You are on page 1of 4

Current Biology

Dispatches
16. Wang, R., Dearing, J.A., Langdon, P.G., human microbiome. Science 336, 1255– P.G., Lenton, T.M., Raworth, K., Brown, S.,
Zhang, E., Yang, X., Dakos, V., and 1262. et al. (2014). Safe and just operating spaces
Scheffer, M. (2012). Flickering gives for regional social-ecological systems.
early warning signals of a critical transition 18. Lenton, T.M., Held, H., Kriegler, E., Hall, J.W., Global Environ. Chang. 28, 227–238.
to a eutrophic lake state. Nature 492, Lucht, W., Rahmstorf, S., and Schellnhuber,
419–422. H.J. (2008). Tipping elements in the Earth’s 20. Carpenter, S.R., Cole, J.J., Pace, M.L., Batt,
climate system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA R., Brock, W., Cline, T., Coloso, J., Hodgson,
17. Costello, E.K., Stagaman, K., Dethlefsen, 105, 1786–1793. J.R., Kitchell, J.F., and Seekell, D.A. (2011).
L., Bohannan, B.J., and Relman, D.A. Early warnings of regime shifts: a
(2012). The application of ecological 19. Dearing, J.A., Wang, R., Zhang, K., Dyke, whole-ecosystem experiment. Science 332,
theory toward an understanding of the J.G., Haberl, H., Hossain, M.S., Langdon, 1079–1082.

Neuropsychology: How Many Emotions Are There?


Julien Dubois and Ralph Adolphs
Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
Correspondence: jcrdubois@gmail.com (J.D.), radolphs@hss.caltech.edu (R.A.)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.037

Psychological theories disagree on how we attribute emotions to people. A new neuroimaging study shows
that such attributions involve a large number of abstract features, rather than a small set of emotion
categories.

The most popular emotion theories of data: neuroimaging. The study had door to see her parents holding a golden
propose either two broad participants attribute emotions by reading retriever puppy.’’
dimensions — arousal and valence short stories while lying in a functional While these stimuli necessarily lack the
(pleasantness) [1] — or a small number magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) full complexity of real-life emotion
(around six) of discrete ‘basic’ emotions scanner, and quantified which of the three attribution, they have the benefit of
[2]. The first, dimensional, view has emotion theories best corresponded with allowing experimental control over the
the virtue of economy and is supported the patterns of evoked brain activations. explicit information on the basis of which
by finding that many kinds of emotion subjects make the attributions. The study
data can be mapped well into a Measuring Emotion Attribution used 10 different stories for each of 20
two-dimensional space [3,4]. The second One reason there are competing theories emotions (200 stimuli in total; the 20
view derives much of its support from the of emotion is that we attribute emotions to emotions were arbitrarily chosen, with half
study of human facial expressions, and people and animals on the basis of a wide of them positive and half negative in
also corresponds well to emotions we range of evidence. Some data come from valence), and controlled for other possible
would typically attribute to people and people describing how they feel; confounds, such as the complexity of the
animals (the list includes anger, fear, and additional clues arise from interpreting sentences used, or the ease with which
disgust). Yet a third theoretical proposal particular kinds of behaviors (facial they could be read.
argues that the rich emotions that people expressions, body posture); and yet more Skerry and Saxe [7] first collected
experience unfold through a complex set information can be derived from the behavioral ratings for their stimuli from
of evaluations and coping mechanisms. circumstances in which people find an independent set of people queried
Such ‘appraisal’ theories invoke a larger themselves. Skerry and Saxe [7] focused over the internet, and then compared
vocabulary of features from which a on the latter type of evidence. As their these to the brain activations evoked in
correspondingly larger set of emotions stimuli, the authors chose short, written the subjects of the neuroimaging study.
can be constructed [5,6]. All three theories vignettes that explicitly described Three different feature spaces were
have some appeal, and all three probably situational causes of emotions; for constructed based on the internet ratings,
reflect aspects of what people actually do example, how do you think Dana feels corresponding to the three different
when they attribute emotions to others as from the following actual sample stimulus: emotion theories investigated. Which of
well as to themselves. Is there any way to ‘‘Dana always wanted a puppy, but her these three spaces best matched the
adjudicate further between the theories? parents said it was too much of a hassle. neural data?
A new study by Skerry and Saxe [7], One summer afternoon, Dana’s parents
reported in this issue of Current Biology, returned from a supposed trip to the Methodological Challenges
now provides such adjudication, based grocery store, and Dana heard barking This question was addressed using a
on an important and relatively new source from inside her garage. She opened the technique called Representational

Current Biology 25, R654–R676, August 3, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved R669
Current Biology

Dispatches
STIMULI CANDIDATE MODELS FEATURAL REPRESENTATION REPRESENTATIONAL GEOMETRY REPRESENTATIONAL
20 emotion categories (3 shown) vs. BRAIN REGION dimensionality differs representational dissimilarity matrix SIMILARITY
10 vignettes per emotion between representational spaces (all 20 emotion categories shown)
“APPRAISAL 38”
compare
expected? full RSA matrix

model A
... patterns
APPREHENSIVE

DM el C
el A
el B
PFC
safe? Euclidean
emotion #8

For months, Naomi

Ratings from M-turk subjects


knowledge

mod
mod
mod
had been struggling distance
change?
to keep up with her model A

(N = 250 total)
BASIC EMOTIONS model B
model C
afraid? DMPFC
model B

...
compare
FURIOUS surprised?
emotion #10

RDMs
After an 18-hour
disgusted?
flight, Caitlin arrived
at her vacation desti - Kendall’s τa
VALENCE/AROUSAL
model C

negative positive noise ceiling


0.12
(lower bound)
mild intense
0.08
JOYFUL OTHER EMOTION MODELS?
emotion #19

τa
Dana always wanted 0.04
a puppy, but her
fMRI subjects
neural data

... (*)
(N = 22)
parents said it was 0.00

model C
model A
model B
... (*)
... (*)
DMPFC (*) not real data Current Biology

Figure 1. Representational similarity analysis of emotion theories.


In the study [7], vignettes for 20 emotion categories (three examples are shown in the first column) were rated along dimensions posited by three different theories
(second column; model A, appraisal theory, 38 dimensions; model B, basic emotions, six dimensions; model C, valence/arousal, two dimensions). These ratings
produced representations of each of the emotions in the feature space corresponding to each model (third column). Since the representational spaces for the
different models have different dimensionalities, a transformation to ‘similarity space’ was then performed to ease comparison. Similarities between the 20
emotions were computed in the representational space of each model, yielding a Representational Dissimilarity Matrix (RDM) for each model (fourth column).
Similarly, an RDM was derived from neural data collected while subjects read the vignettes in the scanner (bottom row). Lastly, the neural RDM was
compared to each of the model RDMs using a rank correlation measure (last column). The winning model was the model with the highest correlation to the
neural RDM — and the goodness-of-fit of this model to the neural data was assessed with respect to the noise in the neural data. Possible future extensions
include the addition of further models of emotion, as well as visualization of the full RSA matrix (last column) with all brain regions included, perhaps using
techniques such as multi-dimensional scaling, to fully explore representational geometries across brain regions and emotion models.

Similarity Analysis (RSA) [8,9], the flow of fourth column; see also Figure 3, top, in rightmost column). The study [7] found
which we summarize in Figure 1. Take a the paper [7]). This analysis essentially compelling evidence that the neural
given stimulus, say a vignette evoking an maps the similarity structure of different RDMs in all brain regions correlate best
attribution of apprehensiveness (blue stimuli, when rated according to the three with the model feature space of appraisal
stimuli in Figure 1, first column). Each of different emotion theories: which theory. With the particular stimuli used
the three theories (second column) posits emotions cluster together as similar, and in the study, the six-emotion and
a representation of this emotion in a which seem very different from one valence/arousal models failed to capture
specific space (38 dimensions for the another? the representational geometry measured
appraisal model, six for the basic An analogous approach can be taken in any of the investigated brain regions.
emotions model, and two for the for the neuroimaging data, generating a Yet appraisal advocates should not
valence/arousal model). The 20 emotions neural RDM (for dorsomedial prefrontal rejoice too hastily. The methods used
can be represented in each feature space cortex as one example: Figure 1, bottom here lack the sensitivity to conclude that a
using average behavioral ratings (the third row; see also Figure 3, center, in the 38-dimensional appraisal theory is the
column shows the representations of original paper [7]). In this case, it is not the ‘‘right’’ model: the noise ceiling
apprehensive, furious, and joyful; see also profiles of how people rate the stimuli on (the expected RDM correlation achieved
Figure 2A–C in the paper [7], which shows various features that determine the by the unknown true model) has a lower
the data for all the emotions used). Next similarity between the stimuli, but rather bound less than 0.1 for all brain regions
one compares the similarity between all the different patterns of activation evoked (see dotted lines in Figure 4 and Figure S3
possible pairs of emotions — how far across voxels in a given brain region when in the paper [7]). As a correlation-based
apart the 20 emotions lie in each of the participants read the stimuli in the measure, the theoretically best value in
three different representational spaces. scanner. the absence of noise is 1.0; Nili et al. [8]
For example, in the valence/arousal The question now is: which feature report a value of about 0.25 for a study of
space, apprehensive and furious appear space (from the behavioral ratings) looks human IT fMRI. One possible reason is
more similar to one another than to joyful most like the neural RDM (from the that the stimuli used in the Skerry and
(Figure 1, third column). This intuition can neuroimaging) — the one for valence/ Saxe study [7] did not elicit sufficiently
be formalized into a Representational arousal, the one for the six basic strong attributions of emotion in the
Dissimilarity Matrix (RDM), computed emotions, or the one built on appraisal subjects’ brains. Another may be the low
using a Euclidean distance measure for theory? This comparison can be done signal-to-noise ratio of the fMRI data.
each of the competing theories (Figure 1, with a rank correlation measure (Figure 1, Though there are complex trade-offs in

R670 Current Biology 25, R654–R676, August 3, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved
Current Biology

Dispatches

neuroimaging, future studies will likely be simplicity of some findings (only six basic view: plausibly, one reason for the relative
able to substantially improve sensitivity emotions; only attributing beliefs) may success of psychological emotion
(raise the noise ceiling) by imaging with have more to do with the simplicity of the theories is that they haven’t got it
smaller voxels, faster acquisition times, stimuli or the task used in a particular completely wrong, even if they haven’t got
and by using higher field strengths. These study, rather than reflecting how the it completely right. By telling us that a
hardware advances will go hand in hand brain actually represents other people’s higher-dimensional abstract feature
with processing refinements, such as minds in general. It thus appears that space best explains how our brains
surface-based analyses and less data there is a neural system for thinking ordinarily represent emotions, the study
smoothing [10]. Finally, it is of course well about other people, but exactly what it is may be telling the emotion scientist that
known that BOLD-fMRI, the that the regions comprising this system this would be a fruitful place to start as
neuroimaging method used in the paper represent is typically complex and well.
[7], is both indirect and macroscopic; abstract. Indeed, the situation is probably
fMRI pattern analysis as used here may more complicated yet, since there may be REFERENCES
not retrieve the relevant information no single neural system, but rather a
present in underlying populations of collection of systems that only partly 1. Russell, J.A. (2003). Core affect and the
psychological construction of emotion. Psych.
single neurons [11], adding uncertainty to overlap [16]. Rev. 110, 145–172.
the interpretation of the current findings. One possible next step may be to
2. Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for
An even more challenging question is extend the investigation by combining the
universals in facial expressions: a reply to
what models of emotion to use in the first competing theories. One could imagine Russell’s mistaken critique. Psych. Bull. 115,
place. The present study compared three constructing a more complex framework 268–287.
emotion models, because these consisting of an underlying dimensionality 3. Russell, J.A., and Bullock, M. (1985).
correspond to well entrenched theories. of valence and arousal, a more Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial
But of course an unbounded number of fine-grained classification into six or so expressions: similarity from preschoolers to
adults. J. Pers. Soc. Psych. 48, 1290–1298.
other models are possible: how many ‘basic’ emotion categories, and a very
features should there be, and which fine-grained and more flexible attribution 4. Rolls, E.T. (1999). The Brain and Emotion (New
York: Oxford University Press).
features should one pick (why did Skerry based on appraisal features [17]. Perhaps
and Saxe [7] choose the particular we use a mixture of these when we 5. Lazarus, R.S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation
(New York: Oxford University Press).
38 features for their Appraisal Space?). attribute emotions to others, or when we
The authors are appropriately cautious in experience them ourselves (another 6. Scherer, K. (1984). On the nature and function
what can be said about the ‘‘best’’ recent study suggests as much [18]). of emotion: A component process approach.
In Approaches to Emotion, K.R. Scherer, and
emotion space at this stage, but that still Perhaps the extent to which one of the P. Ekman, eds. (New York: Lawrence Erlbaum
leaves us with questions about what to three schemes dominates depends on the Associates), pp. 293–317.
conclude. details of the stimuli and the task. Perhaps 7. Skerry, A.E., and Saxe, R. (2015). Neural
this also shows interesting individual representations of emotion are organized
Conceptual Interpretation differences. Maybe children begin with around abstract event features. Curr. Biol. 25,
1945–1954.
Prior studies have suggested that specific the simpler types of attributions, whereas
basic emotions might be associated with adults engage in more appraisal. 8. Nili, H., Wingfield, C., Walther, A., Su, L.,
Marslen-Wilson, W., and Kriegeskorte, N.
specific brain regions. For instance, the Acknowledging this flexibility and (2014). A toolbox for representational similarity
amygdala has often been linked to fear variability in how we attribute emotions analysis. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003553.
[12], and medial prefrontal cortex to would open up a large set of new studies
9. Kriegeskorte, N., and Kievit, R.A. (2013).
valence [13]. Skerry and Saxe [7] looked that would also link to psychopathology. Representational geometry: integrating
everywhere in the brain using a It is important to remember that all such cognition, computation, and the brain. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 17, 401–412.
searchlight approach. The set of brain studies are still studies of how laypeople
regions where the searchlight approach attribute emotions to others in everyday 10. Glasser, M.F., Sotiropoulos, S.N., Wilson, J.A.,
revealed information about the 20 life. The participants in the studies are not Coalson, T.S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J.L., Xu,
J., Jbabdi, S., Webster, M., Polimeni, J.R.,
emotions overlapped largely with an emotion researchers themselves. What et al. (2013). The minimal preprocessing
independently localized set of regions can be said about how scientists should pipelines for the Human Connectome Project.
Neuroimage 80, 105–124.
involved in attributing beliefs to people attribute emotions to people, let alone
(the so-called ‘theory of mind’ network how they should attribute them to other 11. Dubois, J., deBerker, A.O., and Tsao, D.Y.
[14]). As with the present finding [7] that a animals? What is the best science of (2015). Single-unit recordings in the macaque
face patch system reveal limitations of fMRI
larger set of abstract features best emotion? That project might not use the MVPA. J. Neurosci. 35, 2791–2802.
describes how the brain attributes words we normally use for emotions at all
12. Calder, A.J., Lawrence, A.D., and Young, A.W.
emotions, so too might such an expanded [19], and might attempt a broader survey (2001). Neuropsychology of fear and loathing.
feature set best describe ‘theory of mind’ of features that all emotions share across Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 352–363.
more generally [15]. That is, we always species [20]. Could a future science of
13. Kringelbach, M.L. (2005). The human
represent what other people are thinking emotion look completely alien to the orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic
and feeling from a large and diverse set of layperson, as do current scientific experience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 691–702.
features that can flexibly describe many theories in physics, for example? Skerry 14. Saxe, R. (2006). Why and how to study Theory
different situations. The apparent and Saxe [7] suggest a more optimistic of Mind with fMRI. Brain Res. 1079, 57–65.

Current Biology 25, R654–R676, August 3, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved R671
Current Biology

Dispatches
15. Schaafsma, S., Pfaff, D.W., Spunt, R.P., and imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, states are categorically distinct. Soc. Cogn.
Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and 9–34. Affect. Neurosci. pii, nsv032.
reconstructing theory of mind. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 19, 65–72. 17. Barrett, L.F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K.N., and 19. LeDoux, J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional
Gross, J.J. (2007). The experience of emotion. brain. Neuron 73, 653–676.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 373–403.
16. Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, 20. Anderson, D.J., and Adolphs, R. (2014). A
F., and Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory 18. Kragel, P.A., and LaBar, K.S. (2015). framework for investigating emotion across
of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional species. Cell 157, 187–200.

Handedness: What Kangaroos Tell Us about Our


Lopsided Brains
Giorgio Vallortigara
Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Piazza della Manifattura 1, I-38068 Rovereto, Italy
Correspondence: giorgio.vallortigara@unitn.it
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.036

Brain asymmetry is widespread, but the presence of handedness in non-human animals is debated. A new
study now provides evidence for handedness in bipedal — but not quadrupedal — marsupials.

A persistent myth in neuroscience has there might be some geographical weakly correlated with cerebral
been the idea that brain asymmetry — the variation [5], around 90% of humans are dominance for either praxis or language
different functions of the left and right right-handed. In non-human primates, [8]. Besides, the argument based on the
sides of the nervous system — is a whereas there is little doubt that strength of handedness between human
uniquely human trait. Of course, there individuals may show hand preferences, and non-human animals is problematic.
could be uniquely human biological traits differences in the methods used to study First, because the measures of
that also show asymmetry (e.g. language), manual asymmetries (tasks, sample size, handedness in humans (largely based on
but brain asymmetry in itself is so etc.) have sometimes produced questionnaires) rarely compare with those
widespread in the animal kingdom that it discrepant findings [6]. Nonetheless, carried out in non-human animals (for
can be very plausibly considered a some striking evidence for handedness exceptions, see [9]). Second, if we look at
fundamental principle of organization of has emerged, particularly in great apes. other animals, the idea that handedness is
their nervous systems. In recent years, Bill Hopkins and his collaborators [7] have present with maximal strength only in our
besides having been shown in vertebrates collected data for more than 700 species is untenable. Parrots, for
[1], laterality has been found to occur also chimpanzees from four different instance, show preferred use of a foot, the
in invertebrates, such as Caenorhabditis populations who have been tested for left foot in most species, to hold food
elegans [2] and the honeybee [3], thus hand use on a task requiring coordinated objects with percentages of bias at the
revealing that brain size or number of bimanual actions, revealing a significant population level that parallel or exceed
neurons is certainly not a key factor in the right-hand bias in each sampled those of human handedness [10].
emergence of cerebral asymmetry. population. The captive chimp colonies Even among non-human primates the
However, in spite of the huge amount of that Hopkins studied are 60–70 percent presence or absence of handedness
evidence for brain asymmetry in different right-handed, regardless of the should be not taken as a cue of the
animal groups, there is still debate on one proportion of individuals in each colony presence of other biases that indicate the
particular manifestation of cerebral and that were human-reared. Thus, it has presence of functional asymmetry in the
functional asymmetry in behaviour in the been suggested that, whereas there brain. Common marmosets (Callithrix
form of handedness. A recent study in might be a genetic basis for handedness jacchus), for instance, have a preferred
Current Biology by Giljov et al. [4] now in chimpanzees, it must be expressed hand in simple reaching tasks which
provides for the first time evidence for true less strongly than in humans. This may be develops by the time they are
handedness in some species of due to the fact that right-handedness in 8–12 months old and each individual uses
marsupials. humans is associated with the left the same preferred hand across its
The term ‘handedness’ describes hemisphere’s specialization for language lifetime [11], but no population bias is
manual asymmetries at the population and speech production. Yet, the apparent. However, the same group of
level, whereas ‘hand preference’ refers association is far from straightforward, marmosets has been shown to display a
usually to the individual level. Though because handedness in humans is only striking group bias in a completely

R672 Current Biology 25, R654–R676, August 3, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved

You might also like