You are on page 1of 11

Linking Cyber Incivility With Job Performance Through

Job Satisfaction: The Buffering Role of Positive Affect


Gary W. Giumetti,* Lauren A. Saunders, Jeralyn P. Brunette,
Franca M. DiFrancesco, Paul G. Graham
Quinnipiac University

ABSTRACT. Incivility at work has the potential to impact both individual (e.g.,
psychological health) and organizational outcomes (e.g., job performance
and turnover). In line with Affective Events Theory, the current study proposed
a moderated mediation model wherein employee job satisfaction would
mediate the relation between cyber incivility and job performance, and trait
positive affect would buffer the negative impact of cyber incivility on
performance. Two-hundred twenty participants completed measures of cyber
incivility, job satisfaction, job performance, positive affect, negative affect,
and quantitative workload. Hierarchical regression analyses revealed that
cyber incivility was negatively related to job satisfaction (ß = -.19, p = .006), as
well as job performance (ß = -.14, p = .037), while controlling for negative
affect. Additionally, results of mediation analyses indicated that job satisfaction
served as a mediator of the cyber incivility-job performance relationship
because employees who experienced cyber incivility reported lower job
satisfaction, which was linked with lower job performance (indirect effect =
-.020, 95% CIs = -.041, -.009; Sobel z = -2.36, p = .02). Trait positive affect
moderated the relationship between cyber incivility and job performance
such that the performance of those individuals with higher positive affect was
not as negatively impacted by the cyber incivility as individuals with lower
positive affect (b = .08, p = .044). The results are discussed in terms of their
implications for the wired 21st century world of work.

I
ncivility in the workplace is a widespread exposure to incivility has been linked with
phenomenon that can impact employees and decreased physical and psychological health, lower
organizations. According to Andersson and job satisfaction, and increased work withdrawal, as
Pearson (1999), incivility is “low-intensity deviant well as increased absenteeism, greater intentions
behavior with ambiguous intent to harm the to quit, and actual turnover (Cortina et al., 2001;
target, in violation of workplace norms for mutual Johnson & Indvik, 2001; Porath & Pearson, 2012).
respect. Uncivil behaviors are characteristically According to Affective Events Theory (AET;
rude and discourteous, displaying a lack of regard Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), stressful workplace
for others” (p. 457). Evidence has suggested events such as incivility are theorized to impact
that most of the workforce experiences this employees’ job attitudes, which can subsequently
phenomenon, as Cortina, Magley, Williams, and influence job performance. Previous research
Langout (2001) reported that nearly 75% of their has supported this theoretical model, finding
WINTER 2016 sample experienced some type of incivility within that incivility was linked with emotional reactions
the previous 5-year period. Such a widespread and subsequent changes in job satisfaction and
PSI CHI phenomenon has the potential to harm both withdrawal behaviors (Bunk & Magley, 2013). Addi-
JOURNAL OF
PSYCHOLOGICAL individuals and organizations. For example, tionally, Lim and Tai (2014) found that incivility
RESEARCH

230 COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204) *Faculty mentor
Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham | Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance

led to increased psychological distress, which was First, individuals may be more likely to perceive
subsequently linked with lower job performance. communications sent via the Internet as uncivil
However, although much of the existing research because of the lack of instant feedback and reduced
has looked at incivility in a face-to-face context, the nonverbal cues (Byron, 2008). When individuals
21st century working world is one characterized send an e-mail, they may not receive a response for
by frequent online communications (Madden & hours, days, or in some cases, ever. The individuals
Jones, 2008; Purcell & Rainie, 2014). The experi- may be left wondering if the message was delivered
ence of cyber incivility, or rude and discourteous or if they are being ignored. Also, messages sent
treatment that occurs via information and com- via online means lack the rich nonverbal cues that
munication technologies (Giumetti, McKibben, face-to-face communication includes. For example,
Hatfield, Schroeder, & Kowalski, 2012) is also if an individual is having a face-to-face conversation
widespread. A study of financial employees from and something being said is upsetting, the message
Singapore found that 91% of the sample experi- sender may perceive changes in the recipient’s
enced cyber incivility in the last year (Lim & Teo, body language such as arms crossing, a frown, or
2009). Preliminary evidence has also suggested that eyebrows raising, and may decide to change the
cyber incivility is linked with negative outcomes for direction of the conversation. Additionally, face-
employees, as a study of U.S. workers found that to-face conversations have the benefit of other
daily cyber incivility was associated with greater important nonverbal cues including emotional
levels of distress at the end of the workday as well expressions, tone of voice, and gaze direction. In
as the next morning (Park, Fritz, & Jex, 2015). an online message, these cues are missing, which
Beyond this initial research, however, less is may heighten the possibility of miscommunication.
known about the mechanisms through which cyber Additionally, a phenomenon known as the
incivility may be linked to decreased performance online disinhibition effect may make uncivil behav-
and for whom cyber incivility may be more impact- iors more likely (Suler, 2004). This effect occurs
ful. Therefore, the purpose of the current study was because normal constraints on individual behavior
first to examine the main effect of cyber incivility are less salient in an online context, making people
on job satisfaction and job performance, and then feel less restrained and more likely to express them-
to examine the mediating role of job satisfaction selves freely. For example, individuals may feel that
in the relation between cyber incivility and job they can more easily express their disapproval over
performance. Additionally, the current study exam- a new company policy via e-mail by clicking “reply
ined the moderating role of positive affect (PA) to all” to share derogatory remarks about the policy
understand the role of individual differences in makers, something that they would be less likely to
this relationship (see proposed model in Figure 1). do in a face-to-face meeting with the entire com-
pany. As technology use becomes more prevalent
Technology Use at Work in the workplace, interpersonal mistreatment may
Internet use in the workplace is nearly ubiquitous. also become more likely (Weatherbee & Kelloway,
A 2014 survey of employed adults in the United
States found that nearly all workers (96%) use a
FIGURE 1
laptop, desktop, tablet, or smartphone (Harter,
Agrawal, & Sorenson, 2014). In addition, another Hypothesized Model Depicting
2014 survey of U.S. workers found that 61% say that a Moderated Mediation
e-mail is very important to doing their job (Purcell
& Rainie, 2014), and a recent study found that as Job
Satisfaction
many as 83% of workers in the sample used the
Internet to communicate with coworkers, and 70%
used it to communicate with supervisors once or
twice per day (Giumetti et al., 2012). Communica-
tion via online media such as e-mail, text messaging, Cyber Job
Incivility Performance
or chat has several unique qualities compared to
face-to-face communication, which may make both WINTER 2016
the perception of mistreatment as well as the likeli- Positive
hood of engaging in mistreatment greater than Affect PSI CHI
JOURNAL OF
communication in a face-to-face context. PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH

COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204) 231
Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance | Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham

2006). More concerning is that interpersonal link between face-to-face incivility and job attitudes.
mistreatment communicated through information However, considerably less research has focused on
and communication technologies such as e-mail the relation between cyber incivility and job atti-
may be likely to have the same damaging effects as tudes. One exception to this is a study by Lim and
interpersonal mistreatment communicated face-to- Teo (2009) that examined cyber incivility among
face (Weatherbee & Kelloway, 2006). a sample of financial services employees in Singa-
pore. The researchers found that cyber incivility
Theoretical Background was negatively related to both organizational com-
The current study utilized AET to understand mitment and job satisfaction. However, no studies
how cyber incivility (a stressful workplace event) to date have examined the link between cyber
can impact employee attitudes and performance incivility and job attitudes in a U.S.-based sample.
(Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). According to AET, Therefore, we proposed for our first hypothesis
negative workplace encounters such as cyber that cyber incivility would be negatively related to
incivility can lead to negative job attitudes. These job satisfaction.
negative attitudes may impact work performance
because they signal that something at work is Incivility and Job Performance
wrong, and they lead individuals to spend copious Previous research has found a link between inci-
amounts of resources thinking about the situation, vility and job performance. Sliter, Jex, Wolford,
thus distracting their efforts from work (Porath & and McInnerney (2010) examined predictors of
Erez, 2007). customer service performance and found that cus-
Previous research has used AET as a theoretical tomer incivility was linked with decreased perfor-
model to understand how incivility in the work- mance. In other words, when employees were faced
place impacts individuals. For example, the study with an uncivil customer and they faked a positive
by Porath and Erez (2007) found that rudeness emotional state, their sales performance decreased.
was associated with higher levels of negative affect Cortina et al. (2001) also linked experiences of
(NA), which in turn was linked to reduced task per- incivility with increased levels of work withdrawal,
formance. Additionally, Bunk and Magley (2013) suggesting that those employees who experience
found a mediating role for job satisfaction in the rude or discourteous behavior may be more likely
relation between incivility and work withdrawal. to disregard specific tasks that are part of their
Therefore, in the current study, we predicted that role, and therefore suffer a decrease in their job
cyber incivility would be associated with decreases performance. Recent research has also linked
in job satisfaction and job performance, and that cyber incivility with task performance. According
job satisfaction would serve as a mediator of the to a study by Giumetti et al. (2013), participants
relation between cyber incivility and performance. who experienced cyber incivility from a supervisor
We develop these hypotheses further below. performed more poorly on a series of math tasks
as compared to experiencing supervisor support.
Incivility and Job Attitudes Based on this existing research, we proposed our
Incivility has been linked with decreases in several second hypothesis, that cyber incivility would be
different job attitudes including organizational negatively related to job performance.
commitment, job satisfaction (Smith, Andrusyszyn,
& Laschinger, 2010), and perceived organizational Job Satisfaction as a Mediator
support (Miner, Settles, Pratt-Hyatt, & Brady, 2012). With AET, one possible mechanism for the reduced
Further, when incivility is removed from a working job performance that victims of cyber incivility
environment, employees tend to show more posi- may experience may be through a reduction in
tive work attitudes. For example, researchers found job satisfaction (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). That
that a workplace civility intervention was able to is, individuals who experience cyber incivility may
both decrease instances of incivility and increase become dissatisfied with their jobs, causing them to
employee job attitudes including trust, organiza- withdraw effort, and therefore their performance
tional commitment, job satisfaction, and profes- decreases. Previous research has supported this
WINTER 2016 sional efficacy (Leiter, Laschinger, Day, & Oore, mediational model linking uncivil workplace
2011), and these improvements were sustained behaviors with decreases in job satisfaction and
PSI CHI
JOURNAL OF 1 year later (Leiter, Day, Oore, & Laschinger, 2012). subsequent decreases in job performance. For
PSYCHOLOGICAL Clearly, much research has demonstrated a example, Porath and Erez (2007) examined
RESEARCH

232 COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204)
Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham | Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance

rudeness and task performance among college stu- course credit was offered to the undergraduate
dents and found that individuals who experienced student sample, whereas no incentives were offered
rudeness during the experiment performed worse to the other samples. Participants were under no
on the anagram tasks, and this was mediated by time limit while taking the survey, and the mean
disruptions to cognitive processing. Additionally, time to complete the survey was 11 min (SD = 8.2
in a study by Lim, Cortina, and Magley (2008), the min). We set the survey data collection options so
researchers found that, among a sample of U.S. that only one submission would be accepted per
federal circuit court employees, participants who computer to control for potential double submis-
experienced personal incivility tended to report sion of surveys. Two-hundred twenty participants
lower levels of satisfaction with their work, which in completed the survey.
turn was related to turnover intentions. Therefore, The mean age of the sample was 31.05 years
we proposed our third hypothesis, that job satisfac- (SDage = 12.40 years), and there were 80 men (37%)
tion would mediate the relationship between cyber and 127 women (58%). In terms of racial/ethnic
incivility and performance. background, most participants were White (n =
171, 82.93%), and the next most common racial
The Role of Positive Affect groups were Black (n = 17, 8.29%), Asian or Pacific
Previous research has identified several variables Islander (n = 10, 4.88%), or “other” (n = 7, 3.41%).
that may buffer the negative impact of stress on Participants were gathered from four different
strain outcomes. One such moderator is PA, or the samples: business professionals contacted through
tendency to experience positive mood states (Wat- an alumni database (n = 39, 17.7% of the sample),
son, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). A study by Vander professional psychologists contacted through an
Elst, Bosman, De Cuyper, Stouten, and De Witte association listserv (n = 84, 38.2% of the sample),
(2013) found that PA buffered the relationship current MBA students in an executive MBA pro-
between job insecurity and psychological distress. gram (n = 6, 3% of the sample), and undergradu-
Other studies have also found a buffering role for ate students from a large southeastern university
positive emotions in stressor-strain relationships (n = 91, 41.4% of the sample). All participants
at work (Grote, Bledsoe, Larkin, Lemay, & Brown, were at least working part-time in the United States
2007; Riolli & Savicki, 2003; Thomas, Britt, Odle- at the time of the survey. Participants worked an
Dusseau, & Bliese, 2011). In each of these studies, average of 33.87 hours per week (SD = 18.78) and
those individuals who experienced higher levels had an average of 4.29 years of experience (SD =
of PA tended to be less susceptible to the negative 5.75). Participants were employed in numerous
outcomes of the stressor. In the current study, we industries including education (28.29%), health
predicted that PA would protect employees from care (12.68%), professional services (8.90%),
the deleterious impact of cyber incivility on job retail trade (6.80%), food service (6.30%), and
satisfaction and job performance (see Figure 1). manufacturing (4.90%). In terms of Internet use at
Therefore, our fourth hypothesis was that the work, 60% of the sample (n = 151) communicated
indirect effect of cyber incivility on both (a) job at least once or twice per week with their coworkers
satisfaction and (b) job performance would be via the Internet. Fifty-three percent of the sample
moderated by PA such that the negative cyber reported using the Internet at least once or twice
incivility-outcomes relationship would be stronger per week to communicate with supervisors (n =
for those individuals low in PA as compared to those 147), and 50% of the sample reported using it this
individuals high in PA. often to communicate with customers (n = 130).

Method Measures
Participants and Procedure Job performance. Perceived job performance was
The Clemson University institutional review measured with one question pertaining to overall
board reviewed and approved the current study. job performance. The specific question was, “In
Participants were recruited via an e-mail invitation the past six months, do you think that your job
that provided a link to the online survey, and they performance has been . . .” and response options
completed it electronically through Survey Mon- were on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from WINTER 2016
key®, an online survey platform. All participants 1 (significantly below normal) to 5 (significantly above
PSI CHI
first completed an informed consent form to normal). This question was answered based on a JOURNAL OF
acknowledge their agreement to participate. Partial self-assessment from the past 6 months. PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH

COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204) 233
Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance | Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham

Job satisfaction. Job satisfaction was measured the measure of PA (α = .91).


with three questions that were similar to the gen- Control variables.
eral satisfaction measure from the Job Diagnostic Job tenure. We included job tenure as a poten-
Survey (Hackman & Oldham, 1974). Participants tial control variable because previous meta-analytic
were asked to indicate the extent to which they research has found a small positive relationship
agreed with three statements using a 5-point Likert between self-ratings of job performance and job
scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly tenure (Ng & Feldman, 2013). We asked par-
agree). The items are “Overall, I feel satisfied with ticipants to indicate how many years of experience
my job,” “I feel happy with my job for the most they have in their current job at their current place
part,” and “I think that my job generally pleases of employment. Participants chose options from a
me.” Together, the three items demonstrated dropdown list ranging from less than 6 months to
good internal reliability (α = .96), so a composite 50 years, in 1-year increments from 1 to 50.
was created by taking the mean of the three items. Workload. Previous research has also linked
Cyber incivility. For the measure of cyber inci- job stress with incivility and outcomes (Cortina et
vility, participants were first given a list of possible al., 2001; Pearson, Andersson, & Porath, 2000), so
behaviors that define the experience of incivility we included workload as a potential control vari-
such as being condescending, making demeaning able. The Quantitative Workload Inventory (QWI;
remarks, ignoring someone, doubting someone’s Spector & Jex, 1998) was used to gauge general job
judgment, or spreading rumors (Cortina et al., stress resulting from work pace and workload. This
2001). Then, participants read a definition, “The measure included five items with a 6-point Likert-
following questions are about being the victim of type response scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (sev-
cyber incivility at work. When we say cyber incivility, eral times per day). Sample questions included “How
we mean incivility that takes place through e-mail, often does your job require you to work very fast?”
instant messaging, in a chat room, on a website, or and “How often do you have to do more work than
through a text message sent to a cell phone.” After you can do well?” Acceptable internal consistency
reading the definition, participants indicated how reliability for the QWI has been demonstrated in
often they experienced cyber incivility through previous research (Spector & Jex, 1998: α = .82)
seven different media in the last 6 months by using and was also found in the current study (α = .87).
a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to Thus, we created a scale composite by taking the
6 (several times per day). The seven media through mean of the five workload items.
which participants might have experienced cyber NA. The final potential control variable that we
incivility were through instant messaging, in a chat included was trait NA. Previous research has sug-
room, on a social networking site, on another type of gested that NA may inflate the relationships between
website, through an e-mail message, through a text job stress and job strain (Brief, Burke, George, Rob-
message sent to a cell phone, or in another way. This inson, & Webster, 1988) and therefore NA should be
measure is similar to existing measures of cyberbully- statistically controlled. In the current study, trait NA
ing (Kowalski & Limber, 2007). Internal consistency was also measured using 10 items from the PANAS
reliability was acceptable for the current study (α = (Watson et al., 1988), using the same response
.82), so the sum of the seven items was taken to rep- scale as noted above for PA. These items included
resent the frequency of cyber incivility in this study. irritable, distressed, and upset. The PANAS has been
PA. Trait PA was measured using the Positive used in previous research to assess NA in relation to
and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et job stress (Spector, Fox, & Van Katwyk, 1999) and
al., 1988). There were 10 items for PA measured has demonstrated good reliability (α = .91). In the
with a 5-point Likert-type response scale ranging current study, internal reliability was good for our
from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). measure of NA (α = .87), so a scale composite was
Participants indicated to what extent they gen- created by taking the mean of the 10 NA items.
erally felt this way. The items for PA included
enthusiastic, excited, and proud. Previous research Analysis
has utilized the PANAS to measure PA in studies Before testing the hypotheses for the current study,
WINTER 2016 focusing on job-related stress and strain (Zellars, we followed guidelines offered by Carlson and Wu
Perrewé, Hochwarter, & Anderson, 2006), and (2012) for identifying which variables to utilize
PSI CHI
JOURNAL OF found acceptable reliability (α = .88). In the current as control variables in the current study. Specifi-
PSYCHOLOGICAL study, internal consistency reliability was good for cally, we examined the zero-order correlations for
RESEARCH

234 COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204)
Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham | Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance

workload, NA, and job tenure with the key variables involve examination of a third variable that impacts
of the current study (cyber incivility, job satisfac- the strength of the relationship between a predictor
tion, and job performance). As noted in Table 1, variable and a criterion variable (Baron & Kenny,
only NA had a significant relationship with cyber 1986). In this case, we identified PA as a variable
incivility, job satisfaction, and job performance. that will impact the strength of the relationships
Both job tenure and workload were unrelated to between cyber incivility and (a) job satisfaction
these variables. Therefore, we included NA as a and (b) job performance. In other words, our
control variable when testing the hypotheses for the analysis tried to answer the question of for whom
current study. Additionally, we conducted a series cyber incivility would be related to decreased job
of one-way ANOVAs to examine whether there were performance, in this case, for individuals low in
differences across the four samples (undergradu- PA (MacKinnon & Luecken, 2008). To test this
ate students, MBA students, business alumni, and hypothesis, we again used hierarchical regres-
professional psychologists) in the primary variables sion analyses and entered NA and data source in
for the current study (cyber incivility, NA, PA, job Model 1, cyber incivility and PA in Model 2. We
satisfaction, and job performance). Results indi- then entered the interaction term in Model 3 and
cated that there were no significant differences examined the change in R2 from Model 2 to Model
across samples in cyber incivility, NA, PA, and job 3 as an indication of a significant interaction (Aiken
satisfaction (all p’s > .05). However, there were & West, 1991).
significant differences in job performance among
the four samples, F(3, 209) = 7.42, p < .001, η2 = .10. Results
Therefore, we included a variable representing the Descriptive statistics, intercorrelations, and scale
sample from which data originated (referred to as reliabilities can be found in Table 1. Prior to testing
data source below) as an additional control variable the hypotheses of the current study, we first exam-
in the analyses. ined the prevalence rate of cyber incivility in the
To test the first two hypotheses that cyber inci- current sample and found that 61.40% reported
vility would be related to job satisfaction and job never experiencing cyber incivility at work, and
performance, respectively, we conducted a series of 38.60% of the sample reported experiencing cyber
hierarchical linear regression analyses (sometimes incivility through at least one medium less than
called sequential regression; see Tabachnick & Fidell, once per month or more. Whereas this prevalence
2013), with NA and data source entered in Model rate may seem low, it aligns with previous research
1 as control variables, and cyber incivility entered on cyber incivility (e.g., Giumetti et al., 2012; Park,
in Model 2. The third hypothesis involved exami- Fritz, & Jex, 2015).
nation of a mediator, job satisfaction. Mediation The first hypothesis was supported because
hypotheses involve examination of an intervening cyber incivility was significantly related to job sat-
mechanism that occurs between some stimulus isfaction while controlling for NA and data source
and some response (Baron & Kenny, 1986). In this (β = -.19, p = .006; see Table 2). The second hypoth-
case, we predicted that the intervening mechanism esis was also supported because cyber incivility
would be job satisfaction that comes between the
experience of cyber incivility and job performance. TABLE 1
In other words, our analysis tried to answer the
question of how cyber incivility would be related to Descriptive Statistics, Intercorrelations, and Internal
job performance, in this case, through job satisfac- Consistency Reliability Values for All Study Variables
tion (MacKinnon & Luecken, 2008). To test the Variable M SD Min Max 1 2 3 4 5 6
third hypothesis, we utilized the PROCESS SPSS 1 Cyber Incivility 8.21 2.78 5 31 .82
script (Hayes, 2013) with 1000 Bias Corrected (BC) 2 Job Tenure 4.31 5.76 0 29 -.06 --
bootstrap samples, with job satisfaction entered as 3 Workload 3.95 1.22 1 6 .07 .21* .87
the mediating variable, and NA and data source 4 Negative Affect 1.80 0.62 1 4 .24* -.06 .20* .87
entered as control variables. We also examined the
5 Positive Affect 3.51 0.76 1.60 5 -.04 .17* .16* -.16* .91
significance test for the indirect effect (Sobel’s z),
6 Job Satisfaction 3.73 1.08 1 5 -.25* .02 -.07 -.30* .31* .96
and we provided confidence intervals for this effect
7 Job Performance 3.62 0.96 1 5 -.19* .00 .08 -.25* .27* .36*
(Holmbeck, 1997).
Finally, the fourth hypothesis involved examina- Note. Cronbach’s alpha values are presented along the diagonal in italics.
tion of PA as a moderator. Moderator hypotheses *Correlation is significant at p < 0.05.

COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204) 235
Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance | Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham

was significantly related to job performance while significant (ab = -.020, BC 95% CIs = -.041, -.009;
controlling for NA and data source ( β = -.14, Sobel z = -2.36, p = .02) while controlling for NA
p = .037; see Table 3). and data source. These results indicated that the
The third hypothesis examined the mediating third hypothesis was supported.
role of job satisfaction in the relationship between To test our moderated mediation models, the
cyber incivility and job performance. Results indi- fourth hypothesis, we began by examining the
cated that the indirect effect of job satisfaction was interactive effect of cyber incivility and PA on job
satisfaction. Results indicated that the model did
not add significant incremental variance (p = .59).
TABLE 2 The second moderated mediation model involved
Hierarchical Regression Results for Cyber Incivility examining the interactive effect of cyber incivility
Predicting Job Satisfaction While Controlling and PA on job performance. Results indicated that
for Negative Affect and Data Source the model explained significant incremental vari-
Variable b SE ß t ∆ R2 ance (ΔR 2 = .020, p = .026) over the main effects,
Step 1: NA, and data source, and the interaction term was
Negative Affect -0.54 0.12 -0.31 -4.53*
significant (β = .145, p = .026). A simple slopes analy-
0.09 sis revealed that the results were in the expected
Data Source -0.03 0.06 -0.04 -0.55
direction (Aiken & West, 1991). When employees
Step 2:
were low in PA (one SD below the M), cyber inci-
Cyber Incivility -0.07 0.03 -0.19 -2.80* 0.03
vility was significantly related to job performance
Note.* p < .05. (t = -2.85, p = .005). However, when employees were
high in PA (one SD above the M), cyber incivil-
ity was not significantly related to performance
TABLE 3 (t = 0.56, p = .577). We plotted the interaction to
Hierarchical Regression Results for Cyber Incivility help with interpretation (see Figure 2). Finally,
Predicting Job Performance While Controlling for we followed recommendations of Hayes (2013)
Negative Affect and Data Source to examine the conditional indirect effects at
Variable b SE ß t ∆ R2 1 standard deviation above and below the mean of
Step 1: PA. When employees were low in PA, the mediated
Negative Affect -0.34 0.10 -.22 -3.23* model was significant (indirect effect = -.03, SE =
.11 .01, CIs = -.07, -.01). However, when employees were
Data Source 0.18 0.05 0.23 3.48*
high in PA, the mediated model was not significant
Step 2:
(indirect effect = -.01, SE = .01, CIs = -.05, .01).
Cyber Incivility -0.05 0.02 -0.14 -2.10* .02
Together, these results suggested that individuals
Note.* p < .05. who are high in PA are less likely to be affected
by cyber incivility and less likely to report lower
levels of job satisfaction or job performance. Thus,
FIGURE 2 partial support was found for the fourth hypothesis
Interaction of Positive Affect and because the first part was not supported (i.e., PA
Cyber Incivility Predicting Job Performance did not moderate the relationship between cyber
incivility and job satisfaction), but the second part
4.5 – was supported (i.e., PA moderated the relationship
4.0 – between cyber incivility and job performance).

Discussion
Job Performance

3.5 –
Incivility has the ability to negatively impact individu-
3.0 –
als in the workplace. Whereas much of the previous
High Postive Affect
2.5 –
Average Positive Affect
research on incivility has focused on its face-to-face
Low Positive Affect form, the current study extended this research by
2.0 –
WINTER 2016 Low Cyber Incivility High Cyber Incivility focusing on so called cyber incivility, which is inci-
vility experienced through electronic means such
PSI CHI
JOURNAL OF
Note. The slope for each line is as follows: low positive affect b = -.10, p = .005, as e-mail, text messages, or instant messaging. By
average positive affect b = -.04, p = .077, high positive affect b = .02, p = .577.
PSYCHOLOGICAL utilizing the AET model, the current study examined
RESEARCH

236 COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204)
Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham | Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance

how cyber incivility can be identified as a stressful benefit of the doubt. Additionally, organizations
event in the workplace and subsequently impact may consider carefully monitoring employee job
a person’s job satisfaction and performance level. attitudes. If employees begin to report lower levels
The results indicated that, when experiencing cyber of job satisfaction, managers may want to intervene
incivility in the workplace, individuals tend to report to learn about the root of the dissatisfaction to help
lower levels of job satisfaction and also report lower ward off a decline in job performance.
job performance levels. These findings supported The results of the current study also suggested
previous research linking face-to-face incivility to that personality plays a role in the stressor-strain
job attitudes (Miner-Rubino & Reed, 2010) and job process. More specifically, given that we found
performance (Sliter et al., 2010). relationships between NA and cyber incivility, job
Additionally, the findings from the current satisfaction, and job performance, future research-
study indicated that job satisfaction served as a ers are encouraged to measure NA in their studies.
mediator of the relationship between cyber incivil- The experience of cyber incivility may partially be
ity and job performance. This suggests that employ- a function of the personality of the e-mail recipi-
ees who are treated poorly at work through such ent because individuals high in NA may be more
acts as cyber incivility may report lower levels of job sensitive to stressors in their environment or more
satisfaction, which in turn may be related to lower likely to experience increased strain reactions as
levels of job performance. However, PA served as a a result of stress in the workplace (Spector, Zapf,
buffer to the negative effects of cyber incivility on Chen, & Frese, 2000). More research is needed
job performance. Specifically, individuals who were on the mechanisms associated with NA and job
high in PA did not report reduced job performance stressor-strain relationships.
following cyber incivility as compared to those From a practical standpoint, the results of the
individuals who were low in PA. These results help current study also suggested that individuals high in
to show how (through job satisfaction) and for trait NA may be more likely to report lower levels of
whom (those low in PA) that cyber incivility may self-rated performance. Thus, managers may wish
be linked with reduced job performance. Lastly, we to take personality traits into consideration when
did not find support for PA as a moderator of the reviewing self-rated employee performance, and be
link between cyber incivility and job satisfaction. prepared to provide additional coaching to these
This suggests that the relationship between cyber individuals, helping them to see their strengths and
incivility and job satisfaction does not depend on a areas for improvement. In addition, our findings
person’s typical levels of positive emotions. indicated that individuals high in PA were less likely
to report decreased job satisfaction and decreased
Practical Implications performance. Therefore, organizations may wish to
The results of the current study have important encourage positive emotional experiences at work
implications for the 21st century world of work. (Fritz & Sonnentag, 2009).
To try to reduce the incidence of cyber incivility,
supervisors and managers might consider imple- Limitations and Future Research Directions
menting online communication training sessions or There are several limitations that should be consid-
interventions to guide individuals to act in appro- ered when interpreting the results of the current
priate ways online. Such training sessions have been study. First, due to all variables being gathered
effective for reducing the incidence of face-to-face via self-reported surveys, response bias may be a
incivility (Leiter et al., 2012), and also reducing concern (McGrath, Mitchell, Kim, & Hough, 2010).
the negative outcomes associated with incivility, In particular, participants might not have reported
so there may be a high likelihood of success in accurately on their current performance levels.
an online context as well. It is important to note, Whereas much previous research has relied on self-
however, that the current study examined perceived report for measuring a person’s own performance
experiences of incivility, rather than objective (Kim & Glomb, 2014), supervisors or others in the
instances of incivility experienced from others, so workplace may be in a better position to observe
it may be the case that supervisors or coworkers are and report on an employee’s levels of job perfor-
not purposely trying to treat them poorly. Along mance. Therefore, future research should not WINTER 2016
these lines, then, other forms of training may also focus entirely on self-reported measures and should
PSI CHI
be helpful such as training employees to be more instead include measures from coworkers, supervi- JOURNAL OF
forgiving, understanding, or giving others the sors, and subordinates. Additionally, researchers PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH

COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204) 237
Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance | Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham

should attempt to gather objective measures of satisfaction at the same time as cyber incivility. One
performance. For example, test scores could be final limitation was that the working adults samples
used with a student sample or sales numbers for were not offered incentives for participation in
salespersons. An additional limitation with the this study. This might have reduced the number of
performance measure used in the current study responses, and previous research has indicated that
was that participants were asked to judge their even small incentives can improve survey response
performance in comparison to normal. However, rates (Rose, Sidle, & Griffith, 2007). Therefore,
we did not define normal, and what is normal for future research should employ a small incentive
one individual may not be the standard for the with surveys sent to employee-based samples.
organization. Another limitation of the current In conclusion, the current study provided
study was that all data were gathered at a single additional support for AET as a theoretical model
time point and no variables were manipulated. linking negative workplace events with job attitudes
Therefore, we cannot establish that cyber incivility and job performance. Specifically, we found that
caused changes in job satisfaction or performance, cyber incivility was linked with lower levels of job
nor can we establish the temporal order of these satisfaction along with lower levels of self-rated job
variables. For example, it could be the case that performance. In addition, we found evidence for
poor performers are more likely to be victims of a possible mechanism (job satisfaction) through
cyber incivility. Additionally, the design for the which cyber incivility may be linked with job per-
current study only provided weak evidence of true formance as well as an individual difference vari-
buffering. Future researchers should examine the able that helped us to understand for whom cyber
cyber incivility phenomenon as well as a PA buffer- incivility may be more impactful (individuals low in
ing hypothesis using longitudinal study designs or PA). Our study also provided additional informa-
experimental manipulations. tion about the prevalence of cyber incivility among
Another possible limitation was that we were working adults because 38.60% of the sample
unable to control for certain personality traits that indicated that they had experienced at least one
may be related to being targeted more often with form of cyber incivility at work in the past 6 months.
cyber incivility. For example, previous research has Given the rapidly increasing use of communica-
indicated that individuals who are low in agree- tions technologies in today’s workplaces (Purcell
ableness report more occurrences of face-to-face & Rainie, 2014), we feel that additional research is
incivility in the workplace than individuals who warranted on this form of interpersonal mistreat-
are high in agreeableness (Milam, Spitzmueller, & ment. Additionally, we encourage employees to
Penney, 2009). Therefore, future research should be mindful of their electronic communications.
take into consideration the personality of the target
References
employee and obtain other reports of cyber incivil-
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
ity. Additionally, the current study only examined interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
one possible correlate of job performance and Andersson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect
of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24,
job satisfaction (cyber incivility), but these out- 452–471. doi:10.2307/259136
come variables may be influenced by additional Brief, A. P., Burke, M. J., George, J. M., Robinson, B. S., & Webster, J.
workplace factors other than cyber incivility. For (1988). Should negative affectivity remain an unmeasured variable in
the study of job stress? Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 193–198.
example, pay (Judge, Piccolo, Podsakoff, Shaw, & doi:10.1037/0021-9010.73.2.193
Rich, 2010), relationships with supervisors (Har- Bunk, J. A., & Magley, V. J. (2013). The role of appraisals and emotions
ris, Harris, & Brouer, 2009), job characteristics in understanding experiences of workplace incivility. Journal
of Occupational Health Psychology, 18, 87–105. doi:10.1037/
(Cheung & Tang, 2010), individual differences a0030987
(Wu & Griffin, 2012), and growth opportunities Byron, K. (2008). Carrying too heavy a load? The communication and
(Ford & Wooldridge, 2012) may influence job miscommunication of emotion by e-mail. Academy of Management
Review, 33, 309–327. doi:10.5465/AMR.2008.31193163
satisfaction. In addition, the level of work-family Carlson, K. D., & Wu, J. (2012). The illusion of statistical control: Control
conflict that an individuals experience may impact variable practice in management research. Organizational Research
the amount of stress individuals might be under Methods, 15, 415–435. doi:10.1177/1094428111428817
Cheung, F., & Tang, C. (2010). The influence of emotional dissonance
and their reported levels of satisfaction and perfor- on subjective health and job satisfaction: Testing the stress–strain–
WINTER 2016 mance (Odle-Dusseau, Britt, & Greene-Shortridge, outcome model. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 3192–
2012; Spector et al., 2007). Thus, future research 3217. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00697.x
PSI CHI Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001).
JOURNAL OF
is needed to look at other external factors that Incivility in the workplace: Incidence and impact. Journal of
PSYCHOLOGICAL may influence a person’s job performance and job Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 64–80. doi:10.1037/1076-
RESEARCH

238 COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204)
Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham | Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance

8998.6.1.64 incivility on employees’ work attitude and behavior. Information and


Ford, M. T., & Wooldridge, J. D. (2012). Industry growth, work role Management, 46, 419–425. doi:10.1016/j.im.2009.06.006
characteristics, and job satisfaction: A cross-level mediation MacKinnon, D. P., & Luecken, L. J. (2008). How and for whom? Mediation
model. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 17, 492–504. and moderation in health psychology. Health Psychology, 27(2,
doi:10.1037/a0029535 Suppl), S99–S100. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.27.2(Suppl.).S99
Fritz, C., & Sonnentag, S. (2009). Antecedents of day-level Madden, M., & Jones, S. (2008). Networked workers. Retrieved from
proactive behavior: A look at job stressors and positive affect http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2008/Networked-Workers.
during the workday. Journal of Management, 35, 94–111. aspx
doi:10.1177/0149206307308911 McGrath, R. E., Mitchell, M., Kim, B. H., & Hough, L. (2010). Evidence for
Giumetti, G. W., Hatfield, A. L., Scisco, J. L., Schroeder, A. N., Muth, E. R., & response bias as a source of error variance in applied assessment.
Kowalski, R. M. (2013). What a rude e-mail! Examining the differential Psychological Bulletin, 136, 450–470. doi:10.1037/a0019216
effects of incivility versus support on mood, energy, engagement, Milam, A. C., Spitzmueller, C., & Penney, L. M. (2009). Investigating
and performance in an online context. Journal of Occupational individual differences among targets of workplace incivility. Journal
Health Psychology, 18, 297–309. doi:10.1037/a0032851 of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 58–69. doi:10.1037/
Giumetti, G. W., McKibben, E. S., Hatfield, A. L., Schroeder, A. N., & a0012683
Kowalski, R. M. (2012). Cyber incivility @ work: The new age of Miner, K. N., Settles, I. H., Pratt-Hyatt, J. S., & Brady, C. C. (2012).
interpersonal deviance. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Experiencing incivility in organizations: The buffering effects of
Networking, 15, 148–154. doi:10.1089/cyber.2011.0336 emotional and organizational support. Journal of Applied Social
Grote, N. K., Bledsoe, S. E., Larkin, J., Lemay, E. P. Jr., & Brown, C. (2007). Psychology, 42, 340–372. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00891.x
Stress exposure and depression in disadvantaged women: The Miner-Rubino, K., & Reed, W. D. (2010). Testing a moderated mediational
protective effects of optimism and perceived control. Social Work model of workgroup incivility: The roles of organizational trust and
Research, 31, 19–33. doi:10.1093/swr/31.1.19 group regard. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 3148–3168.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1974). The job diagnostic survey: doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00695.x
An instrument for the diagnosis of jobs and the evaluation of job Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2013). Does longer job tenure help or
redesign projects (Technical Report No. 4). Retrieved from http:// hinder job performance? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 83, 305–
files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED099580.pdf 314. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2013.06.012
Harris, K. J., Harris, R. B., & Brouer, R. L. (2009). LMX and subordinate Odle-Dusseau, H. N., Britt, T. W., & Greene-Shortridge, T. M. (2012).
political skill: Direct and interactive effects on turnover intentions Organizational work-family resources as predictors of job
and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39, 2373– performance and attitudes: The process of work-family conflict and
2395. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00530.x enrichment. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 17, 28–40.
Harter, J., Agrawal, S., & Sorenson, S. (2014). Most U.S. workers see doi:10.1037/a0026428
upside to staying connected to work. Retrieved from http://www. Pearson, C. M., Andersson, L. M., & Porath, C. L. (2000). Assessing and
gallup.com/poll/168794/workers-upside-staying-connected-work. attacking workplace incivility. Organizational Dynamics, 29, 123–
aspx 137. doi:10.1016/S0090-2616(00)00019-X
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2007). Does rudeness really matter? The effects
conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New of rudeness on task performance and helpfulness. Academy of
York, NY: Guilford Press. Management Journal, 50, 1181–1197. doi:10.2307/20159919
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical Porath, C. L., & Pearson, C. M. (2012). Emotional and behavioral responses
clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from to workplace incivility and the impact of hierarchical status. Journal
the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of of Applied Social Psychology, 42(Suppl 1), E326–E357. doi:10.1111/
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599–610. doi:10.1037/0022- j.1559-1816.2012.01020.x
006X.65.4.599 Purcell, K., & Rainie, L. (2014). Technology’s impact on workers. Retrieved
Johnson, P. R., & Indvik, J. (2001). Rudeness at work: Impulse from http://www.pewinternet.org/2014/12/30/technologys-impact-
over restraint. Public Personnel Management, 30, 457–465. on-workers/
doi:10.1177/009102600103000403 Riolli, L., & Savicki, V. (2003). Optimism and coping as moderators of the
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., Podsakoff, N. P., Shaw, J. C., & Rich, B. L. relation between work resources and burnout in information service
(2010). The relationship between pay and job satisfaction: A meta- workers. International Journal of Stress Management, 10, 235–252.
analysis of the literature. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 77, 157– doi:10.1037/1072-5245.10.3.235
167. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2010.04.002 Rose, D. S., Sidle, S. D., & Griffith, K. H. (2007). A penny for your thoughts:
Kim, E., & Glomb, T. M. (2014). Victimization of high performers: The roles Monetary incentives improve response rates for company-sponsored
of envy and work group identification. Journal of Applied Psychology, employee surveys. Organizational Research Methods, 10, 225–240.
99, 619–634. doi:10.1037/a0035789 doi:10.1177/1094428106294687
Kowalski, R. M., & Limber, S. P. (2007). Electronic bullying among Sliter, M., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J. (2010). How rude!
middle school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, S22–S30. Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and
doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.08.017 employee outcomes. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15,
Leiter, M. P., Day, A., Oore, D. G., & Spence Laschinger, H. K. (2012). 468–481. doi:10.1037/a0020723
Getting better and staying better: Assessing civility, incivility, distress, Smith, L. M., Andrusyszyn, M. A., & Laschinger, H. K. S. (2010). Effects of
and job attitudes one year after a civility intervention. Journal workplace incivility and empowerment on newly-graduated nurses
of Occupational Health Psychology, 17, 425–434. doi:10.1037/ organizational commitment. Journal of Nursing Management, 18,
a0029540 1004–1015. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2834.2010.01165.x
Leiter, M. P., Laschinger, H. K. S., Day, A., & Oore, D. G. (2011). The Spector, P. E., Allen, T. D., Poelmans, S. A. Y., Lapierre, L. M., Cooper, C.
impact of civility interventions on employee social behavior, distress, L., O'Driscoll, M., . . . Widerszal-Bazyl, M. (2007). Cross-national
and attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 1258–1274. differences in relationships of work demands, job satisfaction, and
doi:10.1037/a0024442 turnover intentions with work-family conflict. Personnel Psychology,
Lim, S., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Personal and workgroup 60, 805–835. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00092.x
incivility: Impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied Spector, P. E., Fox, S., & Van Katwyk, P. T. (1999). The role of negative
Psychology, 93, 95–107. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.95 affectivity in employee reactions to job characteristics: Bias effect WINTER 2016
Lim, S., & Tai, K. (2014). Family incivility and job performance: A moderated or substantive effect? Journal of Occupational and Organizational
mediation model of psychological distress and core self-evaluation. Psychology, 72, 205–218. doi:10.1348/096317999166608 PSI CHI
Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 351–359. doi:10.1037/a0034486 Spector, P. E., & Jex, S. M. (1998). Development of four self-report JOURNAL OF
Lim, V. K. G., & Teo, T. S. H. (2009). Mind your E-manners: Impact of cyber measures of job stressors and strain: Interpersonal Conflict at Work PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH

COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204) 239
Cyber Incivility, Job Satisfaction, and Performance | Giumetti, Saunders, Brunette, DiFrancesco, and Graham

Scale, Organizational Constraints Scale, Quantitative Workload Weatherbee, T., & Kelloway, E. K. (2006). A case of cyberdeviancy:
Inventory, and Physical Symptoms Inventory. Journal of Occupational Cyberaggression in the workplace. In E. K. Kelloway, J. Barling, & J.
Health Psychology, 3, 356–367. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.3.4.356 J. Hurrell Jr. (Eds.), Handbook of workplace violence (pp. 445–487).
Spector, P. E., Zapf, D., Chen, P. Y., & Frese, M. (2000). Why negative Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
affectivity should not be controlled in job stress research: Don't throw Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective Events Theory: A
out the baby with the bath water. Journal of Organizational Behavior, theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of
21, 79–95. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200002)21:1<79::AID- affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw, L. L. Cummings (Eds.),
JOB964>3.0.CO;2-G Research in organizational behavior: An annual series of analytical
Suler, J. (2004). The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychology and essays and critical reviews, Vol. 18 (pp. 1–74). US: Elsevier Science/
Behavior, 7, 321–326. doi:10.1089/1094931041291295 JAI Press.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th Wu, C., & Griffin, M. A. (2012). Longitudinal relationships between core
ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson. self-evaluations and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Thomas, J. L., Britt, T. W., Odle-Dusseau, H., & Bliese, P. D. (2011). 97, 331–342. doi:10.1037/a0025673
Dispositional optimism buffers combat veterans from the negative Zellars, K. L., Perrewé, P. L., Hochwarter, W. A., & Anderson, K. S. (2006).
effects of warzone stress on mental health symptoms and The interactive effects of positive affect and conscientiousness on
work impairment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67, 866–880. strain. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 281–289.
doi:10.1002/jclp.20809 doi:10.1037/1076-8998.11.3.281
Vander Elst, T., Bosman, J., de Cuyper, N., Stouten, J., & de Witte, H.
(2013). Does positive affect buffer the associations between job Author Note. Gary W. Giumetti, Lauren A. Saunders, Jeralyn
insecurity and work engagement and psychological distress? A test P. Brunette, Franca M. DiFrancesco, and Paul G. Graham,
among South African workers. Applied Psychology: An International Department of Psychology, Quinnipiac University.
Review, 62, 558–570. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2012.00499.x Correspondence concerning this article should be
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation addressed to Gary W. Giumetti, Department of Psychology,
of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Quinnipiac University, Hamden, CT 06158. E-mail:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. gary.giumetti@quinnipiac.edu
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

WINTER 2016

PSI CHI
JOURNAL OF
PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH

240 COPYRIGHT 2016 BY PSI CHI, THE INTERNATIONAL HONOR SOCIETY IN PSYCHOLOGY (VOL. 21, NO. 4/ISSN 2164-8204)

You might also like