recycling and the structure
The study also shows that spillover of ERB is more of environmental behavior. Environment and Behavior, 29,
likely when behaving in an environmentally responsible 515–531.
Black, J. S., Stern, P. C., & Elworth, J. T. (1985). Personal and
way is morally important. An important prerequisite of
contextual influences on household energy adaptations. Journal of
the development of personal norms is awareness of the Applied Psychology, 70, 3–21.
consequences of one’s acts for other people and nature Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing
(Schwartz, 1977; Th^gersen, 1999a). Hence, environ- model fit. In J. S. Long (Ed.), Testing structural equation models
mental education will also facilitate appropriate norm (pp. 445–455). Newbury Park: Sage.
development. In addition, governments can send a Cialdini, R. B. (1989). Social motivation to comply: Norms, values and
principles. In J. A. Roth, & J. T. Scholz (Eds.), Taxpayer
strong signal about social norms in the environment compliance, Vol. 2 (pp. 200–227). Philadelphia: University of
field, and thus further facilitate norm development, by Pennsylvania Press.
demonstrating high commitment to solve environmental Collins, B., & Hoyt, M. (1972). Personal responsibility-for-conse-
problems in their choice of policy instruments and in quences: An integration and extension of the ‘forced compliance’
their communication with the citizenry (Th^gersen & literature. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 558–593.
.
Olander, 1997; Frey, 1999). De Kruijk, M.D., & van Raaij, W. F. (1991). Similarities and
frequencies of behaviors in an environmental context. Paper
presented at the IAREP/SASE Conference, Stockholm, Sweden,
June 16–19.
Acknowledgements
Dickerson, C., Thibodeau, R., & Aronson, E. (1992). Using cognitive
dissonance to encourage water conservation. Journal of Applied
This paper is the outcome of a collaborative research Social Psychology, 22, 841–854.
.
project with Folke Olander, Aarhus School of Business, Diekmann, A., & Preisend.orfer, P. (1998). Environmental behavior:
in the framework of Center for Social Science Research Discrepancies between aspirations and reality. Rationality and
on the Environment (CeSaM), a multidisciplinary center Society, 10(1), 79–102.
Du Toit, M., du Toit, S., & Hawkins, D. (2001). Interactive LISREL:
based on collaboration among scholars from a number User’s guide. Chicago: Scientific Software International.
of Danish unversities and other research institutions. I Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort
am grateful to Tommy G.arling, Berit T. M^ller, Folke Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
.
Olander, and Joachim Scholderer for helpful comments Elliot, A. J., & Devine, P. G. (1994). On the motivational nature of
on earlier drafts. cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 382–394.
Enders, C. K. (2001). The impact of nonnormality on full information
maximum-likelihood estimation for structural equation models
References with missing data. Psychological Methods, 6(4), 352–370.
Feldman, S. (Ed.). (1966). Cognitive consistency motivational ante-
Abelson, R. P. (1968). Psychological implication. In R. P. Abelson, E. cedents and behavioral consequents. New York: Academic Press.
Aronson, W. J. McGuire, T. M. Newcomb, M. J. Rosenberg, & P. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston: Row
H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A Peterson.
sourcebook (pp. 112–139). Chicago: Rand McNally. Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of
Aitken, C. K., McMahon, T. A., Wearing, A. J., & Finlayson, B. L. forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58,
(1994). Residential water use: Predicting and reducing consump- 203–210.
tion. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 136–158. Forbrugerstyrelsen og Milj^styrelsen (1996). Milj^belastningen ved
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A familiens aktiviteter (Environmental impacts of household activities).
theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Rapport No. 1996.1, Forbrugerstyrelsen og Milj^styrelsen,
Bulletin, 84, 888–918. Copenhagen.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Th^gersen / Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (2004) 93–103 103

Frey, B. S. (1993). Motivation as a limit to pricing. Journal of Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influence on altruism. In L.
Economic Psychology, 14, 635–664. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology,
Frey, B. S. (1999). Morality and rationality in environmental policy. Vol. 10 (pp. 221–279). New York: Academic Press.
Journal of Consumer Policy, 22, 395–417. Shepard, R. N. (1987). Toward a universal law of generalization for
Gray, D. B. (1985). Ecological beliefs and behaviors: Assessment and psychological science. Science, 237, 1317–1323.
change. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Sitarz, D. (Ed.). (1994). Agenda 21: The earth summit strategy to save
Green-Demers, I., Pelletier, L. G., & Menard, S. (1997). The impact of our planet. Boulder: EarthPress.
behavioural difficulty on the saliency of the association between .
Sjoberg, L., & Thorslund, C. (1979). A classificatory theory of
self-determined motivation and environmental behaviours. similarity. Psychological Research, 40, 223–247.
Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 29, 157–166. Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Black, J. S. (1986). Support for
Guagnano, G. A., Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1995). Influences on environmental protection: The role of moral norms. Population
attitude-behavior relationships. A natural experiment with curbside and Environment, 8, 204–222.
recycling. Environment and Behavior, 27, 699–718. Stern, P. C., & Oskamp, S. (1987). Managing scarce environmental
Heberlein, T. A. (1972). The land ethic realized: Some social resources. In D. Stokols, & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of
psychological explanations for changing environmental attitudes. environmental psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 1043–1088). New York: Wiley.
Journal of Social Issues, 28(4), 79–87. Th^gersen, J. (1997). Facilitating recycling. Reverse-distribution
Hopper, J. R., & Nielsen, J. M. (1991). Recycling as altruistic channel design for participation and support. Social Marketing
behavior. Normative and behavioral strategies to expand partici- Quarterly, 4(1), 42–55.
pation in a community recycling program. Environment and Th^gersen, J. (1999a). The ethical consumer. Moral norms and
Behavior, 23, 195–220. packaging choice. Journal of Consumer Policy, 22, 439–460.
.
Joreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Th^gersen, J. (1999b). Spillover processes in the development of a
Bollen, & J. S. long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models sustainable consumption pattern. Journal of Economic Psychology,
(pp. 294–316). Newbury Park: Sage. 20, 53–81.
.
Joreskog, .
K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1996). LISREL8: Structural equation Th^gersen, J. (2000). Knowledge barriers to sustainable consumption.
modelling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: Scien- In P. F. Bone, K. R. France, & J. Wiener (Eds.), Marketing and
tific Software International. public policy conference proceedings 2000, Vol. 10 (pp. 29–39).
.
Joreskog, .
K. G., Sorbom, D., Toit, S. d., & Toit, M. d. (1999). Chicago: American Marketing Association.
LISREL8: New statistical features. Chicago: Scientific Software .
Th^gersen, J., & Olander, F. (1997). Kender ^konomerne vejen til et
International. b!credygtigt forbrugsm^nster (Do economists know the road to a
Kantola, S. J., Syme, G. J., & Campbell, N. A. (1984). Cognitive sustainable consumption pattern?) In Forbrugeren i centrum
dissonance and energy conservation. Journal of Applied Psychol- (pp. 118–126). K^benhavn: Forbrugerr(adet.
ogy, 69, 416–421. Thibodeau, R., & Aronson, E. (1992). Taking a closer look:
Mackenzie, S. B. (2001). Opportunities for improving consumer Reasserting the role of the self-concept in dissonance theory.
research through latent variable structural equation modeling. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 591–602.
Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 159–166. Verhallen, T. M. M., & Pieters, R. G. M. (1984). Attitude theory
.
Olander, F., & Th^gersen, J. (1995). Understanding of consumer and behavioral costs. Journal of Economic Psychology, 5,
behaviour as a prerequisite for environmental protection. Journal 223–249.
of Consumer Policy, 18, 317–357. Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). Dissonance and the pill: An
Pickett, G. M., Kangun, N., & Grove, S. J. (1993). Is there a general attribution approach to studying the arousal properties of
conserving consumer? A public policy concern. Journal of Public dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29,
Policy & Marketing, 12, 234–243. 703–709.
Raykov, T. (2001). Estimation of congeneric scale reliability using Zanna, M. P., Higgins, E. T., & Taves, P. A. (1976). Is dissonance
covariance structure analysis with nonlinear constraints. British phenomenologically aversive? Journal of Experimental Social
Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 54, 315–323. Psychology, 12, 530–538.

You might also like