You are on page 1of 15

history: from those who wrote it

The process of problem-based learning: what works


and why
Henk G Schmidt,1 Jerome I Rotgans2 & Elaine HJ Yew3

OBJECTIVES In this review, we portray the that knowledge. These activities facilitate the
process of problem-based learning (PBL) as a comprehension of new information related to
cognitive endeavour whereby the learner the problem and enhance its long-term
constructs mental models relevant to problems. memorability. In addition, there is evidence
Two hypotheses are proposed to explain how that problems arouse situational interest that
learning is driven in PBL; an activation– drives learning. Flexible scaffolding provided
elaboration hypothesis and a situational by cognitively and socially congruent tutors
interest hypothesis. also seems to be reasonably effective, as
opposed to ‘hard’ scaffolding represented by,
METHODS Research relevant to these hypoth- for instance, worksheets or questions added to
eses is discussed. In addition, research studying problems. Small-group work protects against
the effects of various support strategies used in dropout and encourages students to study
PBL is reviewed. Finally, we summarise a num- regularly. Initially, students do not study
ber of recent studies in which a new ‘micro- much beyond the learning issues generated;
analytical’ methodology was used to trace the the development of personal agency in self-
process of PBL in the natural classroom setting. study needs time to develop. The extent of
learning in PBL results from neither group
CONCLUSIONS We conclude that there is collaboration only (the social constructivist
considerable support for the idea that PBL point of view) nor individual knowledge
works because it encourages the activation of acquisition only; both activities contribute
prior knowledge in the small-group setting equally to learning in PBL.
and provides opportunities for elaboration on

Medical Education 2011: 45: 792–806


doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2011.04035.x

1
Department of Psychology, Erasmus University, Rotterdam, Correspondence: Professor Henk G Schmidt, Department of
the Netherlands Psychology, Erasmus University, Burgemeester Oudlaan 50,
2
Centre for Research in Pedagogy and Practice, National Institute of Rotterdam 3062 PA, the Netherlands. Tel: 00 31 10 408 1749;
Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Fax: 00 31 10 408 9009; E-mail: schmidt@fsw.eur.nl
Singapore
3
Centre for Educational Development, Republic Polytechnic,
Singapore, Singapore

792 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

INTRODUCTION THE PROCESS OF PBL FROM A COGNITIVE


CONSTRUCTIVIST POINT OF VIEW
In this article we review current knowledge about
what works in problem-based learning (PBL) and The following description of the process of PBL will
why it works. We will not attempt here to review the be brief because its constituent elements from the
entire literature on the topic; the database PubMed perspective of the cognitive constructivist framework
alone presents over 5000 articles that use the term have been sketched previously.7 In PBL, learners are
‘problem-based learning’ in their titles or abstracts. presented with a problem in order to activate their
Nor will we make another attempt to review out- prior knowledge. This prior knowledge is then built
comes of PBL schools in comparison with those upon further as the learners collaborate in small
using conventional curricula. Curriculum compari- groups to construct a theory or proposed mental
son studies have been reviewed extensively over the model to explain the problem in terms of its
past 20 years.1–6 Here, we will focus on reviewing underlying causal structure. As learners continue to
studies that have examined what happens to the study related resources, their initial mental model is
learner in PBL in order to elucidate the process that further modified and refined. Moreover, as the
unfolds when students try to learn new material learners’ preconceptions are activated, they become
through this approach to learning. However, we more easily able to identify gaps in their prior
must first establish which learning process we refer knowledge, thus enabling better learning to take
to. In an earlier paper, we suggested that there are at place (the activation–elaboration hypothesis). Motiva-
least three different perspectives on PBL in the tional processes support these cognitive changes.
literature: PBL as a ‘process of inquiry’; PBL as Situational interest is aroused by the enigmatic
‘learning to learn’, and PBL as a ‘cognitive nature of the problem and acts as the motivating
constructivist’ approach, which defines the purpose force that drives the learner to engage with the
of PBL as helping students to construct mental literature and to continue to seek relevant informa-
models of the world.7 Fortunately, all three per- tion until his or her hunger for new information
spectives concur on the following PBL-defining related to the problem is satisfied (the situational
characteristics: (i) problems are used as a trigger interest hypothesis). These two hypotheses have been
for learning; (ii) students collaborate in small tested in a number of studies that will be reviewed in
groups for part of the time; (iii) learning takes place the next section.
under the guidance of a tutor; (iv) the curriculum
includes a limited number of lectures; (v) learning is
student-initiated, and (vi) the curriculum includes EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE RELEVANT TO THE TWO
ample time for self-study.8–12 As only the cognitive THEORETICAL CLAIMS
constructivist approach has led to a sizable volume
of research, we will confine our review to this In this section, we will summarise the evidence
interpretation. relevant to determining the mechanism by which PBL
affects learning. Firstly, we will present studies
In this paper we will first briefly describe the relevant to the activation–elaboration framework.
process of PBL, emphasising that it is a special way Secondly, we will review studies that relate to the
of acquiring knowledge of a domain. Secondly, we emergence of situational interest in PBL classrooms.
will interpret learning in this approach in terms of
two hypotheses derived from cognitive psychology – The activation–elaboration hypothesis
the activation–elaboration hypothesis and the
situational interest hypothesis – and review the The literature on the constructive nature of learning
evidence supporting these. Thirdly, we will look at and the role of elaboration through self-explana-
research into the educational aids intended to tion,13 discussion with peers,14 practising15 or
support learning based on problems, including: the responding to questions16 is extensive. What needs
problems; the tutorial group; the tutor; scaffolds, to be demonstrated here is that these processes are
and the self-directed learning activities of the also vital to the process of PBL. This involves the
students. Finally, we will discuss a number of review of a number of experiments designed to
recently conducted studies in which attempts were answer the following questions:
made to chart the learning process in PBL in the
natural classroom setting using a micro-analytical 1 Does the initial discussion of a problem lead to
methodology. the activation of previously acquired knowledge?

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 793
H G Schmidt et al

2 Is there evidence that having to explain the watched the video and listened to the discussion, or
problem leads to elaboration? were prompted to actively provide explanations them-
3 Do these activities facilitate the comprehension selves a number of times while watching the discussion.
and retention of relevant new information? In this way, the investigators were able to keep the
information provided by other students constant over
In a series of early studies that aimed to elucidate the the conditions of the experiment (all participants
role of prior knowledge activation and elaboration in saw the same video). Subsequently, both groups
the initial discussion of a problem, Schmidt and studied the same problem-related text for the same
colleagues17–19 presented students who had studied amount of time and were tested for knowledge twice:
the biological process of osmosis during high school immediately and after 1 month. The authors found
with either a problem unrelated to osmosis or with that immediately after studying the text, the elabora-
the following problem: ‘A red blood cell is put in tion group had a 28% higher score. After a month
pure water under a microscope. The cell swells and this difference had increased to 30%.21 It seems that
eventually bursts. Another blood cell is added to a elaboration in a small group not only facilitates the
solution of salt in water. It shrinks. Explain these processing of a study text, but also adds to its longer-
phenomena.’ When subsequently asked to recall term memorability. This view is reinforced by other
whatever they remembered about osmosis, the reports of the long-term effects of PBL.22–24
students who had discussed the blood cell problem
produced almost twice as many ideas about osmosis as The situational interest hypothesis
the students involved in an unrelated task. This
finding suggests that the initial discussion of a The situational interest hypothesis underlying PBL
problem does indeed have a considerable effect on states that problems or puzzles create a desire in
the activation and elaboration of previously learned students to find out more about the topic, which
knowledge. The authors were, however, unable to leads to increased concentration, focused attention
distinguish the influence of activation from the effect and a willingness to learn.25 Situational interest, as
of elaboration (the students were prompted to the term implies, is not a stable or dispositional form
explain) and nor were they able to exclude the of interest, but is situationally aroused by an intrigu-
possibility that the students simply learned from ing or captivating puzzle or problem. The psycho-
one another in the course of providing their expla- logical processes underlying this desire to learn can best
nations and thereby gained more knowledge. be explained in terms of the incongruity hypothesis
To resolve these issues, De Grave et al.20 compared proposed by epistemic curiosity researchers (see
the effects of small-group discussion of a problem Loewenstein26 for a review). Humans seem to have a
with those of individual problem analysis and direct natural tendency to wish to make sense of the world:
prompting for knowledge about osmosis. They dis- when they encounter something they do not under-
covered that small-group discussion had a larger stand or something that violates their expectations,
positive effect than individual problem analysis; direct they experience situational interest because this state
prompting for knowledge had the smallest effect. of affairs makes them aware of a knowledge gap
The assignment to explain leads students to further between what they know and what they want to know.
elaborate on their prior knowledge even in the This phenomenon has been referred to as a cogni-
absence of other students, but group discussion has a tively induced experience of (knowledge) depriva-
considerable additional effect, suggesting that tion.27,28 This experience of deprivation initiates
elaboration on prior knowledge and learning from information-seeking behaviour intended to close the
one another (even before new information is knowledge gap (i.e. learning occurs).29 As the
acquired) are potent means of facilitating knowledge gap closes, through the assimilation of
understanding of problem-relevant information. new information into existing knowledge structures,
situational interest decreases until the knowledge
The text comprehension studies discussed so far had equilibrium is re-established.30
difficulty in studying effects of elaboration (through
the explanatory activities required of the students) in Studies testing the situational interest hypothesis in
isolation. To deal with this problem, van Blankenstein the context of PBL are limited because situational
et al.21 introduced a new research paradigm for study- interest has only become the focus of attention
ing the effects of elaboration in small groups on the recently (but see31,32). The central assumption of this
learning of individual students. They taped a group hypothesis is that a problem presented in the context
discussion about a problem and presented this video to of learning arouses situational interest because it
individual students. These students either only confronts students more extensively with what they

794 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

still do not understand. In turn, this perception of Soppe et al.40 investigated whether problem familiarity
ignorance acts as a driving force to engage in had an influence on student learning. In an exper-
learning. To test this idea, Rotgans and associates imental set-up, students worked with either a ‘famil-
conducted a number of studies using a short rating iar’ or an ‘unfamiliar’ version of the same problem. A
scale that mentions the topic to be studied and measure of perceived problem quality was adminis-
consists of items such as ‘I want to know more about tered and outcome measures, such as the number of
this topic’ and ‘I think this topic is interesting’.30,33 explanations for the problem put forward by the
These studies tested the level of situational interest of students, the quality of the learning issues derived
students at several time-points, including on their from the discussion, the amount of time spent on self-
arrival in the classroom, after a relevant problem had study and the amount of knowledge gained as
been presented and after some initial discussion measured by a test were also obtained. The results
about the problem. They demonstrated that the showed that participants in the familiar-problem
presentation of the problem significantly increased condition perceived the problem to be of higher
the level of situational interest in the students and quality than participants in the unfamiliar-problem
this increase was maintained during the small-group condition. However, no significant differences in
discussion.30,33 learning were found. The authors40 proposed that
problems might be improved by making them more
relevant to the everyday experience of students, an
RESEARCH ON PBL SUPPORT STRATEGIES important consideration also stressed by others.41

In this section, we will review research focusing on the A number of studies have taken a more holistic
means through which learning in PBL is fostered, perspective on the role of problems in PBL.42–45
including: the use of problems; small-group collabo- These studies sought to determine the problem’s
ration; tutors; scaffolds, and self-directed learning influence on learning in the context of other
(SDL). variables influencing learning. To this end, they
tested causal models of PBL using structural equation
Roles of problems in PBL modelling in which all key elements of PBL were
included. They were able to demonstrate that prob-
Most of the research on problems in PBL revolves lem quality (defined in terms of how clear the
around the issue of what makes a good problem in problem appears to students, its ability to stimulate
the views of tutors and students.34–37 The general interest and to trigger group discussion, etc.) as
message from these studies is that a problem should: perceived by students is a major source of influence
(i) be authentic; (ii) be adapted to the students’ level in PBL. Problem quality influences not only the
of prior knowledge; (iii) engage students in discus- quality of small-group discussion, but also time spent
sion; (iv) lead to the identification of appropriate on self-study and interest in the subject matter.
learning issues; (v) stimulate SDL, and (vi) be
interesting. Of course, a key question concerns how Importance of small-group collaboration
these attributes influence student learning in PBL,
such as how problems lead to the identification of The cognitive benefits of small-group cooperation
appropriate learning issues. Dolmans et al.38 investi- have been discussed extensively by various
gated the effectiveness of problems in a course by authors46,47 and need no further elaboration here.
comparing the learning issues identified by students However, we would like to focus on three other
with those intended by faculty members. The aspects of the small-group tutorial. Firstly, the small
assumption behind this investigation was that an tutorial group provides a platform for the develop-
effective problem should lead students to identify ment of friendships among students. Secondly, it
the learning issues referred to by the problem enables closer contacts between students and teach-
designers when they developed the problem. The ers compared with those possible in a larger class.
results of this study revealed that, on average, 64% of Thirdly, the regularity of small-group tutorials in the
the learning issues identified by students matched PBL environment generates peer pressure that is
those intended by faculty, with large differences useful in motivating students to be diligent in their
among individual problems. Interestingly, many of self-study and to meet the deadlines for work agreed
the non-intended learning issues generated by by the group. These two non-cognitive side-effects of
students were found to be considered relevant in small-group collaboration have been found to be
hindsight by the problem designers.38 Similar advantageous in preventing dropout from school48,49
findings were reported by Mpofu et al.39 and may be a reason why students in PBL

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 795
H G Schmidt et al

curricula tend to graduate faster than students at experts were able to generate more than twice as many
conventional schools.50–52 learning issues for SDL and that these learning issues
were almost three times more congruent with case
Role of the tutor objectives compared with those generated by students
who were guided by non-experts. Moreover, com-
The tutor’s role in a PBL tutorial differs from that in pared with the latter group, students with content-
a conventional tutorial. In PBL, tutors are expected expert tutors put in double the amount of time in self-
to facilitate or activate student learning and to study. Similarly, Davis et al.59 found no significant
promote effective group functioning by encouraging differences between expert-led and non-expert-led
the active participation of all members, monitoring groups in terms of teacher-directed and student-
the quality of learning and intervening when initiated interactions, but instead demonstrated in-
necessary.53–56 Tutors also play active roles in the creased student achievement and satisfaction in
scaffolding of student learning by providing a groups led by experts. Both groups of authors59,64
framework that students can use to construct knowl- suggest that the expert tutors, by virtue of their subject
edge on their own.57 Because it encourages students knowledge expertise, were better at posing questions
to think more deeply and offers some modelling of at critical moments, thus positively influencing stu-
the types of questions students should be asking dent learning. However, although the proficiency of
themselves during problem solving, the tutor–student content experts in using their subject matter expertise
relationship can be viewed as supporting a type of to direct student discussion has positive effects on
cognitive apprenticeship.10,58 student learning, their knowledge of when and how to
use this expertise to facilitate learning is more
In view of this shift in the tutor’s role in the student- beneficial. Thus, ideally, a tutor should be expert in
centred PBL process, many studies have sought to both the respective subject matter and in facilitating
better understand how tutors contribute to student student learning processes.
learning in PBL. One subject of considerable debate
has centred around the importance of the tutor’s One theory of the effective tutor merges these two
subject matter expertise: is it sufficient for tutors to qualities.65 A key idea in this theory is the concept of
have good facilitation skills or do they also need ‘cognitive congruence’, which is defined as the
substantial knowledge of the subject matter? Results tutor’s ability to express him or herself in the
from several studies demonstrate that tutor expertise language of the students, using the concepts they use
has a significant effect on student learning out- and explaining things in ways they can easily grasp.
comes,59 whereas other studies indicate that it has no Schmidt and Moust66 suggested that both subject
noticeable effects.60,61 One hypothesis that may matter expertise and social congruence were neces-
explain this contradiction in findings is that the sary conditions for cognitive congruence to emerge.
subject matter expertise of the tutor impacts on In this context, social congruence refers to interper-
student learning more significantly when the cues and sonal qualities such as the ability to communicate
scaffolds within the problems and resources are informally and empathically with students, and hence
insufficient to guide students in the process of the ability to create a learning environment that
identifying what is important to study. In such situa- encourages the open exchange of ideas. Thus, it was
tions, students are more likely to depend on their tutor hypothesised that a tutor who is more socially
for guidance and thus a tutor who is more knowl- congruent and better able to use subject matter
edgeable in the subject matter is of more benefit.62 expertise would be more cognitively congruent.
Using structural equation modelling, the authors66
Another group of studies investigated the influence of demonstrated that both social congruence and
tutors’ content expertise on the tutorial process. Silver subject expertise influenced cognitive congruence,
and Wilkerson63 found that tutors with subject matter which, in turn, influenced tutorial group functioning
expertise were more inclined to play a directive role in and thus indirectly affected the level of student
the tutoring process, to speak more often and for achievement by increasing the time spent on self-
longer periods, to supply more direct answers to study. Social congruence directly influenced group
questions posed by students, and to suggest more functioning during the problem-solving process and
points for discussion. They concluded that these tutor the subject matter expertise of the tutor had a slight
behaviours might have a negative impact on the direct positive impact on student achievement.
development of students’ skills in active SDL and also Hence, this study showed that effective tutoring that
in collaborative learning. By contrast, Eagle et al.64 results in better student achievement requires the
found that students with tutors who were content tutor to have both content knowledge and the ability

796 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

to interact with students on a personal level, as well as facilitator as they worked on a problem over 5 hours
to utilise language students easily understand.66 in two sessions, these authors demonstrate how the
facilitator can support knowledge building through
Chng et al.67 hypothesised that tutors who exhibit the use of open-ended questions that serve as
more cognitively congruent behaviours would influ- scaffolds for students.70
ence knowledge construction and acquisition at each
learning phase in the PBL process. Thus, students The use of scaffolds in PBL
who are under the tutelage of such tutors would be
more extensively involved in the construction of From the descriptions above, it is clear that the role of
knowledge (i.e. they would recall more relevant the tutor as a scaffold to facilitate meaningful learn-
concepts in each PBL phase) and would ultimately ing is one that is generally agreed upon. Although
achieve better results at the end of the learning Kirschner et al.71 suggest that PBL is a minimally
process. Tutor behaviours were assessed by students guided approach and is therefore less effective and
and the tutors were subsequently divided into three efficient in helping students learn, others argue that
groups according to their levels of subject matter PBL provides extensive guidance and scaffolding to
expertise, cognitive congruence and social congru- help students learn.72,73
ence. Student achievement at the end of the PBL
cycle was measured using an essay test. An analysis of The metaphor of scaffolding refers to the temporary
covariance (ANCOVA) using students’ pre-existing support provided for learners to help them complete
grade point average (GPA) scores as the covariate a task they would otherwise not be able to complete on
found that, contrary to the authors’ original hypoth- their own.74 Although scaffolds can take multiple
esis, only the social congruence of tutors had a forms, Saye and Brush75 suggest that most scaffolds
significant influence on the students’ learning can be classified as either ‘soft’ or ‘hard’. Soft scaffolds
process.67 This effect of social congruence was are dynamic and refer to tutor actions that support
evident in the total number of concepts recalled at specific learner needs, as described in the section on
the end of the PBL phases of problem analysis, SDL the role of the tutor in PBL.75–77 By contrast, hard
and reporting. No significant effects were found for scaffolds are static supports that can be developed in
the subject expertise and cognitive congruence of the advance based on typically expected learner difficul-
tutor on any of the learning phases in the PBL ties associated with a task.75 Such scaffolds can take
process.67 (However, in line with the findings of the form of computer- or paper-based cognitive tools,
Schmidt and Moust,66 all three tutor behaviours had such as worksheets.78 In line with the metaphor,
a significant effect on final student achievement.) scaffolds should gradually be withdrawn or fade from
The results indicate that the social congruence of the use as the learner becomes increasingly responsible
tutor influences the learning process more substan- for his or her own learning.79 For example, although
tially than cognitive congruence and subject matter novices in a PBL environment may be supported
expertise, at least in this educational context. The initially with some resources that scaffold their
willingness of a tutor to establish an informal learning, as their expertise increases, these students
relationship with his or her students and to display an should be provided with fewer resources. In this
attitude of genuine interest therefore has significant way, students develop as independent learners
impact. This is possibly because socially congruent through a form of flexible scaffolding.73
tutors are able to create a non-threatening learning
environment and to develop strong tutor–student Studies investigating the use and impact of hard
relationships that support the open exchange of ideas scaffolds in the PBL context are rather limited. A
and questions and promote student engagement in study by Simons and Klein80 examined the impact of
the learning process. hard scaffolds on the learning outcomes of middle-
school learners during the implementation of a PBL
Some studies on tutors move away from investigating unit. Students were subjected to one of three exper-
the tutor’s role in influencing student achievement imental conditions, including one in which no
alone and instead focus on the student learning scaffolding was provided, one in which optional
process and issues such as how and when a tutor scaffolding was provided, and one in which students
should intervene in the event of difficult incidents in were required to use all the scaffolds provided.
tutorial groups.68,69 Hmelo-Silver and Barrows70 Results showed that students in the optional-
describe in detail how ‘collaborative knowledge and compulsory-scaffold conditions performed
building’ is facilitated. By analysing the discourse that significantly better than students in the no-scaffold
took place between five medical students and their condition. In addition, those in the compulsory-

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 797
H G Schmidt et al

scaffold condition produced better organised cluded that the learning issues produced during
project notebooks containing a higher percentage of group discussion may not represent the only factors
relevant entries. The PBL environment described in on which students base their decisions of what to
this study was one with very little consistent teacher study during self-study.86 Other factors may be
support (soft scaffolding), in which students gener- involved, such as tutor suggestions, content covered
ally performed poorly. Simons and Klein80 therefore in previous courses, literature found during self-study
concluded that hard scaffolds seem to enhance and the nature of the learning resources available.
student performance, especially under circumstances Interestingly, first-year students tended to focus on
of limited teacher support. the learning issues agreed upon more than older
students.86 It appears that students become more self-
Another study investigating the effect of worksheets directed as the years of study progress. A similar
as a scaffolding tool on student achievement in a PBL finding was reported by van den Hurk et al.,87 who
environment was carried out by Choo et al.81 Forty- found that students in their first year of study
eight teams (241 students) were randomly assigned to adhered strictly to the learning issues, whereas in
one of two experimental groups; students in one later years students pursued their personal learning
group were provided with a worksheet and students interests to a larger extent. In addition, students who
in the other group were not. No statistically signifi- tended to study beyond the agreed learning issues
cant difference emerged between the two groups in spent more time on individual study and performed
terms of their post-lesson concept recall tests. The better on achievement tests.
findings of Choo et al.81 therefore suggest that
scaffolds such as worksheets may not play a significant As SDL is considered to be a key element in PBL,
role in enhancing student learning within this PBL helping students develop the ability to regulate their
context. Furthermore, results from a survey adminis- own learning is an important priority. Some studies
tered to the students in this study indicated that the have demonstrated outcomes that can be considered
strongest factor to impact on their learning was the to result from the emphasis on SDL in PBL. Students
tutor, followed by the tutorial group; the worksheet in a PBL curriculum have, for instance, been shown
was rated as having the lowest influence.81 Two other to borrow more books from the library than students
experiments have examined the effect of hard scaf- in conventional curriculum schools.88–90 These find-
folds in the form of questions provided as guidelines ings suggest that PBL students are more independent
for self-study in addition to the problem.82,83 In one learners and take more personal responsibility for
of these,82 the presence or absence of the scaffolds their learning.
did not matter; in the other,83 scaffolding was even
detrimental to achievement.
CHARTING PBL IN THE CLASSROOM
In conclusion, the effects of scaffolding in PBL are
rather inconclusive and more research is needed A micro-analytical approach to studying learning
here. Problem-based learning was originally devel- processes in PBL
oped in medical schools for use with relatively mature
and motivated learners, a context in which hard Any attempt to study PBL as it occurs in its natural
scaffolds may have been of limited value. However, in habitat – the PBL classroom – or to survey the
view of the increasing implementation of PBL by underlying mechanisms of learning during PBL must
educators of students at different levels of education seek new approaches to measurement. Convention-
and in various disciplines,84,85 research to shed ally, motivation and learning data are collected at the
further light on the role of scaffolding to support end of a course. These responses provide an
student performance in PBL is necessary. ‘averaged’ impression of how motivated students
were during the course or how much they learned.
Self-directed learning Although such a measurement approach is useful
when the focus of interest is students’ overall moti-
Dolmans et al.86 examined the relationship between vation or learning, this type of measurement may
student-generated learning issues and self-study. As in hide important contextual variations during learning.
an earlier study,38 they found considerable overlap The alternative approach involves carrying out
between the learning issues identified by students detailed classroom observation: a learning event is
and the faculty-intended objectives, but increasing videotaped and subsequently transcribed. Although
this match by improving problem quality did not this measurement approach tends to provide more
result in more self-study time. Therefore, they con- detail on what is happening in a classroom, the

798 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

resulting qualitative dataset usually poses consider- In a follow-up study, Rotgans et al. (unpublished data)
able technical and interpretational challenges. applied the same micro-analytical measurement
approach to determine students’ situational interest
In the subsequent sections, we present a series of in both PBL and direct instruction (DI). In a quasi-
studies on motivation and learning in PBL using an experiment study involving several classes of primary
approach we refer to as micro-analytical measure- school pupils, the authors ensured that all factors
ment.33,91 In this micro-analytical measurement were kept constant, except for the instructional
approach, a short questionnaire or a short knowledge approach. For instance, situational interest measures
test is re-administered several times at critical points were administered at the same time-points to both
during the course of a learning event. These repeated groups, the same teacher was present, the same
measurements are subsequently related to one learning resources were available and the same
another and to final outcomes of learning. Adminis- amount of time was allocated in both groups. The
tering a measure several times over a learning event results of this study revealed that the PBL group was
may provide insights into what actually happens consistently more interested than the DI group on all
during learning. From this, inferences can be made but two measurement occasions (Rotgans et al.,
about the mechanisms underlying PBL. unpublished data). Fig. 1 shows an overview.

Interest is a driving force in PBL but is consumed The first of these differences in interest occurred at
over the course of learning the start of the session, just before the PBL group
read the problem and the DI group was presented
In a study by Rotgans and Schmidt,33 a micro- with some examples to demonstrate the application
analytical measurement approach was applied to of the topic to a real-life context. This was to be
investigate how students’ situational interest develops expected because this time-point gave a measure of
during PBL. Five short measures of situational inter- students’ situational interest before the treatment
est were administered over the course of a 1-day PBL took place. Students in both conditions showed
session. The first two measures were administered, ‘equally little interest in the topic’. However, when
respectively, immediately before and after the pre- students in the PBL group were presented with the
sentation of the problem to determine whether the problem, their situational interest increased signifi-
problem would trigger students’ interest. The third cantly. After the problem had been presented, the
measurement was conducted after the discussion of level of situational interest in the PBL group
the problem and the generation of learning issues. decreased significantly, as in the study by Rotgans
The fourth administration took place after self-study and Schmidt.33 In the DI group, situational interest
to determine whether SDL activities had an effect on decreased significantly from the start when the group
situational interest. The fifth and last measurement was presented with some examples (instead of the
took place after the final discussion. The results of problem). This outcome demonstrates that the DI
the study showed that once the problem had been group did not experience the positive surge in
presented, students’ situational interest increased situational interest generated by the problem and the
significantly. However, as the learning event pro- corresponding feeling of knowledge deprivation.
gressed, situational interest decreased: it seemed as if Their interest decreased immediately from the onset.
the initial increase in situational interest created by However, the DI group showed one significant
the problem was slowly consumed over the course increase during self-study, which occurred when this
of the learning event. The authors used the situa- group was given the opportunity to engage in
tional interest hypothesis to explain this phenome- independent group discussion to discuss some real-
non. The experience of being confronted with a life applications of the topic. At this time-point there
problem that contains unknowns that need to be was no difference in situational interest between the
known triggers awareness of knowledge deprivation PBL and DI groups (Rotgans et al., unpublished
in the form of a knowledge gap that must be bridged data).
by the finding of information about the unknowns.
Knowledge acquired during self-study closes this gap. Overall, applying a micro-analytical approach to
As situational interest is an indicator of the existence the study of student interest in PBL revealed the
of such a gap, its decrease over time provides underlying situational interest processes, which
empirical support for this hypothesis. This study also would have been difficult to achieve with conven-
showed that situational interest predicted students’ tional survey measures or classroom observations. In
academic achievement with considerable accuracy, the following study the micro-analytical measurement
demonstrating that it drives learning.33 approach was applied to measure how students’

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 799
H G Schmidt et al

DI group PBL group


4.00
Identification
of learning 3.80
Presentation goals Self-study
of the 3.72 3.67 Presentation Elaboration
problem of findings 3.63 of what was
learned

Level of situational interest


3.52 3.54
3.50 Students
were
informed 3.28
about the
topic
Assignment
3.17 List of
learning
3.00 objectives Elaboration
Presentation
and group
2.86 Discussion of what was
of examples assignment 2.79 learned 2.82
2.95 activity

Lesson 1 (1 hour) Lesson 2 (1.5 hours) Lesson 3 (1 hour)

2.50
SI1 SI2 SI3 SI4 SI5 SI6
Points at which situational interest was measured

Figure 1 Mean levels of situational interest (SI) over the learning event in the direct instruction (DI) and problem-based
learning (PBL) groups and description of learning activities. (Based on Fig. 1 in Rotgans et al., unpublished data)

knowledge construction develops during the learning in pre- and post-tests, were analysed using structural
process. equation modelling. Results demonstrated that only
the hypothesised model had a good fit with the data
Cumulative process of learning in PBL obtained.92 Fig. 2 shows an overview.

The idea that learning is cumulative and that new This model showed that students’ prior knowledge
learning depends on what has been learned previ- significantly influenced learning in the first PBL phase
ously is generally accepted by most educationalists. (problem analysis), which then strongly influenced
However, most of the evidence for this has come from learning at the end of the SDL phase. Similarly,
experiments in psychology laboratories, particularly learning at the end of the SDL phase significantly
in the field of text comprehension. Yew and impacted on learning at the end of the reporting
Schmidt92 argue that learning outcomes in PBL can phase. Finally, students’ learning achievements were
only reflect collaborative processes because social significantly influenced by learning at the end of the
constructivism suggests that knowledge is constructed reporting phase. By contrast, results from the alter-
through collaborative interactions and co-construc- native hypotheses tested indicated that collaborative
tions.93,94 By contrast, it is also possible to infer that small-group learning or self-directed study alone were
the individual SDL phase more significantly influ- insufficient to account for learning in PBL. These
ences student learning in PBL as studies on self- findings also demonstrate that the sequential nature
regulated learning have demonstrated that the use of of learning in PBL (from the problem analysis phase to
self-regulated learning strategies strongly impacts on SDL to the reporting phase) is essential in influencing
students’ academic outcomes.95 student learning outcomes.92

Yew and Schmidt92 therefore tested the hypothesis


that PBL is cumulative and that learning in PBL DISCUSSION
involves both collaborative and self-directed learning.
They also tested three alternative hypotheses: (i) only We have sketched a picture of PBL that emphasises its
the collaborative learning phases influence learning cognitive constructive nature. We have reviewed
in PBL; (ii) only the self-directed study phase influ- empirical evidence supporting six propositions. The
ences learning in PBL, and (iii) both collaborative first is that the initial discussion of a problem in a
learning and self-directed study influence learning in small group of students leads to the activation of
PBL, but not in a cumulative process. The units of prior knowledge. This prior knowledge is elaborated
analysis used as an indicator of student learning at upon to collaboratively construct a tentative theory
the end of each PBL phase were the relevant concepts explaining the phenomena described in the problem.
recalled by 218 students at the end of each learning The cognitive constructions that result from this
phase. These concepts, together with students’ results exercise in the minds of these students subsequently

800 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

0.33

0.45 Concepts recalled 0.75 Concepts recalled 0.77 Concepts recalled 0.28
Prior knowledge after problem after self-directed after reporting Achievement
analysis phase learning phase phase

Presentation Self- Reporting of


of the directed findings
problem learning

Figure 2 Path model for the hypothetical model of relationships among different phases of problem-based learning. Numbers
indicate standardised regression weights. (Based on Fig. 1 in Yew & Schmidt92; reproduced here with permission)

facilitate the comprehension of new information and than students in conventional programmes, which
its long-term survival.96 suggests that these students are more self-directed in
their learning. However, students need time to
Next, problems drive learning by generating situa- develop this propensity. One study showed that
tional interest. We have demonstrated that the students at the beginning of PBL do not study much
introduction of a problem in the learning situation beyond the learning issues generated in the small
leads to an increase in situational interest in the topic group.87 Real personal agency appears to require
at hand, and that this situational interest is largely time to develop, but when it does, it has surplus value
maintained over the course of learning (although to over learning driven solely by external stimuli.
some extent it seems to be ‘satisfied’ by the new
knowledge acquired and diminishes over time). A Finally, the extent of learning in PBL does not result
higher level of situational interest, in turn, relates to from either group collaboration (the social
higher levels of achievement.33 constructivist point of view), or individual knowledge
acquisition in isolation: both activities contribute
Thirdly, as well as providing cognitive benefits, small- equally to learning in PBL.
group tutorials contribute to students’ feelings of
being ‘at home’ in their class, both socially and In summary, PBL seems to have fairly strong effects on
academically, and these protect against dropout. In learning and achievement compared with conditions
addition, as these groups are focused on interaction in which learning is not driven by the presentation of
in which students are expected to explain subject problems. The studies reviewed generally showed
matter to one another, free riders are discouraged learning gains in PBL students that extended beyond
and students are encouraged to study regularly. This those in students in control conditions in which
may be a reason why PBL curricula tend to have problems were not the focus of attention or in which
higher graduation rates.97 students were not encouraged to elaborate on their
prior knowledge. These findings seem to be at variance
Fourthly, tutors’ subject matter knowledge, ability to with the findings of curriculum comparison studies
relate to students and ability to be cognitively congru- that generally do not report PBL to have effects
ent with students all contribute to learning in PBL. superior to those of forms of conventional training.2 It
Findings in this area suggest that good tutors provide seems that although effects are found at the micro
flexible scaffolding by supporting student learning level, these do not translate into visible effects at the
on a ‘just-in-time’ basis. Experiences with so-called curriculum level. This then raises the question of why
‘hard’ scaffolds, such as those generated when work- this is so. What might explain this apparent paradox?
sheets or additional questions are supplied, were more
equivocal; the few studies conducted in this area We can offer only some tentative hypotheses. The first
suggest limited effects and one even showed hard is a ‘compensation’ hypothesis. There is some evi-
scaffolds to be detrimental to learning.81,83 dence that students who study under less favourable
circumstances tend to compensate by working
Fifthly, students in the PBL classroom have been harder.98,99 This would imply that although students
shown to be more ardent users of library resources profit more from PBL, they compensate for this

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 801
H G Schmidt et al

additional support by working less hard. This is PBL has a particular effect on the long-term
unlikely because students in PBL curricula tend to memorability of learned material, even if initially no
spend more time on individual learning than those effects are found.22–24 If this is true, there may be
in more conventional environments. several reasons for it. For instance, as the problem that
originally drove the learning will be stored in the
A second hypothesis, more parsimonious with the memory along with the material learned, it is possible
micro-level finding reviewed in this article, has been that such a problem acts as a cue for the retrieval of
proposed recently. This hypothesis assumes that the knowledge acquired and thus increases its
differential dropout and study duration mask the accessibility at a later stage.102 A related explanation
effects of PBL at the curriculum level. It takes as its may be that students in PBL organise knowledge in
starting point the observation that PBL curricula memory differently in a manner that facilitates its
tend to show less dropout and shorter study duration later retrieval. Studies of possible differences in
than conventional schools.52,97 Comparisons knowledge organisation that affect the use of that
between PBL and conventional schools may there- knowledge in new contexts – the transfer issue – are
fore be biased against PBL; effects of PBL become virtually non-existent in education research,103 but
hidden by differential attrition and differential study are sorely needed.
duration. To test this hypothesis, Schmidt and
colleagues (unpublished data) re-analysed 104 cur- In the Introduction to this paper, we suggested that
riculum comparisons among schools for which different authors tend to define the ultimate goals
dropout rates and study duration information were of PBL in different ways. This paper took as its point of
available. By correcting for differences on these departure cognitive psychology and well-known prin-
variables, they were able to demonstrate robust ciples of constructivist learning, with an emphasis on
effects of PBL on both knowledge attainment and how people acquire knowledge. However, a point of
diagnostic performance. It seems that micro-level view particularly prevalent in many medical schools
effects of PBL do replicate at the curriculum tends to define PBL as a process of inquiry.104,105 From
level. this perspective, the ultimate objective of PBL is to help
students develop the ‘inquiry’ or ‘problem-solving’
Given that well over 5000 studies referring to PBL are skills of an expert by imitating his or her thinking
represented in PubMed, what interesting avenues processes. Working on a problem is seen as a simula-
remain for further research? We will confine tion of what the doctor does, particularly in its
ourselves to two suggestions here. The tutorial group, emphasis on data gathering and interpretation. In this
vital to the inner workings of PBL, remains in many view of PBL, the role of knowledge acquisition is
respects a black box. For instance, differences in somewhat vague. Although most of its proponents
epistemological beliefs among students have been acknowledge that knowledge is needed to fill gaps in
shown to influence achievement (i.e. students who the student’s knowledge base, from this perspective
believe that learning is knowledge construction PBL is clearly focused on the acquisition of problem-
perform better than other students100). Why is this solving skills. This perspective is attractive to
so? Are groups that are heterogeneous in terms of medical educators because professionals in the field
prior knowledge more successful than homogeneous seem to think that their expertise is indeed partly based
groups, as some studies seem to suggest?101 Despite on the possession of such elusive skills. However, thirty
the importance in the small group of elaboration on years of research in this domain have made it clear that
what one already knows, students tend to avoid it is unlikely that problem-solving skills can be learned
elaboration on the incorrect assumption that every- through education, or even that such things as
body in the group already knows what the individual problem-solving skills, independent of subject matter
knows. Would it help if tutors explicitly prompted for knowledge, exist.106 If this is true, it indicates that there
further elaboration? Tutors were shown to be more are no shortcuts to acquiring expertise in medicine;
effective when they were able to explain things in a that there are no domain-independent problem-
simple way. What does this imply? Do cognitively solving skills the acquisition of which could compen-
congruent tutors explain more items in the tutorial sate to some extent for lack of knowledge acquisition.
group? What would happen if tutors were instructed The cognitive constructivist perspective on PBL incor-
to explicitly provide scaffolds whenever their group porates this view that clinical reasoning is knowledge-
reached a dead end in their discussion? based. It sees learning in the field of medicine as a
series of attempts to construct mental models of the
The second suggestion refers to long-term memory for underlying mechanisms of disease in terms of how it is
what has been learned. Some literature implies that produced, the conditions under which it appears, and

802 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

its consequences in terms of observable signs and 10 Hmelo-Silver CE. Problem-based learning: what and
symptoms in patients. These mental models of disease, how do students learn? Educ Psychol Rev 2004;16
or ‘illness scripts’, are the cognitive tools that doctors (3):235–66.
use when they engage in clinical reasoning.107 The task 11 Schmidt HG. Problem-based learning: rationale and
description. Med Educ 1983;17 (1):11–6.
of medical education is to help students develop these
12 Schmidt HG. Foundations of problem-based learning –
cognitive structures. Problem-based learning may be a
some explanatory notes. Med Educ 1993;27 (5):422–32.
useful way of doing so. 13 Pressley M, Wood E, Woloshyn VE, Martin V, King A,
Menke D. Encouraging mindful use of prior knowl-
edge – attempting to construct explanatory answers
Contributors: HS initiated and coordinated the writing of facilitates learning. Educ Psychol 1992;27 (1):91–109.
this review. He provided the conceptual outline and final 14 Reder LM. The role of elaboration in the compre-
plan of the manuscript. JR contributed sections on hension and retention of prose: a critical review.
situational interest research and the micro-analytical Rev Educ Res 1980;50 (1):5–53.
approach. He also reviewed the role of problems in 15 Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and the acquisition
problem-based learning (PBL). EY reviewed studies on the and maintenance of expert performance in medicine
role of the tutor, scaffolds used in PBL and research on the and related domains. Acad Med 2004;79 (10 Suppl):70–
cumulative nature of learning. All authors contributed to 81.
the critical revision of the paper and approved the final 16 Chi MTH, Siler SA, Jeong H, Yamauchi T, Hausmann
manuscript for submission. RG. Learning from human tutoring. Cogn Sci 2001;25
Acknowledgements: none. (4):471–533.
Funding: none. 17 Moust JHC, Schmidt HG, De Volder ML, Beliën JJJ, De
Conflicts of interest: none. Grave WS. Effects of verbal participation in small-
Ethical approval: not applicable. group discussion on learning. In: Richardson JTE,
Eysenck ME, Piper DW, eds. Student Learning: Research
in Education and Cognitive Psychology. Milton Keynes:
Open University Press 1987;212–18.
REFERENCES
18 Schmidt HG. Activatie van voorkennis en tekstverwer-
king [Activation of prior knowledge and text process-
1 Albanese MA, Mitchell S. Problem-based learning: a
ing]. Ned Tijdschr Psychol 1984;39:335–47.
review of literature on its outcomes and implementa-
19 Schmidt HG, De Grave WS, De Volder ML, Moust
tion issues. Acad Med 1993;68 (1):52–81.
JHC, Patel VL. Explanatory Models in the processing
2 Colliver JA. Effectiveness of problem-based learning
of science text: the role of prior knowledge activation
curricula: research and theory. Acad Med 2000;75
through small-group discussion. J Educ Psychol
(3):259–66.
1989;81:610–9.
3 Dochy F, Segers M, van den Bossche P, Gijbels D.
20 De Grave WS, Schmidt HG, Beliën JJ, Moust JHC, De
Effects of problem-based learning: a meta-analysis.
Volder ML, Kerkhofs LMM. Effecten van verschillende
Learn Instruct 2003;13 (5):533–68.
typen van activatie van voorkennis op recall, gemeten
4 Gijbels D, Dochy F, van den Bossche P, Segers M.
met een aanvultoets [Effects of different types of
Effects of problem-based learning: a meta-analysis
activation of prior knowledge on recall, measured with
from the angle of assessment. Rev Educ Res 2005;75
a completion procedure]. Tilburg: Onderwijs
(1):27–61.
Research Dagen 1985.
5 Schmidt HG, Dauphinee WD, Patel VL. Comparing
21 van Blankenstein FM, Dolmans DHJM, van der
the effects of problem-based and conventional curric-
Vleuten CPM, Schmidt HG. Which cognitive processes
ula in an international sample. J Med Educ 1987;62
support learning during small-group discussion?
(4):305–15.
The role of providing explanations and listening to
6 Vernon DTA, Blake RL. Does problem-based learning
others. Instr Sci 2009;32 (2):1–16.
work? - A meta-analysis of evaluative research. Acad
22 Eisenstaedt RS, Barry WE, Glanz K. Problem-based
Med 1993;68 (7):550–63.
learning: cognitive retention and cohort traits of
7 Schmidt HG, van der Molen HT, Te Winkel WWR,
randomly selected participants and decliners. Acad
Wijnen WHFW. Constructivist, problem-based learn-
Med 1990;65 (9 Suppl):11–2.
ing does work: a meta-analysis of curricular compari-
23 Mårtenson D, Eriksson H, Ingelman-Sundberg M.
sons involving a single medical school. Educ Psychol
Medical chemistry: evaluation of active and problem-
2009;44 (4):227–49.
oriented teaching methods. Med Educ 1985;19:
8 Barrows HS. How to Design a Problem-Based Curriculum
34–42.
for the Preclinical Years. New York, NY: Springer
24 Tans RW, Schmidt HG, Schade-Hoogeveen BEJ,
Publishing 1985.
Gijselaers WH. Sturing van het onderwijsleerproces
9 Evensen DH, Hmelo CE, eds. Problem-Based Learning: A
door middel van problemen: een veldexperiment
Research Perspective on Learning Interactions. Mahwah, NJ:
[Guiding the learning process by means of
Lawrence Erlbaum 2000.

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 803
H G Schmidt et al

problems: a field experiment]. Tijdschr Onderwijsres 42 Gijselaers WH, Schmidt HG. Development and evalu-
1986;11 (1):38–48. ation of a causal model of problem-based learning. In:
25 Hidi S, Renninger KA. The four-phase model of Nooman ZH, Schmidt HG, Ezzat ES, eds. Innovation in
interest development. Educ Psychol 2006;41 (2): Medical Education: An Evaluation of its Present Status.
111–27. New York, NY: Springer Publishing 1990;95–113.
26 Loewenstein G. The psychology of curiosity: a review 43 van Berkel H, Schmidt H. On the additional value of
and reinterpretation. Psychol Bull 1994;116 (1): lectures in a problem-based curriculum. Educ Health
75–98. (Abingdon) 2005;18 (1):45–61.
27 Berlyne DE. Curiosity and learning. Motiv Emotion 44 van Berkel HJM, Schmidt HG. Motivation to commit
1978;2 (2):97–175. oneself as a determinant of achievement in
28 Litman JA. Interest and deprivation factors of problem-based learning. Higher Educ 2000;40 (2):231–
epistemic curiosity. Pers Indiv Differ 2008;44 (7):1585– 42.
95. 45 van den Hurk MM, Dolmans D, Wolfhagen I, van der
29 Litman JA. Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: Vleuten CPM. Testing a causal model for learning in a
wanting and liking new information. Cognition Emotion problem-based curriculum. Adv Health Sci Educ 2001;6
2005;19 (6):793–814. (2):141–9.
30 Rotgans JI, Schmidt HG. The role of teachers in 46 Slavin R. Cooperative Learning: Theory, Research and
facilitating situational interest in an active-learning Practice. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon 1990.
classroom. Teach Educ 2011;27 (1):37–42. 47 Springer L, Stanne ME, Donovan SS. Effects of
31 De Volder ML, Schmidt HG, Moust JHC, De Grave small-group learning on undergraduates in science,
WS. Problem-based learning and intrinsic motivation. mathematics, engineering, and technology: a meta-
In: van der Berchen JHC, Bergen TCM, De Bruyn EEI, analysis. Rev Educ Res 1999;69 (1):21–51.
eds. Achievement and Task Motivation. Berwyn, IL: Swets 48 Severiens SE, Schmidt HG. Academic and social inte-
North America 1986;123–7. gration and study progress in problem-based learning.
32 Schmidt HG. Intrinsieke motivatie en studieprestatie: Higher Educ 2009;58 (1):59–69.
enkele verkennende onderzoekingen [Intrinsic moti- 49 Tinto V. Classrooms as communities – exploring the
vation and achievement: some investigations]. Pedagog educational character of student persistence. J High
Stud 1983;60:385–95. Educ 1997;68 (6):599–623.
33 Rotgans JI, Schmidt HG. Situational interest and aca- 50 Burch VC, Sikakana CNT, Seggie JL, Schmidt HG.
demic achievement in the active-learning classroom. Performance of academically at-risk medical students
Learn Instr 2011;21 (1):58–67. in a problem-based learning programme. A
34 Des Marchais JE. A Delphi technique to identify and preliminary report. Adv Health Sci Educ 2007;12
evaluate criteria for construction of PBL problems. (3):345–58.
Med Educ 1999;33 (7):504–8. 51 Howell H. Ten years of hybrid PBL at Harvard School
35 Jacobs AEJP, Dolmans DHJM, Wolfhagen IHAP, of Dental Medicine. Fourth International Symposium
Scherpbier AJJA. Validation of a short questionnaire to on Problem-Based Learning in Dental Education, 23–
assess the degree of complexity and structuredness of 28 October 2005, Nakorn Pathom.
PBL problems. Med Educ 2003;37 (11):1001–7. 52 Iputo JE, Kwizera E. Problem-based learning improves
36 Kim S, Phillips WR, Pinsky L, Brock D, Phillips K, the academic performance of medical students in
Keary J. A conceptual framework for developing South Africa. Med Educ 2005;39 (4):388–93.
teaching cases: a review and synthesis of the literature 53 Barrows HS, Tamblyn R. Problem-Based Learning: An
across disciplines. Med Educ 2006;40 (9):867–76. Approach to Medical Education. New York, NY: Springer
37 Sockalingam N, Rotgans JI, Schmidt HG. Student and Publishing 1980.
tutor perceptions on attributes of effective problems 54 Maudsley G. Roles and responsibilities of the problem-
in problem-based learning. Higher Educ 2010; 62 (1): based learning tutor in the undergraduate medical
1–16. curriculum. BMJ 1999;318:657–61.
38 Dolmans DHJM, Gijselaers WH, Schmidt HG, van der 55 Mayo WP, Donnelly MB, Schwartz RW. Characteristics
Meer SB. Problem effectiveness in a course using of the ideal problem-based learning tutor in clinical
problem-based learning. Acad Med 1993;68 (3): medicine. Eval Health Prof 1995;18:124–36.
207–13. 56 Wetzel MS. Developing the role of the tutor ⁄ facilita-
39 Mpofu DJS, Das M, Murdoch JC, Lanphear JH. Effec- tor. Postgrad Med J 1996;72:474–7.
tiveness of problems used in problem-based learning. 57 De Grave WS, Dolmans DHJM, van der Vleuten CPM.
Med Educ 1997;31 (5):330–4. Profiles of effective tutors in problem-based learning:
40 Soppe M, Schmidt HG, Bruysten RJMP. Influence of scaffolding student learning. Med Educ 1999;33
problem familiarity on learning in a problem-based (12):901–6.
course. Instr Sci 2005;33 (3):271–81. 58 Collins A, Brown JS, Newman SE, eds. Cognitive
41 Hung W. The 3C3R model: a conceptual framework Apprenticeship: Teaching the Crafts of Reading, Writing,
for designing problems in PBL. Interdisciplinary J Prob and Mathematics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Based Learn 2006;1 (1):55–77. Associates 1989.

804 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806
What works and why in problem-based learning

59 Davis WK, Nairn R, Paine ME, Anderson RM, Oh MS. 74 Wood D, Bruner JS, Ross G. The role of tutoring in
Effects of expert and non-expert facilitators on the problem solving. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1976;17:89–
small-group process and on student performance. 100.
Acad Med 1992;67 (7):470–4. 75 Saye JW, Brush T. Scaffolding critical reasoning about
60 Des Marchais JE, Black R. Effect of Tutor Content Expertise history and social issues in multimedia-supported
on Student Academic Achievement in the Sherbrooke learning environments. Educ Tech Res Dev 2002;50
Problem-Based Curriculum. Sherbrooke, QC: University (3):77–96.
of Sherbrooke 1991. 76 Berk LE, Winsler A. Scaffolding Children’s Learning:
61 Swanson DB, Stalenhoef-Halling BF, van der Vleuten Vygotsky and Early Childhood Education. Washington, DC:
CPM. Effect of tutor characteristics on test perfor- National Association for the Education of Young
mance of students in a problem-based curriculum. In: Children 1995.
Bender W, Hiemstra RJ, Scherpbier AJJA, Zwierstra 77 Roehler LR, Cantlon DJ, eds. Scaffolding: A Powerful
RP, eds. Teaching and Assessing Clinical Competence. Tool in Social Constructivist Classrooms. Cambridge, MA:
Groningen: BoekWerk Publications 1990;129–34. Brookline 1997.
62 Schmidt HG. Resolving inconsistencies in tutor 78 Merriënboer JJGV. Training Complex Cognitive Skills: A
expertise research: does lack of structure cause students Four-Component Instructional Design Model for Technical
to seek tutor guidance? Acad med 1994;69:656–62. Training. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technol-
63 Silver M, Wilkerson LA. Effects of tutors with subject ogy 1997.
expertise on the problem-based tutorial process. Acad 79 van de Pol J, Volman M, Beishuizen J. Scaffolding in
Med 1991;66 (5):298–300. teacher–student interaction: a decade of research.
64 Eagle CJ, Harasym PH, Mandin H. Effects of tutors Educ Psychol Rev 2010;22 (3):271–96.
with case expertise on problem-based learning issues. 80 Simons K, Klein J. The impact of scaffolding and stu-
Acad Med 1992;67 (7):465–9. dent achievement levels in a problem-based learning
65 Moust JHC. De Rol van Tutoren in Probleemgestuurd environment. Instr Sci 2007;35:41–72.
Onderwijs. Contrasten Tussen Student- en Docenttutoren 81 Choo SSY, Rotgans JI, Yew EHJ, Schmidt HG. Effect of
[On the Role of Tutors in Problem-Based Learning: worksheet scaffolds on student learning in problem-
Contrasting Student-Guided with Staff-Guided Tutorials]. based learning. Adv Health Sci Educ 2011 (In Press).
Maastricht: University of Limburg 1993. 82 Schmidt HG, Bouhuijs PAJ. Effecten van Structurering
66 Schmidt HG, Moust JHC. What makes a tutor effective van Patie¨ntenproblemen op Leerresultaat en Satisfactie van
- a structural-equations modeling approach to learning Studenten [Effects of Structuring of Problems on Achievement
in problem-based curricula. Acad Med 1995;70 (8):708– and Satisfaction of Students]. Amsterdam: Vrije Univer-
14. siteit 1977.
67 Chng E, Yew EHJ, Schmidt HG. Effects of tutor- 83 Verkoeijen PPJL, Rikers RMJP, Winkel WWT, van den
related behaviours on the process of problem-based Hurk MM. Do student-defined learning issues increase
learning. Adv Health Sci Educ 2011 (In press). quality and quantity of individual study? Adv Health Sci
68 Gukas ID, Leinster SJ, Walker R. Verbal and non- Educ 2006;11 (4):337–47.
verbal indices of learning during problem-based 84 Gallagher SA, Stepien WJ, Rosenthal H. The effects of
learning (PBL) among first year medical students and problem-based learning on problem solving. Gifted
the threshold for tutor intervention. Med Teach Child Q 1992;36:195–200.
2010;32 (1):5–11. 85 Kolodner JL, Camp PJ, Crismond D, Fasse B, Gray J,
69 Kindler P, Grant C, Kulla S, Poole G, Godolphin W. Holbrook J, Puntambekar S, Ryan M. Problem-based
Difficult incidents and tutor interventions in problem- learning meets case-based reasoning in the middle-
based learning tutorials. Med Educ 2009;43:866–73. school science classroom: putting Learning by
70 Hmelo-Silver CE, Barrows HS. Goals and strategies of a Design into practice. J Learn Sci 2003;12 (4):495–
problem-based learning facilitator. Int J Probl Based 547.
Learn 2006;1 (1):21–39. 86 Dolmans DHJM, Schmidt HG, Gijselaers WH. The
71 Kirschner PA, Sweller J, Clark RE. Why minimal relationship between student-generated learning
guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis issues and self-study in problem-based learning. Instr
of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem- Sci 1995;22 (4):251–67.
based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educ 87 van den Hurk MM, Wolfhagen IHAP, Dolmans D, van
Psychol 2006;41 (2):75–86. der Vleuten CPM. The impact of student-generated
72 Belland B, Glazewski K, Richardson J. A scaffolding learning issues on individual study time and academic
framework to support the construction of evidence- achievement. Med Educ 1999;33 (11):808–14.
based arguments among middle school students. Educ 88 Blumberg P, Michael JA. Development of self-directed
Tech Res Dev 2008;56 (4):401–422. learning behaviours in a partially teacher-directed
73 Schmidt HG, Loyens SMM, Van Gog T, Paas F. Problem- problem-based learning curriculum. Teach Learn Med
based learning is compatible with human cognitive 1992;4:3–8.
architecture: commentary on Kirschner, Sweller, and 89 Marshall JG, Fitzgerald D, Busby L, Heaton G. A
Clark (2006). Educ Psychol 2007;42 (2):91–7. study of library use in problem-based and traditional

ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806 805
H G Schmidt et al

medical curricula. Bull Med Libr Assoc 1993;81 99 Johnson W, Kieras D. Representation saving – effects
(3):299–305. of prior knowledge in memory for simple technical
90 Rankin JA. Problem-based medical education – effect prose. Mem Cognition 1983;11:456–66.
on library use. Bull Med Libr Assoc 1992;80 (1):36–43. 100 Loyens SMM, Rikers RMJP, Schmidt HG. The
91 Zimmerman BJ. Investigating self-regulation and impact of students’ conceptions of constructivist
motivation: historical background, methodological assumptions on academic achievement and dropout.
developments, and future prospects. Am Educ Res J Stud High Educ 2007;32:581–602.
2008;45 (1):166–83. 101 Gutierrez R, Slavin RE. Achievement effects of the
92 Yew EHJ, Schmidt HG. Is learning in problem-based non-graded elementary school – a best-evidence
learning cumulative? Adv Health Sci Educ 2010; doi: synthesis. Rev Educ Res 1992;62 (4):333–76.
10.1007/s10459-010-9267-y. 102 Tulving E, Thomson DM. Encoding specificity and
93 Cobb P. Where is the mind? Constructivist and socio- retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychol Rev
cultural perspectives on mathematical development. 1973;80 (5):352–73.
Educ Res 1994;23:13–20. 103 Eva KW, Neville AJ, Norman GR. Exploring the
94 Driver R, Asoko H, Leach J, Mortimer E, Scott P. etiologic of content specificity: factors influencing
Constructing scientific knowledge in the classroom. analogic transfer and problem solving. Acad Med
Educ Res 1994;23:5–12. 1998;73 (10 Suppl):1–5.
95 Zimmerman BJ. Self-regulated learning and academic 104 Barrows HS. Inquiry: the pedagogical importance
achievement – an overview. Educ Psychol 1990;25 (1):3– of a skill central to clinical practice. Med Educ
17. 1990;24:3–5.
96 De Grave WS, Schmidt HG, Boshuizen HPA. Effects of 105 Hmelo CE, Ferrari M. The problem-based learning
problem-based discussion on studying a subsequent tutorial: cultivating higher order thinking skills. J Educ
text: a randomised trial among first year medical stu- Gifted 1997;20 (4):401–22.
dents. Instr Sci 2001;29:33–44. 106 Norman G. Research in clinical reasoning: past history
97 Schmidt HG, Cohen-Schotanus J, Arends L. Impact of and current trends. Med Educ 2005;39 (4):418–27.
problem-based, active, learning on graduation rates of 107 Schmidt HG, Rikers R. How expertise develops in
ten generations of Dutch medical students. Med Educ medicine: knowledge encapsulation and illness script
2009;43 (3):211–8. formation. Med Educ 2007;41 (12):1133–9.
98 Dubin R, Taveggia TC. The Teaching–Learning Paradox.
Received 7 March 2011; editorial comments to authors 11 March
A Comparative Analysis of College Teaching Methods. 2011; accepted for publication 5 April 2011
Eugene, OR: Center for the Advanced Study of
Educational Administration 1968.

806 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2011. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2011; 45: 792–806

You might also like