Unnecesaria Rudeza. Deportes Escolares y Violencia Adolescente

You might also like

You are on page 1of 21

Unnecessary Roughness?

School Sports, Peer Networks, and Male Adolescent Violence


Author(s): Derek A. Kreager
Source: American Sociological Review, Vol. 72, No. 5 (Oct., 2007), pp. 705-724
Published by: American Sociological Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25472488
Accessed: 05-09-2016 20:20 UTC

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

American Sociological Association, Sage Publications, Inc. are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to American Sociological Review

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Unnecessary Roughness?
School Sports, Peer Networks,
and Male Adolescent Violence
Derek A. Kreager
Pennsylvania State University

This article examines the extent to which participation in high school interscholastic
sports contributes to male violence. Deriving competing hypotheses from social control,
social learning, and masculinity theories, I use data from the National Longitudinal
Study of Adolescent Health to test if (I) type of sport and (2) peer athletic participation,
contribute to the risks of male serious fighting. Contrary to social control expectations,
analyses suggest that athletic involvement fails to inhibit male violence. Moreover, there
is a strong relationship between contact sports and violence. Football players and
wrestlers, as opposed to baseball, basketball, tennis, and other athletes, are significantly
more likely than nonathletic males to be involved in a serious fight. Additionally, the
direct effect of football is explained by the football participation of individuals 'peers.
Males whose friends play football are more likely to fight than other males, supporting
perspectives that emphasize peer contexts as important mediators. Overall, findings are
consistent with the expectations of social learning and masculinity arguments. The
theoretical and policy implications of these results are discussed.

munities, becoming core members of a school's


Inscholastic
manysportsU.S.playsecondary schools, inter
crucial roles in struc "in-crowd" (Bissinger 1991; Coleman 1961;
turing student status hierarchies and peer Holland and Andre 1994). Similarly, nonathletic
friends of popular athletes tend to share elevat
friendship networks. "Star" male athletes are
ed social status and gain membership in more
often venerated by their peers and local corn
exclusive peer groups (Eckert 1989). The pre
dominance and visibility of sports in schools
encourages all students, regardless of their gen
Direct all correspondence to Derek A. Kreager, der or athleticism, to orient their behaviors
Department of Sociology and Crime, Law, and toward these activities and define their own
Justice, Pennsylvania State University, 211 Oswald
identities in relation to the most popular athletes
Tower, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802-6207
and athletic cliques.
(dkreager@psu.edu). I thank Kate Stovel, Jerry
The salience of athletics in adolescent culture
Herting, and Ross Matsueda for their helpful com
ments on drafts of this manuscript. This research fuels ongoing debates about the social role of
uses data from Add Health, a program project youth sports. On the one hand, proponents have
designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and long argued that interscholastic athletics posi
Kathleen Mullan Harris, and funded by a grant P01 tively impact adolescent development. Here,
HD31921 from the National Institute of Child Health
youth sports are viewed as (1) increasing ado
and Human Development, with cooperative funding lescents' bonds to schools, conventional peers,
from 17 other agencies. Special acknowledgment is
due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for and conventional adults (Crosnoe 2001; Larson
assistance in the original design. Persons interested
1994; McNeal 1995); (2) socializing adoles
in obtaining data files from Add Health should con cents into the basic values of American life,
tact Add Health, Carolina Population Center, 123 W. such as competition, fair play, self-restraint,
Franklin Street, Chapel Hill, NC 27516-2524 and achievement (Jeziorski 1994); and (3) help
(www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth/contract.html). ing students develop social and physical com

American Sooological Review, 2007, Vol. 72 (October: 705-724)

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
706 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

petence, leading to increased self-esteem, social field violence toward perceived outsiders and
capital, and upward mobility (Ewing et al. 2002; "weaker" students. Masculinized sports then
Otto and Alwin 1977; Spady 1970). Studies, in become socially sanctioned stepping-stones
fact, concur on many of these points in consis toward privilege and power?sites where coach
tently finding positive relationships between es, peers, parents, and the media encourage
sports participation and a host of individual masculine identities founded on physical aggres
benefits, including increased self-esteem, locus sion and domination.
of control, academic achievement, commitment Seemingly endless reports of high-profile
to graduation, educational aspirations, and eco athlete misbehavior bolster such critical views.
nomic attainment (Eccles and Barber 1999; Accounts of brawling, sexual assault, and bul
Fejgin 1994; Mahoney and Cairns 1997; Marsh lying by prominent athletes regularly stream
1993; McNeal 1995; Otto and Alwin 1977). In across our televisions and newspapers, prompt
addition, some research finds a negative rela ing the question: "Are these activities promot
tionship between sports participation and delin ing the fair play and sportsmanship outlined in
quent behavior (Landers and Landers 1978; their charters, or are they encouraging violence
Langbein and Bess 2002; Mahoney 2000; Stark,
by already privileged elites?" Answering this
Kent, and Finke 1987). The latter lends legiti
question has important implications for school
macy to delinquency prevention programs, such
based sporting programs, yet surprisingly little
as midnight basketball, that promote sports as
research has addressed the youth sports-vio
a means of keeping urban males off of danger
lence relationship. Moreover, studies that have
ous inner-city streets (Hartmann 2001).
been conducted generally suffer from method
Critical scholars, however, assail traditional
ological limitations that strongly curtail sub
views of youth sports as incomplete and prob stantive conclusions.1
lematic. Buoyed by first-hand accounts from
In this article, I move beyond prior research
athletes and coaches, these scholars reveal the
with theoretically grounded hypotheses and
contradictions and inequities underlying much
advanced quantitative methods. I rely on three
of modern sport (see Gatz, Messner, and Ball
distinct theoretical traditions?social control,
Rokeach 2002, for a review). Rather than build
ing socially competent young men and women,
social learning, and masculinity theories?to
it is suggested, the conditions of contemporary derive competing hypotheses for the sports
athletics embed youth in value systems marred violence relationship. I then test these for five
by homophobia, sexism, racism, and ruthless very different sports?football, basketball, base
competition. Within these contexts, middle ball, wrestling, and tennis?using data from
class white males have the most to gain, while the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent
disadvantaged minority and female athletes are Health (Add Health). The unique design of the
either marginalized or forego long-term attain Add Health survey not only allows for the lon
ment in favor of short-term status benefits and gitudinal examination of violent outcomes, but
illusory professional careers.
it also includes a wealth of individual back
Critical feminist scholars have taken partic ground variables and sociometric data. The
ular interest in the relationship between sports measures of friendship networks are of partic
and gendered violence. Rejecting the view that ular interest, as they allow one to move beyond
sports help to curb antisocial behavior, some prior research and gain leverage on a potential
researchers assert that the hypermasculine cul mechanism connecting sports to violence (i.e.,
tures characteristic of many contact sports teach
violence as an acceptable means of maintaining
valued male identities (Burstyn 1999; Coakley
1 Two problems endemic to sport-violence research
2001; Connell 1995; Crosset 1999; Messner
are (1) a reliance on cross-sectional designs that are
1992; Sabo 1994; Young, White, and McTeer unable to distinguish selection from socialization
1994). By rewarding physical aggression with effects and (2) a failure to distinguish effects across
on-the-field success and increased prestige, different types of sports (see Baumert, Henderson,
contact sports are portrayed as both elevating and Thompson 1998; Begg et al. 1996; Jackson et al.
athletes above their peers and increasing off-the 2002; Nixon 1997; Wright and Fitzpatrick 2006).

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 707

embeddedness in sports networks increases the and values of sports are assumed to lie within
likelihood of individual violence). the value system shared by conventional soci
ety, participation in sports should increase ado
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND lescents' belief in the moral order and, thus,
prosocial behavior (Larson 1994). Indeed, many
Social Control Perspectives youth sports programs have explicit written
missions to promote fair play, teamwork, and
Sports scholars commonly invoke social control
concepts, particularly Hirschi's (1969) social conventional values (Fine 1987).
bonding theory, to examine the linkages between Much of the empirical evidence supports the
sports participation and adolescent antisocial dimensions of social control theory outlined
behavior (Crosnoe 2001; Larson 1994; McNeal above. Adolescent athletes are less likely to
1995). Rather than focusing on delinquent moti drop out of high school (Mahoney and Cairns
vations, control theories posit that it is the con 1997; McNeal 1995), more likely to attend col
straining influence of conventional bonds that lege (Eccles and Barber 1999; Marsh 1993;
explain variations in individual-level delin Sabo, Melnick, and Vanfossen 1993), and less
quency. Schools (and positive relationships with likely to behave delinquency (Landers and
peers and adults within schools) are seen as Landers 1978; Langbein and Bess 2002;
important sites for adolescent integration into Mahoney 2000; Stark et al. 1987). None of this
conventional society. Accordingly, youth who work, however, addresses violence specifically,
are tightly bonded to school and to their student nor does it address the possibility of variation
peers are more likely to refrain from violent in antisocial outcomes by forms of athletic
behavior than are other, less bonded, youth. engagement.
Because interscholastic sports are institu Control perspectives assume that the moti
tionally sanctioned activities governed by school vation to commit delinquent acts is constant
connected adults, social control perspectives across persons and that group norms support
predict that sports participation should increase ive of crime are weak or nonexistent (Hirschi
adolescents' bonds to conventional society and 1969). Because organization in favor of crime
reduce antisocial behavior (Crosnoe 2001; is thought inconsequential, control theorists dis
Larson 1994; McNeal 1995). Hirschi's (1969) miss the possibility that individuals may be
elements of the social bond?attachment, tightly bonded to groups or subcultures that
involvement, commitment, and belief?are read promote antisocial behaviors. Following this
ily applied to individual sports participation. logic, violence by male athletes would be inter
First, sports participation should increase attach preted as evidence that either sports are not
ments between athletes and their teammates and
coaches (Coleman 1961; Messner 1992). These
ties should reduce antisocial behaviors by con because these activities increase adolescents' time
straining individual tendencies toward aggression spent in structured situations supervised by conven
and delinquency. Second, athletic participation tional authority figures (Agnew and Petersen 1989;
should build athletes' commitment to conven Osgood et al. 1996). According to these perspec
tional lines of action, because the penalty for tives, the situational properties of sports participation
deviance would include the loss of athletic sta should reduce opportunities for deviance while youth
tus and a related decrease in social standing. are taking part in the activity. This differs from the
social control view, in that no assumptions are made
Third, the time required to practice and succeed
about individuals' motivations or personal charac
in sports should increase adolescents' involve
teristics (e.g., social bonds [see Osgood et al. 1996]).
ment in conventional activities and decrease, by
Instead, the focus is on the activity itself, leaving
default, the time available for antisocial behav
open the possibility that unstructured and unsuper
ior (McNeal 1995).2 Finally, because the rules vised time spent outside of sports may increase ath
letes' deviance. As Hirschi (1969:190) pointed out,
delinquency may not require large amounts of time,
so that time spent in structured activities?such as
2 In a related literature, authors in the routine activ sports?may not prevent off-the-field delinquent
ities tradition assert that sports should curb deviance behaviors in unsupervised settings.

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
708 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

conventional activities or that violent athletes are approach, particularly with regard to the rela
not fully bonded to sports. As there is much evi tionship between peer behavior and individual
dence suggesting that sports involvement is delinquency (Matsueda and Anderson 1998;
generally associated with conventional behav Warr 2002; Warr and Stafford 1991).
ior, we are left to conclude that violent male ath Hughes and Coakley (1991) apply social
letes are mavericks within their programs, learning ideas to the seeming paradox of athlete
alienated from other players and the conven deviance. Rather than suggesting that athletes'
tional institutions of school and family. It would antisocial behaviors result from social alien
be this lack of social integration that frees an ath ation or the rejection of cultural values, they con
lete to behave violently. tend that such behaviors stem directly from the
normative definitions learned in sports, a con
Social Learning Perspectives cept they call "positive deviance." They state that
the values associated with sports?striving for
In contrast to social control theories, social distinction, sacrificing for The Team, playing
learning perspectives allow for subgroup vari through pain, and refusing to accept limits?are
ation in attitudes toward violence and law vio generally associated with individual success
lation. Accordingly, individuals learn antisocial and conventional behavior. Yet, these norms
values and techniques within intimate social may also create situations where athletes "do
relations, particularly among friends and fam harmful things to themselves and perhaps oth
ily members (Akers 1998; Sutherland 1947). ers while motivated by a sense of duty and
Because some individuals and social groups honor" (p. 311). They point to the widespread
are thought to have positive attitudes toward use of performance enhancing drugs as a clear
criminal behavior (or at least justify such behav example of such behavior. These drugs are con
ior under certain circumstances), social learn sidered deviant by broader society, but within
ing theorists assume that individuals may be sporting contexts, they are often modeled and
tightly bonded to others while simultaneously reinforced as acceptable means to boost per
holding attitudes favorable to law violation. It formance in a highly competitive environment.
is this assumption that most separates learning A similar argument may be applied to aggres
theories from those of the social control tradi sive behavior, in that aggression is often an
tion (Matsueda 1997). essential element for on-the-field success. By
At the heart of social learning approaches is applying lessons learned in sports, athletes may
the idea that delinquency, like any other behav perceive violence and intimidation as acceptable
ior, is learned in social interaction. Sutherland means of achieving off-the-field goals and solv
(1947), in his classic work, postulates that delin ing problems unrelated to sports.
quency results from individuals learning Peer relationships play central roles in the
prodelinquency situational definitions (or atti learning process, particularly during the status
tudes) within intimate social contacts. conscious adolescent years (Coleman 1961).
Accordingly, delinquency occurs when a person As noted previously, sports provide males with
holds more positive than negative delinquent clear avenues toward increased peer status (Eder
definitions of a situation. Akers and colleagues and Kinney 1995; Holland and Andre 1994).
(Akers 1998; Akers et al. 1979; Burgess and Team sports in particular may also direct indi
Akers 1966) have expanded on this by includ vidual behavior toward group norms. Ridicule,
ing concepts of operant conditioning from appeals to group loyalty, and status competition
behavioral psychology. This more general model are primary mechanisms for ensuring individ
adds specific mechanisms, such as imitation ual conformity to group expectations (Warr
and personal and vicarious reinforcement, to 2002). For male athletes, derisive comments
the learning process. Individual violence and such as "pussy" and "chicken" pose deep threats
delinquency are thus assumed to emanate from to status within the peer group, prompting
continual and reciprocal processes of social behaviors meant to regain face. Perceived threats
observation, attitude internalization, and real to masculine reputations or social status may
and perceived reinforcements from the behav escalate into "character contests" where vio
ior of self and others. Research has confirmed lence becomes an acceptable means of resolv
the explanatory power of the social learning ing the encounter (Goffman 1967; Luckenbill

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 709

1977). Situational research of such "honor con ordinated males. Displays of aggression, inde
tests" consistently finds that peers play impor pendence, competition, and a rejection of fem
tant roles in the violent transaction (Polk 1999). ininity are thought to be culturally honored
Audiences both heighten the visibility of status ways of being a man, so that enacting these
threats and become resources for conflict res qualities allows men to "do" gender while also
olution. As Curry (1998) notes in his study of reproducing a system of gender inequality. For
athlete barroom violence, peers may simulta masculinity theorists, understanding the repro
neously encourage violence against perceived ductive processes associated with hegemonic
outsiders and bear witness to a group member's masculinity allows for the recognition of alter
fighting prowess: "These fights with other males native gender forms and opens possibilities for
(never members of one's own team) had a way less oppressive gender regimes.
of building team cohesion and expressing mas Within the masculinity literature, heavy-con
culine courage" (p. 211). Moreover, such activ tact sports are typically portrayed as important
ities also serve to further insulate the athlete avenues for males to construct hegemonic mas
from nonathletic peers and increase disdain for culine identities. Accordingly, these sports
those who have not made the sacrifices of sports become "endlessly renewed symbolfs] of mas
(Hughes and Coakley 1991; Messner 2002). culinity" that promote the "violence and homo
Violence by male athletes may thus bind team phobia frequently found in sporting milieus"
mates into exclusive peer groups where indi (Connell and Messerschmidt 2005:833). In con
viduals are forced to jockey for status with tact sports, on-the-field violence is intertwined
displays of aggression, risk taking, and ridicule. with success, prestige, and essentialist images
of "maleness." Contact sport athletes are
Masculinity Perspectives and Contact admired for their strength and determination
Sports and rewarded with increased prestige and access
to exclusive peer groups, the latter serving to
Aggressiveness and feelings of superiority may insulate athletes from alternative gender con
be endemic to sport culture and increase vio ceptions. The connection between on-the-field
lence among all athletes. However, qualitative violence and identity should then increase con
research by masculinity scholars suggests that tact athletes' risks of violence beyond the play
sports are not equal in their relationships to ing field (Crosset 1999; Pappas, McKenry, and
individual violence. The contention here is that Catlett 2004). Such behavior confirms the con
"hypermasculine" contact sports (e.g., sports tact athlete's sense of self, connects him to his
where physical domination, the use of the body teammates, and protects his powerful position
as a weapon, and brutal bodily contact are nec relative to subordinated masculinities and fem
essary for on-the-field success) create condi ininities.
tions where violence becomes an acceptable In their qualitative study of a middle school,
means of "doing" masculinity and maintaining Eder, Evans, and Parker (1997) document how
valued masculine identities (Coakley 2001; young boys are able to construct masculine
Connell 1995; Crosset 1999; Messner 1992; identities within heavy-contact sports. On foot
Young et al. 1994). By differentiating sporting ball fields and wrestling mats, the authors
contexts and emphasizing the gendered nature observe boys setting up "a pattern in which
of sport-related violence, masculinity theorists higher status is associated with intimidation of
extend social learning ideas and provide addi others and lower status is associated with sub
tional hypotheses for the relationship between missive behavior" (p. 69). Responses to insults
sports and violence. and physical confrontations establish the mas
Stemming from critical feminist perspec culine pecking order and provide the contact ath
tives, masculinity theories focus on male groups letes with access to high-status groups. Although
to illuminate the processes underlying gendered coaches attempt to confine boys' combative
hierarchies (Connell and Messerschmidt 2005). behavior to the playing field, violence extends
A central concept for these arguments is hege to informal settings. Memories of "good hits"
monic masculinity, commonly defined as the and "take-downs" establish the informal social
cultural patterns of action that allow some men order and identify the leaders as the best fight
to maintain dominance over females and sub ers. Simultaneously, the aggression modeled

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
710 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

on the playing field carries over to disdain for According to masculinity arguments, varia
nonathletes, who are derided as being weak and tions in physical contact across these sports
effeminate (e.g., "pussies" and "fags") and, arguably relate directly to the masculine defi
thus, become subject to violent victimization nitions fostered by players. For football ath
(Ederetal. 1997:76-78). letes, on-the-field violence is likely entwined
with male status and identity (Coakley 2001;
Physical Contact and American Messner 1992). Social learning approaches
Sports would also expect football players' networks to
reinforce violent behavior as a means of main
To understand the relationship between ado taining peer status and avoiding ridicule (Akers
lescent sports and violence in the United States, 1998; Warr 2002). Although basketball and
we must first understand this country's current baseball players, like virtually all athletes, would
sports landscape. Unquestionably, the most associate aggressiveness with definitions of
prominent team sports in contemporary U.S. self, these definitions should fall short of vio
society are football, basketball, and baseball. lence because physical violence is an unneces
Each corresponds to its own season and is well sary and unreinforced dimension for athletic
represented nationally at both the collegiate and success.
professional levels. Huge industries help pro Combining learning and masculinity con
mote and broadcast their games, advertise their cepts creates an expectation that football will be
merchandise, and capitalize on the celebrity a stronger predictor of fighting than noncontact
status of successful players. More important team sports. But what of individual sports?
for this study, these sports are also found in Social learning theories would expect peers to
most secondary schools and are at different have greater impact on individual behavior in an
points on the "contact" continuum. interdependent team sport than in the more
Football is considered a heavy-contact sport autonomous environments of individual sports.
because physical bodily contact is an acceptable If so, then comparing team and individual sports
and necessary component for on-the-field suc with similar levels of physical contact would
cess. It is impossible for a team to win a foot provide insights into the group nature of male
ball game without physically dominating violence. Football and wrestling appear to be
opposing players through tackles, blocks, hits, ideal candidates for such a comparison. Similar
and other forms of "brutal body contact" to football, wrestling is an exclusively male
(Coakley 2001:176). In contrast, the rules of contact sport, but the small size of wrestling
basketball prohibit play that is physically vio teams (typically fewer than 15 wrestlers) and the
lent, allowing contact only when it is inciden individual nature of competition may temper
tal to the normal course of a game. Although wrestling's peer effects. In addition, a noncon
physical and verbal (e.g., trash-talking) intim tact and gender neutral individual sport, such as
idation of opponents are key strategies (Eveslage tennis, provides a useful comparison at the
and Delaney 1998), bodily contact to the point opposite end of the contact continuum. Not
of violence or to "take down" an opponent is only does tennis have a long history of gender
expressly forbidden and severely sanctioned inclusiveness, but it is also played on a surface
(Shields 1999). If basketball is less contact where opponents are physically separated by a
oriented than football, baseball lies at the oppo net. For social learning and masculinity per
site end of the contact continuum. At most
spectives, the lack of physical contact, low con
points in a baseball game, opposing players nection with masculinity, and individual nature
occupy separate physical spaces and the goals of tennis should limit its connection to male vio
of the game do not require physically defend lence.
ing those spaces. Only on rare occasions (e.g.,
preventing a double-play, sliding into home, or Self-Selection and Spuriousness
pitchers "brushing back" batters) are baseball
players able to threaten opponents physically, The above perspectives suggest a direct causal
and these instances are closely monitored by relationship between sports and violent behav
officials to ensure that actions are not meant to ior. It is likely, however, that factors prior to ath
hurt or injure. letic participation are influential in explaining

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 711

subsequent outcomes. For example, athletes DATA AND MEASURES


may be more likely to possess aggressive traits Sample
that increase the likelihood of sports participa
tion, success, and individual violence. If this isI draw from the National Longitudinal Study of
the case, then the relationship between sports Adolescent Health (Add Health). Add Health is
and violence would be spurious and explained a school-based, nationally representative study
by the stable trait of aggressiveness. Aside from of American adolescents in grades 7 to 12. From
population heterogeneity in aggressive propen a list of all high schools in the United States,
sities, a spurious sports-violence relationship Add Health selected a stratified sample of 80
may also result from early socialization expe schools with probabilities proportional to size.
riences. Messner (1992) finds that childhood Schools were stratified by region, urbanicity,
school type, ethnic mix, and size. Additionally,
relationships with fathers, brothers, uncles, and
for those high schools not covering grades 7 to
peers contribute to individuals' definitions of
12, the sample included the middle school that
masculinity and subsequent desires to partici
contributed the most students to the high
pate in sports. These definitions, as well as the
school's incoming cohorts. The result is a sam
environments encountered in early athletic
ple of 145 schools of varying sizes, affiliations,
teams, may select individuals into secondary
and community contexts.
school athletic programs and explain subse
From 1994 to 2001, the study collected four
quent violence. Parents and coaches may play waves of data from students, parents, and school
similar selection roles. If parents encourage administrators. For this analysis, I use data from
aggressive children to play sports, or if coach the first (in-school) and second (Wave I in
es select players based on their aggressiveness, home) questionnaires. The in-school survey
again it is a pre-existing trait, and not sports per was administered to all available students in
se, that causes later violent behavior. Addressing each of the sampled schools. In total, 90,118
these issues of selection and spuriousness is a students (approximately 80 percent of those
key component?and, indeed, contribution? listed on school rosters) were surveyed. In each
of this article. school, surveys were administered in a single
Below, I test several competing hypotheses day during one 45- to 60-minute class period.
about the relationship between sports partici The questionnaire included basic demograph
pation and male adolescent violence. From ic characteristics, school-related activities
social control theory, one would expect sports (including sports participation), and risk behav
participation to inhibit violent behavior by bond iors (including a measure of violence). Also, stu
dents nominated their five best male and five
ing youth to conventional institutions. This con
best female friends. This allows for the con
trasts sharply with the hypothesis derived from
masculinity perspectives that the contexts of struction of friendship data taken directly from
heavy-contact sports produce conditions sup friends, thereby avoiding possible measurement
error associated with self-reported friends'
portive of male violence. Importantly, the mas
behavior. Sixteen schools had less than 50 per
culinity hypothesis adds a sports-specific
cent of the students complete the nomination
dimension and an explicit focus on male behav
portion of the survey and were dropped from the
ior. From social learning perspectives, I also
analysis. The resulting sample consists of 75,871
examine the possibility that peer athletic par students nested in 129 schools.
ticipation is an important mediating link.
The Wave I in-home survey took place in the
Notably, my final modeling explores whether
year following the in-school survey and con
pre-existing conditions, such as prior levels of sisted of a random sample of approximately
fighting, delinquency, or background charac 200 students from each of the originally sam
teristics, make spurious the sports-violence rela pled schools (N = 20,745). The 90-minute inter
tionship. This offers both leverage on the views were administered in individuals' homes
question of potential selection effects and con and, to ensure confidentiality, questions were
fidence in interpretations of any sports-vio completed using laptop computers. Nested with
lence association highlighted in the earlier in 120 schools, 14,396 students completed both
analyses. the in-school and in-home questionnaires. As my

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
712 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

hypotheses are primarily concerned with dis within 12 months of the in-home interview.
cerning the prevalence of male violence, I Descriptive statistics for this outcome are list
restricted my analyses to the 6,397 males who ed in Table 1. The variable is coded 0 if a respon
completed both surveys and attended schools dent reported not fighting in the last 12 months
with adequate network measures. As with any
and 1 if he reported fighting one or more times.
large survey, missing data due to item nonre
Approximately 40 percent of male respondents
sponse creates problems of representativeness
that may bias estimated coefficients. Although reported getting into a serious physical fight.
none of the variables in my analyses have greater The serious fighting item was originally
than 10 percent missing values, I use imputation measured on an interval scale, with values rang
techniques in STATA to maintain statistical ing from 0 (never) to 4 (seven or more times).
power and regain a nationally representative As the distribution for this variable is highly
sample of male adolescents. skewed, I chose to present findings using a bina
ry measure capturing the prevalence of serious
Dependent Variable fighting. In preliminary analyses, however, I

The dependent variable for this analysis is self also explored alternative modeling specifica
reported violence taken from the first in-home tions of the ordinal scale (e.g., using ordered
interview. This measure captures students' self logit and linear regressions) and found similar
reports of getting into a serious physical fight results to those presented here.

Table 1. Variable Descriptives (Survey Adjusted) (N = 6,397)


Mean
Variable (Percent) (SE) Minimum Maximum
Dependent Measure (Wave I In-Home Survey)
Serious Fighting .40 (.01) 0 1
Independent Variables (In-School Survey)
Age 14.83 (.13) 10 1
Black .15 (.02) 0 1
Intact Family .73 (.01) 0 1
Parent Attachment 4.71 (.01) 1 5
School Commitment 3.18 (.02) 1 4
Self-Esteem 4.14 (.02) 1 5
Family SES 6.12 (.10) 0 10
Club Member .41 (.01) 0 1
Body Mass Index 22.59 (.13) 11.22 54.28
Athlete .63 (.01) 0 1
Football .26 (.01) 0 1
Basketball .27 (.01) 0 1
Baseball .22 (.01) 0 1
Wrestling .07 (.00) 0 1
Tennis .04 (.00) 0 1
Other Sport .36 (.01) 0 1
Prior Physical Fighting .57 (
Minor Delinquency 7.03 (.17) 0 36
No Reciprocated Male Friends .44 (.02) 0 1
Friends Outside School 1.02 (.08) 0 10
Percent Male Friends Football .16 (.01) 0 1
Percent Male Friends Basketball .17 (.01) 0 1
Percent Male Friends Baseball .15 (.01) 0 1
Percent Male Friends Wrestling .02 (.00) 0 1
Percent Male Friends Tennis .03 (.00) 0 1
Percent Male Friends Other Sport .20 (.01) 0 1
Male Friends Average Delinquency 3.84 (.19) 0 36
Percent Male Friends Physical Fight_31_C01)_0_1

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 713

Individual-Level Independent ticipation rates for these activities, as well as the


Measures percentage of male nonathletes. As expected,
males are most likely to play baseball, football,
The primary independent variables are indiand basketball. Approximately 25 percent of
viduals' sports participation, friends' sports parthe sampled males participated in each of these
ticipation, and prior levels of violence and riskactivities. The individual sports of interest,
behaviors. Descriptive statistics for these meas wrestling and tennis, have lower participation
ures, as well as for the background control varirates (8 percent and 4 percent, respectively),
ables, are listed in Table 1. Add Health asked
but they remain adequately represented and
respondents about their participation in 12 athhave relatively low correlations with the team
letic activities (baseball/softball, basketball,sports (highest r: wrestling-football = .21).
field hockey, football, ice hockey, soccer, Along with being the most common male
swimming, tennis, track, volleyball, wrestling,athletic activities, the "big three" sports are also
positively related to peer status. Figure 2 shows
and other sports). Figure 1 displays the male par

0.40 -j-j
0.35

0.30

0.25-- - - -

0.20-- -1- - -

__ - Jl I I I
J^ J^ > ^ A ^ & <& J" -& J^ J" ^
y S j S s S ? S ^VVVV
Figure 1. Male Adolescent Sports Participation
6-i-i

-. 5.5 --

i""111
I 4'5"'
I ITi''ill
I) tit
'
3.5 -- .
I
T
3 -I-1-1-1-1-1-1-1-1-1-1-1-1

Figure 2. Popularity by Male Adolescent Sports Participation

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
714 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

the mean number of friendship nominations meaning that a respondent either (1) lacks
received by athletes participating in the various reciprocated male friendships or (2) has male
sports, as well as the mean number of nomina friends that do not play the sport in question,
tions going to nonathletes. The greater popu to 1, indicating that all of the respondent's
larity of athletes as compared to nonathletes is male friends play the designated sport. To dis
most apparent. On average, athletes have over tinguish between individuals with no recipro
one friendship nomination more than non cated male friendships and adolescents whose
athletes (meanAthlete = 4.6, meanNonathlete = 3.3,
male friends do not play sports, I include a
p < .001). Among the different sports, base dummy variable for respondents who lack
ball, basketball, and football rate high in their
reciprocated male friendship nominations. In
association to peer status. Moreover, the "big
addition, I control for the number of reported
three" sports are very similar in their popular
friendships not listed in the high school or sis
ity levels, which when added to their similar par
ter middle school rosters. Approximately 15
ticipation rates, help to reduce the risk that
percent of all friendship nominations were to
differences on these qualities could make spu
unknown peers.
rious any associations found between the sports
To account for potential selection effects, I
and male violence. Of the individual sports,
wrestling has a relatively high association to include two measures of prior antisocial behav
peer status, while tennis is among the lower ior. The first, prior violence, is taken from the
status sports. in-school survey and indicates whether a
I use two sets of sports measures. First, I cre respondent was involved in a fight within 12
ate a global sports measure, coded 1 if the months prior to the survey. Unlike the in-home
respondent answered yes to participating in any measure, the in-school violence measure does
of the listed athletic activities and 0 otherwise. not refer to the "seriousness" of the fight. It is
This measure allows me to test if athletes, ver therefore not a true lagged dependent measure.
sus nonathletes, are more likely to be involved However, its reference to fighting should con
in serious fights. Second, I look at the inde tinue to capture much of the effects of unob
pendent effects of football, basketball, base served population heterogeneity. The
ball, wrestling, and tennis on future violence. distribution for this variable is similar to the
These sports are interesting because they vary Wave I measure, but a moderate correlation
along a variety of theoretical dimensions, includ (.35) between the in-school and Wave I meas
ing physical contact, popularity, and peer con ures suggests that there remains sufficient vari
texts. I then compare these sports to other sports ation to explain. The second measure, prior
and the omitted category of nonathletes.3 delinquency, is a mean index of six minor
I operationalize friends' sports participation delinquency items (smoking, drinking, getting
using six variables, each capturing the propor drunk, skipping school, doing something dan
tion of the respondents' male friends who play
gerous on a dare, and racing a vehicle) with
football, basketball, baseball, wrestling, tennis,
possible responses ranging from 0 (never) to
or another sport. To more closely approximate
6 (nearly everyday). The Cronbach alpha for
objective friendships, I rely on reciprocated
the prior delinquency index is .71. To control
nominations, meaning that a tie sent by the
for friends' levels of fighting and minor delin
respondent (ego) had to be returned by the
quency, I also construct measures that average
receiver (alter) for it to be considered a "friend
these behaviors across respondents' male
ship."4 Values for these measures range from 0,
friends. The peer measure of violence cap
tures the proportion of male friends who have
been in a fight, while the peer delinquency
3 Correlations between the sports measures range
between .04 (tennis-football) to .24 (baseball-foot measure captures the average delinquency
ball), suggesting that each measure has ample unique among a respondent's male friends.
variance to minimize problems of collinearity.
4 This measure is often termed the send-and-receive
network and overcomes potentially problematic sit same perceptions. I examined alternative network
uations where individuals perceive that they have measures (send-or-receive, just send, and just receive)
friends while the supposed "friends" do not share the with similar results.

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 715

Self-esteem and socioeconomic status may MALE VIOLENCE AND SPORTS


also be related both to violence and sports par
I use survey-corrected logistic regressions to
ticipation (Hughes and Coakley 1991). I con predict the binary measure of serious fighting.
struct two indices. The self-esteem measure is
I estimate models in STATA using the SURVEY
an index created from three items ("I have a lot commands (Chantala and Tabor 1999). This
of good qualities," "I have a lot to be proud of," method adjusts standard errors to correct for the
and "I like myself just the way I am") with a correlated error structure resulting from indi
Cronbach alpha of .76. The SES measure cap viduals sharing similar school contexts. In addi
tures the highest parents' educational and occu tion, the inclusion of poststratification weights
pational attainment, as reported by students in gains population estimates by correcting for (1)
the in-school survey. Values in this scale range unequal selection probabilities resulting from
from 0 (neither parent achieved a high school the oversampling of specific subpopulations,
such as disabled students and siblings, and (2)
education or is currently employed) to 10 (at
the loss of respondents due to survey attrition.6
least one parent pursued postgraduate education
Table 2 reports five survey-adjusted models
and has a professional job).
of male serious fighting. The first model
I include individual background and demo includes individual background and control
graphic variables to control for concepts that variables, as well as a dichotomous measure of
prior research has found to be related to delin athletic participation. The second model dis
quency or sports participation. These variables aggregates the athletic variable into the six
include measures for age, black race, family sports categories with nonathletes as the refer
structure, attachment to parents ("How much do ence category. The third model addresses issues
you think your mother/father cares for you?" of selection by including measures of prior indi
1 = not at all to 5 = very much), and commit vidual fighting and minor delinquency. The
fourth model adds measures of peer athletic
ment to school ("How hard do you try and do
behavior and network structure. Finally, the
your school work well?" 1 = I never try at all to
fifth model examines the effects of peer-report
4 = I try very hard to do my best). Of the latter,
ed violence and minor delinquency.
parent attachment and school commitment are
Looking at Model 1, we find few surprises
commonly viewed as indicators of social bond regarding the relationships between individual
ing and are therefore important controls for background characteristics and violence.
examining the independent effects of sports on Consistent with prior research, age, intact fam
violence. Membership in nonathletic extracur ily, socioeconomic status, parent attachment,
ricular clubs may also indicate school bonding and school commitment are significant negative
and confound the relationship between sports risk factors for subsequent male violence, while
and violence. I create a dichotomous measure black males are at greater risk of fighting.
of club membership with values of 1 for respon Interestingly, involvement in a nonathletic
extracurricular activity also decreases the like
dents who participated in one of 20 nonathlet
lihood of getting into a fight by over 25 percent.
ic activities and 0 otherwise. Finally, I include
This finding, along with the negative effects of
a measure of respondents' body mass index
parental attachment and school commitment,
(BMI), calculated as: BMI = (kilos)/(meters2).
Physical size may be positively related to both
male violence (Felson 1996) and sports partic
ipation.5 6 In a previous draft of this article, I used HLM to
correct for the correlated error structure and explore
school-level effects. However, I found little between
school variance in serious fighting and few school
5 Add Health's BMI measure does not distinguish level effects. Moreover, the individual-level effects
physical size from strength or muscularity. Males in the HLM models were virtually identical to those
with relatively high BMI values may therefore be presented in this article. For the sake of parsimony,
either more muscular or more obese as compared to I therefore present only the survey-adjusted esti
other males. mates.

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
716 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 2. Survey-Adjusted Logistic Regressions of Male Adolescent Violence (N = 6,397)


Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Beta Odds Beta Odds Beta Odds Beta Odds Beta Odds
Variable_(SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio
Intercept 4.37*** 4.29*** 1.73** 1.82** 1.59*
(.51) (.52) (.61) (.63) (.65)
Age -.17*** .84 -.16*** .85 -.14*** .87 -.14*** .87 -.13*** .87
(.03) (.03) (.03) (.03) (.03)
Black .37** 1.44 .34** 1.41 .42** 1.52 .42** .84 .42** .85
(.11) CU) (.12) (.12) (.12)
Intact Family -.27** .77 -.27** .77 -.18 .84 -.17 .97 -.17 .97
(.09) (.09) (.09) (.10) (.10)
Parent Attachment -.12 .89 -.11 .90 .00 1.00 .01 .96 .01 .96
(.06) (.06) (.06) (.06) (.06)
School Commitment -.23*** .80 -.21*** .81 -.04 .97 -.03 .99 -.03 .99
(.05) (.05) (.07) (.07) (.07)
Self-Esteem -.12* .89 -.14** .87 -.11 .89 -.11 1.02 -.12 1.01
(.05) (.05) (.06) (.06) (.06)
Family SES -.04** .96 -.04* .96 -.04* .96 -.04* 1.25 -.04* 1.23
(.02) (.02) (.02) (.02) (.02)
Club Member -.31*** .74 -.28** .76 -.24** .79 -.24** .90 -.24** .90
(.08) (.08) (.08) (.08) (.09)
Body Mass Index .002 1.00 -.007 .99 -.013 .99
(.01) (.01) (.01) (.01) (.01)
Athlete .10 1.10
(.08)
Football .34*** 1.41 .23* 1.26 .18 1.19 .18 .81
(.09) (.10) (.11) (.11)
Basketball -.01 .99 -.02 .98 .02 1.02 .01 .94
(.09) (.10) (.10) (.10)
Baseball -.01 .99 -.05 .96 -.02 .98 -.02 .89
(.10) (.10) (.10) (.10)
Wrestling .37* 1.45 .24 1.27 .22 1.25 .21 1.37
(.14) (.16) (.15) (.15)
Tennis -.43* .65 -.48* .62 -.45* .64 -.45* .64
(.20) (.20) (.20) (.20)
Other Sport -.13 .88 -.14 .87 -.11 .90 -.10 .90
_C09)_009)_009)_(.09)
(continued on next page)

provides support for social control arguments Model 2 examines the independent effects of
that conventional bonds inhibit youth violence. football, basketball, baseball, wrestling, and
Model 1 also suggests, though, that athletic partennis participation on male adolescent vio
ticipation may not hold similar inhibitorylence. I also include an indicator of participa
effects. Contrary to the hypothesis derived from tion in other sports, leaving nonathletes as the
social control theory, I find that athletic particomitted category. The results demonstrate that
ipation shows a positive relationship to serioussports differ significantly in their relationships
fighting. Albeit nonsignificant, this runs count to serious fighting. Of the "big three" U.S.
er to arguments suggesting that sports particisports, only football shows a significant and
pation encourages conventional lines of action positive relationship with fighting. Playing foot
and reduces antisocial behavior. It remainsball increases the risk of getting into a serious
unclear, however, if the relationship between fight by over 40 percent, compared to nonath
violence and athletic participation varies byletes, while basketball and baseball participation
type of sport (as suggested by masculinity argu show no relationship to fighting. Of the two
ments). I now turn to this question. individual sports, wrestling shows a positive

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 717

Table 2. (continued)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Beta Odds Beta Odds Beta Odds Beta Odds Beta Odds
Variable (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio (SE) Ratio
Prior Physical Fighting 1.40*** 4.07 1.41 ***4.09 1.40***4.07
(.08) (.08) (.08)
Minor Delinquency .04*** 1.04 .04***1.04 .04***1.04
(.01) (.01) (.01)
Peer Network Measures
No Reciprocated Male Friends -.08 .93 .04 1.04
(.09) (.12)
Friends Outside School .04 1.04 .04 1.04
(.02) (.02)
Percent Male Friends Football .35* 1.42 .33* 1.38
(.16) (.16)
Percent Male Friends Basketball -.20 .82 -.22 .81
(.15) (.15)
Percent Male Friends Baseball -.20 .82 -.22 .80
(.15) (.14)
Percent Male Friends Wrestling -.02 .98 -.07 .94
(.24) (.24)
Percent Male Friends Tennis -.55 .58 -.52 .59
(.34) (.34)
Percent Male Friends Other Sport -.14 .87 -.12 .89
(.14) (.14)
Male Friends Average Delinquency -.01 .99
(.01)
Percent Male Friends Physical Fight .31 * 1.37
(.14)

F-Statistic(dfl,df2)_12.88(10,128)10.97(15,107) 37.64(17,105) 25.64(25,97) 27.05(27,95)


***p<.001; **/?<.01; */?<.05.

effect on fighting that is similar to football, with ciation to male violence. Together, these results
wrestlers being 45 percent more likely than suggest a continuum of physical contact and
nonathletes to get into a fight. Playing tennis masculinity whereby highly masculinized contact
shows the opposite effect, significantly decreas sports increase the risks of violence. Sports low
ing the risks of fighting by 35 percent. These in physical contact and less associated with mas
results provide strong support for masculinity culinity, in contrast, seem to curb such behavior.
arguments, in that the two heavy-contact and It is possible that the effects of football and
exclusively male sports (i.e., football and wrestling are spurious and explained by latent
wrestling) show the strongest positive relation characteristics or prior socialization. Model 3
ships to male violence, while tennis, the sport gains leverage on this question by including
with the least amount of physical contact and his measures of prior fighting and minor delin
torically the least male dominated, has the quency. These variables control for much of the
strongest negative association with male vio effects of state dependence or selectivity into
lence. On the other hand, there appears to be lit sports, while also providing a more conservative
tle support for the expectation that involvement test of the sports-violence relationship. There is,
in high visibility sports alone increases levels of notably, considerable stability in antisocial behav
violence. Neither baseball nor basketball is asso ior over time. Self-reported fighting during the
ciated with an increased risk of fighting. in-school survey increases the risk of being in a
Moreover, wrestling, an individual sport with serious future fight by over 300 percent. Similarly,
low participation rates, has a strong positive asso minor acts of delinquency increase the likeli

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
718 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

hood of serious fighting, suggesting the gener reported), yet this interaction was nonsignifi
ality of antisocial behavior (Gottfredson and cant. This suggests that, if anything, football
Hirschi 1990). Indeed, these variables attenuate players in football networks are less likely to
over one-third of the football coefficient and behave violently than similarly situated non
almost one-half the wrestling coefficient, mak football males. Perhaps this null effect is not sur
ing the latter nonsignificant. Football, however, prising, as a nonathlete in a football group would
remains a strong and significant predictor of vio have more to prove and be in a more precarious
lence, suggesting that self-selection does not social position than a football player in the same
account for the entire relationship. situation. Violence for the nonathlete would
Model 4 explores the potential mediating role, become a means of demonstrating worthiness
derived from social learning theories, of friend to the football-dominated group.
ship network composition on the sports-violence To illustrate the effects of athletic friend
relationship. This model includes peer network ships, Figure 3 presents predicted probabilities
measures for each of the six sports categories. The of serious fighting across different proportions
results provide support for combined social learn of football, basketball, baseball, wrestling, and
ing and masculinity predictions. Males with a tennis friends (holding other variables at their
high proportion of reciprocated friends playing means). The risk of fighting increases with
football are significantly more likely to behave higher proportions of football friends. Males
violently than those without football friends. Net with all-football friends are expected to have a
of the number of friends playing other sports, 45 percent probability of getting into a serious
individuals whose friends all play football are 38 fight, more than 8 percentage points higher
percent more likely to get into a serious fight than than similar individuals with no football friends
those without football friends. All other measures and almost 20 percentage points higher than
of peer sports participation are nonsignificant. males with all-tennis friends. Like Haynie's
It is particularly interesting that embeddedness (2002) examination of the relationship between
in wrestling networks shows a small negative delinquency and delinquent friendship networks,
relationship to violence (a finding that persists this analysis suggests that embeddedness in
when all other peer measures are removed from homogeneous peer networks most effectively
the model). This suggests that it is the combina constrains individual behaviors toward group
tion of heavy physical contact and a team setting norms and increases opportunities for group
(e.g., football), and not just the physical contact related behavior. In this case, the norms and
associated with wrestling, that encourages male opportunities associated with all-football net
violence. Indeed, football friendships fully atten works are positively associated with increased
uate the direct effect of individual football par male violence.
ticipation, decreasing the size of the football As with the potentially spurious relationship
coefficient by 35 percent from Model 3. Much between individual sports participation and
of the violence associated with football is thus
fighting, it is possible that the effects of peer net
explained by athletes in this sport having greater works may be explained by friends' violence or
contact with other football players. Moreover, delinquent activities. Such a pattern would
embeddedness in a football network significantly reflect the possibility that individuals seek
increases the risk of serious violence, regardless friends who are violent and that shared violent
of an individual's level of football participation. tendencies predict both involvement in sports
But is the effect of football-playing friends and future individual violence. Model 5 exam
greater for football players than for non-football ines this by including measures of the propor
players? It could be that the relationship between tion of friends who self-report being in a fight
football and violence increases as players are (in-school survey) and the average minor delin
more immersed in football peer networks.7 To test quency of those same friends. As one would
this possibility, I included an interaction between expect, the proportion of friends who have been
football and football networks in Model 4 (not in a fight is positively associated with individ
ual fighting. Individuals whose friends all report
getting into a fight are themselves 38 percent
7 This idea was raised by a reviewer of a previous more likely to get into a fight. The delinquen
manuscript draft. cy of male friends shows no relationship to seri

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 719

0.50 T-1

I - - Football I
-, ._ '-*-Baseball
0 4%- -_=*
' J -A- Basketba
-X- Wrestling ^^4^-""""""^
^> -3K- Tennis _^~~-~"~~^
a '-? ^-""^
| 0.40-_^-^""^^^

bO ^*\^^
1)0.30-^^"-^
0.25 -I-1-1-1-1
0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00
Proportion of Friends

Figure 3. Predicted Probability of Serious Fighting by Friends' Sports Participation

ous fighting, net of other covariates. competing


More hypotheses from prominent pe
importantly for my stated hypotheses, thespectives.
intro Contrary to the social control hypot
duction of the two measures does not signifi
esis, my results suggest that sports fail to protec
cantly attenuate the peer football measure.
males from interpersonal violence. Indeed, con
Having a high proportion of friends playing
tact sports (e.g., football and wrestling) are po
football maintains a significant association with associated with male serious fighting.
itively
serious violence. This effect is mediated by peer football parti
To test whether the effects of playing sports
ipation, such that embeddedness in all-footbal
or having friends who play sports differ by
networks substantially increases the risk for
race, social class, or self-esteem, I include
serious fighting. These findings are consistent
interactions between the sport-specific vari
with hypotheses derived from social learning
ables and individual background variables in
and masculinity theories and provide impo
the final model (not shown). None of tanttheseimpetus for further research. Although
interactions are significant atp < .05. caveats
Thus, exist, this study offers leverage on a
there appears to be little evidence that norma
potential paradox of youth sports, and thi
tive pressures resulting from sports participa
understanding may help inform schools' sport
tion should be higher for more disadvantaged
policies.
or insecure youth.
Playing hypermasculine contact sports shapes
subsequent violence. Some of this relationship
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS is explained by selection effects, with the intro
duction
This analysis represents a quantitative foray of prior fighting and delinquency atten
into the relationship between adolescent sportssignificant proportion of the football
uating a
and male interpersonal violence. Whileand wrestling direct effects. This is not sur
prior
prising, given that aggressive kids are likely to
research has demonstrated that sports partici
pation is associated with many positive enter
outcontact sports and the coaches of these
comes, few studies have focused on theare likely to choose aggressive kids to
activities
fill more competitive teams. However, selection
connections between high school interscholas
does rea
tic sports and violence, despite compelling not appear to tell the entire story. Net of
sons for doing so. The sports-violence prior fighting and delinquency, football remains
relationship is a theoretically rich area with
a significant predictor of serious fighting and

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
720 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

is only fully mediated with the introduction of tations and maintain their superiority within
friends' football participation. gendered peer hierarchies.8
The theoretical implications of a positive This argument parallels Sutherland's (1940)
football-violence relationship are apparent. explanation for white-collar crime. For
If we believe that football players generally Sutherland, white-collar crime results from over
lie at the center of a school's peer culture (as conformity to the competitive norms within
suggested by prior adolescence research and business and is therefore unlikely to stem from
the sports-popularity relationship observed deviant identities or social isolation from con
above), it is difficult to explain the football ventional society. Like athletes in heavy-contact
violence connection as resulting from weak team sports, businesspeople are often reinforced
social bonds. The social and individual ben for behaviors that are at odds with legal rules.
efits accrued by male contact-sport athletes Because their behaviors occur within respect
do not suggest that they reject conventional ed and high-status institutional settings, how
norms and lie on the fringes of conventional ever, both businesspeople and athletes are able
society, and yet increased rates of violence to behave in socially unacceptable ways while
from these athletes also suggest that they are avoiding deviant labels and identities. It is only
not altogether conforming. This seemingly when misbehavior within these institutions
paradoxical pattern was recognized at foot becomes particularly egregious or highly visi
ball's inception. Raymond Gettell, a physical ble that the public reevaluates its perceptions and
educator at Amherst College at the beginning increases social controls.
of the twentieth century, echoed the views of W^hile my findings contribute to our under
many Americans when he described football standing of the relationship between sports and
as an essential tool for preparing young men violence, there are obvious limitations. First,
for war. According to Gettell, football simul although I control for prior levels of fighting and
taneously (1) provides young males with minor delinquency, it remains possible that
opportunities for "physical combat," (2) sat unobserved heterogeneity explains the associ
isfies a "primitive lust for battle," and (3) ation between peer athletic participation and
provides a "higher and distinctively civilized violence. One possibility for addressing unob
interest in organization, cooperation, and the served heterogeneity is to look at within-indi
skilled interrelation of individual effort direct
vidual change over time using a fixed effects
ed to a common purpose" (see Burstyn approach. In a fixed effects model, longitudinal
1999:73). GettelFs statements reveal the com dependent and independent variables measure
peting demands facing many athletes. change while controlling for time stable char
On the one hand, parents, coaches, and acteristics. Unfortunately, such an approach is
communities expect athletes to abide by con impossible in the current analysis due to a lack
ventional rules, with the threat of team expul of longitudinal sports measures. Add Health
sion potentially deterring misbehavior. On only asked sports-specific questions during the
the other hand, these same groups provide in-school survey. Without longitudinal data on
contact-sport athletes with situational defi
sports participation, population heterogeneity
nitions that support violence as a means of and selection effects can only be addressed by
attaining "battlefield" victories, increasing identifying plausible instrumental variables
peer status, and asserting "warrior" identi
ties. Given these conflicting definitions,
whether or not male contact-sport athletes
behave violently likely depends on the situa
8 Recent research also suggests that athletes are at
tional contexts in which they find themselves.
greater risk of alcohol use (Eitle, Turner, and Eitle
In the classroom, constraint and conformity
2003) and drunk driving (Hartmann and Massoglia
validate the conventional identities expected forthcoming). These findings, along with positive
of contact-sport athletes, as well as help to connections between minor delinquency and popu
solidify their high status positions within larity (Allen et al. 2005; Kreager forthcoming), sup
schools. In informal peer situations, howev port views that "in-crowd" athletes are under pressure
er, gendered displays of power and aggression to maintain peer status with demonstrations of mas
allow the same males to meet group expec culinity and risky behaviors (see also Curry 1998).

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 721

(notoriously difficult in social science research) contributors to a school's gender regime. The
or by including theoretically relevant controls. results of this project clearly demonstrate a link
I take the latter approach but recognize the between contact sports and violence, meaning
imperfection in this strategy. that these activities may be appropriate sites
Second, this study is unable to identify the for disrupting male violence. Strong selection
causal mechanisms that explain the observed effects also suggest, though, that much of the
relationships. Although some of the results are connection between contact sports and violence
consistent with arguments derived from mas occurs prior to individuals' high school athlet
culinity and socialization theories, an inability ic experiences. This possibility makes it all the
to identify specific mechanisms (e.g., subjects' more important that coaches, parents, and school
identification with hegemonic masculinity, administrators be conscious of their gate-keep
objective reinforcement for violence, or vic ing roles and use their positions to prevent con
tims as "weaker" peers) leaves open the possi tinued athlete violence at all developmental
bility for alternative explanations. This is a stages. Precluding problematic youth from play
problem often associated with cultural, identi ing contact sports, not tolerating athletic vio
ty, and values research. These concepts are elu lence, and fostering a more tolerant atmosphere
sive and open to interpretation. Qualitative are three means of breaking the contact sport
research provides the best hope for under violence relationship. These changes necessitate
standing the mechanisms underlying this arti de-emphasizing the "winning is everything"
cle's findings. Similar to the work of Eder and mentality, an unlikely proposition given the
colleagues (1997) and Curry (1998), researchers demands placed on coaches and players to make
must return to the field to identify the person
their schools and communities proud. However,
al and situational characteristics associated with
the positive benefits for individual trajectories
athlete misbehavior. Only ethnographic studies
may outweigh the costs of a losing team record.
can gain leverage on the intersections of context,
As Trulson (1986) found when researching mar
opportunity, and motivations that surround
tial arts programs, contact sports that emphasize
sports-related violence. Based on situational
respect for others, self-control, patience, and
research of male-on-male violence (Luckenbill
humility can serve to reduce the violence of
1977; Polk 1999), I am inclined to believe that
aggressive male adolescents. Programs devel
athlete violence typically involves threats to
oped according to these ideals may not neces
masculinity, the presence of an audience, and an
sarily win as many matches as those built on
informal setting such as a party or schoolyard.
aggression and competitiveness, but their attrac
It may also be, though, that male athletes active
tiveness lies in positively affecting the lives of
ly seek violent encounters to demonstrate their
problematic youth while fostering an environ
masculine prowess and group worth. Likewise,
ment of inclusiveness and respect.
individual characteristics (e.g., athletic ability
or position played) and team characteristics Derek A. Kreager is an Assistant Professor of
(e.g., win-loss record) may facilitate athlete Sociology and Crime, Law, and Justice at the
violence. Addressing these issues is beyond the Pennsylvania State University. His research interests
scope of this article, but future qualitative include crime, the life course, deterrence, and social
research could better disentangle the proximal network analysis. He is currently working on a proj
causes of sports-related violence. ect with Ross Matsueda and David Huizinga that
Despite such limitations, results suggest that analyzes life course trajectories of drug use and
sports, particularly male-dominated contact crime. His prior work, focusing on decision making
sports, have important consequences for male and delinquent friendships, has been published in
ASR (with Ross Matsueda and David Huizinga) and
adolescent violence. Such findings may also Social Forces.
have certain policy implications. As Connell
(1996) states in his insightful look at gender and
education, the most important step in address REFERENCES
ing violence and bullying in schools is to Allen, Joseph P., Maryfrances R. Porter, F. Christy
become aware of the masculinizing practices McFarland, Penny Marsh, and Kathleen Boykin
that contribute to gender privilege and hege McElhaney. 2005. "The Two Faces of Adolescents'
monic domination. Sports, he argues, are major Success with Peers: Adolescent Popularity, Social

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
722 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Adaptation, and Deviant Behavior." Child Room: Campus Bars and College Athletes."
Development 76:747-60. Sociology of Sport Journal 15:205-15.
Agnew, Robert and David M. Petersen. 1989. Eccles, Jacquelynne S. and Bonnie L. Barber. 1999.
"Leisure and Delinquency." Social Problems "Student Council, Volunteering, Basketball, or
36:332-50. Marching Band: What Kind of Extracurricular
Akers, Ronald L. 1998. Social Learning and Social Involvement Matters?" Journal of Adolescent
Structure: A General Theory of Crime and Research 14:10-43.
Deviance. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Eckert, Penelope. 1989. Jocks and Burnouts: Social
Press. Categories and Identity in High School. New York:
Akers, Ronald L., Marvin D. Krohn, Lonn Lanza Teachers College Press.
Kaduce, and Marcia Radosevich. 1979. "Social Eder, Donna, Catherine C. Evans, and Stephen
Learning and Deviant Behavior: A Specific Test of Parker. 1997'. School Talk: Gender and Adolescent
a General Theory." American Sociological Review Culture. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University
44:636-55. Press.
Baumert, Paul W., John M. Henderson, and Nancy Eder, Donna and David A. Kinney. 1995. "The Effect
Thompson. 1998. "Health Risk Behaviors of of Middle School Extracurricular Activities on
Adolescent Participants in Organized Sports." Adolescents' Popularity and Peer Status." Youth
Journal of Adolescent Health 22:460-65. and Society 26:298-324.
Begg, Dorothy J., John D. Langley, Terrie Moffit, and Eitle, David, R. Jay Turner, and Tamela McNulty
Stephen W. Marshall. 1996. "Sport and Eitle. 2003. "The Deterrence Hypothesis
Delinquency: An Examination of the Deterrence Reexamined: Sports Participation and Substance
Hypothesis in a Longitudinal Study." British Use Among Young Adults." Journal of Drug Issues
Journal of Sports Medicine 30:335-41. 33:193-222.
Bissinger, H. G. 1991. Friday Night Lights: A Town, Eveslage, Scott and Kevin Delaney. 1998. "Talkin
A Team, A Dream. New York: Harper Perennial. Trash at Hardwick High: A Case Study of Insult
Burgess, Robert L. and Ronald L. Akers. 1966. "A Talk on a Boys' Basketball Team." International
Differential Association-Reinforcement Theory Review of Sport Sociology 33:239-53.
of Criminal Behavior." Social Problems Ewing, Martha, Lori Gano-Overway, Crystal Branta,
14:128-47. and Vern Seefeldt. 2002. "The Role of Sports in
Burstyn, Varda. 1999. The Rites of Men: Manhood, Youth Development." Pp. 31-47 in Paradoxes of
Politics, and the Culture of Sports. Toronto, Youth and Sport, edited by M. Gatz, M. Messner
Canada: University of Toronto Press. and S. J. Ball-Rokeach. Albany, NY: State
Chantala, Kim and Joyce Tabor. 1999. "Strategies to University of New York Press.
Perform a Design-Based Analysis Using the Add Fejgin, Naomi. 1994. "Participation in High School
Health Data." Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Competitive Sports: A Subversion of School
Carolina Population Center. Retrieved January 12, Mission or Contribution to Academic Goals."
2007 (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/ Sociology of Sport Journal 11:211-30.
files/weight l.pdf). Felson, Richard B. 1996. "Big People Hit Little
Coakley, Jay. 2001. Sport in Society: Issues and People: Sex Differences in Physical Power and
Controversies. 7th ed. Boston, MA: McGraw Hill. Interpersonal Violence." Criminology 34:433-52.
Coleman, James. 1961. The Adolescent Society: The Fine, Gary Alan 1987. With the Boys: Little League
Social Life of the Teenager and its Impact on Baseball andPreadolescent Culture. Chicago, IL:
Education. New York: The Free Press of Glencoe. University of Chicago Press.
Connell, Robert W. 1995. Masculinities. Berkeley, Gatz, Margaret, Michael A. Messner, and Sandra J.
CA: University of California Press. Ball-Rokeach. 2002. "Introduction." Pp. 1-12 in
-. 1996. "Teaching the Boys: New Research Paradoxes of Youth and Sport, edited by M. Gatz,
on Masculinity, and Gender Strategies for M. Messner, and S. J. Ball-Rokeach. Albany, NY:
Schools." Teachers College Record 98:206-35. State University of New York Press.
Connell, Robert W. and James W. Messerschmidt. Goffman, Erving. 1967. Interaction Ritual: Essays on
2005. "Hegemonic Masculinity: Rethinking the Face-to-Face Behavior. Garden City, NJ:
Concept." Gender and Society 19:829-59. Doubleday.
Crosnoe, Robert. 2001. "The Social World of Male Gottfredson, Michael and Travis Hirschi. 1990. A
and Female Athletes in High School." Sociological General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford
Studies of Children and Youth 8:87-108. University Press.
Crosset, Todd. 1999. "Male Athletes' Violence Hartmann, Douglas. 2001. "Notes on Midnight
Against Women: A Critical Assessment of the Basketball and the Cultural Politics of Recreation,
Athletic Affiliation, Violence Against Women Race, and At-Risk Urban Youth." Journal of Sport
Debate." Quest 51:244-57. and Social Issues 25:339-71.
Curry, Timothy Jon. 1998. "Beyond the Locker Hartmann, Douglas and Michael Massoglia.

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
SPORTS AND MALE VIOLENCE 723

Forthcoming. "Re-Assessing the Relationship Matsueda, Ross L. and Kathleen Anderson. 1998.
Between High School Sports Participation and "The Dynamics of Delinquent Peers and
Deviance: Evidence of Enduring, Bifurcated Delinquent Behavior." Criminology
Effects." Sociological Quarterly. 36:269-308.
Haynie, Dana. 2002. "Friendship Networks and McNeal, Ralph B. 1995. "Extracurricular Activities
Delinquency: The Relative Nature of Peer and High School Dropouts." Sociology of
Delinquency." Journal of Quantitative Criminology Education 68:62-81.
18:99-134. Messner, Michael. 1992. Power at Play: Sports and
Hirschi, Travis. 1969. Causes of Delinquency. the Problem of Masculinity. Boston, MA: Beacon
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Press.
Holland, Alyce and Thomas Andre. 1994. "Athletic -. 2002. Taking the Field: Women, Men, and
Participation and the Social Status of Adolescent Sports. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota
Males and Females." Youth and Society Press.
25:388-407. Nixon, Howard L. 1997. "Gender, Sport, and
Hughes, Robert and Jay Coakley. 1991. "Positive Aggressive Behavior Outside Sport." Journal of
Deviance Among Athletes: The Implications of Sport and Social Issues 21:3 79-91.
Overconformity to the Sport Ethic." Sociology of Osgood, D. Wayne, Janet K. Wilson, Patrick M.
Sport Journal 8:307-25. O'Malley, Jerald G. Bachman, and Lloyd D.
Jackson, James S., Shelley Keiper, Kendrick Brown, Johnson. 1996. "Routine Activities and Individual
Tony Brown, and Warde Manuel. 2002. "Athletic Deviant Behavior." American Sociological Review
Identity, Racial Attitudes, and Aggression in First 61:635-55.
Year Black and White Intercollegiate Athletes." Pp. Otto, Luther and Duane Alwin. 1977. "Athletics,
159-72 in Paradoxes of Youth Sport, edited by M. Aspirations and Attainments." Sociology of
Gatz, M. Messner, and S. J. Ball-Rokeach. New Education 50:102-13.
York: State University of New York Press. Pappas, Nick, Patrick McKenry, and Beth Catlett.
Jeziorski, Ronald M. 1994. The Importance of School 2004. "Athlete Aggression on the Rink and off
Sports in American Education and Socialization. the Ice." Men and Masculinities 6:291-312.
New York: University Press of America. Polk, Kenneth. 1999. "Males and Honor Contest
Kreager, Derek A. Forthcoming. "When It's Good to Violence." Homicide Studies 3:6-29.
be 'Bad': Violence and Adolescent Peer Status." Sabo, Donald. 1994. "Pigskin, Patriarchy and Pain."
Criminology. Pp. 82-88 in Sex, Violence, and Power in Sports:
Landers, Daniel M. and Donna M. Landers. 1978. Rethinking Masculinity, edited by M. Messner and
"Socialization via Interscholastic Athletics: Its D. Sabo. Freedom, CA: Crossing Press.
Effects on Delinquency." Sociology of Education Sabo, Donald, Merrill J. Melnick, and Beth E.
51:299-303. Vanfossen. 1993. "High School Athletic
Langbein, Laura and Roseana Bess. 2002. "Sports in Participation and Postsecondary Educational
School: Source of Amity or Antipathy?" Social and Occupational Mobility: A Focus on Race
Science Quarterly 83:436-54. and Gender." Sociology of Sport Journal
Larson, Reed. 1994. "Youth Organizations, Hobbies, 10:44-56.
and Sports as Developmental Contexts." Pp. 46-65 Shields, Edgar W 1999. "Intimidation and Violence
in Adolescence in Context, edited by R. K. by Males in High School Athletics." Adolescence
Silberiesen and E. Todt. New York: Springer 34:503-21.
Verlag. Spady, William. 1970. "Lament for the Letterman:
Luckenbill, David F. 1977. "Criminal Homicide as a Effect of Peer Status and Extracurricular Activities
Situated Transaction." Social Problems 25:176-86. on Goal and Achievement." American Journal of
Mahoney, Joseph L. 2000. "School Extracurricular Sociology 75:680-702.
Activity Participation as a Moderator in the Stark, Rodney, Lori Kent, and Roger Finke. 1987.
Development of Antisocial Patterns." Child "Sports and Delinquency." Pp. 115-24 in Positive
Development 71:502-l 6. Criminology, edited by M. R. Gottfredson and T.
Mahoney, Joseph L. and Robert B. Cairns. 1997. Hirschi. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
"Do Extracurricular Activities Protect Against Sutherland, Edwin H. 1940. "White Collar
Early School Dropout?" Developmental Criminality" American Sociological Review
Psychology 33:241-53. 5:1-12.
Marsh, Herbert W. 1993. "The Effects of Participation -. 1947. Principles of Criminology. 4th ed.
in Sport During the Last Two Years of High Philadelphia, PA: J. B. Lippincott.
School." Sociology of Sport Journal 10:18-^3. Trulson, Michael E. 1986. "Martial Arts Training: A
Matsueda, Ross L. 1997. '"Cultural Deviance Novel 'Cure' for Juvenile Delinquency." Human
Theory': The Remarkable Persistence of a Flawed Relations 39:1131-40.
Term." Theoretical Criminology 1:429-52. Young, Kevin, Philip White, and William McTeer.

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
724 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

1994. "Body Talk: Male Athletes Reflect on Sport, Influence of Delinquent Peers: What They Think
Injury, and Pain." Sociology of Sport Journal or What They Do?" Criminology 29:851-66.
11:175-94. Wright, Darlene and Kevin Fitzpatrick. 2006. "Social
Warr, Mark. 2002. Companions in Crime: The Social Capital and Adolescent Violent Behavior:
Aspects of Criminal Conduct. Cambridge, UK: Correlates of Fighting and Weapon Use Among
Cambridge University Press. Secondary School Students." Social Forces
Warr, Mark and Mark C. Stafford. 1991. "The 84:1435-53.

This content downloaded from 181.118.149.24 on Mon, 05 Sep 2016 20:20:32 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like