You are on page 1of 7

Conditioning Factors in Opiate Addiction 279

Conditioning Factors in Opiate Addiction and Relapse

ABRAHAM WIKLER

THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS Possibly for these reasons, the clinician’s interests


in the problem of drug addiction and relapse have
In achieving an impressive degree of mastery over the
been focused on the occurrence of drug-induced
world about us, the growth of the natural sciences
“euphoria.” It has been debated whether or not mor-
has been characterized by an ever-increasing supple-
phine, heroin, and other narcotic drugs (henceforth,
mentation of “private operations” (sensing, feeling,
morphine will be used as a prototype) produce eu-
inducing, deducing) with “public” ones (control and
phoria in all persons or only in atypical or deviant
manipulation of measurable variables). As one result,
ones (16, 21), but few have ventured to question the
even our “private” ways of perceiving the world have
decisive importance of euphoria as the main determi-
changed from those of our prescientific ancestors, so
nant of the addict’s behavior, both in his initial ad-
that at sunset, we no longer “see” the sun sinking into
diction and in his subsequent relapses- for this is
the sea, but “see” it disappearing beneath the horizon.
also the addict’s explanation. Likewise in agreement
Such subtle but far-reaching changes have also
with the addict, clinicians have generally regarded
occurred in our dealings with our hearts, livers, and
morphine-induced physical dependence merely as an
kidneys, but in dealing with our own and other peo-
unpleasant complication, serving as a deterrent rather
ple’s behaviors, most of us are still content with men-
than as a motivating factor in relapse, for who but a
talistic explanations, the terms of which, “private” as
masochist would see any virtue in those properties of
they are, remain undefined though they may be quan-
a drug that produced “the agonies of the damned”
tified in the sense of counting the frequency of their
when withheld after long-continued use?
occurrence in one or another situation. This men-
The present writer would not deny that certain ef-
talistic orientation has characterized most attempts
fects of single doses of morphine may be decisive in
to describe and explain drug (including opiate) addic-
determining the initial choice of that drug from among
tion and relapse no less than other forms of deviant
many other “euphorogenic” agents for continued use
human behavior. In consequence, the clinician has
by some persons. Indeed, in collaboration with sev-
tended to accept his patient’s mentalistic interpre-
eral colleagues (9, 10, 15, 11, 7, 8) he has presented
tation of his addiction and his frequent relapses to
data acquired by “public” operations indicating one
drugs as a valid one - for who else would know better
class of effects of morphine on nontolerant postad-
what his feelings are than the patient himself? And
diets, which theoretically at least may play such a role
does not one behave in accordance with one’s feel-
-namely, reduction of the influence on behavior of
ings? To be sure, the psychoanalysts have often an-
motivating variables in general, and of nociceptive
swered both questions in the negative, designating the
ones in particular, but whether or not such effects
patient’s verbally expressed feelings as “conscious,”
have anything to do with euphoria is indeterminable
and their own dissenting conscious feelings about the
and, in this writer’s opinion, quite irrelevant. Of
patient’s feelings as “unconscious” ones of the patient.
more importance would be the development of meth-
This certainly represents a commendable attempt to
ods for testing the hypothesis that, other factors be-
break out of the solipsistic straitjacket which, had it
ing equal, any drug possessing such effects, regard-
been allowed to restrain Columbus, would have left
less of whether or not it also produces euphoria,
us convinced that the earth is flat because we “see” it
would be chosen for repeated use by the same kind of
that way. Unfortunately, however, the psychoan-
person who uses morphine in this manner.
alyst’s explanations of human behavior, including
Be this as it may, however, the writer has stated
drug addiction and relapse, are also couched in men-
reasons for doubting the prevailing opinion that the
talistic terms and hence cannot be tested for validity
quest for euphoria and the fear of abstinence dis-
in the accepted manner of the natural sciences-i.e.,
tress are responsible for self-maintenance of addic-
by use of “public” operations, at least directly.
tion to morphine (25, 27). Mainly, these are three:
(1) tolerance to the euphoric effect of morphine de-
Reprinted with permission from Nurcorics, edited by D.I. Wilner velops quickly and is not achieved again in anything
and G.G. Kassebaum, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965, 85-100. like the intensity of the initial effect despite progres-
280 A. Wikler

sive increase in dosage; (2) even with unlimited sup- ing of a narcotic he becomes conditioned to taking one in
plies of the drug and the privilege of self-injection in response to most any situation that may arise.
amounts and at intervals ad libitum, the addict, at
least under experimental conditions, is a miserable Expressed for the most part in mentalistic terms,
creature, beset by remorse, guilt, and anxiety; and the validity of such speculations is difficult, if not im-
finally (3) there appears to be no adequate reason possible, to test directly, but to those who are familiar
why experienced addicts cannot withdraw themselves with behavioristic-conditioning theory, their similar-
in a relatively painless manner either by gradual re- ity to certain concepts definable in terms of “public”
duction of dosage or substitution and subsequent operations should be apparent. Restating the writer’s
withdrawal of methadone. In mentalistic terms, the views in such terms (28,29) relapse may be attributed
conclusion drawn from these considerations and at least in part to two factors operating during previ-
other subjective data gathered in a study of a patient ous episodes of addiction: (1) classical conditioning
during self-regulated readdiction to morphine (25) of physical dependence through repeated temporal
was that the motivations for self-maintenance of ad- contiguities between a specific environment and the
diction in man are largely unconscious ones, above occurrence of morphine-abstinence phenomena; and
all the gratification by each dose of morphine of the (2) reinforcement of instrumental activity (morphine-
need for the drug engendered by physical depen- acquisitory behavior) through repeated reduction by
dence. Furthermore, it was suggested (27) that “being the drug of such abstinence phenomena as developed
hooked” furnishes the addict with a motivational during intervals between doses. This “two-factor
basis for sustained activity (“hustling for drugs”) learning theory” of relapse could be expanded further
directed to recurring but attainable goals, thereby by introduction of some additional concepts such as
preventing boredom and also securing approval from “secondary reinforcement,” “drive (or stimulus) gen-
his peers, i.e., “addict society.” eralization,” and “scheduling” of reinforcement for
Likewise, the writer has doubted that the quest for the purpose of redefining and testing other factors in
euphoria constitutes the only, or even the major, mo- the genesis of relapse already described in mentalistic
tivation to relapse, in spite of the addict’s conscious terms, but only brief allusions to these concepts will
insistence thereon, for the acceptance of such an ex- be made in this paper because actual research to date
planation would entail acceptance of the improbable has been confined to investigation of factors 1 and 2.
assumption that the addict “remembers” only the
euphoric effects of the first few doses of morphine
and “forgets” the months or years of misery experi- EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATIONS
enced during addiction. Rather, statements occasion- Though some of the consequences of the two-factor
ally made by postaddicts to the effect that they have learning theory of relapse may eventually be testable
experienced symptoms resembling those of acute ab- in man, both ethical and practical considerations re-
stinence from morphine long after withdrawal of the quire the use of animals for direct testing of the con-
drug (26) led the author to hypothesize that physical ditionability of physical dependence and the putatively
dependence may become conditioned to environmen- reinforcing processes operating during addiction.
tal situations specifically associated with availability With regard to the latter, similar hypotheses and data
of morphine, and hence “abstinence distress,” or bearing upon them have been reported by Nichols
something very much like it, may be reactivated long et al. (19), Nichols and Davis (18), and Davis and
after “cure” when the postaddict finds himself in a Nichols (2), who employed forced drinking of dilute
similar situation, thus providing an unconscious mo- (0.5 mg per ml) aqueous solutions of morphine under
tivation to relapse and renewed self-maintenance of water-deprivation conditions for reinforcement of
addiction as described above (24). “choice” drinking of the same solution by morphine-
Another way in which relapse might come about abstinent rats; by Headlee et al. (6). who reinforced
as a result of conditioning was suggested by Kolb (14) head turning in a particular direction by making in-
in the following terms: traperitoneal injection of morphine contingent upon
this operant in morphine-abstinent rats; by Beach (l),
The addict, even if he has sufficient narcotics, becomes who reinforced running of rats to one arm of a Y
uncomfortable several times a day when the last dose wears maze by establishing temporal contiguity between
down. If another dose is not available, he suffers acute dis- “residence” in that arm and presumed reduction of
tress in about 18 hours. Over a period of years, he relieves
morphine-abstinence phenomena there by prior in-
such discomfort or distress thousands of times by injection
of morphine. During this same period he enjoys the drug in
traperitoneal injection of morphine; by Weeks (22)
pleasurable association with friends and by taking it to get and Weeks and Collins (23), who developed a tech-
the effect that many of them describe by the statement, “It nique for self-maintenance of addiction through
makes my troubles roll off my mind.“,By thus building up a intravenous (intracardiac) self-injection of morphine
strong association between pleasure and pain and the tak- in rats; and by Yanagita et al. (32) and by Schuster
Conditioning Factors in Opiate Addiction 281

and Thompson (20), who utilized a similar method wet-dog frequencies, as well as other early abstinence
for reinforcing intravenous self-injection of mor- phenomena, become manifest at least as early as 22
phine in monkeys. to 24 hr after the last previous injection of morphine.
In our own investigations (carried out exclusively 3. When E’s are permitted to drink a 10 mcg per ml
in the rat so far) we circumvented difficulties encoun- aqueous solution of etonitazene instead of water over
tered with forced drinking of morphine solutions or the preceding 17-hr period, no significant differences
parenteral injections of morphine for reinforcement from normal rats receiving single intraperitoneal in-
by use of a relatively new drug, etonitazene [ l-(beta- jections of saline at 8 A.M. daily (C’s)* can be found
diethylaminoethyl)-2-(p-ethoxybenzyl)-5-nitrobenz- on comparison 22 to 24 hours after the last previous
imidazole methane sulfonate] in very dilute aqueous injections.
solution for drinking. Though structurally unrelated 4. Whereas E’s drink very much larger volumes of
to morphine (13) it has been shown to be 1,000 times 5 or 10 mcg per ml aqueous solutions of etonitazene
as potent as morphine by parenteral injection for than they do of water when only one or the other
analgesia in the rat (5) and to have morphine-like ef- fluid is available from 3 P.M. to 8 A.M. next morning
fects not only in this species but also in monkey and (during the latter part of which period E’s are mor-
man, in both of which it has been shown to suppress phine-abstinent), the volumes of water or etonitazene
morphine-abstinence phenomena as well (3, 4). Pre- consumed by C’s during the same period under the
liminary studies (30) revealed that water-deprived same conditions are not significantly different (though
rats would drink a 5 mcg per ml concentration of one of a group of 4 C’s died after drinking the 10 mcg
etonitazene in distilled water as avidly as tap water per ml drug solution).
and that within 4 to 7 min after starting to drink, nor- In light of this information, the studies on condi-
mal rats would show typical morphinelike effects (tail tioning of physical dependence and reinforcement of
rigidity, exophthalmos, and stupor alternating with drug-acquisitory behavior were designed in the fol-
quick, jerky movements). This was also true for rats lowing manner. Experimental conditions were ar-
acutely (18 hr) abstinent from morphine, in which ranged in such a way that over a 6-week “training”
drinking of this solution also abolished an easily period, the occurrence of nocturnal morphine-absti-
observable sign of abstinence, namely, increased fre- nence phenomena in E’s (N = 7) was repeatedly
quency of “wet-dog” shakes (so-called because of associated with “residence” in one (preferred) end of
their resemblance to those of a dog shaking water off a three-compartment linear maze, where only distilled
its back). Later it was found that rats, morphine- water was available for drinking, and relief from
addicted or not, will readily drink such dilute aqueous nocturnal morphine-abstinence phenomena was as-
solutions of etonitazene even without any prior water sociated with “residence” in the other (nonpreferred)
deprivation, thus eliminating the necessity of in- end, where only a 10 mcg per ml aqueous solution of
troducing that condition as a complicating variable etonitazene was available for drinking, the drug solu-
in the design of experiments on conditioning and tion being also “tagged” with anise flavor to provide
reinforcement. additional discriminative cues (food was available ad
Because of these promising early results, more sys- libitum in both ends at all times), Another group of
tematic investigations (17, 3 1) were then undertaken E’s (N = 7) was “trained” in exactly the same manner
to characterize as precisely as possible the phenomena except that the drinking fluid in the nonpreferred end
of the morphine abstinence syndrome in the rat, and consisted only of anise-flavored water, and both
the effects thereon of drinking dilute aqueous solu- training schedules were replicated concurrently for
tions of etonitazene, without prior water deprivation. two groups of c’s (N = 7 each) except that for the
Those results bearing most directly on the condition- first of these two groups, the anise-flavored etonita-
ing and reinforcement studies to be described later zene solution contained 5 mcg per ml of that drug.
may be summarized as follows: For convenience, the four groups will be designated
1. Elevated wet-dog frequencies are reliable in- in the order described as ET, ENT, CT, and CNT.
dicators of early morphine abstinence in the rat, Following the training period, all injections were
roughly paralleling other signs of early abstinence terminated, and all rats were transferred to home
such as increased activity, hypothermia, loss of body cages (food and tap water ad libitum), where they re-
weight, and increased defecation, urination, and hos- mained thenceforth except on “relapse” and other
tility - all compared with observations made concur- test days conducted at intervals up to 155 days after
rently on normal control rats. termination of injections. On the morning of each
2. In rats maintained on single intraperitoneal in- relapse test day (9, 23, 44, 58, 72, 87, 94, and 142
jections of morphine at 8 A.M. daily (E’s),* increased days after termination of injections) wet-dog fre-
quencies for each rat were measured immediately
after removal from home cage, weighing, and return-
*E, “experimental animals”; C, “control animals.” ing to home cage, and also immediately after removal
282 A. Wikier

from home cage, weighing, and placing in the linear sumptive evidence of “conditioned abstinence” was
maze (preferred end), in alternate order from rat to manifested by E’s long after “cure’‘-i.e., after body
rat; from 8 P.M. the same day to 8 A.M. next morning, weight, 24hr tap-water consumption (home cage),
each rat was afforded “free choice” between distilled and wet-dog frequencies in the home cage had re-
water in the preferred and an anise-flavored, 5 mcg turned to the levels of C’s (from the 23d day of absti-
per ml aqueous solution of etonitazene in the non- nence onward, although through the 72nd day, wet-
preferred end of its linear maze. Between the seventh dog frequencies of E’s were generally slightly higher
(94th day of abstinence) and eighth (142d day) re- than in C’s). As between ET and ENT, or between
lapse tests, two successive extinction procedures were CT and CNT, no significant differences in wet-dog
carried out. In the first free choice was permitted frequencies were found.
between distilled water in the preferred and anise- 2. Reinforcement of Instrumental Activity (Drug-
flavored water in the nonpreferred end, and in the acquisitory Behavior). In each of the first four re-
second, between distilled water in both ends. In addi- lapse tests (through the 58th day of abstinence) and
tion, six tests on forced drinking, without prior water again on the eighth (142d day) relapse test (after com-
deprivation, were made on all rats from 8 P.M. to pletion of the extinction procedures), the percentage
8 A.M. at intervals throughout the study-four on of fluids consumed in the form of anise-flavored
forced drinking of the anise-flavored 5 mcg per ml etonitazene solution (5 mcg per ml) by choice was
aqueous solution of etonitazene, one of the anise- significantly greater for E’s (ET + ENT) than for C’s
flavored water, and one of the distilled water, each in (CT + CNT) by the Mann-Whitney “U” test. The
the nonpreferred end. The purpose of these tests was ratios, volumes of the anise-flavored etonitazene so-
to provide comparative data on how much each rat lution consumed by constraint to those consumed by
would drink of each of the fluids mentioned “by con- choice, were about 8:3 for E’s and 5:l for C’s at 9 to
straint” (forced-drinking tests) and “by choice” (re- 10 days, and about 2:l for E’s and approaching in-
lapse tests). Finally, a supplementary study was also finity (almost zero consumption of the drug solution)
carried out on four new E’s and four new C’s in which for C’s at 142 to 155 days of abstinence. In the sup-
both groups were trained over a g-day period in a plementary study, the percentage of fluids consumed
manner similar to that already described for ENT in the form of anise-flavored water on the two relapse
and CNT, and then subjected, 9 and 23 days after tests (9 and 23 days abstinent) was less for E’s than
termination of injections, to relapse tests conducted for C’s, though the difference was significant only on
exactly as described above, except that the fluid in the first test. These results indicate that the greater
the nonpreferred end was only anise-flavored water, intake of anise-flavored etonitazene solution by E’s
to determine whether or not previous addiction to than by C’s in the first four and in the eighth relapse
morphine results in an increased affinity of rats for tests cannot be explained merely as a reflection of
the anise flavor per se. “residual cross-tolerance” (between morphine and
Considered in relation to the theory, the results etonitazene) in E’s or of a greater affinity of E’s for
were as follows: anise flavor per se. Rather, the data suggest that rela-
1. Classicial Conditioning of Physical Depen- tive to C’s, etonitazene solution is less negatively (or
dence. On every relapse test, wet-dog frequencies of more positively) reinforcing for E’s, even long after
E’s (ET + ENT) were higher in the linear mazes than cure. Not predicted by theory, however, were the
in the home cages, and although on some tests wet- findings that the etonitazene solution was reinforcing
dog frequencies of C’s (CT + CNT) changed in the to equal degrees for ET and ENT on the first relapse
same direction, the magnitude of change was less. A test, and that whereas such reinforcement continued
mixed type of analysis of variance revealed that on with little change for the ENT group, it fell progres-
the second relapse test (23 days abstinent from injec- sively for the ET group over the next four relapse
tions), F ratios for variances due to previous treat- tests. These observations suggest that such “self-
ment (E’s vs. C’s), test conditions (linear maze vs. training” as the ENT rats may have undergone during
home cage), and “interactions” (E’s vs. C’s linear the 12-hr (8 P.M. to 8 A.M.) free choice drinking pe-
maze vs. home cage) were all significant. On the first riod in the first relapse test could have been sufficient
relapse test (9 days) only one, but on the third (44 for maximal positive reinforcement; whereas the
days), fourth (58 days), and fifth (72 days), two of forced drinking of the anise-flavored drug solution
the three F ratios were significant. All three F ratios which the ET group underwent in the programmed
were again significant when the analysis was made on training period could have produced, in addition,
means obtained for each rat on twelve tests (including some negative reinforcement based on pharmacologic
the sixth, seventh, and eighth relapse tests as well as properties of etonitazene other than morphine-absti-
the single-day tests during the extinction procedures) nence reducing ones. Presumably, the negatively re-
from the 84th through the 155th days of abstinence. inforcing properties of etonitazene were even more
Especially significant theoretically is that such pre- pronounced for the CT and CNT groups, since they
Conditioning Factors in Opiate Addiction 283

were resistant to the extinction procedures, whereas sures - would need to be modified only in details, not
the latter were effective for the ET and for the ENT in principle.
group as well, which likewise exhibited evidence of If, on the other hand, the behavior of E’s relative
negative reinforcement on the sixth and seventh re- to C’s in the relapse tests was due to conditioning and
lapse tests. reinforcing procedures employed during training,
then the implications of the experimental results for
the drug-withdrawal phase of treatment in man are
DISCUSSION
quite far-reaching, for methods would have to be de-
Though the data are consistent with the two-factor vised for extinguishing “conditioned abstinence” and
learning theory of relapse, this conclusion must be a reinforced drug-acquisitory behavior. This is not ac-
tentative one at present, because the experiments de- complished by passive withdrawal of, and prolonged
scribed have not ruled out with assurance the possi- abstention from, morphine in a “drug-free environ-
bility that the differences between E’s and C’s in the ment” with or without nonspecific psychotherapy,
relapse tests were due to latent but long-enduring any more than satiating a rat with food and keeping
hyperirritability in the former as a result of previous it away from the Skinner box for a period of time will
addiction to morphine, rather than to the putative “cure” it of its lever-pressing “habit,” previously rein-
interactions between morphine abstinence and the forced by food rewards under conditions of food
specified stimulus arrangements in the linear mazes deprivation. Rather, true extinction of both processes
during training. While the low frequencies of wet would require an attack on each separately in reverse
dogs in the home cage on the relapse tests would seem order under very different conditions. Thus, extinc-
to rule out such a possibility, they were somewhat tion of morphine-acquisitory behavior would require
higher in E’s than in C’s through the 72d day of absti- maintenance of the state of morphine deprivation,
nence as already noted, and in an earlier study (17) i.e. prolongation of the abstinence syndrome by some
rats withdrawn from morphine at a final “stabiliza- means passively, coupled with non-reinforcement of
tion” dose level of 360 mg/kg per day showed small instrumental activity directed toward (unsuccessful)
but consistent differences from control rats for 4 to 6 acquisition of the drug. Practical realization of such
months. Conceivably, therefore, the duration of time an extinction procedure would depend on the devel-
over which relapse tests in the present study were opment of at least two new drugs, namely, one which
made may not have been sufficient to reveal the ef- if substituted for morphine would produce on abrupt
fects of conditioning and reinforcement during train- withdrawal a prolonged, though not necessarily severe,
ing as such. abstinence syndrome, and another which, though not
These two interpretations (alternative or com- effective in suppressing abstinence phenomena pro-
bined) of the data have a number of different conse- duced by withdrawal of the first drug, would be suf-
quences for further research and therapy in man. ficiently reinforcing on other grounds so that the
Thus, if the behavior of E’s (compared with C’s) in addict would “work” for it on some schedule of rein-
relapse tests was due only to residual hyperirritabil- forcement proven to be optimal by experiment. Of
ity, then the method of morphine withdrawal in the necessity, this phase of treatment would have to be
treatment of human addicts is not a crucial issue, carried out in an institution where, after its comple-
provided it is sufficiently painless to ensure the ad- tion and withdrawal of the abstinence-maintaining
dict’s cooperation. Rather, the problem is to deter- drug, therapy could be directed toward elimination
mine the exact physiological nature and the duration of the hypothetical residual hyperirritability discussed
of such residual hyperirritability and to devise meth- above.
ods of treatment to shorten or control it by pharma- In addition, some elementary principles of rein-
cologic or other means. By use of the cold pressor forcement therapy could be applied to what may be
test, Himmelsbach (12) was able to show that the called “positive reconditioning” of the patient while
autonomic reactivity of postaddicts is slightly greater he is still in the institution. This refers to scheduling
than that of normal subjects for over 6 months after of rewards for “work therapy.” Monetary payment
withdrawal of morphine. It is conceivable that with for socially useful work in crafts and industries within
more refined techniques, differences of this sort the institution would serve more effectively than ver-
would be revealed for even longer periods. If such bal reinforcement for supplanting physical depen-
proves to be the case, currently prevailing methods of dence on narcotic drugs as a motivational basis for
treatment-withdrawal of morphine by substitution “hustling” especially if such payment is made on a
and subsequent withdrawal of methadone, super- piecework (fixed ratio) schedule, and opportunities
vised abstention from drugs of all kinds in a drug- are provided for spending part of the wages for ob-
free environment for as long a period of time as is jects (other than drugs) that are immediately satisfy-
practicable combined with institutional and postinsti- ing to the patient.
tutional psychotherapy and social rehabilitative mea- Ideally, however, extinction of conditioned physi-
284 A. Wikier

cal dependence should be carried out in the addict’s 10. HILL, H.E. KORNETSKY, C.H., FLANARY,H.G., & WIXLER,A.:
natural “drug-available” environment, so that he Studies on Anxiety Associated with Anticipation of Pain: I.
Effects of Morphine, A.M.A. Arch. Neurol. Psychior., 67:612
could be exposed to that conditioned stimulus and
(1952).
the secondary reinforcers (addict society) repeatedly, 12. HIMMELSBACH, C.K.: Studies on the Relation of Drug Addic-
provided that the hoped-for extinction of drug-ac- tion to the Autonomic Nervous System: Results of Cold
quisitory behavior previously carried out in the insti- Pressor Tests, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap., 73:91 (1941).
tution, coupled with close surveillance and, if neces- 13. HUNOER,A. J., KEHRLE,J., ROSSI,A., & HOFFMAN,H.: Syn-
these basisch substituirter, analgetisch wirksamer Benzimid-
sary, appropriate nonnarcotic pharmacologic therapy
azolderivate, Experienriu, 13401 (1957).
and psychotherapy for suppression of conditioned 14. KOLB,L.: Drug Addiction As a Public Health Problem, Sci.
abstinence and reduction of anxieties probably in- Monthly, 48:391 (1939).
volving the same physiologic systems (stimulus or 15. KORNETSKY,C.H.: Effects of Anxiety and Morphine on the
drive generalization) is sufficiently effective to ensure Anticipation and Perception of Painful Radiant Thermal
Stimuli, J. Comp. and Physiol. Psycho/, 47: 130 (1954).
against reexposure to the unconditioned response,
16. LA~ACNA,L., VON FELSINOER,J.M., & BEECHER,H.K.: Drug
namely morphine-abstinence phenomena generated Induced Changes in Man: I. Observations on Healthy Sub-
by readdiction. jects, Chronically Ill Patients and “Post-addicts,” J. Am.
These speculations have been presented not for the Med. Assoc., 157:1006 (1955).
purpose of recommending immediate changes in our 17. MARTIN, W.R., WIKLER, A., EADES, C.G., & PESCOR,F.T.:
Tolerance to and Physical Dependence on Morphine in Rats
treatment programs for addicts but to illustrate how
(abstract), Phurmocologisf, 4: 154 (1%2).
restatement of some mentalistic concepts about drug 18. NICHOLS,J.R. AND DAVIS, W.M.: Drug Addiction: II. Varia-
addiction and relapse in behavioristic terms may en- tion of Addiction, J. Am. Phorm. Assoc., Sci. Ed., 48:259
able us to test their validity by experiment. Though, (1959).
as stated, the role of conditioning factors in mor- 19. NICHOLS,J.R., HEADLEE,C.P., & COPPOCK, H.W.: Drug Ad-
diction: I. Addiction by Escape Training. J. Am. Pharm.
phine addiction and relapse is not yet fully eluci-
Assoc., Sci. Ed., 44:229 (1955).
dated, the “public” operations by which they may be 20. SCHUSTER,C.R., & THOMPSON, T.: A Technique for Studying
are quite readily discernible, and further research Self-administration of opiates in Rhesus Monkeys, presented
along the lines indicated may yet enable us to deal at 25th Meeting, Committee on Drug Addiction and Nar-
with these clinical problems more effectively than cotics, National Academy of Sciences, National Research
Council, Ann Arbor, Mich., 16 February, 1963.
heretofore.
21. VON FELSINOER,J.M., LASAGNA, L., & BEECHER,H.K.: Drug
Induced Changes in Man: 2. Personality and Reactions to
REFERENCES Drugs, J. Am. Med. Assoc., 157-1113 (1955).
1. BEACH, H.D.: Morphine Addiction in Rats, Con. J. psychol., 22. WEEKS, J.R.: Experimental Morphine Addiction: Method for
11:104 (1957). Automatic Intravenous Injections in Unrestrained Rats,
2. D~vts, W.M. & NICHOLS,J.R.: Physical Dependence and Sus- Science, 138:143 (1%2).
tained Opiate-directed Behavior in the Rat: A Preliminary 23. WEEKS, J.R., & Cows, R.J.: Some Factors Affecting Per-
Report, Psychopharmacologia, 3:139 (1962). formance of Self-maintained Addict Rats, presented at 25th
3. DENEAU, GA., MCCARTHY, D.A., & SEEVERS, M.H.: Meeting, Committee on Drug Addiction and Narcotics, Na-
Physical Dependence Liability Studies in the Monkey, Add. I, tional Research Council, National Academy of Sciences, Ann
Min. 20th Meeting, Committee on Drug Addiction and Nar- Arbor, Mich.. 16 February, 1%3.
cotics, National Research Council, Washington, D.C., Na- 24. WIKLER, A.: Recent Progress in Research on the Neurophys-
tional Academy of Sciences, 10-l 1 January, 1959. iological Basis of Morphine Addiction, Am. J. Psychiat.,
4. FRASER, H.F., ISBELL, H., & WOLBACH,A.B., JR.: Addic- 105:329 (1948).
tiveness of New Synthetic Analgesics, Add. 2, Min. 21st 25. WIKLER,A.: A Psychodynamic Study of a Patient during Self-
Meeting, Committee on Drug Addiction and Narcotics, Na- regulated Readdiction to Morphine, Psychiut. Quart., 26:270
tional Research Council, Washington, D.C., National Acad- (1952).
emy of Sciences, 11-12 January, l%o. 26. WIKLER, A.: “Opiate Addiction: Psychological and Neuro-
5. Gross, R., B TURRJAN,H.: Ueber Benzimidazolderivate mit physiological Aspects in Relation to Clinical Problems,”
starker analgetischer Wirkung, Experientiu, 13:401 (1957). Charles C. Thomas, Publisher, Springfield, Ill., 1953.
6. HEMLEE, C.P., COPPOCK, H.W., & NICHOLS, J.R.: Ap- 27. WIKLER,A.: Rationale of the Diagnosis and Treatment of Ad-
paratus and Technique Involved in a Laboratory Method of diction, Conn. St&e Med. J., 19:5&l (1955).
Detecting Addictiveness of Drugs, J. Am. Phorm. Assoc., Sci. 28. WWER, A.: Mechanisms of Action of Opiates and Opiate
Ed., 44:229 (1955). Antagonists, Public Health Monograph, no. 52, Government
7. HILL, H.E., BELLEVILLE, R.E., & WIKLER,A.: Studies on Anx- Printing Office, Washington, D.C.. 1958.
iety Associated with Anticipation of Pain: II. Comparative 29. WIKLER, A.: On the Nature of Addiction and Habituation,
Effects of Pentobarbital and Morphine, A.M.A. Arch. Brit. J. Addict., 57~73 (1961).
Neurol. Psychiat., 73602 (1955). 30. WIXLER, A., GREEN, P.C.. Sa~rr~, H.D., & PESCOR,F.T.: Use
8. HILL, H.E., BELLEVILLE,R.E., & WIKLER, A.: Motivational of a Dilute Aqueous Solution (5 mcg/ml) of a Benzimidazole
Determinants in Modification of Behavior by Morphine and Derivative with Potent Morphine-like Actions Orally As a
Pentobartibal, A.M.A. Arch. Neural. Psychiat., 77~28(1957). Presumptive Reinforcing Agent in Conditioning of Drug-
9. Hnt., H.E., FLANARY,H.G., KORNETSKY, C.H., & WXLER, A.: seeking Behavior in Rats (abstract), Pederufion Proc., 19:22
Relationship of Electrically Induced Pain to the Amperage (1960).
and the Wattage of Shock Stimuli, J. Clin. Invest., 31~464 31. WIKLER, A., MARTIN,W.R., PESCOR, F.T., & EADES, C.G.:
(1952). Factors Regulating Oral Consumption of Etonitaxene Solu-
Conditioning Factors in Opiate Addiction 285

tion by Morphine-addicted Rats (abstract), Pharmacologist, Presented at 25th Meeting, Committee on Drug Addiction and
4:154 (1962). Narcotics, National Research Council, National Academy of
32. YANAGITA, T., DENEAU, GA., & SEEVERS,M.H.: Physical Sciences, Ann Arbor, Mich., 16 February, 1963.
Dependence to Opiates in the Monkey, with Demonstration.

You might also like