You are on page 1of 12

Preparation of Patients for Group Therapy

A Controlled Study
Irvin D. Yolom, MD; Peter S. Houts, PhD; Gary Newell, AB; and Kenneth H. Rand, AB, Palo Alto, Calif

WILL an explanatory session preparing therapeutic framework has reoriented us to


prospective patients increase the efficacy of an appreciation of the patient's adaptive
group therapy? This article describes a con- coping mechanisms, and toward recruitment
trolled research project designed to answer of these processes in the therapeutic frame¬
this question. work.6 The utilization of patient as thera¬
The query springs from many sources. pist in staffless groups in many psychiatric
Laboratory and clinical group research has hospitals, the programming of leaderless
demonstrated the crucial importance of ear- outpatient groups,7 the instrumented Hu¬
ly meetings in shaping the future course of a man Development Institute programs for
group.1 Group norms established early in interpersonal learning (unpublished materi¬
the life of the group tend to persist, outliv- al by J. Berlin and B. Wyckoff), the use
ing even a complete turnover in the group of peers or near peers as therapists,8 and the
population.2 Approximately one third of all many responsible programs in progress for
patients beginning group therapy in a uni- training nonprofessionals in psychotherapy
versity outpatient clinic drop out unim- are all manifestations of the démystification
proved during the first dozen meetings.3 trend. Psychotherapy, these approaches may
Conversely, patients who in the first 12 argue, is a rational, teachable process the
meetings achieve high group popularity, or efficacy of which is enhanced rather than
who show high satisfaction with the group, impaired by explication.
are more apt to show clinical improvement Another source of impetus for this study
at the end of 50 meetings.4 These observa- comes from the nonclinical small group
tions suggest the rationale of therapeutic in- field. Human development laboratories or
tervention early in the life of the group. A sensitivity training groups have, for many
recent study demonstrating that a pre- years, augmented the group process with ex¬
therapy "role induction interview" could planatory reading material and periodic lec-
beneficially influence the early course and turettes designed to provide a cognitive map
outcome of individual therapy provided ad¬ for the proceedings.9 Although the compara¬
ditional impetus to this study.5 If a cogni¬ bility of these groups with therapy groups is
tive orientation to therapy can so affect a still controversial, it is undeniable that there
dyadic interactional system, then its impli¬ exists much overlap in structure and process
cations for group therapy are exciting be¬ between them.
cause of the early group culture building These converging factors, then—from
which, once set into motion, is powerful and theory, research, and clinical practice—all
relatively irrevocable. suggest the importance and timeliness of a
Recent developments in the entire field of controlled investigation into the effects on
psychotherapy also suggest that a cognitive patient behavior and attitudes of a systema¬
orientation of the prospective patient might tic explicatory session prior to the beginning
beneficially influence therapy. Clearly there of group therapy. This explicatory session
is an important trend toward demys¬ instructs patients to engage in here-and-now
tification of the psychotherapeutic process, interaction focusing on relationships among
toward a defrocking of the therapist, toward group members and, in addition, was de¬
a more collaborative venture between pa¬ signed to increase faith in group therapy
tient and therapist. An ego-based psycho-N and attractiveness of the therapy group. The
Submitted for publication March 31, 1967. experimental groups, which had an explica¬
From the Department of Psychiatry, Stanford tory session, were compared with control
University School of Medicine, Palo Alto, Calif. groups which had no such session. Three hy¬
Reprint requests to Stanford Medical Center, Palo
Alto, Calif 94304 (Dr. Yalom). potheses were studied.

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


Hypothesis 1.—Patients in the experi¬ formed of the immediate availability of
mental groups were expected to have greater group therapy. Excluded from consideration
faith in group therapy than patients in con¬ flagrantly psychotic patients, addicts,
were
trol groups. alcoholics, and patients with mental retarda¬
Hypothesis 2.—Patients in the experi¬ tion or organic syndromes.
mental groups were expected to have greater The (»therapists of the six groups were 12
attraction (cohesiveness) to their groups first-year residents. During the first seven
than in control groups. months of their residency they had had an
Hypothesis 3.—Patients in the experi¬ intensive group therapy training course con¬
mental groups were expected to engage in sisting of weekly didactic sessions and clini¬
more here-and-now discussion of interper¬ cal seminars, and a 50-hour sensitivity train¬
sonal relations within the group than pa¬ ing group experience. They had observed
tients in control groups. experienced group (»therapists conduct ap¬
proximately 20 group therapy meetings
Method (with a similar patient population) and par¬
ticipated in postgroup "rehash" sessions.
Summary of Research Design.—Sixty pa¬ The general orientation of the program was
tients on the group therapy waiting list of a a dynamic, interactional one. One of the
university outpatient clinic were interviewed therapist's chief tasks was formulated as
by one of us (I. D. Y.) prior to the first meet¬ helping the group mature into the thera¬
ing with their group therapists, and were peutic agent. Emphasis was placed on the
randomly assigned to one of two conditions. importance of immediate expression of feel¬
The experimental subjects were given a sys¬ ings and here-and-now interaction, Therapy
tematic 25-minute preparatory lecture on centered on the understanding and correc¬
group therapy, whereas the control subjects,
tion of parataxic distortions among the
although seen by the same researcher for an group members and of general maladaptive
equal period of time, did not receive the interpersonal stances.
group orientation lecture. The patients then Following their sensitivity group experi¬
went into six therapy groups—three groups ence, the residents selected (»therapists.
of experimental subjects and three groups of The six (»therapist pairs were then divided,
control subjects. These groups were studied on the basis of the faculty's general assess¬
for the first 12 meetings to determine what ment of their competence, into two equal
differences, if any, resulted from our manip¬ groups of three (»therapists each. Three of
ulation. Throughout this study the group these cotherapist pairs were randomly se¬
therapists were totally unaware of the na¬ lected to lead the three groups of experimen¬
ture or design of this research. tal subjects, and the other three cotherapist
Clinical Setting.—Our sample was repre¬ pairs to lead the three control groups. It is
sentative of the population in the Stanford important to repeat that the therapists were
University outpatient clinic and has been never aware of the nature of this research.
described in detail in previous reports.3·4 It is common clinic practice for one of us
The mean age was 28, with a range from 18 (I. D. Y.) to see group patients in a pre¬
to 49. The population, largely from the mid¬ liminary screening interview. Postgroup pa¬
dle socioeconomic class, was a sophisticated, tient questionnaires and tape-recordings of
well educated one; 94% had graduated from the meetings for research purposes are also
high school and 72% had had some college part of routine clinic practice and have been
education. Fees ranged from $1 to $10 per continuously utilized in the clinic for four
session. Diagnostic classification primarily years.
indicated characterologic or neurotic disor¬ Procedure.—The 60 patients on the group
ders. Occasionally a patient was labeled waiting list were randomly divided into two
as a borderline schizophrenic or psycho¬ equal groups of experimental and control
physiological reaction. Approximately two subjects. All 60 were asked to come to the
months before the groups were scheduled to clinic to meet with the associate director of
begin, a group therapy waiting list was the clinic (I. D. Y.) for registration and for
formed. All patients applying for therapy a discussion regarding their request for
during this time were considered for group group therapy. The first patients were seen
therapy. Local referring sources were in- in small groups of three to five, but schedul-

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


ing made this design impractical people in their lives now, as well as with people
pressures
and the great majority of patients were seen they had yet to meet.
individually. The patients were advised that the way they
The experimental patients were given a could help themselves most of all was to be
25-minute group preparatory lecture. The and honest direct with their feelings in the
group at that moment, especially their feelings
major goals of this presentation were: (1) towards the other group members and the ther¬
To enhance the patient's faith in group apists.
This point was emphasized many times
therapy. (2) To enhance the attractiveness and was referred to as the "core of group thera¬
(cohesiveness) of the patient's specific py." They were told that they may, as they de¬
group. (3) To direct the patient toward con- velop trust in the group, reveal intimate aspects
frontive, here-and-now interaction in the of themselves, but that the group was not a
group. (The greatest proportion of the pre¬ forced confessional and that people had
paratory session was devoted to this goal.) differential rates of developing trust and re¬
To accomplish these goals the orientation vealing themselves. It was suggested to them
presented the patients with a rational de¬ that the group could be seen as a forum for
scription of the practice, the theoretical ba¬ risk-taking, and that as learning progressed,
new types of behavior might be tried in the
sis, and the results of group therapy. Possi¬
ble sources of stress were identified. Finally, group setting.
Certain stumbling blocks were predicted. Pa¬
patients were instructed to discuss their feel¬ tients were forewarned about a feeling of puz¬
ings about other group members. The pres¬ zlement and discouragement in the early meet¬
entation was informal and questions were
ings; it would, at times, not be apparent how
solicited and answered. A descriptive ac¬ working on group problems and intragroup re¬
count of the orientation follows. lationships could be of value in solving the
A brief explanation of the interpersonal problems which brought them to therapy. This
theory of psychiatry began with the statement puzzlement, they were told, was to be expected
that everyone seeking help in group therapy in the typical therapy process, and they were
had in common the basic problem of encounter¬ strongly urged to stay with the group and not
ing difficulty in establishing and maintaining to heed their inclinations to give up therapy.
close and gratifying relationships with others, They were told that many patients found it
although each group member manifested his painfully difficult to reveal themselves or to ex¬
problems differently. They were reminded of press directly positive or negative feelings. The
the many times in their lives that, undoubtedly, tendencies of some to withdraw
emotionally, to
they wished to clarify a relationship, to be real¬ hide their feelings, to let others express feelings
ly honest about their positive or negative feel¬ for them, or to form concealing alliances with
ings with someone and get reciprocally honest others were discussed. The therapeutic goals of
feedback from others. The general structure of group therapy were described as ambitious: we
society, however, does not often permit totally desired to change behavior and attitudes many
open communication. The therapy group, it years in the making; treatment, therefore,
was emphasized, is a special microcosm where would be gradual and many, many months in
this type of honest interpersonal exploration duration. We discussed with them the
vis-a-vis the other members is not only permit¬ velopment of feelings of frustration or annoy¬
likely de¬
ted, but encouraged. If people are conflicted in ance with the therapist, how they would in vain
their methods of relating to others, then ob¬ expect answers from him. The source of help
viously a social situation which encourages would be primarily the other patients. We
honest interpersonal explorations can provide knew, we told them, of their difficulty in ac¬
them with a clear opportunity to learn many cepting this fact, of their probably wondering
valuable things about themselves. It was em¬ "How can the blind lead the blind?"
phasized to them that working on their rela¬ Next they were told about the history and
tionships directly with other group members development of group therapy. We described,
would not be easy; in fact, it might be very for example, how group therapy passed from a
stressful; but it was of the utmost importance stage during the second world war when it was
because if one could completely understand valued because of its economic feature in allow¬
and work out one's relationships with the other ing psychiatry to reach a large number of pa¬
group members, there would be an enormous tients, to its present position in the field where
carry over. They would then find pathways to it is clearly seen as having something unique to
more rewarding relationships with significant offer and is often the treatment of choice. Re-

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


suits of psychotherapy outcome studies were groups with a total of 20 control subjects,
cited in which group therapy was shown to be and three groups with a total of 23 experi¬
as efficacious as any mode of individual thera¬ mental subjects. The (»therapists, of course,
py. An outcome project4 at Stanford conducted made the final decision as to whether an as¬
last year was described in which it was shown
that a very high proportion of group therapy
signed patient should begin therapy in his
group. Seventeen (nine experimental and
patients who remained in group therapy for a
year were significantly improved. Our remarks
eight control patients) did not begin group
in this area were focused toward instilling faith therapy: either they were not accepted by
in group therapy and dispelling the false notion the group therapists, or they themselves
of many patients that group therapy is "second elected not to begin.
class therapy," to be used when individual Each pair of (»therapists was supervised
treatment is unavailable. one hour a week by a highly experienced
Lastly, we attempted to enhance the attrac¬ clinician. Each supervisor was assigned two
tiveness of their particular group. Before the groups, an experimental and a control
interview they were asked to hand in two lists group. Everyone involved—therapists, pa¬
of traits: one characteristic of people whom tients, and supervisors—was unaware of the
they liked, and another characteristic of those nature of the research. The residents knew
whom they disliked. During the interview they that some type of research was in progress,
were told that we would place them in a group
which was maximally compatible with them,
because we collected tapes of each meeting
and that we would use their Usta in this task.
and postgroup questionnaires; but they were
This device, although a well-accepted small not aware, even a year later, that there had
group research technique, was the only part of been
experimental manipulation.
an
the group preparatory interview which was de¬ Comparison Measures.—The six groups
ceptive. were studied throughout their first 12 meet¬
The control patients were also seen by the ings. Six different measures
were selected to
same interviewer for 25 minutes; registration
compare the three experimental groups with
data and some historical material were ob¬
the three control groups.
tained. They were then given a brief "orienta¬
tion" to group therapy; however, all the points Hill Interaction Matrix.—Inasmuch as
covered were facts that they would have our major emphasis in orienting patients
learned at their first therapy meeting. For ex¬ was on how they should behave in the group
ample, we told them about the size of the sessions, our main concern was with how
group, the number of therapists, the probable they did, in fact, behave in the groups. The
age range of the group members, the length of Hill Interaction Matrix10 method of scoring
the meetings, and the fees. They were informed interaction in therapy groups was well-suited
that attendance was very important for their to measure the effects of our manipulation.
therapy and, like the experimental group, they While we cannot describe the scoring sys¬
were urged not to drop out but to make a com¬ tem in detail here, a brief description is in
mitment to attend at least a dozen meetings be¬ order to understand the results. Hill's scor¬
fore attempting to evaluate the value of the
ing method consists of a 4 X 4 matrix, which
group to them. Both the experimental and con¬ is shown in Table 1. All statements are
trol patients were informed that a group thera¬
pist would call them within two weeks. scored and placed in one of 16 cells. For
The next step was patient assignment. purposes of clarity we have used a different
The clinic psychiatric social worker was giv¬ lettering system from the one described in
the Hill manual. Hill described a fifth row,
en two patient lists, A (control) and a
(experimental), as well as a list of three Responsive, which is intended for groups of
chronic regressed patients but which is not
pairs of A group therapists and three pairs
group therapists; they were then asked applicable
of to our population.
to assign the patients to the appropriate Hill considers the speculative and con-
groups. The social worker was not aware of frontive statements (rows Y and Z) to be
the identity of the A or condition. He at¬ "work" statements because someone is tak¬
tempted to fill one A or group at a time, ing the role of a patient and actively seeking
striving for an equal male-female, single- self-understanding. He considers the asser¬
married ratio. Finally there were three tive and conventional statements (rows W

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


and X) to be "non-work" because the pa¬ Table 1.—Modified Hill Interaction Matrix
tient role is avoided. 3 4
12
Before analyzing the Hill ratings, the 16 Relation-
types of statements were divided into three Topic Group Personal ship
major categories: (1) directly oriented state¬ W Conventional 1W 2W 3W 4W
ments (cells 2W, 2X, 2Y, 2Z, 4W, 4X, 4Y, X Assertive IX 2X 3X 4X
3Y
4Z) ; (2) indirectly oriented statements Y Speculative
Confrontive
1Y
1Z
2Y
2Z 3Z
4Y
4Z
(cells 3X, 3Y, 3Z) ; and (3) nonoriented The columns refer to types of content:
statements (cells 1W, IX, 1Y, 1Z, 3W). 1. Topic statements are about topics of general in¬
The directly oriented category is the one terest exclusive of the group or its members.
2. Group statements deal with the group as an entity.
most relevant to our preparatory session. If 3. Personal statements are about a group member
this sessionwere effective, then we would independent of his relationships with other members of
the group.
anticipate that the three experimental 4. Relationship statements deal with relationships
between members and between a member and the
groups would score higher than the three group.
control groups in this category, which con¬ The rows refer to levels of work:
W. Conventional statements are socially appropriate
sists of group and relationship statements. for any group. They consist largely of social pleasantries
Since the preparatory session did not spe¬ and discussions of one's problems in a conventional,
socially appropriate manner without putting oneself in
cifically stress different work levels (rows), a patient role.
all levels of group and relationship state¬ X. Assertive statements are argumentative and hos¬
tile. They often involve blaming others -ather than
ments were placed in this category. oneself for problems, and so evade the patient role.
Y. Speculative statements deal with therapeutic issues
The indirectly oriented category consists in a speculative intellectual way.
of all personal statements made by a mem¬ Z. Confrontive statements confront patients or the
group with aspects of their behavior usually avoided,
ber with the exclusion of superficial, conven¬ 3nd with some documentation to allow reality testing.
tional statements; we speculated that the
experimental groups would score higher groups had been oriented when they scored
than the control groups in this category. Al¬ the interaction.
though the preparatory session did not spe¬ Interaction was scored from tape record¬
cifically orient patients to speak on personal ings of the second, fifth, and twelfth meet¬
levels, we reasoned that the decreased un¬ ings of each of the six therapy groups. The
certainty and increased trust in group thera¬ second meeting was chosen because first
py would indirectly permit them to speak meetings of therapy groups are often ritual¬
more freely about personal problems with ized introductions with little opportunity
less need to engage in superficial conven¬ for serious interaction. The twelfth meeting
tionalities. In combining assertive with spec¬ was the last one scored because we had asked
ulative and confrontive levels we differ subjects to stay through 12 meetings before
somewhat from Hill, who would call asser¬ deciding whether to remain or leave their
tive level interaction nonwork because the groups. This procedure, it was hoped, would
patient is avoiding the patient role. How¬ Table 2.—Postgroup Questionnaire
ever, we felt that assertive statements can in¬
dicate emotional involvement and are often 1. I found the meeting today to be
good bad
part of serious therapy. .

2. Compared with how much you would tell a casual


The nonoriented category consisted of all friend, how frankly did you express your feelings about
the remaining statements, mostly superficial other group members?
very frank not frank
interaction often referred to as group flight .

3. Compared with how much you would tell a casual


or resistance. If the preparatory session friend, how much of your private life did you tell the
were effective in enabling the experimental group today?
a great deal none at all
groups to engage in group work more quick¬
.

4. How would you rate your mood right now?


ly, then we would expect them to score low¬ a. angry .
not angry
tense
er in this category. b. relaxed .

c. discouraged . hopeful
Three of us were trained in scoring inter¬ d. involved with others.. .withdrawn from others
action with the Hill Interaction Matrix and 5. The group worked together today
notatali very well
had achieved an 80% agreement with Hill's
.

6. I made progress ¡n achieving my goals in therapy


criterion test prior to scoring these proto¬ today
deal
cols. The three raters did not know which none . a great

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


minimize the number of dropouts during the perimental group scores were combined for
research period. each meeting, as were the scores for the
The middle half-hour of each meeting was three control groups. The percentage of
scored. Two raters listened to each statements occurring in the three statement
recording and peached a consensus on the categories for each of the meetings are
scoring of each statement. A scorable state¬ shown in Fig 1.
ment consisted of one person's continuous The percentage of directly oriented state¬
talk within one of the Hill categories. Thus ments is higher for the experimental than
a long, uninterrupted soliloquy by one pa¬ for the control groups on all three of the
tient entirely in one category would be rated meetings, whereas the percentage of
scored only once. If he were interrupted by nonoriented statements is lower for the ex¬
someone else, even if the interrupter were perimental groups on all three meetings.
also speaking in the same category, the in¬ The indirectly oriented statements show a
terruption itself would be scored, as would more complex pattern, with the oriented
the resumption after it. If talk shifted to an¬ groups higher on meetings two and five, but
other category and then returned, the other lower on meeting 12.
category would be scored, as would resump¬ Since the three-statement categories form
tion of the first category. Since most state¬ a rank order from oriented to indirectly ori¬
ments were terminated by another person's ented to nonoriented, a Mann-Whitney two-
speaking rather than the same speaker shift¬ sample rank test was used to test the
ing to another category, the total number of differences between the experimental and
statements scored in a half-hour period is a control groups for weeks two and twelve.
rough index of the amount of interaction The differences on week five were in the ex¬
during the period. pected direction, but did not reach statisti¬
Each rater scored an equal number of ori¬ cal significance. The lack of significant dif¬
ented and nonoriented groups for each ferences on week five raised the possibility
meeting studied. that the effect of the orientation was curvi¬
Postgroup Questionnaires.—At the end of linear over time: high at the beginning and
every therapy meeting the patients filled out later in the group, but overshadowed by oth¬
a questionnaire consisting of nine items, er factors around the fifth meeting. It is also
each answered on a 7-point scale; this ques¬ possible that the lack of significant results
tionnaire is reproduced in Table 2. was due to sampling error.
Cohesiveness Questionnaire.—A ten-ques¬ In order to investigate the alternatives
tion cohesiveness questionnaire, described further, we scored the sixth meetings of all
elsewhere,11 was filled out by the patients groups. (Raters were, again, not aware of
at the end of the fourth, eighth, and twelfth which were the experimental and which the
meetings. control groups.) The results are shown in
Faith in Group Therapy Questionnaire.—- Fig 2 and Table 4. In meeting six the experi¬
Before therapy (immediately after the mental groups had more directly and indi¬
group preparatory or control interview), rectly oriented statements and fewer non¬
and once again after the twelfth meeting, oriented statements than did the control
the patients answered two questions: (1) groups. These differences are in the same
What percent of people who start group direction as in meeting five and are now sta¬
therapy do you think are helped by group tistically significant using the Mann-Whit¬
therapy? (2) How long do you think it takes ney rank test. This indicates that the lack of
the average person to get definite benefit significant differences between experimental
from group therapy? and control groups in the fifth meeting were
Attendance and dropout records were not indicative of a curvilinear effect of the
kept. preparatory session over time and may have
Clinical data were available on each been due to sampling error.
group from tapes and from a summary of In order to test in more detail how the
each meeting dictated by the therapists. three statement categories differed between
Results the oriented and nonoriented groups, the
Hill Interaction Matrix.—The three ex- 2X3 contingency tables in Tables 3 and 4

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


were each partitioned into three 2 2 2 ta¬ Week 1
bles. The results, shown in Table 5, tell us Il Experimental qroups
60
more about the differences over time be¬ 1 I Control groups
tween the oriented and nonoriented groups.
All three 2 are significant for week two, 40
indicating that the experimental groups ex¬
ceed the control groups to a greater degree
20
on oriented than on nonoriented statements
and have a greater proportion of both direct¬
ly and indirectly oriented statements than of § O
nonoriented statements. This is exactly Week 5
what we would have expected from our ma¬ 60
nipulation: its strongest effects were shown
-

on directly oriented, and the next strongest


on indirectly oriented statements. £40
O
+-
Week five has no statistically significant
differences among the three statement cate¬ O 20
gories, whereas week six again shows a sig¬ c
nificantly greater proportion of both directly
and indirectly oriented statements than of g 0
Week \Z
nonoriented statements for the experimental 60
groups. However, counter to our expecta¬
tions that the largest difference between ex¬
perimental and control groups would be 40
with oriented statements, the experimental
groups exceeded the control groups to a 20
greater degree on indirectly oriented than on
oriented statements.
Week twelve is similar to week six, except Indirectly Non¬
Oriented
that this time the difference in proportion of statements oriented oriented
statements statements
indirect vs nonoriented statements is not
significant for the two types of groups. Fig 1.—Percent of oriented, indirectly oriented, and
nonoriented statements made in weeks two, five, and
The most consistent finding over the twelve.
weeks is that, for the oriented groups, the
proportion of directly oriented statements is
significantly greater than the proportion of we would expect five of them to exceed this
nonoriented statements. The results with probability level. We therefore must con¬

the indirectly oriented statements are more clude that these two differences may have
equivocal and, since they do not follow a been due to chance.
consistent pattern, are difficult to interpret.Cohesiveness Questionnaire.—Cohesive¬
Postgroup Questionnaires.—The experi¬ ness scores of the experimental and control
mental and control groups were compared groups were compared for the fourth, eighth,
on each of the nine postgroup questionnaire and twelfth meetings. No statistically sig¬
items for each of the first 12 meetings. Out nificant differences were found between the
of these 108 comparisons, two had probabili¬ two groups on any of these meetings.
ty levels of less than 0.05: at meeting ten, Faith in Group Therapy.—Immediately
members of oriented groups rated their mood following the orientation session, (prior to
as more tense than did members of non¬ therapy) and again at the twelfth meeting,
oriented groups, and at meeting five, mem¬ all patients were asked to estimate the per¬
bers of oriented groups rated themselves as centage of people helped by group therapy
making more progress toward their goals and the amount of time required for benefit.
than did members of nonoriented groups. The Mann-Whitney U-test was used as a
However, if this number of comparisons test of statistical significance. There were no
were to be made from a random population, statistically significant differences between

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


Table 3.—Statements in the Three Categories dropouts often presents a formidable ob¬
tor Experimental and Control Groups on stacle in group therapy research. The tech¬
Weeks 2, 5, and 12
nique of urging patients to remain in therapy
Statements for a minimum set period may prove useful
Non¬
to other workers.
Groups Oriented Indirect oriented Week
Comment
Experimental 67 104 124 2*
Control 17 53 118
Experimental 37 84 143
5
Hypothesis 1 (greater faith in group
Control 33 85
55
173
41
therapy among experimental subjects) re¬
Experimental 118 12* ceived some support from our findings.
Control 62 96 109
*
P<0.05, Mann-Whitney two-sample rank test cor¬ Hypothesis 2 (greater cohesiveness
rected for ties. among the experimental subjects) was not
the experimental and control groups prior to
supported by our findings.
Hypothesis 3 (greater here-and-now inter¬
therapy. At the twelfth meeting, however, personal interaction among experimental
the experimental patients estimated signifi¬
subjects) was strongly supported by our
cantly shorter times required for benefit findings.
from group therapy (P < 0.05). They also To interpret these findings let us review
estimated that a larger percentage of pa¬ those aspects of the preparatory session rele¬
tients was helped by group therapy than did
vant to each hypothesis.
the control group, but this difference did not
reach statistical significance (P < 0.10). Hypothesis 1.—In the preparatory session
we attempted to enhance the patients' faith
Attendance and Dropout Records.—The in group therapy by citing encouraging out¬
experimental and control groups did not come studies, by describing the unique fea¬
differ significantly in the number of drop¬
tures of group therapy, and, no doubt, by
outs. At the end of ten months, ten experi¬
mental and ten control patients had dropped transmitting the interviewer's enthusiasm
for the modality. There is considerable re¬
out from group therapy. The mean stay of search evidence that positive structuring of
the experimental patient was longer (18.2 the patients' initial expectations beneficially
meetings) than for the controls (11.7 meet¬ influences the course and outcome of indi¬
ings). This difference was not, however, sig¬ vidual therapy,12·13 and our results sug¬
nificant. The early attendance pattern is in gest that a similar process may occur in
itself quite interesting. All 43 patients were
strongly urged to remain in therapy for at
least 12 weeks, and only 14% of the group Table 4.—Statements in the Three Categories
for Experimental and Control Groups on Week 6*
members dropped out during the first 12
meetings—less than half the number of Statements

dropouts observed in the same population in Groups Oriented Indirect Nonoriented


previous studies.3 The high rate of early Experimental 139 153 92
Control 83 55 123
c *
P<0.05 Mann-Whitney two-sample rank test cor¬

Week 6 rected for ties.


§60
+-
O
I 1
Experimental groups
I I Control groups
. - Table 5.—x~ Values tor Partitioned Tables
(
Comparing Experimental and Control Groups
40
+- Week
o
+-
Statements 2 5 6 12
%20 Oriented vs indirect 4.85* 0.20 5.91* 27.93:
Oriented vs non¬
0L oriented 20.91t 1.33 17.22; 47.88t
Oriented Indirectly Non¬ Indirect vs non¬
statements oriented oriented oriented 8.75f 0.88 41.42 í 2.86
statements statements
* P<0.05.
Fig 2.—Percent of oriented, indirectly oriented, and t P<0.01.
nonoriented statements made in week six. î P<0.001.

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


group therapy. We tested for this effect by crease anxiety. In addition, we attempted to
asking the patients (pretherapy, and again clarify the goals of therapy.
at the twelfth meeting) to estimate the per¬ One important source of perplexity and
centage of patients helped by group therapy discouragement for patients in the initial
and the duration of time required. At the stages of group therapy is a perceived goal
twelfth meeting the experimental groups incompatibility. They will often be unable
tended to be more optimistic than the con¬ to discern the connection between group
trol groups. goals (group integrity, construction of an
Hypothesis 2.—In the preparatory inter¬ atmosphere of trust, and an interactional
view we also attempted to increase the mem¬ confrontive focus) and the individual goals
bers' satisfaction with their groups by telling of the patient (relief of suffering). Part of
them that, on the basis of their question¬ the task of the preparatory interview was to
naire data, they would be placed in a maxi¬ disconfirm the apparent incompatibility,
mally compatible group. However, the cohe¬ and to clarify how these goals are, in fact,
siveness questionnaire scores (obtained at confluent. The preparatory interview, fur¬
fourth, eighth, and twelfth meetings) of the thermore, attempted to clarify expected role
experimental groups were not significantly behavior. The patients were presented with
higher than those of the control groups. This unambiguous guidelines for their behavior
procedure, one which has traditionally been in the group. Types of efficacious and self-
successful in laboratory research, may have defeating behavior were concretely described,
failed in this research for several reasons. and the rationale behind our value judge¬
First, the emphasis on cohesiveness in the ments was explained. The ambiguity sur¬
preparatory interview was very weak, con¬ rounding the role of the therapist was also
sisting of only a single sentence. Secondly, lessened by outlining his goals and purpose
the cohesiveness questionnaire, adapted in the group.
from laboratory small-group research, may The importance of clarity of goals and
have overemphasized being comfortable in role expectations in effective group function¬
the group, and the more interactive task-ori¬ ing has been demonstrated by an abundance
ented experimental groups may well have of small group research, recently re¬
been less comfortable for the members, viewed.15 In laboratory groups, increased
though more cohesive or binding. It is inter¬ clarity of goals and increased explication of
esting to note that two of the control groups the methods of goal attainment result in
fragmented shortly after the end of the for¬ greater member attraction to the group,
mal study, losing three members each be¬ increased sympathy for group emotions,
tween the 13th and 16th meetings. The im¬ decreased intermember hostility, increased
pact and stress of the early meetings of the intermember influencability, increased moti¬
experimental groups may well have neutral¬ vation, increased member security, in¬
ized the weak initial favorable expectation creased efficiency, and decreased member
we implanted. In fact, Goldstein14 cites evi¬ frustration.1619 Ambiguous group-member
dence that individuals who are exposed to role expectations reduce group satisfaction
more stress than anticipated may react with and group productivity and increase mem¬
hostility at the disconfirmation of their ex¬ ber defensiveness. Many psychotherapy and
pectations. group dynamic studies have demonstrated
Hypothesis 3.—The third goal and major that the more discrepant the member's ex¬
emphasis of the preparatory interview was pectancies of the group leader's role, the
to increase the development of interpersonal "less the attraction to the group, the less the
interaction in the group. Our method was to satisfaction of group members, and the more
provide a cognitive structure for the patient. the strain or negative affect between leader
We clarified the rationale of group therapy and led or therapist and patient."20
by briefly explaining the interpersonal theo¬ The results of our study indicate that pa¬
ry of psychiatry and the advantages of an tients systematically prepared for interac¬
interactional focus in the group. Through tional group therapy, will engage themselves
this explanation, and through predictions of more quickly in the therapeutic task than
anticipated obstacles, we attempted to de- patients not so prepared. Considerable evi-

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


dence, cited above, suggests that increased guity, absence of cognitive anchoring, and
clarity of goals and paths to attain these frustration of conscious and unconscious
goals, as well as increased clarity of role ex¬ wishes all facilitate a regressive reaction to
pectations, result in conditions more condu¬ the therapist and help create an atmosphere
cive to effective therapy. Why, then, are pa¬ favorable for the development of transfer¬
tients not routinely prepared for group ence toward the therapist and parataxic dis¬
therapy in a systematic manner? In fact, tortions toward other group members. These
all group therapists do attempt to clarify therapists wish to encourage regressive phe¬
the therapeutic process and expected role be¬ nomena and the emergence of unconscious
havior, and the difference between therapists impulses so they may be identified and
or between therapeutic schools is largely worked through in therapy.29
a difference in timing and style of prepara¬ Other therapists, who deemphasize the
tion. Some group therapists initially pre¬ centrality of the patient-therapist relation¬
pare the new patient by providing him with ship in group therapy and look to total
written material about group therapy,21 or group forces as the therapeutic agent, also
by having him hear a tape of a model group argue against the initial clarification of
therapy work meeting (as noted by B. Ber- group goals and process. If, as Bion states,30
zon in an oral communication, May 1965), the chief task of the group is to analyze its
or by having him attend a trial meeting,22 own tensions, then the anxiety stemming
or by a long series of individual intro¬ from initial uncertainty surrounding the ex¬
ductory lectures or an instrumented pro¬ pected group process forms the initial group
gram of therapy and insight aids.23·24 task. Others31 point out that the initial anx¬
However, even the therapists who deliber¬ iety encourages the group toward an early
ately abrogate initial preparation and orien¬ problem-solving culture and eventual group
tation of the patient nevertheless have in autonomy; one cannot, it is argued, establish
mind goals and preferred modes of group group solutions by edict. Slavson insists that
procedure which eventually are transmitted initial anxiety is a desirable feature, since it
to the patient. By subtle, or even subliminal, delays the development of group cohesive¬
verbal and nonverbal reinforcement, even ness.32 The group thus does not develop a
the most nondirective therapist structures degree of comfort incompatible with thera¬
his group so that inevitably it adopts his val¬ peutic work.
ue system of high and low priority content An authoritative discussion of these issues
and process.25·26 This was illustrated by is difficult because of the dearth of relevant
the fact that even the control groups consist¬ sound research. The identification and the
ently increased in the percentage of oriented rank ordering of the curative factors in
statements throughout the 12 meetings. (Fig
group therapy is entirely problematic. Some
1 and 2).
pertinent research studies4·33·84 suggest that
If we accept the research evidence that the important curative forces are total
the group therapist's goals and preferred
group and interactive in nature; for exam¬
procedural paths to goal attainment are ulti¬ ple, group support, group cohesiveness, and
mately transmitted to the group, why, then, popularity in the group. Nevertheless, there
is an initial systematic preparation of pa¬
are many different group therapies, and it is
tients uncommon in group therapy prac¬
tice? possible that the relative importance of the
curative forces varies depending on the ther¬
Some therapists hold that ambiguity of
both patient and therapist role expectation apist's goals and techniques. Thus there
is a desirable condition of the early phases may exist well thought out and appropriate
reasons for deliberate perpetuation of initial
of therapy.27·28 If one accepts the premise
that the development and eventual resolu¬ unclarity. However, if interpersonal interac¬
tion of patient-therapist transference distor¬ tion is considered a desired condition in
tions is a key curative factor in therapy, group therapy, and we contend that it is in
then it follows that one should seek, during the great majority of present-day group
the early stages of therapy, to enhance the therapy, then our research demonstrates
development of transference. Enigma, ambi- that a systematic preparatory interview will

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


facilitate the appearance of interaction in ment for our patients. For learning to occur,
therapy groups. and for positive reinforcement of adaptive
A systematic preparation for group thera¬ interpersonal behavior to transpire, the
py by no means implies a structuring of the patient must feel that he is effectively in¬
group experience. We do not espouse didac¬ vestigating and producing a purposeful, ap¬
tic or directive group therapy, but, on the propriate effect on his environment. If, as
contrary, suggest a technique which will en¬ research evidence holds, anxiety is the enemy
hance the formation of a freely interacting of environmental exploration,37 then a reduc¬
autonomous group. By averting lengthy rit¬ tion of the extrinsic anxiety of uncertainty
ualistic behavior in the initial sessions, and should enhance the formation of an atmos¬
by diminishing initial anxiety stemming phere conducive to interpersonal explora¬
from unclarity, the group is enabled to tion. If patients are unclear about the long-
plunge into work more quickly. In our view, range goals of the group and the stepping
anxiety caused by deliberate unclarity is not stones to those goals, then the possibility
necessary to prevent the groups from becom¬ exists that constructive behavior will not
ing too socially comfortable. Our patients be recognized as such by the patient. Poten¬
are conflicted in their interpersonal relation¬ tially adaptive exploration may not be grati¬
ships and any groups—-for example our fying or reinforcing for the patient who sus¬
three experimental groups which have an pects that this behavior is irrelevant, or even
increased rate of interpersonal interaction— counter, to therapeutic objectives.
will continually present challenging and In conclusion, there is considerable evi¬
anxiety-fraught interpersonal confronta¬ dence from a number of sources that exces¬
tions. Therapy groups which are too com¬ sive initial anxiety, frustration, and unclari¬
fortable are groups which engage in flight ty may inhibit learning and be disconsonant
from the task of direct interpersonal con¬ with successful psychotherapy. In interac¬
frontation. tional group therapy, in which the thera¬
We would suggest that anxiety stemming peutic process is considered to be mediated
from unclarity of the group task, process, through free interpersonal interaction and
and role expectations in the early meetings the therapeutic relationship is conceptual¬
of the therapy group may, in fact, be a de¬ ized as a therapeutic alliance, we demon¬
terrent to effective therapy. Considerable strated the feasibility and efficacy of a sys¬
evidence exists that, although anxiety with tematic preparation for therapy. Through a
accompanying hypervigilance may be adap¬ pretherapy explication of the group process
tive, excessive degrees of anxiety will ob¬ and expected role behavior the therapy
struct coping with stress.35·36 In his review group is more quickly able to engage in the
of evidence supporting the concept of an ex¬ therapeutic task.
ploratory drive, White37 notes that anxiety Summary
and fear retard learning and result in de¬ In controlled investigation the effects of
a
creased exploratory behavior to an extent a pretherapy preparatory session on the ear¬
correlated with the intensity of the fear. It ly course of group therapy are studied. The
has been postulated that man has a primary findings indicate that the preparatory session
drive to explore and master his environ¬ increases the development of interpersonal
ment, and that there is an intrinsic pleasure interaction, ie, the discussion of intermember
and positive reinforcing value in the investi¬ relationships in the group. In addition, there
gation of environment, the experiencing of was some evidence that patients' faith in
competence, and the production of an effect group therapy was strengthened by the pre¬
on the environment.38 The concept that paratory session. There was no effect of the
learning is facilitated by frustration has experimental procedure on the patients' at¬
been challenged by many psychotherapists traction to their particular groups.
who conceptualize the patient-doctor rela¬ Implications of these findings for the
tionship as a therapeutic alliance. Kardi- theory and practice of group therapy were
ner39 states that it is the successful and discussed. A preparatory interview clarify¬
gratifying experiences, not the frustrations, ing group process and role expectations can
that will lead to integration. enhance the efficacy of interactional group
The early group therapy experience is a therapy by hastening the appearance of
strange, usually threatening social environ- effective levels of group communication.

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015


This research was partially financed by a Stan¬ 18. Cohen, A.R.; Stotland, E.; and Wolfe, D.M.:
ford University general research support grant, and An Experimental Investigation of Need for Cogni-
by NIMH grant No. MH 8304. tion, J Abnorm Soc Psychol 51:291-294, 1955.
William F. Hill, PhD, and Donald Staight, MSW, 19. Cohen, A.R.: "Situational Structure, Self-
assisted in this research.
Esteem and Threat-Oriented Reactions to Power,"
in Cartwright, D. (ed) : Studies in Social Power, Ann
References Arbor, Mich: Research Center for Group Dynamics,
1959, pp 35-52.
1. Bales, R.F.: "The Equilibrium Problem in 20. Goldstein, A.P., et al: Psychotherapy and the
Small Groups," in Parsons, T.; Bales, R.F.; and Psychology of Behavior Change, New York: John
Shils, E. (eds): Working Papers in the Theory of Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1966, p 405.
Action, Glencoe, Ill: Free Press, 1953. 21. Martin, H., and Shewmaker, K.: Written In-
2. Jacobs, R.C., and Campbell, D.T.: The Perpet- structions in Group Therapy, Group Psychother
uation of an Arbitrary Tradition Through Several 15:24, 1962.
Generations of a Laboratory Microculture, J Ab- 22. Bach, G.R.: Intensive Group Psychotherapy,
norm Soc Psychol 62:649-658, 1961. New York: Ronald Press, 1954.
3. Yalom, I.D.: A Study of Group Therapy Drop- 23. Malamud, D.I., and Machover, S.: Toward
outs, Arch Gen Psychiat 14:393-414, 1966. Self-Understanding: Group Techniques in Self Con-
4. Yalom, I.D., et al: Prediction of Success in frontation, Springfield, Ill: Charles C Thomas Pub-
Group Therapy, Arch Gen Psychiat 17:159-168, 1967. lishers, 1965.
5. Hoehn-Saric, R., et al: Systematic Preparation 24. Bettis, M.D.; Malamud, D.I.; and Malamud,
of Patients for Psychotherapy, J Psychiat Res R.F.: Deepening a Group's Insight Into Human Re-
2:267-281, 1964. lations, J Clin Psychol 5:114-122, 1949.
6. Bandler, B.: Health Oriented Psychotherapy, 25. Murray, E.J.: A Content Analysis for Study
Psychosom Med 21:176-181, 1959. in Psychotherapy, Psychol Monogr 70 No. 13, 1956.
7. Berzon, B.: The Self Directed Therapeutic 26. Goldstein, A.P., et al: Psychotherapy and the
Group, Int J Group Psychother 14:366-369, 1964. Psychology of Behavior Change, New York: John
8. Goodman, G.: "Companionship as Therapy: Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1966, pp 422-423.
The Use of Non-Professional Talent," in Hart, J.T., 27. Horwitz, L.: Transference in Training Groups
and Tomlinson, T.M. (eds): New Directions in and Therapy Groups, Int J Group Psychother
Client-Centered Psychotherapy, New York: Hough- 14:202-213, 1964.
ton-Mifflin Co., to be published. 28. Wolf, A.: "The Psychoanalysis of Groups," in
9. Bradford, L.P.; Gibb, J.R.; and Benne, K.D.: Rosenbaum, M., and Berger, M. (eds): Group Psy-
T-Group Theory and Laboratory Method, New chotherapy and Group Function, New York: Basic
York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1964. Books, Inc., 1963, pp 273-328.
10. Hill, W.F.: HIM: Hill Interaction Matrix, Los 29. Schiedlinger, S.: The Concept of Repression in
Angeles: Youth Study Center, University of South- Group Psychotherapy, read before American Group
ern California, 1965. Psychotherapy 24th annual conference, New York,
11. Yalom, I.D., and Rank, K.: Compatibility and January, 1967.
Cohesiveness in Therapy Groups, Arch Gen Psy- 30. Bion, W.R.: Experiences in Groups, New
chiat 15:267-276, 1966. York: Basic Books, Inc., 1959.
12. Goldstein, A.D., and Shipmen, W.G.: Patient 31. Whitaker, P.S., and Lieberman, M.A.: Pscyho-
Expectancies, Symptom Reduction and Aspects of therapy Through the Group Process, New York:
the Initial Psychotherapeutic Interview, J Clin Psy-
Atherton Press, 1964, p 208.
chol 17:129-133, 1961. 32. Slavson, S.: A Textbook in Analytic Group
13. Frank, J.: The Dynamics of the Psychothera-
peutic Relationship, Psychiatry 22:17-39, 1959. Psychotherapy, New York: International Universi-
ties Press, 1964.
14. Goldstein, A.P.; Heller, K.; and Secrest, L.B.:
33. Dickoff, H., and Lakin, M.: Patients' Views of
Psychotherapy and the Psychology of Behavior
Change, New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., Group Psychotherapy: Retrospections and Interpre-
1966, p 405. tations, Int J Group Psychother 13:61-73, 1963.
15. Goldstein, A.P.; Heller, K.: and Secrest, L.B.: 34. Kapp, F.T., et al: Group Participation and
Psychotherapy and the Psychology of Behavior Self-Perceived Personality Change, J Nerv Ment
Change, New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., Dis 139:255-265, 1964.
1966, pp 376-377. 35. Janis, I.L.: Psychological Stress: Psychoana-
16. Rauer, B.H., and Reitsema, J.: The Effects of lytic and Behavioral Studies of Surgical Patients,
Varied Clarity of Group Goal and Group Path Upon New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1958.
the Individual and His Relation to His Group, Hu- 36. Basowitz, H., et al: Anxiety and Stress, New
man Relations 10:29-45, 1957. York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., Inc., 1955.
17. Wolfe, D.M.; Snock, J.D.; and Rosenthal, 37. White, R.W.: Motivation Reconsidered: The
R.A.: Report to Company Participants at 1960 Uni- Concept of Competence, Psychol Rev 66:297-333,
versity of Michigan Research Project, Ann Arbor, 1959.
Mich: Institute of Social Research, 1961, in Gold- 38. Hendrick, I.: Instinct and the Ego During In-
stein et al: Psychotherapy and the Psychology of fancy, Psychoanal Quart 11:33-58, 1942.
Behavior Change, New York: John Wiley and Sons, 39. Kardiner, A., and Spiegel, H.: War Stress and
Inc., 1966, p 391. Neurotic Illness, New York: Hoeben Co., 1947.

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Michigan User on 06/14/2015

You might also like