You are on page 1of 11

The Gerontologist

cite as: Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2, S149–S159


doi:10.1093/geront/gnx071

Research Article

Couples’ Shared Beliefs About Aging and Implications for


Future Functional Limitations

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


Shannon T. Mejía, PhD1,* and Richard Gonzalez, PhD2
Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign. 2Institute for Social Research,
1

University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

*Address correspondence to: Shannon T. Mejía, PhD, Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign,
110 Huff Hall, 1206 South Fourth Street, Champaign, IL 61820. E-mail: shannon.t.mejia@gmail.com

Received September 12, 2016; Editorial Decision Date April 21, 2017

Decision Editor: Catherine W. Gillespie, PhD

Abstract
Purpose of the Study:  Individual beliefs are known to be predictive of health. This study examines the co-construction of
couple norms and links couples’ shared beliefs about aging to future individuals’ and couples’ functional limitations.
Design and Methods: Data from the 2008 and 2014 waves of the Health and Retirement Study (1,231 couples; age
range = 51–90) were analyzed using latent variables that estimated shared and individual variance in beliefs about aging in
2008 and functional limitations at follow-up in 2014. Spouses’ individual processes of physical activity and disease burden
were modeled to contribute to couples’ shared beliefs about aging and subsequent functional limitations. Models progres-
sively controlled for indicators of partner selection, couples’ shared health experiences, and similarities and differences in
age.
Results:  Couples’ beliefs about aging predicted future functional limitations. The effect magnitude decreased but remained
significant in all models. Physical activity predicted couples’ future functional limitations but was largely explained by
shared health experiences and similarities and differences in age for wives and husbands, respectively. Disease burden con-
tributed to couples’ shared beliefs about aging. Husbands’ contributions were explained by partner selection, but wives’
contributions remained significant in all models. The effect of couples’ shared beliefs on change in couples’ functional limi-
tations was explained by couples’ shared health experiences.
Implications:  Beliefs about aging and health occur within the context of close relationships and shared experiences.
Knowledge of couples’ beliefs and health is necessary to support their individual and collective efforts to age successfully
together.
Keywords:  Self-perceptions of aging, Dyadic analysis, Common fate model, Health, Longitudinal analysis

Beliefs about one’s own aging are known to have signifi- and well-being (Meyler, Stimpson, & Peek, 2007). Their
cant consequences for future experiences of aging, includ- health behaviors, perceived relationship quality, coping
ing health, health behaviors, and longevity (Westerhof behaviors, and goals have been shown to influence their
et al., 2014). Although aging is not experienced in isolation, partners’ experiences (see Hoppmann & Gerstorf, 2009).
but rather in concert with close others (Antonucci, Fiori, In addition to directly influencing each other, spouses may
Birditt, & Jackey, 2010; Settersten, Richard, & Hagestad, also co-create shared norms—through shared experiences
2015), little is known about how couples co-construct and collective action—that is greater than the sum of its
common beliefs about aging over their extended shared his- parts and exerts independent influence on individual out-
tories. Spouses are known to be connected in their health comes (Gonzalez & Griffin, 2002). In this study, we use

© The Author 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. S149
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
S150 The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2

older couples’ shared beliefs about aging to predict future


individuals’ and couples’ functional limitations and exam-
ine how individual health and health behaviors contribute
to these shared processes.

Self-perceptions of Aging and Functional


Limitations
Subjective experiences of aging are multidimensional
(Brothers, Miche, Wahl, & Diehl, 2015) and develop
through experiences within societies (Barrett & Montepare,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


2015), personal experiences of aging (Hubley & Russell,
2009), and interactions with others (Schafer & Shippee,
2010). Beliefs about aging encompass attitudes toward
one’s own aging, are largely driven by age stereotypes and
personal experiences of aging (Diehl et al., 2014), and con-
sistently predict markers of successful aging such as func-
tional ability and longevity (Westerhof et al., 2014). Beliefs
about aging affect future health through cognitive and
behavioral pathways (Levy, 2009). As a cognitive frame,
beliefs about aging shape how individuals experience age-
related change in their bodies, relationships, and roles
(Stephan, Chalabaev, Kotter-Grühn, & Jaconelli, 2013;
Figure 1.  Common fate model of couples’ self-perceptions of aging on
Stewart, Chipperfield, Perry, & Weiner, 2012). In addition,
future functional limitations with contribution of individual processes
beliefs about aging motivate health behaviors and have on shared processes. Note: Loadings within constructs are constrained
been prospectively linked to physical activity and preventa- to 1.  Variances within constructs across gender are constrained to
tive care (Kim, Moored, Giasson, & Smith, 2014). Taken equal. Individual variances (i) were allowed to covary within gender
together, beliefs about aging form self-fulfilling prophecies and were tested for equality/inequality across gender.
for future experiences of health and well-being in old age
(Levy, 2009) and are predictive of individuals’ future health predict future functional limitations. We selected physi-
(Sargent-Cox, Anstey, & Luszcz, 2014). cal activity and disease burden because of their relevance
How individuals understand their own aging is inter- for subjective experiences of aging (Hubley & Russell,
subjective—the meaning of age and the experience of 2009), relationship processes (Rook, 2015), and health
aging is constructed through interactions in society and policy and interventions (Martire, Schulz, Helgeson,
with others (Settersten et  al., 2015). Married couples are Small, & Saghafi, 2010). Spouses establish norms that
intimate family units of unrelated individuals who elected guide how they support one another in managing chronic
each other to navigate a shared life course. Many couples illness, as well as encourage each other to engage in posi-
in older adulthood share extended life histories and have tive health behaviors (Tucker & Mueller, 2000). Evidence
traveled together through age-graded roles and responsi- also suggests that one spouse’s physical activity and dis-
bilities. Within their tightly interwoven lives, a spouse’s ease burden has implications for both spouses’ health
beliefs have implications for the partner’s experience of and well-being. For example, one spouse’s engagement
aging (Drewelies, Chopik, Hoppmann, Smith, & Gerstorf, in physical activity could influence the partner’s activ-
2016). Similarly, one spouse’s experiences of health and ill- ity engagement (Franks, Wendorf, Gonzalez, & Ketterer,
ness are known to shape relationship processes (Martire 2004; Li, Cardinal, & Acock, 2013) and potentially shape
et  al., 2006). This study examines how individual beliefs beliefs about aging (Levy, 2009). Similarly, partners are
about aging, health behaviors, and health experiences con- intimately involved in the disease process (Franks, Lucas,
tribute to the co-construction of norms that are unique to Stephens, Rook, & Gonzalez, 2010; Monin et al., 2010).
the couple and exert an independent and unique influence Managing chronic conditions can increase caregiving bur-
on health outcomes (see Figure 1). den and limit opportunities for both partners (Polenick,
Martire, Hemphill, & Stephens, 2015), to the detriment
of couples’ beliefs about aging and functional limitations.
Couples’ Co-construction of Shared Beliefs Experiences of aging are also gendered (Calasanti, 2010;
Individual beliefs about aging are constructed in part Kornadt, Voss, & Rothermund, 2013). Husbands and
through experiences of health. In this study, we examine wives may therefore differ in how their individual experi-
how physical activity and disease burden—two individual ences contribute to couples’ shared norms of health and
processes—shape couples’ shared beliefs about aging and shared beliefs about aging.
The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2 S151

Couple-level processes stem from partner selection and Beginning in 2006, 50% of households were randomly
shared experiences, which have implications for how inter- assigned to an in-person interview at their place of resi-
ventions and policy can support couples in constructing pos- dence and the other 50% was interviewed by telephone.
itive shared beliefs about aging. Partners select one another Interviews included questions about functional limitations,
across the axes of age, education, and race (Schwartz, health, and demographics. Following the in-person inter-
2013), which distribute opportunities and exert constrains view, respondents were left a paper self-administered psy-
on the couple unit and guide how couples move together chosocial questionnaire (SAQ) to complete and return by
through their shared life course. Through their shared expe- mail. Starting in 2008, the baseline for this study, the SAQ
riences, couples co-construct norms of support, companion- included questions on beliefs about aging.
ship, and social control (Rook, 2015), which shape couple
processes and amplify individual experiences (Gonzalez &

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


Griffin, 2002). In this study, we focus on couples’ shared Participants
experiences of health and health behaviors because health In the 2008 wave, 1,992 married couples (3,984 indi-
involves both spouses and health behaviors are formed in viduals), where both spouses were age eligible (M = 68,
response to experiences within the couples’ shared environ- SD  =  9.01, range  =  51–95), community-residing, and
ment (Franks et al., 2004; Li et al., 2013; Tucker & Mueller, non-proxy respondents, were assigned to the in-person
2000). In addition to guiding partner selection, age indicates interview. Among eligible couples, 79% identified as
the experience of historical events, biological and psycho- White/Caucasian, 9% identified as African American,
logical aging processes, and transitions through age-graded 12% identified as Hispanic/other, and 29% had com-
roles and responsibilities (Settersten et al., 2015). Similarities pleted at least some college. From this sample of eligi-
and differences in age within couples therefore have impli- ble couples, there were 1,883 couples (86%) where both
cations for how couples’ shared beliefs are shaped by indi- (n = 1,723) or at least one (n = 160) spouse completed
vidual processes and predict future health. and returned the SAQ. Logistic regression showed those
who completed the SAQ tended to be older and identify
as White.
Present Study
Together, extant research on the interdependence of
health and well-being within couples offers a compel-
Measures
ling foundation to examine older couples’ shared beliefs The measures of individual and shared processes follow.
about aging. Our study has three aims. Our first aim is Descriptive statistics are provided for the analytic sample
to quantify the extent to which beliefs about aging are (1,231 couples).
shared within couples and linked to individuals’ and cou-
ples’ future functional limitations. Our second aim is to Shared Processes
examine how individual processes contribute to shared Beliefs about aging and functional limitations were mod-
processes of couples’ beliefs about aging and functional eled as the couples’ shared processes and were adjusted for
limitations. We test the contributions of spouses’ indi- variance at the individual level.
vidual physical activity and disease burden to couples’
beliefs about aging and subsequent functional limitations. Beliefs About  Aging. Respondents’ beliefs about aging
Our third aim is to examine the extent to which indica- were measured at the 2008 wave using the Philadelphia
tors of partner selection, shared health experiences, and Geriatric Center Morale subscale on attitudes toward on
similarities and differences in age within couples account aging (Lawton, 1975). The subscale included three posi-
for these associations. tive (e.g., I have as much as pep as I  did last year) and
two negative (e.g., Things keep getting worse as I get older)
statements about one’s own aging. Respondents rated each
Methods statement from (1) strongly disagree to (6) strongly agree.
The current study examines couples’ shared beliefs about The beliefs score was constructed for each respondent by
aging and future functional limitations using data from reverse coding the negative items and then calculating the
the 2008 and 2014 waves of the Health and Retirement mean (M = 4.12, SD = 1.08). The scale showed good inter-
Study (HRS). The HRS is a National Institute of Aging nal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .72).
(NIA U01AG009740) sponsored biennial panel study that
is conducted by the University of Michigan and nationally Functional Limitations. Respondents’ reports of difficulty
representative of Americans aged 51 and older (Sonnega completing basic physical activities were measured in the
et  al., 2014). Sampling is conducted using a multistage 2008 and 2014 waves (Fonda & Herzog, 2004). Activities
probability design at the household level. If the primary (walking several blocks, jogging 1 mile, walking 1 block,
respondent is coupled, both partners are interviewed and sitting for 2  hr, getting up from a chair, climbing stairs,
brought into the HRS panel. climbing 1 flight of stairs, stooping, reaching arms above
S152 The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2

shoulders, pushing and pulling large objects, lifting weights, Loss to Follow-up
and picking up a dime) that respondents reported having In total, 562 (14%) respondents died before the 2014 fol-
difficulty completing due to health problems were summed low-up. Logistic regression found couples’ and individu-
(M = 2.59, SD = 2.71, range = 0–10). als’ functional limitations, age, disease burden, frequency
of physical activity, and smoking to predict vital status at
Individual Processes follow-up. In addition, 53 couples (106 individuals) were
Physical activity and disease burden were adjusted for alive but formed separate households between 2008 and
interdependence within couples and examined separately 2014, and 239 individuals were alive but missed the 2014
as individual processes that contribute to couples’ shared interview. The likelihood of exclusion from analysis due
beliefs about aging and future functional limitations. to the couples’ separation or missed interview did not sys-
tematically vary across study covariates. Across all causes

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


Physical Activity. The frequency of engagement in moder- of loss to follow-up, those who attritted tended to have
ate activity was measured in the 2008 wave. Respondents more negative couples’ (but not individual) beliefs about
reported their frequency of doing moderately energetic aging as well as greater functional limitations at the cou-
activities such as gardening, cleaning the car, or walking. ple level and fewer functional limitations relative to their
Responses on the single-item scale ranged from (4) “hardly couple norm (see Supplementary Table  1). The final ana-
ever or never” to (1) “more than once a week” and were lytic sample included 1,231 couples where at least one
reverse coded so that a higher score indicated greater fre- spouse completed the SAQ in 2008 and both spouses were
quency of moderate activity (M  =  3.11, SD  =  1.16). We alive and completed the follow-up interview in 2014 (see
acknowledge the conceptual overlap in the items that com- Supplementary Figure 1).
prise functional ability and physical activity. We assume
the future ability to do an activity (functional ability) to be
distinct from the frequency of actually doing the activity Analytic Plan
(physical activity). Common fate models (Ledermann & Kenny, 2012), also
known as latent dyadic models (Gonzalez & Griffin, 2002),
Disease  Burden. Respondents’ disease burden was meas- were used to link couples’ shared variance in beliefs about
ured in 2008 as the count of reported diagnoses of: heart aging to their shared variance in functional limitations at
disease, lung disease, diabetes, cancer, stroke, and arthri- follow-up 6 years later. The common fate model is a form
tis. Diagnoses of cancer and stroke were counted if the of structural equation model that parses observed variables
respondent reported receiving treatment for the condition into variation that is attributed to the couple and individual.
within the last 2  years. Signs of depression were counted A latent variable represents couples’ shared variance (simi-
toward disease burden if respondents reported more than lar to a couple mean). The residual represents the spouses’
four symptoms in the 8-item Center for Epidemiological remaining individual (or unique) variance to depict individ-
Studies Depression scale (CES-D; Steffick, 2000). Disease ual variation above and below the couple mean. The com-
burden ranged from 0 to 7 (M = 1.53, SD = 1.19). mon fate model allows couples to vary from other couples
(some couples may have more positive beliefs about aging
Covariates than others) and for spouses within the dyad to vary within
Covariates were added in blocks to explain how couples’ couples (spouses may differ from each other in their beliefs
individual and shared processes predicted future functional about aging). We used the common fate model to link cou-
limitations. The first block addressed partner selection and ple variances over and above effects at the individual level.
included sociodemographic characteristics of educational This approach is distinct from the commonly used actor-
attainment (25% with at least some college), race, and partner interdependence model, which adjusts for inter-
sample quintiles of household wealth. The second covari- dependence within couples and then links the remaining
ate block added indicators of couples’ shared health experi- individual variance to spouses’ individual outcomes.
ences and adjusted for: body mass index (BMI categorized In preparation for analysis, separate measurement mod-
as <18.5–24.9 [27%]; 25–29.9 [40%]; 30+ [33%]), alco- els were constructed to parse couple from individual vari-
hol use (59% currently drink alcohol), and whether or ance. Factor loadings were constrained to 1 and residuals
not the respondent had ever smoked (57% have smoked). were constrained to be equal across gender. This model is
The health and health behavior block adjusted for disease mathematically equivalent to an unconditional model in
burden in physical activity models, and physical activity the multilevel context.
in disease burden models. The final model added age to To accomplish Aim 1, we first used the unconditional
adjust for similarities and differences in age within couples. models to estimate the intraclass  correlations, which repre-
All covariates, except for BMI (missing = 19) and smoking sent both the proportion of total variation attributed to cou-
(missing = 18) were fully present within the analytic sam- ple membership and the extent of similarity within couples.
ple. Missing values were accommodated using full informa- We then regressed couples’ shared processes of functional
tion maximum likelihood. limitations at follow-up on couples’ beliefs about aging,
The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2 S153

adjusting for individual covariances. To address Aim 2, we SE = 0.04, p < .001). Although allowing variation in indi-
modeled physical activity and disease burden as individual vidual effects across gender did not significantly improve
processes—adjusted for interdependence within couples—to model fit (χ2(1) = 1.40, ns), the following models remained
contribute to couples’ beliefs about aging and predict cou- unconstrained across gender due to our interest in within-
ples’ functional limitations. Individual variances were con- couple differences.
strained to be equal within each construct to scale the betas
and then covaried within gender to simultaneously estimate
couple and individual processes. Functional limitations and Contributions of Individual Physical Activity and
disease burden were measured as continuous rather than Disease Burden
count variables because an error term must be estimated To accomplish Aim 2, individual processes of physical
to simultaneously examine individual and couple-level pro- activity (Table 2) and disease burden (Table 3) were added

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


cesses using this analytic framework. The key couple-level to the models. Spouses’ individual physical activity was
effects were similar in both their magnitude and significance significantly linked to more positive couples’ beliefs about
when functional limitations were modeled as a count vari- aging and predicted lower couples’ functional limitations
able. Husbands and wives’ covariances and paths from indi- at follow-up. Individual disease burden was associated
vidual parameters were tested for gender invariance using the with more negative couples’ beliefs about aging and pre-
log likelihood test. dicted greater couples’ functional limitations at follow-
To complete Aim 3, covariates were added sequentially up. The likelihood test found neither individual physical
in blocks to test explanations for couples’ individual and activity nor disease burden to vary significantly by gender
shared processes. All exogenous observed variables (all in their contributions to couples’ shared processes (see
covariates) were correlated with one another. We concluded Supplementary Table 3). At the individual level, the covari-
analysis by testing the effect of individuals’ and couples’ ance of chronic conditions and functional limitations was
beliefs about aging on residual change in functional limita- stronger for wives than husbands (χ2(1) = 8.58, p < .01).
tions. In these models, both beliefs about aging in 2008 and
functional limitations in 2014 were adjusted for functional
limitations in 2008. Residual change was examined first as Selection, Shared Experiences, and Age
unadjusted and then sequentially adjusted by the partner Similarities/Differences
selection, shared health experiences, and age similarity/dif- To address Aim 3, we progressively adjusted for indicators
ferences blocks. Data were analyzed using MPLUS. of partner selection (sociodemographics), shared health
experiences (health and health behaviors), and similarity
and differences in age to explain how couples’ shared and
Results
individual processes were predictive of future functional
Couples’ Beliefs About Aging and Functional limitations. We began with adjusting for sociodemograph-
Limitations ics of race, education, and wealth, which are known to be
Descriptive statistics of study covariates are presented implicated in partner selection. As presented in Tables 2
in Table  1. Chi-squared tests and analyses of variance and 3, the effect of couples’ beliefs about aging was damp-
showed that husbands tended to be older and have higher ened but continued to predict couples’ future functional
educational attainment, greater disease burden, greater limitations. Adjusting for partner selection rendered the
physical activity, negative beliefs about aging, and fewer effects of physical activity on couples’ beliefs about aging
functional limitations than wives. To accomplish our first nonsignificant (Table  2, sociodemographics), but spouses’
aim, we examined the extent to which beliefs about aging physical activity continued to significantly predict cou-
were similar within couples and predicted couples’ future ples’ future functional limitations. In contrast, the effects
functional limitations. In total, 35% of the variation in of spouses’ disease burden on couple processes remained
beliefs about aging, 21% of variation in functional limi- robust, although more so for wives’ than husbands (see
tations, 17% of variation in physical activity, and 19% Table 3, sociodemographics).
of variation in disease burden was attributed to couple In our second set of models, we adjusted for health and
membership (see Supplementary Table 2). Couples’ beliefs health behaviors—covariates that are representative of
about aging predicted couples’ functional limitations at couples’ shared health experiences. The effect of couples’
follow-up, (β = −.60, SE = 0.03, p < .001). Couples with beliefs about aging on future functional limitations was
more positive beliefs tended to have fewer functional slightly reduced in both the physical activity (Table 2) and
limitations at follow-up. The likelihood test (Gonzalez & disease burden (Table 3) models. The remaining contribu-
Griffin, 2001) found this path to be significantly different tion of physical activity to couples’ functional limitations
from zero (χ2(1) = 26.78, p < .001). Individual beliefs about was rendered nonsignificant once shared health experi-
aging were also correlated with individual functional ences, including disease burden, were also accounted for
limitations at follow-up; (rhis(beliefs, limitations)  =  −.29, (Table  2, health). The contribution of husbands’ disease
SE  =  0.03, p < .001; rher(beliefs, limitations)  =  −.35, burden to couples’ beliefs about aging was explained by
S154 The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2

Table 1.  Descriptive Characteristics of Study Covariates Across Gender (N = 1,231 Couples)

Men Women

M % SD M % SD p

Age 67.94 8.07 65.00 7.88 <.001


 51–64 34% 48% <.001
 65–80 58% 49%
 80+ 8% 4%
Race
 White 81% 80%

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


  African American 8% 8%
 Hispanic/other 12% 12%
Some college 35% 30% =.003
Disease burden 1.39 1.13 1.25 1.04 =.002
 Diabetes 23% 14% <.001
 Cancer 13% 9% =.002
  Lung disease 8% 8%
  Heart disease 28% 16% <.001
 Stroke 5% 3% =.01
 Arthritis 55% 63% <.001
 Depression 10% 14% =.002
Physical activity 2.43 1.97 2.47 1.91
Self-perceptions of aging 4.20 1.06 4.30 1.03 =.05
N functional limitations 2008 1.58 1.98 2.04 2.32 <.001
N functional limitations 2014 2.25 2.60 2.51 2.70 =.02
  Walking several blocks 28% 30%
  Jogging 1 mile 66% 66%
  Walking 1 block 14% 13%
  Sitting for 2 hr 14% 17%
  Getting up from chair 37% 37%
  Climbing flights of stairs 41% 47% =.002
  Climbing 1 flight of stairs 14% 18% =.005
 Stooping 45% 45%
  Reaching arms above shoulders 16% 15%
  Pulling/pushing large objects 21% 27% <.001
  Lifting heavy objections 16% 25% <.001
  Picking up a dime 9% 8%

Note: Gender differences tested using chi-squared analysis for categorical variables and analysis variance for continuous variables. N = number.

couples’ shared health experiences. The effect of spouses’ Discussion


disease burden on couples’ functional limitations was ren-
Signs of one’s own aging are ubiquitous in daily life. Coded
dered nonsignificant (Table 3, health).
signals from institutions, the media, and interactions with
The third model added adjustments for similarities and
others make changes in physical ability and appearance,
differences in spouses’ age. Couples’ beliefs about aging
relationships, and roles salient in an age-structured society
continued to predict couples’ future functional limitations
(Levy, 2009; Settersten et al., 2015). Aging is inescapable,
in the fully adjusted models. Accounting for husbands’ and
but beliefs about aging are malleable (Stephan et al., 2013),
wives’ age rendered all remaining effects of physical activity
can remain positive until the end of life (Kotter-Grühn,
on couples’ functional limitations nonsignificant (Table 2,
Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, Gerstorf, & Smith, 2009), and
chronological age). The effect of wives’ disease burden on
are predictive of future health, well-being, and longevity
couples’ beliefs about aging remained significant.
(Westerhof et al., 2014). Our research extended the propo-
We concluded by testing the effects of couples’ beliefs
sition that aging is not experienced alone, but in concert
about aging on couples’ shared processes of change in func-
with close others (Antonucci et al., 2010; Settersten et al.,
tional limitations (see Table 4). The contributions of cou-
2015). We examined the extent and implications of older
ples’ beliefs about aging on couples’ shared processes of
couples’ shared beliefs about their own aging. We found
change in functional limitations was largely explained by
couples to be similar in beliefs about aging, their shared
indicators of shared health experiences.
beliefs to predict future functional limitations, and their
The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2 S155

Table 2.  Individual Physical Activity and Couples’ Shared and Individual Self-perceptions of Aging in 2008 Predicting Couples’
Shared and Individual Functional Limitations in 2014 (N = 1,231 Couples)

Unadjusted Sociodemographicsa Healthb Chronological agec

β SE β SE β SE β SE

Couple level
  Couples’ SPA ←
   His physical activity .17** 0.06 .08 0.06 .04 0.06 .04 0.06
   Her physical activity .17** 0.06 .11† 0.06 .10 0.06 .10 0.06
  Couples’ limitations ←

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


  Couples’ SPA −.55*** 0.09 −.50*** 0.12 −.41** 0.14 −.46** 0.17
   His physical activity −.28** 0.09 −.26* 0.11 −.24* 0.11 −.25† 0.14
   Her physical activity −.26** 0.08 −.24* 0.10 −.21† 0.11 −.20 0.13
Individual level (residual correlations)
 SPA ←
   His physical activity .19*** 0.04 .20*** 0.04 .17*** 0.04 .16*** 0.04
   Her physical activity .11* 0.04 .09* 0.05 .04 0.05 .05 0.05
 Limitations ←
  His SPA −.27*** 0.04 −.28*** 0.03 −.22*** 0.04 −.21*** 0.04
  Her SPA −.31*** 0.03 −.30*** 0.03 −.21*** 0.04 −.22*** 0.04
   His physical activity −.21*** 0.05 −.20*** 0.05 −.17*** 0.04 −.16*** 0.04
   Her physical activity −.22*** 0.04 −.21*** 0.04 −.16*** 0.04 −.15*** 0.04
Model fit indices
 RMSEA .04 .03 .02 .01
 CFI .99 .98 .99 .99
 BIC 25475.574 30556.105 49811.146 51982.25
  χ2 (model vs. saturated) χ2(4) = 9.95* χ2(22) = 40.82* χ2(52) = 69.73 χ2(64) = 76.03

Note: Standardized β coefficients presented. BIC  =  Bayesian Information Criterion; CFI  =  Cumulative Fit Index; RMSEA  =  Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation; SPA = self-perceptions of aging.
Models are additive and adjusted for: aIndividual’s race and education, and sample quintiles of household wealth. bIndividual smoking status, use of alcohol
(1 = drinks alcohol, 0 = does not drink alcohol), body mass index (<18.5–24.9 [ref.], 25–29.9, 30+), and number of chronic conditions. cIndividual age group
(51–64, 65–79, 80+).

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

individual experiences of health and health behaviors to most often directed at the individual, and attention to the
shape these processes. couple environment provides an opportunity to amplify a
Beliefs about aging develop over a lifetime of experi- program’s intended effects (Martire et al., 2010). Our find-
ences with age stereotypes, age-graded social institutions, ings justify further research on couple-level effects. For
and interactions with others. Our research showed that example, cognitive framing interventions could include
a striking 35% of the variation in beliefs about aging is components where couples discuss aging beliefs together.
shared within couples and hold enduring implications for Alternatively, an estimation of spouses’ shared beliefs could
future health. The couple’s shared environment represents assist practitioners in placing assessment of an individual
co-constructed couple norms, rituals, and tendencies that within the context of the couple’s norms.
shape how spouses interact with and support one another Individuals’ beliefs, resources, and behaviors are
(Rook, 2015; Tucker & Mueller, 2000). Our research iden- known to be important for both partners’ future health
tified a unique and separate pathway by which beliefs about (e.g., Drewelies et al., 2016; Franks et al., 2004; Kim et al.,
aging are linked to future functional limitations—that the 2014; Westerhof et  al., 2014). We expanded this body
shared environment exerts an independent influence on of research and illustrated how individual processes of
future health. Beliefs about aging influence health through physical activity and disease burden contribute to couples’
behavioral and cognitive pathways (Levy, 2009) and shared co-constructed norms of beliefs about aging and future
beliefs may shape how couples jointly appraise, interpret, functional health. Relationship processes are known to
and respond to experiences of aging (Rook, 2015; Tucker be essential to health behavior and disease management
& Mueller, 2000). In designing policies and interventions (Martire et al., 2010; Rook, 2015). In addition to shaping
to improve individuals’ beliefs about aging, our findings individual beliefs about aging and future health, we found
highlight the importance of recognizing individual beliefs husbands’ physical activity to predict couples’ future
as part of a larger whole. Policies and interventions are functional health, whereas wives’ disease burden shaped
S156 The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2

Table 3.  Individual Disease Burden and Couples’ Shared and Individual Self-perceptions of Aging in 2008 Predicting Couples’
Shared and Individual Functional Limitations in 2014 (N = 1,231 Couples)

Unadjusted Sociodemographicsa Healthb Chronological agec

β SE β SE β SE β SE

Couple level
  Couples’ SPA ←
   His disease burden −.21*** 0.06 −.13* 0.06 −.10† 0.06 −.09 0.06
   Her disease burden −.32*** 0.06 −.26*** 0.07 −.23*** 0.07 −.21*** 0.06
  Couples’ limitations ←

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


  Couples’ SPA −.39** 0.13 −.33* 0.15 −.38* 0.16 .49** 0.18
   His disease burden .21*** 0.11 .25* 0.12 .22† 0.13 .02 0.13
   Her disease burden .32*** 0.11 .24† 0.13 .16 0.13 .02 0.14
Individual level (residual correlations)
 SPA ←
   His disease burden −.24*** 0.04 −.25*** 0.04 −.22*** 0.04 −.21*** 0.04
   Her disease burden −.26*** 0.05 −.25*** 0.05 −.24*** 0.04 −.24*** 0.04
 Limitations ←
  His SPA −.29*** 0.03 −.29*** 0.03 −.26*** 0.03 −.25*** 0.03
  Her SPA −.30*** 0.04 −.29*** 0.04 −.27*** 0.04 −.28*** 0.04
   His disease burden .29*** 0.04 .29*** 0.04 .27*** 0.04 .27*** 0.04
   Her disease burden .45*** 0.03 .44*** 0.03 .42*** 0.03 .41*** 0.03
Model fit indices
 RMSEA .04 .04 .02 .02
 CFI .99 .97 .98 .98
 BIC 25149.31 30236.69 49828.50 52012.07
  χ2 (model vs. saturated) χ2(4) = 12.06* χ2(22) = 64.21** χ2(52) = 87.08** χ2(64) = 105.85***

Note: Standardized β coefficients presented. BIC  =  Bayesian Information Criterion; CFI  =  Cumulative Fit Index; RMSEA  =  Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation; SPA = self-perceptions of aging.
Models are additive and adjusted for: aIndividual’s race and education, and sample quintiles of household wealth. bIndividual smoking status, use of alcohol
(1 = drinks alcohol, 0 = does not drink alcohol), body mass index (<18.5–24.9 [ref.], 25–29.9, 30+), and frequency of moderate physical activity. cIndividual age
group (51–64, 65–79, 80+).

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

couples’ concurrent shared beliefs about aging. These find- norms to amplify individual beliefs and behaviors would
ings suggest that first, individual processes are important— be a fruitful topic for further research.
they shape one’s own beliefs, which then form the couples’ The source of couples’ shared processes is important
shared beliefs. Therefore, an intervention that “treats” an because it assists researchers, practitioners, and policy
individual has the potential for broader impact through makers in positively affecting older adults’ health and
its contribution to shared norms. However, these shared well-being. Couple processes are grounded in the distal
norms can also dampen the effects of individual treatments history of how couples selected one another, their shared
if not accounted for (Martire et al., 2010). Second, although historical and current experiences and behaviors, and also
husbands and wives were found to be similar in how their by their similarities and differences in chronological age
own physical activity and disease burden contributed to (Choi & Vasunilashorn, 2014; Meyler et al., 2007). In this
their own beliefs about aging, our research suggests that study, the contribution of spouses’ physical activity to cou-
contribution of individual experiences to couples’ norms ples’ shared beliefs was entirely explained by indicators of
is gendered (Calasanti, 2010; Kornadt et  al., 2013). Our partner selection. Distal statuses such as race and educa-
research suggests that interventions aimed at modifying tion are known to open and constrain access to health-
married couples’ beliefs about aging should be sensitive promoting environments (Calasanti, 2010; Settersten
to how husbands and wives perceive their disease burden. et  al., 2015). Building on this earlier work, our findings
For example, previous research has shown how individuals suggest that interventions aimed at improving couples’
interpret the experience of chronic conditions—as actiona- shared beliefs through increased engagement in physical
ble or an unavoidable consequence of aging (Stewart et al., activity must be sensitive to the constraints couples face.
2012). Based on the findings from this study, these beliefs Additionally, we found couples’ shared health experiences
are expected to be amplified through their contribution to explain the contributions of not only husbands’ disease
to the couples’ shared beliefs. The potential for couples’ burden to functional limitations, but also the contribution
The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2 S157

Table 4.  Couples’ Shared and Individual Self-perceptions of Aging on Residual Change in Functional Limitations (N = 1,231
Couples)

Unadjusteda Sociodemographicsb Healthc Chronological aged

β SE β SE β SE β SE

Couple level
  Δ Couples’ limitations ←
  Couples’ SPA −.44** 0.15 −.38* 0.15 −.32† 0.17 −.41 0.28
Individual level (residual correlations)
  Δ Limitations ←

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


  His SPA −.14*** 0.03 −.15*** 0.04 −.13*** 0.04 −.13*** 0.04
  Her SPA −.14** 0.05 −.12** 0.04 −.09** 0.04 −.09* 0.04
Model fit indices
 RMSEA .18 .03 .02 .02
 CFI .85 .99 .99 .99
 BIC 27942.66 32782.03 59314.97 61541.74
  χ2 (model vs. saturated) χ2(7) = 298.34*** χ2(19) = 40.83** χ2(43) = 67.83** χ2(51) = 72.09*

Note: Standardized β coefficients presented. BIC  =  Bayesian Information Criterion; CFI  =  Cumulative Fit Index; RMSEA  =  Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation; SPA = self-perceptions of aging.
Models are additive and adjusted for: aFunctional limitations in 2008. bIndividual’s race and education, and sample quintiles of household wealth. cIndividual
smoking status, use of alcohol (1 = drinks alcohol, 0 = does not drink alcohol), body mass index (<18.5–24.9 [ref.], 25–29.9, 30+), and frequency of moderate
physical activity. dIndividual age group (51–64, 65–79, 80+).

p < .07. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

of couples’ beliefs about aging to couples’ change in func- experiences as care givers and care receivers (e.g., Calasanti,
tional limitations. This is important because health and 2010). Additional time points would allow examination of
health behaviors are accessible to intervention and are also these processes within the context of couples’ shared beliefs.
known to be deeply influenced by couples’ relationships Additionally, modeling functional limitations as a continu-
processes (Martire et  al., 2006; Rook, 2015; Tucker & ous variable estimates a linear function on the raw scale,
Mueller, 2000). Finally, adjusting for age did not further rather than a linear function on the log scale. This limits
explain how couples’ physical activity and disease burden the ability to think of our independent variables as risk
contribute to couples’ beliefs about aging. This suggests multipliers. However, this limitation is most relevant for
that the relevance of similarities and differences in age for the individual effects, as the couple-level effects were com-
couples’ beliefs about aging is encompassed by age-related parable in the count model (see Supplementary Table  4).
variation in health and health behaviors, rather than char- This research does illustrate how the couple environment
acteristics of age such as age-graded roles, responsibilities, encompasses previous unexplored variance in the relation-
and experiences. ship between beliefs about aging and future functional limi-
The results from this study must be interpreted within tations. The findings from this study lay the groundwork to
the context of its limitations. First, beliefs about aging and further examine individual beliefs, health, and behaviors as
functional limitations maintain a reciprocal relationship. parts of a larger whole. Additionally, due to the relatively
Although this study was longitudinal, its design prohibits small sample size, our models assumed invariance across
conclusions about the direction of causality. We acknowl- couples’ age difference and marriage tenure. The heteroge-
edge that physical activity and functional limitations are neity of effects across subgroups of within-couple age dif-
reciprocally related. Within the context of this study, hus- ferences, marriage tenure, and relationship intensity is an
bands’ and wives’ frequency of moderate activity—actual important avenue for future research. Finally, due to con-
activity—was found to be predictive of future functional straints in the available data, this study focused on married
limitations—the ability to do activities—at both the indi- heterosexual couples in older adulthood. Future research
vidual and couple level. Additionally, couples’ health and should examine similarity and differences in shared beliefs
beliefs about aging contributed to selective attrition. It is about aging in other couple formations as well as consider
therefore important for future research to examine the the lingering effect of previous relationships and critical
implications of couple-level constructs on survival. We also periods of shared time together.
acknowledge that with only two points of measurement, In conclusion, couples are not only similar in their
it was not possible to examine proximal within-couple health and behavior, but also in their beliefs about aging.
relationship processes. This is important because aging is a The quality of couples’ shared beliefs predicts both indi-
gendered process, where husbands and wives have distinct vidual and couple-level differences in functional limitations
S158 The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2

6 years in the future. Knowledge of the couples, including Drewelies, J., Chopik, W. J., Hoppmann, C. A., Smith, J., &
their norms, rituals, beliefs, and general health, will assist Gerstorf, D. (2016). Linked lives: Dyadic associations of mas-
gerontologists and policy makers in supporting couples in tery beliefs with health (behavior) and health (behavior) change
their individual and collective efforts to age successfully among older partners. The Journals of Gerontology, Series
together. B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. doi:10.1093/
geronb/gbw058
Fonda, S. J., & Herzog, A. R. (2004). Documentation of physical
Supplementary Material functioning measured in the Health and Retirement Study and
the Asset and Health Dynamics among the oldest old. Ann
Supplementary data are available at The Gerontologist online.
Arbor: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.
Franks, M. M., Lucas, T., Stephens, M. A. P., Rook, K. S., & Gonzalez,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


R. (2010). Diabetes distress and depressive symptoms: A dyadic
Conflicts of Interest
investigation of older patients and their spouses. Family Relations,
The authors S. T. Mejía and R. Gonzalez certify that they have no 59, 599–610. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2010.00626.x
affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with Franks, M. M., Wendorf, C. A., Gonzalez, R., & Ketterer, M. (2004).
any financial or nonfinancial interest in the subject matter or materi-
Aid and influence: Health-promoting exchanges of older mar-
als discussed in this manuscript.
ried partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21,
431–445. doi:10.1177/0265407504044839
Gonzalez, R., & Griffin, D. (2001). Testing parameters in struc-
Funding tural equation modeling: Every “one” matters. Psychological
The authors’ contributions were partially funded by the National Methods, 6, 258–269. doi:10.1037//1082-989X.6.3.258
Institute on Aging (R01AG040635). The content is solely the respon- Gonzalez, R., & Griffin, D. (2002). A statistical framework for mod-
sibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official eling homogeneity and interdependence in groups. In G. J.  O.
views of the funders. This paper was published as part of a supple- Fletcher & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook on social
ment sponsored and funded by AARP. The statements and opinions psychology: Interpersonal processes (pp. 505–534). Blackwell
expressed herein by the authors are for information, debate, and dis-
Publishers Ltd.
cussion, and do not necessarily represent official policies of AARP.
Hoppmann, C. A., & Gerstorf, D. (2009). Spousal interrela-
tions in old age: A  mini review. Gerontology, 55, 449–459.
doi:10.1159/000211948
References Hubley, A. M., & Russell, L. B. (2009). Prediction of subjec-
Antonucci, T. C., Fiori, K. L., Birditt, K. S., & Jackey, L. M.  H. tive age, desired age, and age satisfaction in older adults:
(2010). Convoys of social relations: Integrating life-span and Do some health dimensions contribute more than others?
life-course perspectives. In R. M. Lerner, M. E. Lamb, & A. M. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 12–21.
Freund (Eds.), The handbook of life-span development (Vol. 2, doi:10.1177/0165025408099486
pp. 434–473). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Kim, E. S., Moored, K. D., Giasson, H. L., & Smith, J. (2014).
Barrett, A. E., & Montepare, J. M. (2015). “It’s about time”: Satisfaction with aging and use of preventive health ser-
Applying life span and life course perspectives to the study of vices. Preventive Medicine, 69, 176–180. doi:10.1016/j.
subjective age. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, ypmed.2014.09.008
35, 55–77. doi:10.1891/0198-8794.35.55 Kornadt, A. E., Voss, P., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Multiple
Brothers, A., Miche, M., Wahl, H. W., & Diehl, M. (2015). standards of aging: Gender-specific age stereotypes in differ-
Examination of associations among three distinct subjec- ent life domains. European Journal of Ageing, 10, 335–344.
tive aging constructs and their relevance for predicting devel- doi:10.1007/s10433-013-0281-9
opmental correlates. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Kotter-Grühn, D., Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, A., Gerstorf, D., &
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/ Smith, J. (2009). Self-perceptions of aging predict mortality and
gbv085 change with approaching death: 16-year longitudinal results
Calasanti, T. (2010). Gender relations and applied research on aging. from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 24, 654–
Gerontologist, 50, 720–734. doi:10.1093/geront/gnq085 667. doi:10.1037/a0016510
Choi, K. H., & Vasunilashorn, S. (2014). Widowhood, age heterog- Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale
amy, and health: The role of selection, marital quality, and health Scale: A  revision. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 85–89.
behaviors. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological doi:10.1093/geronj/30.1.85
Sciences and Social Sciences, 69, 123–134. doi:10.1093/geronb/ Ledermann, T., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). The common fate model for
gbt104 dyadic data: Variations of a theoretically important but under-
Diehl, M., Wahl, H.-W., Barrett, A. E., Brothers, A. F., Miche, utilized model. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 140–148.
M., Montepare, J. M., … Wurm S. (2014). Awareness of doi:10.1037/a0026624
aging: Theoretical considerations on an emerging con- Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A  psychosocial approach
cept. Developmental Review, 34, 93–113. doi:10.1016/j. to aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 332–
dr.2014.01.001 336. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x
The Gerontologist, 2017, Vol. 57, No. S2 S159

Li, K. K., Cardinal, B. J., & Acock, A. C. (2013). Concordance of Schafer, M. H., & Shippee, T. P. (2010). Age identity in context stress
physical activity trajectories among middle-aged and older mar- and the subjective side of aging. Social Psychology Quarterly,
ried couples: Impact of diseases and functional difficulties. The 73, 245–264. doi:10.1177/0190272510379751
Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Schwartz, C. R. (2013). Trends and variation in assortative mat-
Social Sciences, 68, 794–806. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt068 ing: Causes and consequences. Annual Review of Sociology, 39,
Martire, L. M., Keefe, F. J., Schulz, R., Ready, R., Beach, S. R., Rudy, 451–470. doi:10.1146/annurev-soc-071312-145544
T. E., & Starz, T. W. (2006). Older spouses’ perceptions of part- Settersten, J., Richard, A., & Hagestad, G. O. (2015). Subjective
ners’ chronic arthritis pain: Implications for spousal responses, aging and new complexities of the life course. Annual
support provision, and caregiving experiences. Psychology and Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 35, 29–53.
Aging, 21, 222–230. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.222 doi:10.1891/0198-8794.35.29
Martire, L. M., Schulz, R., Helgeson, V. S., Small, B. J., & Saghafi, E. Sonnega, A., Faul, J. D., Ofstedal, M. B., Langa, K. M., Phillips,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/57/suppl_2/S149/3913390 by guest on 27 August 2021


M. (2010). Review and meta-analysis of couple-oriented inter- J. W., & Weir, D. R. (2014). Cohort profile: The Health and
ventions for chronic illness. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40, Retirement Study (HRS). International Journal of Epidemiology,
325–342. doi:10.1007/s12160-010-9216-2 43, 576–585. doi:10.1093/ije/dyu067
Meyler, D., Stimpson, J. P., & Peek, M. K. (2007). Health concordance Steffick, D. E. (2000). Documentation of affective functioning meas-
within couples: A systematic review. Social Science & Medicine, ures in the Health and Retirement Study. Ann Arbor: Survey
64, 2297–2310. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.007 Research Center, University of Michigan.
Monin, J. K., Schulz, R., Martire, L. M., Jennings, J. R., Lingler, J. H., Stephan, Y., Chalabaev, A., Kotter-Grühn, D., & Jaconelli, A. (2013).
& Greenberg, M. S. (2010). Spouses’ cardiovascular reactivity “Feeling younger, being stronger”: An experimental study of
to their partners’ suffering. The Journals of Gerontology, Series subjective age and physical functioning among older adults. The
B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B, 195–201. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and
doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp133 Social Sciences, 68, 1–7. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs037
Polenick, C. A., Martire, L. M., Hemphill, R. C., & Stephens, M. A. P. Stewart, T. L., Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., & Weiner, B. (2012).
(2015). Effects of change in arthritis severity on spouse well- Attributing illness to ‘old age’: Consequences of a self-directed
being: The moderating role of relationship closeness. Journal of stereotype for health and mortality. Psychology & Health, 27,
Family Psychology, 29, 331–338. doi:10.1037/fam0000093 881–897. doi:10.1080/08870446.2011.630735
Rook, K. S. (2015). Social networks in later life weighing Tucker, J. S., & Mueller, J. S. (2000). Spouses’ social control of
positive and negative effects on health and well-being. health behaviors: Use and effectiveness of specific strategies.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 45–51. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1120–1130.
doi:10.1177/0963721414551364 doi:10.1177/01461672002611008
Sargent-Cox, K. A., Anstey, K. J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2014). Westerhof, G. J., Miche, M., Brothers, A. F., Barrett, A. E., Diehl, M.,
Longitudinal change of self-perceptions of aging and mor- Montepare, J. M., … Wurm, S. (2014). The influence of subjec-
tality. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological tive aging on health and longevity: A meta-analysis of longitu-
Sciences and Social Sciences, 69, 168–173. doi:10.1093/ dinal data. Psychology and Aging, 29, 793–802. doi:10.1037/
geronb/gbt005 a0038016

You might also like