You are on page 1of 3

commentaries

Resolving the 50-year debate around using and


misusing Likert scales
James Carifio & Rocco Perla

How Likert type measurement scales are interval in nature and, thus, may produce interval data, particularly if
should be appropriately used and be analysed parametrically with all the scale meets the standard psy-
analysed has been debated for over the associated benefits and power of chometric rule-of-thumb criterion
50 years, often to the great confu- these higher levels of analyses. of comprising at least eight reason-
sion of students, practitioners, allied Jamieson3 replied that she failed to ably related items. Based on the
health researchers and educators. be convinced by Pell’s three points. empirical evidence available, then,
Basically, there are two major com- Although both views are prototypi- the argument clearly goes to Pell and
peting views that have evolved cal of this long and ongoing debate, the intervalist view of the issues he
somewhat independently of one neither the letter nor the original represents in this debate. However,
another and of the associated commentary made use of a great to resolve the debate we must also
empirical research literature on this deal of empirical evidence that ask where this contention that Likert
‘great debate’. Most recently in this should enable a resolution. scales produce ordinal data, and all
journal, Jamieson1 outlined the view that this unsupported contention
that ‘Likert scales’ are ordinal in implies, comes from, and why it has
character (i.e., produce rank order Monte Carlo studies of the F-test have persisted as a view long past the
data) and that they, therefore, must shown the F-test to be extremely robust to point when adequate data and
be analysed using non-parametric violations of its assumptions, which must arguments became available to
be extreme before the F-test is biased
statistics. Non-parametric statistics, decide the debate.
however, are less sensitive and less
powerful than parametric statistics
and are, therefore, more likely to Monte Carlo studies of the F-test, A variety of studies have shown that the
miss weaker or emerging findings. performed by Glass et al.,4 have Likert response format produces empiri-
convincingly shown that the F-test cally interval data at the scale level
is extremely robust to violations of its
Historically, there has been debate be- assumptions, except for the homo-
tween those who maintain the ordinalist geneity of variance assumption, and The views of Likert scales and
(rank order) and intervalist views in violations of this assumption must their uses and misuses presented by
Likert scales truly be extreme before they bias the Jamieson1 can be characterised as
F-test. Utilising the F-test to analyse representative of Stevens’ non-para-
ordinal data, therefore, produces metric view (and fallacy). Stevens’8,9
Pell2 responded to Jamieson’s arti- unbiased results, which is an empiri- view is that what is or might be
cle with a letter that pointed out cal fact. Further, a variety of studies ordinal data at the item (i.e. atom)
three of the chief reasons why Likert on the nature of Likert scales (as level cannot be interval data at the
scales (collections of items) as op- opposed to single Likert items) have scale (i.e. molecular) level, whereas
posed to individual Likert items are shown that the Likert response for- all in medicine know that molecules
not ordinal in character, but rather mat produces empirically interval almost always have properties that
data5,6 and, in fact, can approximate their individual atoms do not have
ratio data, in theory and actuality , if a but are functionally reliant on them
Lowell, Massachusetts, USA hundred millimeter response line is nonetheless. It is the ‘emergent
used for marking responses which properties’ of scales (versus items)
Correspondence: James Carifio, University of has ‘always’ and ‘never’ as anchors.7 that are key in framing and settling
Massachusetts-Lowell, Lowell, Massachusetts,
01854, USA. Tel: 00 1 617-513-6279;
The weight of the empirical evi- disagreements in this area.
Fax: 00 1 978-934-3005; dence, therefore, clearly supports
E-mail: james_carifio@hotmail.com the view and position that Likert Stevens’ view is a logical argument
scales (collections of Likert items) based upon extrapolating various
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03172.x

1150 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2008. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2008; 42: 1150–1152
commentaries 1151

rank ordering methodologies he Likert (graded valence) question intervalists contend, perfectly
was investigating at the time to (or stem) or a Likert scale appropriate to summarise the
‘Likert scales’, which, parentheti- (collection of items), which are the ratings generated from Likert scales
cally, he and others mischaracter- root of many of the logical using means and standard
ised in several ways relative to what problems with the ordinalist deviations, and it is perfectly
Likert wrote and data that were position and many of the incorrect appropriate to use parametric
available at the time.10 Empirical claims ordinalists make in this techniques like Analysis of Variance
data and studies often do not debate. The intervalist position, by to analyse Likert scales. It is also
significantly impact logical argu- contrast, brings all of these (and perfectly appropriate to calculate
ments or logical theories, even if we other) elements together to form a Pearson correlation coefficients
acknowledge that the best empiri- true and coherent measurement using the summative ratings from
cal evidence did not become avail- system whose construct validity may Likert scales and to use these
able until 20 years after this view be easily and quickly assessed, correlations as the basis for various
had become firmly rooted in sev- representing another critical multivariate analytical techniques,
eral disciplines and communities. difference between the two views. such as multiple regression,
factor analysis and meta-analysis,
We believe that reading the article to obtain more powerful and
Those who hold the ordinalist view of we have written on this topic and nuanced analyses of the data and
Likert scales rarely mention the abundant the sources it cites will convince research hypotheses being investi-
empirical findings about Likert scales most paradigmatically neutral gated. Treating the data from
researchers and practitioners of the Likert scales as ordinal in character
correctness of the points made and prevents one from using these
Those who hold the ordinalist (i.e., supported by the article, and the more sophisticated and powerful
Stevens’) view of Likert scales, and of correctness of the view of the modes of analyses and, as a result,
how the data from these scales debate we summarise in this from benefiting from the richer,
should be analysed, rarely mention commentary. Likert methodology is more powerful and more nuanced
or address the empirical findings one of the most commonly used understanding they produce.
and facts outlined in this commen- methodologies in all fields of
tary. These facts and findings are research, but particularly so in
simply ignored, or seem not to allied health, medicine and REFERENCES
be familiar to those holding the medical education.
ordinalist view of Likert scales. 1 Jamieson S. Likert scales: how to
Beliefs that live on independently of (ab)use them. Med Educ
empirical evidence and key studies It is perfectly appropriate, therefore, to 2004;38:1212–8.
2 Pell G. Uses and misuses of Likert
are typically called misconceptions, sum Likert items and analyse the sum- scales. Med Educ 2005;39:97.
myths or urban legends. We have mations parametrically, both univariately 3 Jamieson S. Author’s reply. Med
identified and codified 10 major and multivariately Educ 2005;39:970.
misconceptions, myths and urban 4 Glass GV, Peckham PD, Sanders JR.
legends about Likert scales and Consequences of failure to meet
assumptions underlying the
their analysis in detail elsewhere, Therefore, we do not want to see analyses of variance and covariance.
and include in that paper a wide researchers and practitioners Rev Educ Res 1972;42:237–88.
array of additional supporting unwittingly misusing and misun- 5 Carifio J. Assigning students to
empirical evidence.11 derstanding Likert scales, their career education programs by
nature and characteristics, or how preference: scaling preference
data for program assignments.
In the article cited above11, we data obtained using them should Career Education Quarterly 1976;1,1,
focused on a number of the chief be analysed with maximal sensitivity Spring, 7–26.
misconceptions and logical flaws and power. The debate on Likert 6 Carifio J. Measuring vocational
in the ordinalist position in this scales and how they should be preferences: ranking versus
50-year debate about Likert scales analysed, therefore, clearly and categorical rating procedures.
Career Education Quarterly 1978;3,1,
because these misconceptions strongly goes to the intervalist posi- Winter, 34–66.
and logical flaws are as important as tion, if one is analysing more than a 7 Vickers A. Comparison of an ordi-
the empirical evidence that strongly single Likert item. Analysing a single nal and a continuous outcome
falsifies this view. For example, the Likert item, it should also be noted, measure of muscle soreness. Int J
ordinalist view makes no distinction is a practice that should only occur Technol Assess Health Care 1999;
15:709–16.
between a Likert response format, a very rarely. It is, therefore, as the

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2008. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2008; 42: 1150–1152 1151
commentaries

8 Stevens S. On the theory of scales John Wiley & Sons 1951;1– tions, persistent myths and urban
of measurement. Science 1946;103 49. legends about Likert scales and
(67):668–90. 10 Likert R, Hayes S. Some Applications Likert response formats and their
9 Stevens S. Mathematics, measure- of Behavioural Research. Paris: antidotes. Journal of the Social
ment and psychoanalysis. In: Unesco 1957. Sciences 2007;3(3), 106–116.
Stevens SS, ed. Handbook of 11 Carifio J, Perla R. Ten common
Experimental Psychology. New York: misunderstandings, misconcep-

Thinking ‘no’ but saying ‘yes’ to student presence in


general practice consultations: politeness theory
insights
Charlotte E Rees1 & Lynn V Knight2

We’ve all done it. We’ve all agreed previously unpublished excerpt GP’s request when the student is
to something when we really from Knight and Rees.2 present to hear the response? To
wanted to say no. It was no surprise answer this question, we turn to
to us, therefore, that 20.8% of politeness theories.4,5
patient participants who agreed to The patient’s ability to refuse a request for
have a student present during their student presence relates to who asks for Drawing on Goffman,6 Brown and
consultation in the Price et al.1 consent (and the patient’s relationship Levinson4 employed the concept of
with the requester) and who listens to the
study would have preferred to see face (a person’s public self-image)
response
their general practitioner (GP) to illustrate how people go to great
alone. This finding is consistent efforts in social interaction to
with results from our qualitative maintain their own and others’ self-
research in which medical students This excerpt raises two important image needs using politeness strat-
articulated their anxieties about issues, the resolution of which egies. They differentiate between
patient consent.2 Although stu- may help us understand the condi- positive face (maintaining a positive
dents were keen for patients to give tions necessary for some patients to self-image) and negative face
consent because this would provide agree to the presence of medical (maintaining autonomy as in free-
the students with learning oppor- students despite their wish to say no. dom from imposition and freedom
tunities, they were worried that These issues concern who requests of choice) and suggested that
patients sometimes gave consent consent (and the patient’s relation- speech acts, such as requests and
without understanding what they ship with the requester) and who refusals, threaten interlocutors’
were consenting to and without hears the patient’s response. From positive and negative face needs.
really wanting to consent. This Ann’s comments in turn 5, we can Requests not only threaten the
tension between patient consent see that patients seem able to requestee’s freedom from imposi-
and student learning has been decline a request for students to be tion,4 but threaten multiple faces,
articulated in earlier literature3 and present if they are asked by a recep- including the requester and reque-
is illustrated in turns 1 and 5 of the tionist (a third party extrinsic to the stee’s positive self-image.5 Likewise,
patient–doctor relationship) and if refusals threaten the requester’s
their response will not be heard by autonomy and other types of face,
1
Sydney, New South Wales, Australia either the GP or the student. By depending on the reasons behind
2
Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK contrast, Matt implies that patients the refusal (known as obstacles).7
are unable to decline a request if
Correspondence: Charlotte E Rees, OPME,
Mackie Building (K01), University of Sydney, they are asked by the GP (a person
Sydney, New South Wales 2006, Australia. central to the patient–doctor rela- Requests and refusals not only threaten
Tel: 00 61 (0) 2 9351 2814; Fax: 00 61 (0) 2 tionship) and if their response will the negative face of the recipient but
9351 6646; E-mail: crees@med.usyd.edu.au threaten multiple faces
be heard by the student. So, why is it
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03173.x that patients feel unable to decline a

1152 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2008. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2008; 42: 1152–1154

You might also like