You are on page 1of 16

Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, 148–163

doi:10.1093/deafed/enx057
Advance Access publication March 5, 2018
Empirical Manuscript

EMPIRICAL MANUSCRIPT

Reading Comprehension and Phonics Research:


Review of Correlational Analyses with Deaf and
Hard-of-Hearing Students
Pamela Luft*
Kent State University
*Correspondence should be sent to Pamela Luft, School of Lifespan Development and Educational Sciences, 150 Terrace Drive, 405 White Hall, Kent State
University, Kent, OH 44242-0001, USA (e-mail: pluft@kent.edu)

Abstract
This manuscript reviews 28 studies of reading research on deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) students published since 2000
that used correlational analyses. The examination focused on assessment issues affecting measurement and analysis of
relationships between early phonological or orthographic skills and reading comprehension. Mixed outcomes complicate
efforts to determine evidence-based practices, and to develop an accurate model of reading. Across the 28 studies, DHH
participants represented a wide age range with potential floor and ceiling effects that reduce score variability for valid
correlations. Many studies assessed readers beyond the optimal ages during which early skills develop and are most useful
for reading. Reading skills also were assessed using a diverse array of measures and skill definitions. Particularly for reading
comprehension, word-level and text-level abilities appear to be different constructs. Suggestions include more consistent
skill definitions and differential timing for early- versus later-developing skill assessments to ensure more robust
correlational relationships.

The Search for Evidence-based Practices


DHH students. None of these studies met standards for strong
in Reading
evidence of effectiveness or even possible evidence of effective-
The federal No Child Left Behind Act of (2001) stipulated that edu- ness. Easterbrooks and Stephenson (2006) reviewed 10 literacy
cators use “scientifically-based research” to guide choices regard- practices routinely cited either in the literature or as field-
ing instructional interventions. Subsequently, the Individuals supported practices with DHH students. Several had weak or
with Disabilities Education Act (2004) indicated that education for developing evidence leading the authors to recommend their use
children with disabilities should include the use of scientifically while accumulating further substantiation.
based instructional practices (Section 1400(c)(5)(E)). The difficulty One recommended practice for early readers is that they
for teachers of deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) students is that receive instruction in phonemic awareness and phonics
few scientifically validated practices have been identified. Two (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development,
meta-analyses of literacy research on DHH students found few 2000a, 2000b). For students with typical hearing, phonemic
practices that met recommended standards identified by Cook awareness and letter knowledge have been the two best school-
et al. (2014) and the Institute for Educational Sciences (U.S. entry predictors of how well they learn to read during the first 2
Department of Education, 2003). Luckner, Sebald, Cooney, Young, years of instruction (Cunningham, 2001; Ehri, Nunes, Willows,
and Muir’s (2006) reviewed 22 studies of literacy practices recom- Shuster, Yaghoub-Zadeh, & Shanahan, 2001; National Institute
mended by the National Reading Panel (National Institute of of Child Health and Human Development, 2000b). In contrast,
Child Health and Human Development, 2000a, 2000b) used with research of early phonics-based instruction for DHH individuals

Received August 29, 2017; revisions received November 20, 2017; editorial decision November 21, 2017; accepted November 24, 2017
© The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com.

148

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
P. Luft | 149

has not shown consistent or strong support. Luckner et al.’s unconstrained skills with each encompassing multiple content
(2006) review identified 13 reading practices with large effect domains.
sizes; yet, none included use of phonics. Easterbrooks and Statistical correlations between early- and late-developing
Stephenson (2006) characterized phonics instruction with DHH and across constrained and unconstrained skills are problem-
students as a developing research base and concluded that in- atic in that optimal variabilities between these skill clusters
struction using visual and other support strategies allowed have little timing overlap. Analyses may require sophisticated
some DHH students to develop these skills, although some did causal modeling or exclusion methods in addition to assuring
not. adequate score dispersion (Thompson, Diamond, McWilliam,
As these reviews suggest, individual study outcomes on the Snyder, & Snyder, 2005). Strong correlational analyses require
contributions of phonological skills to reading comprehension adequate variability among identified factors in order to ensure
of DHH children remain mixed. Several have reported signifi- valid relationships. The need to optimize variability for con-
cant relationships (Easterbrooks, Lederberg, Miller, Bergeron, & ducting robust correlational analyses (Gersten et al., 2005;
Connor, 2008; Paul, Wang, Trezek, & Luckner, 2009; Trezek, Thompson et al., 2005) is particularly complicated in identifying
Wang, Woods, Gampp, & Paul, 2007). Yet, others have found low relationships between these reading skill clusters. Phonological
or non-significant correlations (Alvarado, Puente, & Herrara, awareness is an early pre-reading skill with its greatest variabil-
2008; Bélanger, Baum, & Mayberry, 2011; Clark, Gilbert, & ity during the first 18 months of acquisition, typically first grade
Anderson, 2011; Izzo, 2002; Kyle & Harris, 2006; Miller, 2009). and early second grade (Paris, 2005). Other early-acquired and
The effects of early and substantial hearing loss are likely to constrained skills have similar developmental trajectories.
affect and perhaps alter, the acquisition and importance of However early reading comprehension, an unconstrained skill,
phonics-based skills for young DHH readers. Hearing loss re- has low initial variability until students attain initial fluency.
stricts attainment of language fluency if not addressed early Assessment during periods of greatest score variability for early
and intensively (Anderson, 2006; Boudreault & Mayberry, 2006; code-based skills occurs when RC variability is minimal due to
Friedmann & Szterman, 2005; Lederberg, 2003; Marschark, floor effects; and RC achieves score variability after code-based
Schick, & Spencer, 2006; Mayberry, Chen, Witcher, & Klein, 2011; skills are minimally variable due to asymptote (Paris, 2005).
Moeller, Toblin, Yoshinaga-Itano, Connor, & Jerger, 2007; Correlational studies assessing across these categories without
Nicholas & Geers, 2003). Reduced access to the sounds of lan- adequate variation may be a reason for mixed results with DHH
guage likely affect the brain’s utilization for reading such that individuals.
the mixed and inconclusive research reflect unique develop- Wang and Williams’ (2014) meta-analysis identified timing
mental trajectories. Identifying consistent skill relationships for limitations in that phonics- and code-based (early constrained)
DHH students could provide insights into strategies that may skills were more effective up to grade one. After this point, com-
meet standards for evidence-based practice and result in more prehension and mixed interventions on language- and
effective instruction. thinking-based skills yielded greater effect sizes. Other research
has likewise found that mature and experienced readers are
Concerns with Correlational Research of Phonemic less dependent on individual letter codes in comparison with
metacognitive and text-based psycholinguistic reading skills
Awareness and Phonological Skills
(Goodman, 1994/2003; Goodman, Goodman, & Paulson, 2009;
Despite the apparent predictive strength of phonemic aware- Kyle & Harris, 2010; Miller, 2009; Paris, 2005). Differential time-
ness and letter knowledge for most young readers with normal based developmental trajectories appear to effect measurement
hearing, a number of researchers have raised concerns about accuracy when analyzing relationships between various early
the National Reading Panel’s conclusions. Several question their and later-acquired reading skills.
statistical assumptions or the practical significance of research Another complicating factor is that the multiple skill compo-
cited in support of their findings (Allington, 2013; Almasi, Garas- nents comprising reading comprehension affect the acquisition
York, & Shanahan, 2006; Burns, 2003; Hammill & Swanson, processes. Yet, the range of contributing factors may not be con-
2006; Paris, 2005). One important concern is that the variables sistently included in research models that target strong and
used to measure phonemic awareness (PA) and reading com- direct effects. Storch and Whitehurst’s (2002) longitudinal study
prehension (RC) do not demonstrate properties of equal vari- of preschool through fourth grade students found that oral lan-
ability (Paris, 2005). Optimal variability for PA occurs within a guage abilities had strong and direct effects on later reading,
very limited time during acquisition. In contrast, RC develops but only beyond Grade 2. Before this, effects were mediated by
across an individual’s lifespan with increasing and more stable code-related skills so that although oral language abilities were
score variability over time. essential, they had only an indirect role until Grade 3. Another
These statistical properties extend to variables beyond PA outcome was that reading accuracy and reading comprehension
and RC such that reading skills may be divided in two separate were found to be two separate abilities, each of which was influ-
clusters. One cluster consists of constrained skills, character- enced by different skills. The authors noted a danger in empha-
ized by a limited number of elements that typically are learned sizing phonological processing skills to the extent that other
quickly. Young readers progress quickly from floor to asymptote language skills were underestimated.
during early reading instruction. Examples include learning the Other studies have identified effects of language abilities on
26 letter names in English (orthographic awareness, OA) and reading acquisition. Cheung, Chen, Lai, Wong, and Hills (2001)
the 43 letter-sound relationships (phonemic awareness, PA; and found that oral language influenced acquisition of phonological
visually/print-supported phonological skills, PS). The second skills, which also were influenced by orthographic abilities.
cluster consists of unconstrained reading skills that are acquired, Dickinson, McCabe, Anastasopoulous, Peisner-Feinberg, and
developed, and refined over long periods of time. They consist of Poe (2003) identified shortcomings in vocabulary development
multiple components that are utilized in various ways, may that limited aspects of initial literacy development to include
never be entirely mastered, and comprise non-identical content. phonological sensitivity. Language fluency appears to be a poten-
Reading comprehension and vocabulary development both are tially critical, but often under-identified, factor in examining the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
150 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2

development of reading skills. The complexity of statistical The format allows for searches by keywords, title, or author
modeling to reflect both direct and indirect effects, and changing with additional features to further limit the search, as needed.
relationships over time leaves most studies unavoidably imper- Searches used a range of terms for hearing loss and deafness,
fect (Thompson et al., 2005). reading comprehension, and phonological skills in both the key-
The contributions of language fluency to reading skills is of word and title functions. Each article’s reference list was re-
particular importance for DHH individuals. Childhood delays in viewed for additional articles to ensure a comprehensive pool.
achieving a first language affect both functional and structural Articles were eliminated that did not include an element of cor-
development of the brain with neuroanatomical differences relational analyses (to include regression) in their statistical
that contrasted with those due to auditory deprivation (Mayberry procedures, or did not include either phonological or reading
et al., 2011; Pénicaud et al., 2013). MacSweeney, Waters, Brammer, comprehension skills.
Woll, and Goswami (2008) found comparable neural networks Due to the paucity of research in this low-incidence popula-
that supported phonological similarity judgments made in both tion category, studies were not confined to a single language
English and British Sign Language but which were negatively (English) but included other alphabetic languages. The acquisi-
impacted by delayed language acquisition. DHH children’s often- tion of phonological processes to support reading skills has
diminished linguistic abilities likely affect acquisition of phono- been identified as similar across alphabetic languages (Cheung
logical and code-based processing, and a range of associated et al., 2001; Goswami, Ziegler, & Richardson, 2005). Specifically,
reading skills. Yet, early indirect relationships are not consis- phonological processes have been found to remain similar
tently assessed in studies of reading development (Storch & across languages despite some variation in letter-sound consis-
Whitehurst, 2002) and may not be of sufficient strength to meet tency. Some of these differences are pronunciation of letter and
standards for evidence-based practice, potentially eliminating a letter clusters in Greek, Italian, German, Spanish which are
critical factor in research efforts. more often consistent, whereas English, French, and Hebrew
An additional issue regards the assessment of research vari- orthographies have less consistency (Goswami et al., 2005).
ables. Luckner et al. (2006) found that across 22 studies and 40 However, the reading acquisition processes use similar alpha-
years of literacy investigations, no two studies examined the betic learning methods.
same dimensions of literacy. In that even a single construct can Each article was examined to identify: (a) the age of partici-
be measured in multiple ways greatly complicates efforts at pants, (b) the analytical methods used, (c) the reported correla-
cross-study comparisons and accruing a body of evidence- tional results, (d) the study conclusions regarding PA/PS, OA,
based practices. For example, within the domain of reading with RC, and (e) measures of these skills. The target age for in-
comprehension, Storch and Whitehurst (2002) found that read- struction in examining early-developing skills was defined as
ing accuracy and text comprehension were separate abilities. the 18 months for optimal PA/PS variability which coincides
Hannon (2012) reported weak relationships between lower, with the period of intensive OA instruction during first and early
word-level skills in comparison with higher-level reading pro- second grade, calculated to be between 6 and 8 years of age for
cesses. She suggested that word reading was a construct sepa- the DHH population. This also is consistent with studies indic-
rate from text-level comprehension skills and each required ating that early constrained skills were most effective through
differentiated instruction. Burns (2003) similarly identified word first grade (National Institute of Child Health and Human
reading and contextualized reading as very different skills. Development, 2000b; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002; Wang &
Overall, the developmental processes for acquiring reading Williams, 2014).
skills in DHH students appears to be multifaceted and complex, Examination of the articles could not verify that measure-
and potentially unique. The present study sought to re-examine ment of phonological awareness (PA) utilized an auditory-only
reading research on DHH children conducted since 2000 that presentation and therefore, the review combined PA with pho-
utilized correlational analyses to examine relationships nological skills (PS). The range of PA/PS are consistently early
between PA/PS, OA, and RC. An examination of timing and developing and infrequently utilized by mature readers
assessment issues could clarify reasons for mixed and inconclu- (Goodman et al., 2009; Goodman, 1994/2003; National Institute
sive research results with DHH students. A more accurate of Child Health and Human Development, 2000b; Wang &
model could offer a potential pathway toward evidence-based Williams, 2014). In addition, a number of commonly used tests
and effective instructional strategies and improved reading out- combine PA with other PS in order to create composite scores of
comes for these individuals. sufficient statistical variability (Comprehensive Test of
The research questions examined the timing of assessing Phonological Processing, Dynamic Indicators of Basic Literacy
component skills of reading, and the measures used to identify Skills, the Texas Primary Reading Inventory, the Phonological
and examine these skills. They are as follows: Awareness Survey; Paris, 2005). A number of studies included
OA in their analyses, also an early-developing constrained skill
1. What is the age-based timing of measurements in correlat-
so this was added to the analyses, when present.
ing early-developing and constrained PA/PS and OA skills
with later-developing and unconstrained skills of reading
comprehension in DHH individuals? Results
2. In what ways are these reading skill components measured?
The megadatabase search and examinations of reference sec-
tions of potential studies resulted in a total of 28 journal articles
that utilized correlation and/or regression analyses. Table 1 lists
Method
the studies and the components examined. The primary lan-
This review identified research articles published between 2000 guage of the participants indicated that 18 used English and the
and 2017 that utilized correlational analyses to examine reading remaining used a variety of languages, all of which had alpha-
skills of DHH individuals. The search used the university li- betic orthography.
brary’s megadatabase “Discovery”, a service that performs a Research question one examined the ages of the participants
simultaneous search of over 300 individual research databases. in measuring PA/PS, OA, and RC which affects statistical

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
Table 1. Studies of reading comprehension with PA and OA

Study Participants Method Reading variables Correlation or regression results Study conclusions

Alvarado, • 28 deaf children, 7–16 years • ANOVA • RC: reading level Correlation • Orthography and fingerspelling offer
Puente, and old • Correlation • Phonology • Sig: Reading level with sign language visual processing strategies for reading
Herrara (2008) • 15 hearing children of for • Regression • Speechreading abilities (p < .01); with orthography comprehension in conjunction with
level control • Orthography (p < .05) sign language
• Fingerspelling • NS: reading level with phonology, with
• Sign language speechreading, with visual similarity
Regression:
• Sig: age with sign language to predict
reading (R2 = .84)

Bélanger, Baum, • 29 Deaf adults, 22–55 years • ANOVA • RC: reading level—groups Regression • Less-skilled deaf readers’ reading
and Mayberry • 16 hearing controls • Linear regression defined by median split • NS: phonological prediction of reading difficulties are not caused by the lack of
(2011) • Phonology level (R2 = .001, p = .86) use of phonological codes
• Speechreading
• Orthography

Clark, Gilbert, • 50 DHH college participants • ANOVA • RC: English reading fluency Correlation • No clear link between PA, reading, and
and Anderson • 51 hearing college • Correlation • PA • Sig: ASL skills with total score, (r = .34, decoding skills.
(2011) participants • Morphological knowledge p = .01); ASL with monomorphemic • Some deaf students demonstrate
• ASL (bilingual) Proficiency words (r = .30, p = .03) and with phonological knowledge and skills
multimorphemic words correct (r = .35,
p = .01)
• NS: ASL with phonology

Colin, Magnan, • 21 deaf 6-year-old • 2 year • RC: written word test Regression • PA was a significant correlate even after
Ecalle and prereaders longitudinal study • Phoneme identification • Sig: word recognition score predicted by controlling for hearing loss
Leybaert • 21 age-matched hearing • ANOVA • Speech intelligibility rhyme decision task (R2 change =28.3%)
(2007) children • Regression • Rhymes

Cupples, Ching, • 101 5-year-old DHH • Correlation • RC: word and nonword Correlation: • PA predicts aspects of early reading in
Crowe, Day, children, 71 with hearing • Multiple reading, passage • NS: PA with passage comprehension 5-year olds with HL who use speech
and Seeto aids, 30 with implants regression comprehension Regression: • Phonological skills are most important
(2013) • PA • Sig: PA with real-word reading, with in the earliest stages of reading
• Letter knowledge word attack, with letter knowledge • Vocabulary at 5 years may be associated
• Receptive vocabulary (ps < .001) when controlling for HA/CI, with reading at later stages of
communication, cognitive ability, development
receptive vocabulary, and demographic
variables

Daigle and • 24 DHH children who used • ANOVA— • RC zigzag test Correlation • Syllabic sensitivity correlated with age
• Graphemic (homophone) • Sig: Age with reading (r = .726, p < .001);

P. Luft
Armand sign language, 10–18 years repeated and reading scores
(2008) in 3 age groups measures similarity of pseudowords DHH graphemic sensitivity with age • DHH had better scores when using both
• 24 age-matched hearing • Correlation • Syllabic similarity (r = .492, p = .015), with reading (r = .719, orthography and phonology
peers • Response time p < .001); DHH syllabic sensitivity with • Not all DHH show graphemic sensitivity

|
age (r = .485, p = .016), with reading

151
(r = .599, p = .002)

(Continued)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
Table 1. (Continued)

152
Study Participants Method Reading variables Correlation or regression results Study conclusions

|
Daza, Phillips- • 30 DHH students 8–16 years; • Correlation • RC: sentence-picture Correlation • Good readers were better at spoken

Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2
Silver, Ruiz- 23 used spoken language, 7 • Covariates: matching and sentence • Sig: partial correlations—reading language, vocabulary, spatial attention,
Cadra, and used sign language; 50% demographic or completion comprehension with vocabulary (r = .56; visuospatial short-term and working
López-López used CIs, 50% used HAs clinical variables • PA: rhyme judgment p = .003), with spatial memory (r = .47; memory, and executive functions.
(2014) to differentiate • Vocabulary p = .014), with visuospatial memory span • Differences in RC of readers was not
between good and • Visual attention and (direct order: r = .63; p < .001) related to PA skills.
poor readers memory

Dillon, de Jong, • 27 DHH children 6–14 years • Comparison of • RC: letter-phoneme match, Correlation • Percentile ranks were in the bottom half
and Pisoni (8 = K-2), all used CIs and standard scores word and nonword reading • Sig: PA scores with all reading: word on PA and reading
(2012) spoken language • Correlation sentence reading reading (r = 1.82, p < .001); word attack • Students had poorer vocabulary scores
• PA: isolated sounds, (r = 1.74, p < .001); PIAT-RC; PIAT-Total despite relatively high reading
monosyllables & syllable (r = 1.86, p < .001), (r = 1.85, p < .001) • Older readers were generally less
count successful
• Vocabulary

Dyer, • 49 DHH, mean age 13 years, • Correlation • RC: cloze Correlation • Deaf readers can make use of
MacSweeney, RA ≈ 7 years; all use BSL • PA and decoding • Sig: Rhyme with reading delay (r = –0.30, phonological structure to some extent
Szczerbinski, and TC • Picture rhyme p < .05), with RAge (r = .39, p < .01); RAge in reading.
Green, and • 81 hearing controls CA • Pseudohomophones with pseudohomophones (r = .46, • Strong relationship between
Campbell matched & RA matched • Rapid automatized naming p < .01); Rapid naming-sign and reading pseudohomophone task, reading, and
(2003) groups • Speech or sign repetition delay (r = –.47, p < .01) (unexpected IQ in the BSL-first language subgroup
direction)
• NS: Reading delay with the
pseudohomophone matching

Easterbrooks, • 44 DHH children, 3–6 years, • T-tests Fall/Spring • RC: passage comprehension Correlation • All measures generally correlated with
Lederberg, 32 in oral-only classes, 7 in • Correlation (graphics and words) • Sig: Age with all raw scores and between Spring word identification and RC
Miller, TC, 5 in bilingual/TC classes • PA measures: Negative for age with passage • Majority of children performed poorly,
Bergeron, and • Speech perception comprehension (-.67), with letter-word particularly on rhyming words with
Connor (2008) • Rhyming alliteration identification (-.58), with vocabulary little improvement; DHH gaps increased
• Syllable-segmentation (-.30) with age
• Phonological processing

Furlonger, • 30 DHH adults, ages 18-61, • ANOVA lo/hi • Word reading Correlation • More proficient deaf readers were better
Holmes, and profound HL, 17 preferred reading groups • RC: word reading, passage • Sig: PGC with word reading (r = .41, at word reading
Rickards Auslan, 13 preferred spoken using medial split reading with comprehension p < .05); word reading with RC (r = .85, • Both deaf groups made rhyme
(2014) • Hearing controls matched • Correlation questions p < .001) judgments above the level of chance.
on age, gender, & NVIQ. • Linear regression • PA: phoneme, syllable, • NS: PGC and RC (r = .24) • Deaf participants showed a marked
rhyme; phoneme-grapheme Regression effect for orthographic condition; less-
match • Sig: Three PA measures with reading skilled readers showed greater reliance
• Sign comprehension (at comprehension, (F [3, 27] = 10.17, on orthography
ceiling). p < .001); PA measures with orthography
• Response time information, (F [3, 27] = 10.84, p < .001);
rhyme efficiency was the only significant
unique predictor of RC (14% & 9%) across
tasks

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
Geers (2003) • 181 DHH, 8–9 years; all • Correlation • RC: modified cloze, sentence Correlation • Reading outcome was most highly
implanted by 5 ½ years; 98 • Multiple with picture • All significant (p < .0001) for reading with predicted by linguistic competence.
using oral and 83 using regression • Lexical and rhyming tasks— homophones, rhyme, and digit span • Reading processing skills of word
Total Communication. orthographic or Regression attack, word recognition, and
phonological strategies • Variance in reading principal educational placement did not make a
components score predicted by multiple substantial contribution to reading
student and demographic factors outcomes after demographic
characteristics were accounted for.

Gibbs (2004) • Group 1: 15 DHH, moderate • Correlation • RC: word recognition Correlation • Ensuring use of language in advance of
HL, 7–9 years; • Hierarchical • PA • Sig: Vocabulary with word recognition reading would reduce potential barriers
• Group 2: 15 DHH children regression to • Vocabulary (r = .64, p < .01); vocabulary with word and support acquisition of PA
6;2–7;10, all in mainstream partial out • Syntax reading (r = .613, p < .01). • Development of PA may be contingent
and primary language was variance in • NS: Initial phonemes or rhymes with on vocabulary
spoken English 3 groups of reading due to PA. single-word reading; DHH reading with • Some reading may be possible without
30 hearing children controls PA, with memory span closely associated phonological skills.
Regression
• Vocabulary accounted for a further 27%
of the variance in word recognition
(F = 7.13; p < .01) following awareness of
rhyme scores, 26% of the variance
(F = 7.26; p < .01) following awareness of
initial phonemes scores
• Rhyme scores and initial phonemes
accounted for 19.5% (F = 4.0; p < .05) and
12% (F = 5.46; p < .05) of the variance in
word reading

Harris and • Two groups of 30 DHH • ANOVA • Reading age: word reading Regression • Orthography was a significant predictor
Moreno (2004) British children, 7–8 & 13–14 • MANOVA • OA • Sig: Reading of younger deaf by age of reading ability for both older and
years • Regression • Phonology-spelling (p < .05) and orthography (p < .01); younger deaf children
• CA and RA matched hearing reading of older deaf for memory span • Phonology was not a significant
peers (p < .01) and orthography (p < .05); predictor for younger, and marginally
phonology was marginally significant so for older DHH children
(p = .06) • Results suggest little DHH reliance on
phonological coding

Izzo (2002) • 29 primary DHH students, • Descriptive • RC: retelling Correlation • Reading ability was significantly
4.33–13.16 years, from analyses • Age • Sig: Reading with language ability = .58 (p correlated to language ability but not to
residential schools (unlikely • Multiple • Language ability (Signed ≤.001); with age = .50 (p ≤.001); Language PA
to use phonological coding regression English-to-ASL continuum) ability with age = .51 (p ≤.001) • PA may not facilitate reading
strategies) • PA • NS: Reading with PA (r = .09), language development for DHH who use other
ability with PA (r = .03) strategies
Regression • Some DHH with low PA achieved high

P. Luft
• NS: PA with reading reading comprehension
• Sig: Language, age, and PA (R2 = .397,
p = .005); Individual predictors of

|
language ability (p = .025)

153
• NS: Individual predictors: age (p = .1280)
and PA (p = .665)

(Continued)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
Table 1. (Continued)

154
Study Participants Method Reading variables Correlation or regression results Study conclusions

|
Johnson and • 43 DHH, 5–15 years divided • ANOVA • RC: single-word reading, Regression • CI age was associated with

Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2
Goswami into early/late implant • Multiple word chains, read-aloud • Sig: Composite of 4 reading scores with development of oral language, auditory
(2010) groups. Regression with comprehension PA when entered before receptive memory, and PA skills necessary for
• 16 HA user controls questions vocabulary; age at cochlear implantation developing efficient word recognition
• 19 RA hearing controls, 6–9 • PA: rhyme, initial and final with vocabulary and reading outcomes skills. There is a benefit to earlier
years phonemes when using quotient scores; RC with CI, implantation.
• Vocabulary controlling for age & NVIQ: rhyme =.584
• Visual and auditory memory (p < .001), initial phoneme =.364 (p < .05),
• Auditory discrimination final phoneme =.594 (p < .001)
• Speech intelligibility • Receptive vocabulary: up to 38% of
• Speechreading of single additional variance in each reading
words quotient measure
• NS: PA with reading composite scores
after removing variance due to
vocabulary

Koo, Crain, • 51 DHH college students • ANOVA across 5 • RC: silent word reading, Correlation • Deaf native ASL users had the lowest PA
LaSasso, and grouped into native users of groups passage comprehension • Sig: PDT with passage comprehension accuracy (p < .05).
Eden (2008) ASL, cued speech, oral • Correlation • Phoneme Detection (p < .006) • Silent word reading utilized more sight
• Hearing native users of ASL, • Sig: Negative RC with reaction time for words and perhaps was NS for that
and hearing native speakers PDT, with silent word reading (τ = −.291, reason.
of English p < .01)
• NS: word-recognition fluency with PA

Kyle and Harris • 29 British deaf children 7-8 • Descriptive • RC: single-word reading, Correlation • DHH and hearing children did better
(2006) years analyses sentence comprehension • Sig: PA with speechreading (r = .46, when rime items were orthographically
• Hearing controls matched • ANOVA • PA p < .05); vocabulary with SWR (r = .46, and phonologically congruent
for RA • Correlation • Speechreading p < .05), with sentences (r = .70, p < .01), • After controlling for hearing loss,
• Spelling with speechreading (r = .48, p < .01). productive vocabulary and
• Vocabulary • NS: PA with SWR, sentences, or spelling speechreading (language factors) were
significant predictors of reading
• PA did not correlate with RC after
controlling for hearing loss

Kyle and Harris • 29 DHH children, 7-8 years • 3-year • RC: single-word reading, Regression • Vocabulary was the strongest and most
(2010) of age; 7 = oral, 18 = BSL, 4 = longitudinal study cloze sentence • Sig: Word reading at T1 with hearing loss consistent predictor of all reading
combination with 4 assessment comprehension, passage (R2 = .77), with word reading at T2; measures across all time periods
periods comprehension productive vocabulary at T1 accounted • PA not a significant longitudinal
• Correlation • PA for an additional 12% of sentence predicator after Time1 reading levels
• Multiple • Speechreading comprehension at T2; speechreading • DHH children may acquire phonology
regression • Productive vocabulary with productive vocabulary at T1 as a consequence of reading with early
contributed a further 4% and ≈6%, reading associated with later PA
respectively
• NS for Word reading at T2: PA at T1,
hearing loss, and speechreading at T1

Kyle and Harris • 24 British DHH children, 5–6 • 2-year • RC: single-word reading Correlation • Vocabulary at T1 was the most
(2011) years longitudinal • PA • Sig: PA with speechreading vocabulary, consistent significant correlate of
spelling, and word reading (ps < .01)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
• 23 hearing children 5–6 comparative • Letter name and letter- • NS: PA at T1 was not significantly related reading at T2 and T3 even after
years, matched for word study sounds to reading at T2 or T3 controlling for NVIQ and T1 reading.
recognition • Descriptive • Picture spelling • Sig: Reading score at T1 with PA at T2 • Earlier PA was not a longitudinal
analyses • Productive vocabulary (p < .05) correlate of reading after controlling for
• Correlation • Speechreading earlier levels
• ANOVA • Young DHH readers used a whole-word
strategy and 2 years later used a more
alphabetic strategy.

Luetke- • 31 DHH students, 7.9–17.9 • ANCOVA • RC-passage comprehension, Correlation • Reading strongly correlated with
Stahlman and years, no CIs; 9 in general • Correlation word comprehension, word • Sig: Word passage with word blending phonemes and syllables,
Nielsen (2003) education, 22 in self- identification comprehension (r = .970, p = .0001), with segmenting sentences into words, and
contained • PA word identification (r = .832, p = .0001), written English.
• Receptive, expressive and with phonemic substitution (r = .826, • Length of Signed English exposure did
written English p = .0001) not yield higher reading measures or
• Sig: RC with blending phonemes written language.
(r = .723, p = .0001), with syllables • Deaf students may use some different
(r = .720, p = .0001), with written reading strategies.
language (r = .702, p = .0001), with letter
identification (r = .700, p = .0001).

Miller (2009) • 31 Israeli high school and • Correlation • RC of word pairs Correlation • No significant evidence that DHH
post-graduate students • MANOVA • PA and phonological • NS: PA, response time, and error rates participants processed word pairs with
with prelingual deafness decoding overall and for each study condition: less efficiency across conditions
• 59 hearing students • Response time visual, phonological, and control; DHH • Phonemic skills do not significantly
for sentence comprehension, response impact reading
time, and error rate

Miller (2010) • 83 Israeli prelingually deaf, • MANOVA • RC: sentences and Correlation • DHH participants did not demonstrate
21 in primary school (3rd– • Correlation questions, semantically • Sig: OA with PA (r = .54, p < .001), by PA growth over time
4th), 36 in high school • Cluster analysis plausible and implausible grade (p ≤ .05); SI with SP sentences • Sentence-level processing showed
(10th–11th), 26 university sentences (r = .56, p < .001); PA and OA with all individual word meaning using
(21–29 years); • PA sentence types (r = .48, p < .001; r = .36, integration with syntactic (structural)
• 85 control: 29 primary, 29 • OA p < .001) knowledge
high school, 27 university • OA plays a central role in processing
written text; youngest DHH were non-
strategic in their reading
• DHH participants had poorer PA and OA
than hearing peers

Miller and • 40 prelingually deaf Arab, ½ • MANOVA • RC: word reading Correlations • Young and old DHH categorized real
Achmed from primary (9.17–11.08 • Correlation • Word categorization: real • Sig: Categorization accuracy across real words significantly more accurately
(2009) years) and half from word and and pseudo conditions for young deaf than pseudohomophones which
middle-to high school pseudohomophones (r = .84, p < .001) and older deaf (r = .60, improved developmentally
(14.25–16.00 years) • Rapid word naming p < .01); real word and • Older deaf and hearing did not differ in

P. Luft
• 40 control group pseudohomophone categorization for recognition of real words, which was
young (r = .39, p < .05) and older deaf not related to phonological
(r = .37, p = .05) development

|
• NS: categorization accuracy associated • Older DHH had rates equal to hearing

155
with speed of processing for the real- controls
word condition

(Continued)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
Table 1. (Continued)

156
Study Participants Method Reading variables Correlation or regression results Study conclusions

|
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2
Miller, Kargin, • 213 DHH, 6th–10th grade; • Cluster analysis • RC: sentence Correlations • Failure to find significant positive
Guldenoglu, Hebrew, Arabic, English, • ANOVA comprehension with • Sig: RC of the 2 sentence types for correlations between PA, phonological
Rathmann, German participants • Correlation questions: semantically syntactic readers (r = .47, p < .001) word decoding skills, and RC.
Kubus, plausible & implausible. • Sig: Negative for syntactic readers for • Reading skills appear to develop
Hauser, and • Phonology: phonological processing with overall independently of phonological
Spurgeon pseudohomophones and sentence comprehension scores processing skills.
(2012) real words (r = −.25, p < .05) • Syntactic deficits offer an explanation
• Response time • NS: RC for semantic or unspecified for difficulties in reading of DHH
• Native written language strategy readers; phonemic processing students
• Strategies: syntactic, among the 3 reader profiles despite large
semantic, or unspecified intergroup differences

Most, Aram, and • 42 DHH, 62–84 months (5–7 • ANONVA • RC: word recognition & Correlations • PA, letter ID, knowledge and vocabulary
Andorn (2006) years) in 3 placements • Correlation explanation of choice • Sig: PA with vocabulary, with general was better for those in individual
• 11 hearing peers • PA knowledge, with writing, with reading inclusion placements
• OA explanations (ps < .01), with letter • No statistically significant differences
• Letter identification identification (p < .05) between individual and group inclusion
• Word writing programs regarding reading, writing, or
• Receptive vocabulary OA

Spencer and • 72 pediatric CI users, tested • Correlation • RC: word comprehension, Correlation • Standard scores for reading were in the
Oleson (2008) Simultan-eous (speech & SE) • Multiple word identification, word • Sig: Speech production measures with low average range for word
Approximately 5.1–11.3 years regression antonyms & synonyms, each other (r > .73, p < .0001) identification and passage
(48 months after implant) analogy Multiple Regression comprehension
• Speech perception: word- • Sig: Word reading with correct • Both speech perception and production
picture identification, vowel phonemes produced (short-long & skills were strongly correlated with
perception retell), with speech perception, with age word identification and passage
• Speech production: sentence at testing (R2 = .59); paragraph comprehension; early speech skills may
repetition, story retelling comprehension with correct phonemes predict later reading
• Demographic background produced, consonant test, speech
perception (R2 = .62)

Spencer and • 29 CI children, 7;2–17;8; • RC: word comprehension, Correlation • The A/V condition was significantly
Tomblin • 32 hearing controls HC, word attack • Sig: Elision: with word attack (r = .63, correlated with two word reading tasks
(2009) matched on mothers • PA p = .01), with word reading (r = .70, and may be a more accurate measure of
education & word • Rhyme judgment p = .01) phonological processing.
comprehension, nearly 2 • Phonological memory • Sig: Blending and A/V condition with • CI children may hear some sounds (PA)
years younger than DHH • Rapid letter and number word attack (r = .42, p = .05), with word then learn to associate full
naming reading (r = .37, p = .05) pronunciation after seeing print form
• A/O (auditory only) or A/V • Sig: Nonword repetition with A/V (r = .41, (RC); they may learn PA from print (PS)
(auditory-visual) conditions p = .05, r = .38, p = .05); with rapid letter rather than the reverse
naming (r = .49, p = .01, r = .75, p = .01)
• NS: rapid letter naming, nonword
repetition with A/O

Note. CI = cochlear implant; Sig: = statistically significant; NS = non-significant.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
P. Luft | 157

variability needed for correlational analyses. Few studies re- The second research question examined measurements
ported variabilities of factors and therefore, this could not be used for targeted reading skills. PA/PS were measured by tests
directly examined. Across the 28 studies there was broad age of phoneme detection, elision, blending, matching, phonologi-
range. Half (n = 14) used participants that were within the target cal similarity, use of words and nonwords, rhyme decision &
ages; however, many included those who were above or below generation, syllabic similarity, and spelling. Not all studies
these ages. Three studies included younger participants who examined OA skills; however, those that did also used a variety
were potentially just beginning early reading skill acquisition. of measures to identify: letters in words, contrasting ortho-
Seven studies included older participants expected to have graphically similar & dissimilar words & nonwords, phoneme-
achieved asymptote for early constrained skills. Four studies grapheme correspondence, lexical & rhyming tasks, words in
included both below and above target-age participants. Only letter strings (word chains), and rapid letter and number nam-
three studies used participants solely within the target ages, ing. A number of studies identified letter naming as an early
with 11 using participants who were entirely outside of the tar- reading skill and did not report these outcomes as a component
get ages, 10 of which were older individuals. The “Participants” of OA (Easterbrooks et al., 2008; Geers, 2003; Most et al., 2006).
column of Table 1 lists the ages of the participants for each The present study did not re-categorize outcomes or reclassify
study. measures if not so reported in the original articles.
The examination of ages with correlations indicated that of Studies also used a variety of measures for reading compre-
the 17 studies reporting significant correlations between PA/PS hension skills. Alvarado et al. (2008) defined this skill based on
and RC, 1 study used below target-age participants (Cupples academic level while controlling for age. Izzo (2002) assessed
et al., 2013); 3 used both younger and target-age participants reading comprehension through story retelling and while
(Easterbrooks et al., 2008; Kyle & Harris, 2011; Most et al., 2006); Spencer & Oleson (2008) also used story retelling, it was to
1 used only target-age participants (Colin et al., 2007); 2 used assess speech/phonemic production skills. Several studies mea-
younger, target age, and older participants (Johnson & sured RC through letter identification, word and/or pseudoword
Goswami, 2010; Spencer & Oleson, 2008); 4 used target age and reading/identification, or word chain tests (Colin et al., 2007;
older participants (Dillon et al., 2012; Geers, 2003; Luetke- Cupples et al., 2013; Dillon et al., 2012; Easterbrooks et al., 2008;
Stahlman, & Nielsen, 2003; Spencer & Tomblin, 2009); and 6 Furlonger et al., 2014; Geers, 2003; Gibbs, 2004; Harris & Moreno,
used older participants (Daigle & Armand, 2008; Dyer et al., 2004; Johnson & Goswami, 2010; Koo et al., 2008; Kyle & Harris,
2003; Furlonger et al., 2014; Koo et al., 2008; Miller & Achmed, 2006, 2010, 2011; Luetke-Stahlman & Nielsen, 2003; Miller &
2009; Miller, 2010). Achmed, 2009; Miller, 2009; Spencer & Tomblin, 2009). Most
A total of 12 studies reported non-significant correlations et al. (2006) used word recognition to examine RC but asked par-
between PA/PS and RC including 2 that used participants within ticipants to explain their answer. Of the 18 studies using letter
the target-age range (Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010); 5 that used and word-based measures, all but 5 reported significant correla-
within and beyond target-age participants (Alvarado et al., 2008; tions between PA/PS and RC (Gibbs, 2004; Harris & Moreono,
Daza et al., 2014; Gibbs, 2004; Harris & Moreno, 2004; Izzo, 2002); 2004; Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010; Miller, 2009).
and 5 that used only participants beyond the target age A number of the studies used text-level reading to assess RC
(Bélanger et al., 2011; Clark et al., 2011; Koo et al., 2008; Miller such as sentence and short-passage cloze procedures (Cupples
et al., 2012; Miller, 2009) with Koo et al. (2008) reporting both sig- et al., 2013; Daza et al., 2014; Dyer et al., 2003; Geers, 2003; Koo
nificant and non-significant correlations. Bélanger et al. (2011) et al., 2008; Kyle & Harris, 2010; Luetke-Stahlman & Nielsen,
also used linear regression analyses, finding non-significant 2003; Spencer & Oleson, 2008) with two of these eight reporting
relationship of phonological skills for predicting reading levels non-significant PA/PS and RC correlations (Daza et al., 2014;
of older (adult) participants (R2 = .001, p = .86). Kyle & Harris, 2010). Others used longer text with RC scores
Six studies reported correlations between orthographic aware- based on comprehension questions, correct choices, or pictures
ness (OA) and RC. Significant correlations included two studies (Bélanger et al., 2011; Clark et al., 2011; Cupples et al., 2013;
using target age and older participants (Alvarado et al., 2008; Harris Daigle & Armand, 2008; Daza et al., 2014; Dillon et al., 2012;
& Moreno, 2004); and three using older participants (Bélanger et al., Easterbrooks et al., 2008; Furlonger et al., 2014; Johnson &
2011; Daigle & Armand, 2008; Furlonger et al., 2014). One study Goswami, 2010; Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010; Luetke-Stahlman &
reported non-significant correlations for participants beyond Nielsen, 2003; Miller et al., 2012; Miller, 2009; Miller, 2010;
the target ages (Miller, 2010). Spencer & Oleson, 2008). Of the 16 studies using text-based RC
Several studies reported significant relationships between stimuli, nearly half reported non-significant correlations with
unconstrained skills that would demonstrate variability long PA/PS (Bélanger et al., 2011; Clark et al., 2011; Daza et al., 2014;
after initial acquisition. Significant relationships between Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010; Miller et al., 2012; Miller, 2009).
vocabulary and reading comprehension were reported by two Overall, the 28 studies employed a notable variety of measures
studies using target-age participants (Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010), across these reading skills.
one using both target age and older participants (Daza et al.,
2014), and one using older participants (Miller & Achmed, 2009).
Significant correlations between sign language and reading
Discussion
comprehension were reported for mixed younger-through-older
participants (Izzo, 2002), and for older participants (Miller, 2009). This study reviewed research on DHH readers that reported on
Alvarado et al. (2008) used regression to find a significant rela- correlations between early- and late-developing constrained
tionship between age and sign language acquisition in predict- (PA/PS, OA) and unconstrained (RC) skills. The purpose was to
ing reading (t (age) = 4.32, p < .01; t (sign language) = 3.32, p < .01) examine potential effects of assessment timing and constructs
for target age and older participants. Clark et al. (2011) also that may contribute to inconsistent outcomes regarding these
found significant relationships between ASL skills and RC (r = skills. The search of research databases identified 28 studies
.34, p = .01). Vocabulary was significantly correlated with RC in that fit criteria and reported either significant or non-significant
seven studies, and with PA/PS in seven studies. correlations between PA/PS or OA and RC.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
158 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2

The first research question examined the age of participants number of their participants unable to perform several reading
in correlating early-acquired and constrained skills of PA/PS tasks. This emphasizes the importance of timing assessments
and/or OA with reading comprehension. Across the 28 studies, to ensure maximum variability to achieve robust correlations,
10.7% (n = 3) used participants within the target-age group and avoidance of both floor and ceiling effects among develop-
defined as 6–8 years of age; 1 was below the target age (3.6%), 10 ing skill sequences.
were older (35.7%), with most using mixed ages (n = 14, 50%). Of Age of participants was not a factor in studies reporting cor-
the 17 studies reporting significant correlations between PA/PS relations between unconstrained skills. Significant relation-
and RC, 6 (35.3%) were with older participants and 4 (23.5%) ships were reported by four studies for vocabulary and RC, two
were with target age and older participants. More studies used for language and RC, and two for sign language and reading
older readers despite PA/PS and OA being early-acquired skills level or RC. Similarly, a meta-analysis of 57 studies examining
with attainment of asymptote expected by 8 or 9 years of age. Of phonological coding with DHH individuals (Mayberry, del
the 12 studies that did not report significant PA/PS and RC corre- Giudice, & Lieberman, 2011) identified the importance of lin-
lations, 5 (41.7%) were with target age and older participants, 4 guistic skills. Half of the studies had significant effect sizes for
(33.3%) were with older participants, with Koo et al. (2008) re- PA/PS with RC; however, the mean effect size was .35 (low to
porting both significant and non-significant correlations. Six medium) and represented just 11% of the variance in RC scores.
studies reported on relationships between OA and RC with five By comparison, language ability predicted 35% of the variance
that were significant (two with target age and older, three with in RC. The importance of these linguistic skills, also reported by
older participants) and one that indicated non-significant corre- Storch and Whitehurst (2002), suggests a need for further exam-
lations using older participants. ination of unconstrained skills that may contribute to the read-
Those studies using participants within the target ages for ing comprehension of DHH individuals. Such correlations
PA/PS and OA would not likely have had sufficient variability for would be less affected by timing once individuals had attained
the later-developing skill of reading comprehension. This is initial fluency.
most salient for correlations with text-based reading which is Research question two examined the measures used for as-
not attained until a child is beyond floor levels, at age 9 or above sessing component reading skills as a source of potential dispar-
(National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, ity in reported relationships. PA/PS was measured in more than
2000b; Wang & Williams, 2014). Only studies correlating PA/PS 10 different ways with this variety suggesting that these discrete
or OA with word recognition would tend to have equal variabil- skills may be dissimilar enough to warrant caution in making
ity across these factors. Of the three studies using only target- cross-study generalizations. Assessment of OA skills also used
age participants, one reported a significant correlation between approximately 10 different measures with some that contrasted
PA/PS and RC (Colin et al., 2007) and two reported non- phonemic with graphemic strategies in conjunction with PA/PS
significant correlations (Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010). None of the development. Spelling was analyzed for orthography in Harris
studies examining OA and RC used target-age participants. and Moreno (2004) but not in other studies. Orthographic skills
Outcome patterns are difficult to ascertain and interpret were included in several other studies but reported in terms of
among the remaining studies and varying ages. For those using other linguistic competencies (e.g., Miller & Achmed, 2009). Again,
older participants, six reported significant correlations between the variety of assessment targets within OA suggests caution in
PA/PS and RC (Daigle & Armand, 2008; Dyer et al., 2003; reporting potential acquisition patterns across studies.
Furlonger et al., 2014; Koo et al., 2008; Miller & Achmed, 2009; Notably diverse was the range of skills and the types of mea-
Miller, 2010) while five reported non-significant correlations sures used to assess reading comprehension. These included
(Bélanger et al., 2011; Clark et al., 2011; Koo et al., 2008; Miller individual word or pseudoword reading, sentence reading, and
et al., 2012; Miller, 2009). Five studies reported significant OA passage comprehension measured by retelling, explanation of
and RC correlations with three that used only older participants answers, correct picture or answer choice, or cloze procedures.
(Bélanger et al., 2011; Daigle & Armand, 2008; Furlonger et al., Research into testing and test construction has identified partic-
2014) and two that used target age and older participants ular difficulties for DHH students that result from wording of
(Alvarado et al., 2008; Harris & Moreno, 2004). questions and items, and use of shorter versus longer passages
The correlation results for older participants would be ex- such that scores do not accurately reflect their abilities
pected to approach asymptote or ceiling effects with reduced (Alvermann & Phelps, 2002; Johnson & Mitchell, 2008; Lollis &
variability although few studies identified these. Daigle and LaSasso, 2009; Martin & Mounty, 2005; Weinstock & Mounty,
Armand (2008) reported ceiling effects although only with hear- 2005). Explanations or responses that are evaluated based on
ing participants for graphemic coding. Miller (2009) reported comparisons with hearing participant norms are likely to pro-
ceiling effects on PA/PS for his older participants and Luetke- duce relatively poorer scores.
Stahlman and Nielsen (2003) reported ceiling effects for initial Tests of pseudoword word reading fluency and differentia-
and final phonemes for their mixed-age participants. Although tion between words and pseudowords may be negatively
their study used ANOVA, James, Rajput, Brinton, and Goswami affected by DHH students’ frequently diminished linguistic
(2008) reported that their results for PA/PS and RC for DHH chil- skills (Luckner, 2013). A panel review of the Dynamic Indicators
dren approximately 5–10 years of age were impacted by ceiling of Basic Literacy Skills (DIBELS) identified only one of seven
effects for syllable awareness. These studies suggest that DHH subtests as genuinely assessing reading comprehension. Many
readers experience similar age-based ceiling and asymptote ef- commercially available reading assessments and several used
fects although reported by only 3 of the 21 studies using mixed in these studies have similar tests and subtests. Other test con-
and older participants. cerns were raised by LaSasso (1980) with regard to cloze proce-
Another statistical concern is with floor effects. The National dures such that DHH individuals’ cloze scores did not correlate
Reading Panel characterized children below first grade as being well with predicted passage difficulty. DHH students tend to
nonreaders, with this skill being acquired gradually through have much poorer performance levels when held to typical
first and second grade. About 2 of the 28 studies (Cupples et al., scoring standards (Kelly & Ewoldt, 1984). A number of the 28
2013; Easterbrooks et al., 2008) reported floor effects with a studies utilized cloze procedures.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
P. Luft | 159

The differences in assessing and comparing comprehension defined among studies or assessments. These issues likely have
achievement across single-word and connected text reading contributed to inconsistent findings and ongoing obstacles to
scores likely contributes to the mixed outcomes reported by identifying evidence-based literacy practices with DHH indivi-
these studies. Single-word reading utilizes sight-word and word duals (e.g., Luckner et al., 2006).
attack or phonemic skills and is dissimilar to sentence or pas- Although these 28 studies used similar research methodol-
sage comprehension skills that require multiple psycholinguis- ogy and skill targets, the variety of skill and construct measure-
tic processes (Goodman et al., 2009; Goodman, 1994/2003; Kyle & ments, ages of participants, and stages of skill development
Harris, 2010; Miller, 2009; Paris, 2005). The National Reading preclude drawing any conclusions. A particular challenge is
Panel’s conclusion that PA is foundational for assisting with the that the formulation of robust and parsimonious research mod-
alphabetic relationships for reading and spelling is based on its els limits examination of the potentially multiple related skills
utility for word and pseudoword reading, with much smaller ef- that contribute to fluent reading comprehension. One observa-
fects reported for contextualized reading (National Institute of tion is that assessment of skill constructs that reflect age-based
Child Health and Human Development, 2000b). Word- and passage timing constraints across early- and later-developing skills, and
reading have been identified as separate constructs (Allington, that capture multiple and varying contributions of component
2013; Burns, 2003; Hannon, 2012; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002) with skills across readers’ maturation may be best achieved through
Dillon et al. (2012) describing even sentence-level comprehension longitudinal research designs.
as inferior to passage comprehension in relying more on phono- One consistent relationship regardless of age, was among
logical knowledge than authentic comprehension skills. unconstrained skills of vocabulary, language (English or sign
Among the 17 studies finding significant correlations language), and reading comprehension. Importantly, Miller
between PA/PS and RC, more used word-level reading alone et al. (2012) found that linguistic skills based on syntactic pro-
(29.4%) or in combination with text reading responses (41.2%) cessing were more effective for adolescent DHH readers than
for a total of 82.3%. This is in contrast with studies that used semantic (word-based) strategies. Wang and Williams (2014)
only using text reading with 17.6% reporting significant relation- also reported that language and thinking skills yielded larger RC
ships between PA/PS and RC. For the 11 studies reporting non- effect sizes once readers understood the written code. The diffi-
significant correlations, 54.6% used word or word measures in culty for many DHH readers is with their struggle to attain pri-
combination with others, whereas 45.5% used only text reading mary language fluency prior to reading instruction. Studies
measures. The Alvarado et al. (2008) method for determining using hearing children rarely examine linguistic abilities in that
reading level in correlation with age was not included in these most achieve basic skills in early childhood. Yet, Storch and
calculations. Whitehurst (2002) confirmed the importance of these skills for
The five studies examining OA and RC correlations included later reading with normal hearing readers. Linguistic skills are
just one study using only word-level reading to measure RC at least as important for DHH readers (Mayberry et al., 2011).
(Harris & Moreno, 2004), the others using text-level responses This review limited itself to correlational studies which does
alone (Bélanger et al., 2011; Daigle & Armand, 2008; Furlonger not allow for identification of causal relationships. The field
et al., 2014, Miller, 2010), and the Miller study reporting non- continues to wrestle with isolating significant contributing fac-
significant correlations. tors and the interrelationships that characterize the develop-
The difficulty in the current review is that the varying mea- mental trajectory of skillful readers. These relationships evolve
sures likely obscured patterns as well as differences among the over time and show unequal rates of growth, with further com-
reading constructs and PA/PS and OA subskills. This was further plications from skill co-dependencies and effects due to co-
complicated by participant age effects on variability across early, developing cognitive and linguistics competencies (Paris, 2005).
constrained and later, unconstrained skills. Even for studies Research needs to move beyond cross-sectional and correla-
within the target-age range reading measures varied with Colin tional studies into longitudinal and multi-skill component
et al. (2007) using a word recognition test, and with the Kyle & analyses.
Harris studies using word reading, word recognition, and sentence Another significant issue raised by this review is the dispar-
comprehension. Assessing the contributions of early-acquired ity in skill and construct measurement. This likely contributes
skills to those that occur later is important, but require differential to the difficulty in accumulating a consistent body of research
timing that may be best addressed using longitudinal methodol- that: (a) describes the nature and timing of the important fac-
ogy, such as used by the Kyle and Harris studies. This would tors that contribute to the development of fluent reading, and
accommodate differential developmental trajectories among com- (b) isolates potential evidence-based instructional practices.
ponent skills while also monitoring fluctuating direct and indirect Word- and text-level reading typically are treated as inter-
relationships that are characteristic of developing readers. changeable aspects of “reading comprehension” but instead, are
unique skill clusters that should be differentiated for assess-
ment and instruction. The comingling of these two constructs
Summary and Recommendations obscures potentially productive research avenues that other-
wise could more accurately identify optimal developmental pat-
This review identified several age-based and construct mea-
terns and processes that lead to fluent text reading.
surement concerns in studies of relationships between early-
To summarize, this review has several potential implica-
and later-developing reading skills. Participant ages suggested
tions for the planning of future research:
probable floor and ceiling effects with reduced variability for
robust and valid correlations, although relatively few were re- • Research methodologies should examine reading skills
ported. Most of the 28 studies tested participants outside the across the developmental trajectory that expand beyond
optimal ages for PA/PS, OA, or RC. Another issue was with as- common correlational approaches in order to accurately
sessments that created composite scores to enhance statistical represent causal connections, maturational change, and
robustness but that confounded tracking of discrete skills. varying skill contributions across the acquisition-to-mastery
Reading skills of PA/PS, OA, and RC were not consistently continuum. Models should allow for potential direct and

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
160 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2

indirect and evolving effects of component skills in order to References


identify potentially effective and evidence-based practices.
Albertini, J., & Mayer, C. (2011). Using miscue analysis to assess
• Researchers, diagnosticians, and educators need to develop
comprehension in deaf college readers. Journal of Deaf Studies
and adhere to consistent definitions of critical reading skills
and Deaf Education, 16, 35–46. doi:10.1093/deafed/enq017
and subskills. For example, Scarborough and Brady (2002)
Allington, R. L. (2013). What really matters when working with
defined reading and speech skill terms beginning with the
struggling readers. The Reading Teacher, 66, 520–530. doi:10.
morpheme “phon” to ensure consistency. At a minimum,
1002/TRTR.1154
word reading should be clearly differentiated from compre-
Almasi, J. F., Garas-York, K., & Shanahan, L. (2006). Qualitative
hension of connected text, and subskills should be unambig-
research on text comprehension and the report of the
uously identified and defined.
National Reading Panel. The Elementary School Journal, 107,
• Research on instruction should not only distinguish between
37–66. doi:10.1086/509526
the two major reading constructs but also reflect the distinc-
Alvarado, J. M., Puente, A., & Herrara, V. (2008). Visual and pho-
tive cognitive skills from which each construct develops.
nological coding in working memory and orthographic skills
Many early-acquired skills (e.g., OA, PA, PS, and word identifi-
of deaf children using Chilean sign language. American
cation) are learned through memorization, rote practice, and
Annals of the Deaf, 152, 467–479. doi:10.1353/aad.2008.0009
direct instruction processes. In contrast, comprehension of
Alvermann, D. E., & Phelps, S. F. (2002). Assessment of students.
connected text is a higher-order cognitive skill that requires
In Richard-Amato P. A., & Snow M. A. (Eds.), Academic success
analysis and interpretation through inquiry and discovery
for English language learners: Strategies for K-12 mainstream tea-
learning processes (Borich, 2014). Connected text comprehen-
chers (pp. 103–110). White Plains, N.Y: Longman.
sion is more than a sum of individual word identifications.
Anderson, D. (2006). Lexical development of deaf children
The historic struggles of DHH individuals to become fluent read- acquiring signed languages. In Schick B., Marschark M., &
ers may have created a situation in which singular subskills Spencer P. E. (Eds.), Advances in the sign language development
have become overly emphasized and presumed as prerequisites of deaf children (pp. 135–160). Oxford, Great Britain: Oxford
for text comprehension. Examining the higher-order cognitive University Press.
skills necessary to develop connected text comprehension may Atwell, W. R. (2013). Deaf readers and phrasal verbs: Instructional re-
prompt investigations into more effective instructional strate- fficacy of chunking as a visual tool (Unpublished doctoral disser-
gies. These should consider phrase-level reading or “chunking” tation). Lamar University, Beaumont, TX.
which provide the foundations for comprehension of sentences, Barrera, M., Liu, K., Thurlow, M., & Chamberlain, S. (2006). Use of
paragraphs, and increasingly larger segments of text. This has chunking and questioning aloud to improve the reading comprehen-
been an approach used with hearing students (Barrera, Liu, sion of English language learners with disabilities (ELLs with
Thurlow, & Chamberlain, 2006; Rasinski, 1994; Rasinski, Disabilities Report 17). Minneapolis, MN: University of
Yildirim, & Nageldinger, 2011) or students with learning disabil- Minnesota, National Center on Educational Outcomes.
ities (Casteel, 1990). DHH students also have abilities to acquire Borich, G. D. (2014). Effective teaching methods: Research-based prac-
phrase-level skills (Albertini & Mayer, 2011; Kelly, 2003; Luft & tice. Boston: Pearson.
Fochman, accepted with revision) although not consistently Boudreault, P., & Mayberry, R. I. (2006). Grammatical processing
(Albertini & Mayer, 2011; Atwell, 2013; Kelly, 2003; Schirmer, in American Sign Language: Age of first-language acquisition
Bailey, & Lockman, 2004), perhaps affected by varying levels of effects in relation to syntactic structure. Language and
language fluency. The goal of reading instruction is to develop Cognitive Processes, 21, 608–635. doi:10.1080/
fluent comprehension of connected text. This is not achieved 01690960500139363
through a linear progression that begins with discrete phono- Burns, M. K. (2003). Reexamining data from the National
logical, orthographic, and single-word reading (Hannon, 2012). Reading Panel’s meta-analysis: Implications for school psy-
Nor should it be assumed that phrase-level reading cannot be chology. Psychology in the Schools, 40, 605–612. doi:10.1002/
introduced until after mastery of these discrete and early- pits.10110
developing skills. Bélanger, N. N., Baum, S. R., & Mayberry, R. K. (2011). Reading
The complex and interrelated nature of achieving fluent text difficulties in adult deaf readers of French: Phonological
reading suggests potential insights from Goodman’s sociopsy- codes, not guilty! Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 263–285.
cholinguistic theory of reading (1994/2003). Its foundations in doi:10.1080/10888438.2011.568555
language and cognitive processes of literacy may help in identi- Casteel, C. A. (1990). Effects of chunked text-material on reading
fying practices that also address the linguistic challenges of comprehension of high and low ability readers. Reading
DHH readers, and their effects on connected text reading (Kelly, Improvement, 27, 269–275.
2003; Mayberry et al., 2011; Miller et al., 2012). We need a better Cheung, H., Chen, H. C., Lai, C. Y., Wong, O. C., & Hills, M. (2001).
understanding of DHH reading skill development that results in The development of phonological awareness: Effects of spo-
a more accurate model of the relative contributions and timing ken language experience and orthography. Cognition, 81,
of skills that comprise fluent reading comprehension and the 227–241. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00136-6
causal processes by which developing readers assemble related Clark, M. D., Gilbert, G., & Anderson, M. L. (2011). Morphological
skills into a fluent cognitive package. At that point, educators knowledge and decoding skills of deaf readers. Psychology, 2,
and researchers can finally remedy the many decades of read- 109–116. doi:10.4236/psych.2011.22018
ing mediocrity that still characterize the abilities of too many Cook, B., Buysse, V., Klinger, J., Landrum, T., McWilliams, R.,
DHH students. Tankersley, M., & Test, D. (2014). Council for Exceptional
Children standards for evidence-based practices in special educa-
tion. Alexandria, VA: Council for Exceptional Children.
Conflict of Interest
Cunningham, J. W. (2001). The National Reading Panel report.
No conflicts of interest were reported. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 326–335. doi:10.1598/RRQ.36.3.5

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
P. Luft | 161

Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner- readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 230–249.
Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive lan- 10.1093/deafed/eng013
guage approach to early literacy: The interrelationships Kelly, L., & Ewoldt, C. (1984). Interpreting nonverbatim cloze re-
among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print sponses to evaluate program success and diagnose student
knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of needs for reading instruction. American Annals of the Deaf,
Educational Psychology, 95, 465. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.465 129, 45–51. 10.1353/aad.2012.0857
Easterbrooks, S. R., & Stephenson, B. (2006). An examination of Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and predic-
twenty literacy, science, and mathematics practices used to tors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf and hearing
educate students who are deaf or hard of hearing. American school children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11,
Annals of the Deaf, 151, 386–397. doi:10.1353/aad.2006.0043 273–288. 10.1093/deafed/enj037
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Shuster, B. V., Yaghoub- Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2010). Predictors of reading development
Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness in- in deaf children: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of
struction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 229–243. 10.1016/j.jecp.
National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research 2010.04.011
Quarterly, 36, 250–287. doi:10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2 Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2011). Longitudinal patterns of emerging
Friedmann, N., & Szterman, R. (2005). Syntactic movement in literacy in beginning deaf and hearing readers. Journal of Deaf
orally trained children with hearing impairment. Journal of Studies and Deaf Education, 16, 289–304. 10.1093/deafed/enq069
Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11, 56–75. doi:10.1093/deafed/ LaSasso, C. (1980). The validity and reliability of the cloze proce-
enq052 dure as a measure of readability for prelingually, profoundly
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D., Coyne, M., Greenwood, C., & deaf students. American Annals of the Deaf, 125, 559–563. 10.
Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality indicators for group experimen- 1353/aad.2012.1497
tal and quasi-experimental research in special education. Lederberg, A. R. (2003). Expressing meaning: From communica-
Exceptional children, 71, 149–164. 10.1177/001440290507100202 tive intent to building a lexicon. In Marschark M., & Spencer
Goodman, K. G. (1994/2003). Reading, writing, and written texts: P. E. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and educa-
A transactional sociopsycholinguistic view. In Flurkey A., & tion (pp. 247–260). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Xu J. (Eds.), On the revolution of reading: The selected writings of Lollis, J., & LaSasso, C. (2009). The appropriateness of the NC
Kenneth S. Goodman (pp. 3–45). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. state-mandated reading competency test for deaf students
Goodman, K., Goodman, Y., & Paulson, E. J. (2009). Beyond word as a criterion for high school graduation. Journal of Deaf
recognition: How retrospective and future perspectives on Studies and Deaf Education, 14, 76–97. 10.1093/deafed/enn017
miscue analysis can inform our teaching. In Hoffman J. F., & Luckner, J. L. (2013). Using the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early
Goodman Y. M. (Eds.), Changing literacies for changing times: An Literacy Skills with students who are deaf or hard of hearing:
historical perspective on the future of reading research, public pol- Perspectives of a panel of experts. American Annals of the Deaf,
icy, and classroom practices (pp. 146–161). New York: Routledge. 158, 7–19. 10.1093/deafed/ent015
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., & Richardson, U. (2005). The effects of Luckner, J. L., Sebald, A. M., Cooney, J., Young, J., III, & Muir, S. G.
spelling consistency on phonological awareness: A compari- (2006). An examination of the evidence-based literacy
son of English and German. Journal of Experimental Child research in deaf education. American Annals of the Deaf, 150,
Psychology, 92, 345–365. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.06.002 443–456. 10.1353/aad.2006.0008
Hammill, D. D., & Swanson, H. L. (2006). The National Reading Luft, P., & Fochtman, M. (accepted with revision). Strengths-
Panel’s meta-analysis of phonics instruction: Another point Based reading assessment: Applying miscue analysis to an
of view. The Elementary School Journal, 107, 17–26. 10.1086/ African American native ASL-user. In Wang, Q. Y., &
509524 Andrews, J. F. (Eds.), Toward a global understanding of literacy
Hannon, B. (2012). Understanding the relative contributions of education for deaf students. Washington, DC: Gallaudet
lower-level word processes, higher-level processes, and University Press. Targeted publisher.
working memory to reading comprehension performance in MacSweeney, M., Waters, D., Brammer, M. J., Woll, B., &
proficient adult readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, Goswami, U. (2008). Phonological processing in deaf signers
125–152. 10.1002/RRQ.013 and the impact of age of first language acquisition.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act of 2004. 20 U.S.C. § NeuroImage, 40, 1369–1379. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.047
1400 et seq. Marschark, M., Schick, B., & Spencer, P. E. (2006). Understanding
Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awareness and reading ability: An sign language development of deaf children. In Schick B.,
investigation with young readers who are deaf. American Marschark M., & Spencer P. E. (Eds.), Advances in the sign lan-
Annals of the Deaf, 147, 18–28. doi:10.1353/aad.2012.0242 guage development of deaf children (pp. 3–19). Oxford, Great
James, D., Rajput, K., Brinton, J., & Goswami, U. (2008). Britain: Oxford University.
Phonological awareness, vocabulary, and word reading in Martin, D. S., & Mounty, J. L. (2005). Overview of the challenge. In
children who use cochlear implants: Does age of implanta- Mounty J. L., & Martin D. S. (Eds.), Assessing deaf adults: Critical
tion explain individual variability in performance outcomes issues in testing and evaluation (pp. 3–10). Washington, DC:
and growth? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, Gallaudet University.
117–137. doi:10.1093/deafed/enm042 Mayberry, R. I., Chen, J., Witcher, P., & Klein, D. (2011a). Age of
Johnson, R. C., & Mitchell, R. E. (2008). Introduction. In Johnson acquisition effects on the functional organization of lan-
R. C., & Mitchell R. E. (Eds.), Testing deaf students in an age guage in the adult brain. Brain and Language, 119, 16–29. 10.
of accountability (pp. 1–15). Washington, DC: Gallaudet 1016/j.banl.2011.05.007
University. Mayberry, R. I., del Giudice, A. A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2011b).
Kelly, L. P. (2003). The importance of processing automaticity Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and
and temporary storage capacity to the differences in compre- awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf
hension between skilled and less skilled college-age deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16, 164–188. 10.1093/deafed/enq049

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
162 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2018, Vol. 23, No. 2

Miller, P. (2009). The nature and efficiency of the word reading friendly guide. Washington, DC: Institute of Educational
strategies of orally raised deaf students. Journal of Deaf Studies Sciences. Available from https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pdf/evidence_
and Deaf Education, 14, 344–361. 10.1093/deafed/enn044 based.pdf.
Moeller, M. P., Toblin, J. B., Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Connor, C., & Wang, Y., & Williams, C. (2014). Are we hammering square pegs
Jerger, S. (2007). Current state of knowledge: Lanugage and into round holes? An investigation of the meta-analyses of
literacy of children with hearing impairment. Ear & Hearing, reading research with students who are d/Deaf or hard of
28, 740–753. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e318157f07f hearing and students who are hearing. American Annals of the
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Deaf, 159, 323–345. 10.1353/aad.2014.0029
(2000a). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to Weinstock, R. B., & Mounty, J. L. (2005). Test-taking for deaf and
read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research litera- hard of hearing individuals: Meeting the challenges. In
ture on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Mounty J. L., & Martin D. S. (Eds.), Assessing deaf adults: Critical
Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government issues in testing and evaluation (pp. 27–36). Washington, DC:
Printing Office. Gallaudet University.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
(2000b). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to
read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research litera-
Appendix: Reviewed Studies
ture on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports
of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington, Alvarado, J. M., Puente, A., & Herrara, V., (2008). Visual and
DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. phonological coding in working memory and orthographic
Nicholas, J. G., & Geers, A. E. (2003). Hearing status, language skills of deaf children using Chilean sign language,
modality, and young children’s communicative and linguis- American Annals of the Deaf, 152, 467–479. doi: 10.1353/
tic behavior. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, aad.2008.0009
422–437. 10.1093/deafed/eng029 Bélanger, N.N., Baum, S. R., & Mayberry, R. K. (2011). Reading dif-
No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, P.L. 107-110, Title IX, Sec 9101 ficulties in adult deaf readers of French: Phonological codes,
(23)(A&B). not guilty! Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 263–285. doi:
Paris, S. G. (2005). Reinterpreting the development of reading skills. 10.1080/10,888,438.2011.568555
Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 184–202. 10.1598/RRQ.40.2.3 Clark, M. D., Gilbert, G., & Anderson, M. L. (2011). Morphological
Paul, P. V., Wang, Y., Trezek, B. J., & Luckner, J. L. (2009). knowledge and decoding skills of deaf readers. Psychology, 2,
Phonology is necessary, but not sufficient: A rejoinder. 109–116. doi:10.4236/psych.2011.22018
American Annals of the Deaf, 154, 346–356. 10.1353/aad.0.0110 Colin, S., Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., & Leybaert, J. (2007) Relation
Pénicaud, S., Klein, D., Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J.-K., Witcher, P., between deaf children’s phonological skills in kindergarten
Hyde, K., & Mayberry, R. I. (2013). Structural brain changes and word recognition performance in first grade. Journal of
linked to delayed first language acquisition in congenitally Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48, 139–146. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
deaf individuals. NeuroImage, 66, 42–49. 10.1016/j. 7610.2006.01700.x
neuroimage.2012.09.076. Cupples, L., Ching, T. Y. C., Crowe, K., Day, J., & Seeto, M. (2013).
Rasinski, T. V. (1994). Developing syntactic sensitivity in reading Predictors of early reading skill in 5-year-old children with
through phrase-cued texts. Intervention in School and Clinic, 29, hearing loss who use spoken language. Reading Research
65–168. 10.1177/105345129402900307 Quarterly, 49, 85–104. doi:10.1002/rrq.60
Rasinski, T., Yildirim, K., & Nageldinger, J. (2011). Building flu- Daigle, D., & Armand, F. (2008). Phonological sensitivity in
ency through the phrased text lesson. The Reading Teacher, 65, severely and profoundly deaf readers of French. Reading and
252–255. 10.1002/TRTR.01036 Writing, 21, 669–717. doi: 10.1007/s11145-007-9087-5
Scarborough, H. S., & Brady, S. A. (2002). Toward a common ter- Daza, M. T., Phillips-Silver, J., del Mar Ruiz-Cuadra, M., & López-
minology for talking about speech and reading: A glossary of López, F. (2014). Language skills and nonverbal cognitive
the “phon” words and some related terms. Journal of Literacy processes associated with reading comprehension in deaf
Research, 34, 299–336. 10.1207/s15548430jlr3403_3 children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 3526–3533.
Schirmer, B. R., Bailey, J., & Lockman, A. S. (2004). What verbal doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.030
protocols reveal about reading strategies of deaf students: A Dillon, C.M., de Jong, K., & Pisoni, D.B. (2012). Phonological
replication study. American Annals of the Deaf, 140, 5–16. 10. awareness, reading skills, and vocabulary knowledge in chil-
1353/aad.2004.0016 dren who use cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code- Deaf Education, 17, 205–226. doi:10.1093/deafed/enr043
related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal Dyer, A., MacSweeney, M., Szczerbinski, M., Green, L., &
structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947. 10. Campbell, R. (2003). Predictors of reading delay in deaf ado-
1037//0012-1649.38.6.934 lescents: The relative contributions of rapid automatized
Thompson, B., Diamond, K. E., McWilliam, R., Snyder, P., & naming speed and phonological awareness and decoding.
Snyder, S. W. (2005). Evaluating the quality of evidence from Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 215–229. doi:
correlational research for evidence-based practice. 10.1093/deafed/eng012
Exceptional Children, 71, 181–194. 10.1177/001440290507100204 Easterbrooks, S. R., Lederberg, A. R., Miller, E. M., Bergeron, J. P., &
Trezek, B. J., Wang, Y., Woods, D. G., Gampp, T. L., & Paul, P. V. Connor, C. M. (2008). Emergent literacy skills during early
(2007). Using visual phonics to supplement beginning read- childhood in children with hearing loss: Strengths and weak-
ing instruction for students who are deaf or hard of hearing. nesses. The Volta Review, 108, 91–114
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 373–384. 10.1093/ Furlonger B., Holmes, V. M., & Rickards, F. W. (2014).
deafed/eni028 Phonological awareness and reading proficiency in adults
U.S. Department of Education (2003). Identifying and implementing with profound deafness. Reading Psychology, 35, 357–396. doi:
educational practices supported by rigorous evidence: A user- 10.1080/02,702,711.2012.726944

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018
P. Luft | 163

Geers, A. E. (2003). Predictors of reading skill development in Luetke-Stahlman, B., & Nielsen, D. C. (2003). The contribution of
children with early cochlear implantation. Ear & Hearing, 24, phonological awareness and receptive and expressive
595–685. doi: 10.1097/01.AUD.0000051690.43989 English to the reading ability of deaf students with varying
Gibbs, S. (2004) The skills in reading shown by young children with degrees of exposure to accurate English. Journal of Deaf
permanent and moderate hearing impairment. Educational Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 464–484. doi: 10.1093/deafed/
Research, 46, 17–27. doi: 10.1080/0,013,188,042,000,178,791 eng028
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonologi- Miller, P. (2009). The nature and efficiency of the word reading
cal coding: Evidence from reading, spelling, and working strategies of orally raised deaf students. Journal of Deaf Studies
memory. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 9, 253–268. and Deaf Education 14, 344–361. doi:10.1093/deafed/enn044
doi: 10.1093/deafed/enh016 Miller, P. (2010). Phonological, orthographic, and syntactic
Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awareness and reading ability: An awareness and their relation to reading comprehension in
investigation with young readers who are deaf. American prelingually deaf individuals: What can we learn from skilled
Annals of the Deaf, 147, 18–28. doi: 10.1353/aad.2012.0242 readers. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22,
Johnson, C., & Goswami, U. (2010). Phonological awareness, 549–580. doi:10.1007/s10882-010-9195-z
vocabulary, and reading in deaf children with cochlear im- Miller, P., & Achmed, R. A. (2009). The development of ortho-
plants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, graphic knowledge in prelingually deaf individuals: New
237–261. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0139) insight from Arab readers. Journal of Developmental and
Koo, D. Crain, K., LaSasso, C, & Eden, G. F. (2008). Phonological Physical Disabilities, 22, 11–31. doi: 10.1007/s10882-009-9160-x
awareness and short-term memory in hearing and deaf indi- Miller, P., Kargin, T., Guldenoglu, B., Rathmann, C., Kubus, O.,
viduals of different communication backgrounds. Annals of Hauser, P., & Spurgeon, E. (2012). Factors distinguishing
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 83–99. doi: 10.1196/ skilled and less skilled deaf readers: Evidence from four
annals.1416.025 orthographies. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 17,
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and predic- 439–461. doi:10.1093/deafed/ens022
tors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf and hearing Most, T., Aram, D., & Andorn, T. (2006). Early literacy in children
school children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11, with hearing loss: A comparison between two educational
273–288. doi:10.1093/deafed/enj037 systems. The Volta Review, 106, 5–28.
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2010). Predictors of reading development Spencer, L. J., & Oleson, J. J. (2008). Early listening and speaking
in deaf children: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of skills predict later reading proficiency in pediatric cochlear
Experimental Child Psychology 107, 229–243. doi:10.1016/j. implant users. Ear and Hearing, 29, 270–80. doi: 10.1097/01.
jecp.2010.04.011 aud.0000305158.84403.f7
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2011). Longitudinal patterns of emerging Spencer, L. J., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Evaluating phonological
literacy in beginning deaf and hearing readers. Journal of Deaf processing skills in children with prelingual deafness who
Studies and Deaf Education 16, 289–304. doi:10.1093/deafed/ use cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
enq069 Education 14, 1–21. doi:10.1093/deafed/enn013

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/23/2/148/4921154


by University of Durham user
on 29 March 2018

You might also like