You are on page 1of 27

Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc

1994, Vol. 8, No, 4, 390-416 0893~3200/94/$3.00

What We Do and Don't Know About the Process of


Family Therapy
Myrna L. Friedlander, Julie Wildman, Laurie Heatherington, and
Elizabeth A. Skowron

Despite convincing outcome evidence and the popularity of family therapy, little is
known about how interpersonal change actually comes about in this context. To
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

synthesize and integrate what is known and to offer recommendations for future,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

clinically relevant research, all published process studies on family therapy were
reviewed. Included were naturalistic studies of conjoint treatment in which the focus
was the in-session verbal behavior of the participants or their self-reported perceptions
of actual interactions. Dating from 1963, 36 studies met the inclusion criteria. The
review was organized according to 3 hierarchically ordered levels of in-session
processes (L. S. Greenberg, 1986): observations of (a) speech acts, followed by studies
of (b) important incidents or change episodes and (c) the therapeutic relationship. The
implications of what is "known" are followed by a discussion of what is not "known"
and suggested directions for future research.

Since the 1950s, when family therapy was treatment for various psychological disorders.
considered an unusual, if not heretical, mode of Although outcome research was needed to es-
treatment, its popularity has been rapidly accel- tablish the efficacy of family treatment, and
erating. Family therapy is now routinely taught the evidence is indeed convincing (Gurman,
to students in all of the major mental health Kniskern, & Pinsof, 1986), such research
professions and to practitioners seeking to im- tends to be of little value to the practicing
prove their skills in professional workshops, therapist, who is concerned with effective
conferences, and free-standing training centers. strategies and interventions. Practitioners tend,
The process of family treatment is described, instead, to turn to theoretical or technical arti-
debated, and analyzed not only in practice-ori- cles, books, and workshops for such clinically
ented periodicals and books but also in well- relevant information.
respected journals of psychotherapy research. Another reason for the lack of knowledge
Despite the ever-growing popularity of fam- concerning interpersonal change processes is
ily therapy, little is known about how interper- that the overriding concern of theorists is what
sonal change actually comes about in this therapists should do to effect change (Kuehl,
context. One reason for this lack of knowl- Newfield, & Joanning, 1990). Rarely do theo-
edge may be because, to date, investigators rists discuss— or even comment on—what fam-
have emphasized the global efficacy of family ily members need to do. Furthermore, it is rare
for theorists to focus on processes common to
Myrna L. Friedlander, Julie Wildman, and Eliza- many therapeutic approaches. Instead, the con-
beth A. Skowron, Department of Counseling Psy- cern of most theorists is how to distinguish their
chology, University at Albany, State University of approach from others.
New York; Laurie Heatherington, Department of The purpose of this article is to redress these
Psychology, Williams College. imbalances (across all schools of family ther-
This article was presented in June 1993 at the apy) by reviewing the existing research on pro-
annual conference of the Society for Psychotherapy
Research in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. cesses of change in family therapy and by of-
Correspondence concerning this article should be fering recommendations for future research.
addressed to Myrna L. Friedlander, Department of We have made an attempt to provide a com-
Counseling Psychology, Education 220, University at prehensive review of all the published process
Albany, State University of New York, 1400 Wash- research on family therapy. Both computer-
ington Avenue, Albany, New York 12222. assisted and human searches were used to iden-
390
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 391

tify publications with empirical evidence (qual- levels, so that speech acts can be studied in the
itative as well as quantitative) on family treat- context of specific change events or episodes,
ments. Only studies of naturally occurring and each of these units can be "nested" within a
processes in family therapy were included in the study of the relational aspects of the therapy
review. Although there are a number of excel- (Greenberg, 1986). From this perspective, pro-
lent controlled studies of treatment components cess variable C is not only composed of other,
(e.g., Jacobson, 1984), these investigations were more microscopic process variables (A, B) but
designed to test the effectiveness of those com- is also, itself, a component of the therapeutic
ponents rather than to observe the naturally process (D) at a macroscopic level (e.g., Green-
unfolding process of change. Our intent was not berg, 1986; Greenberg & Pinsof, 1986a; Pinsof,
to disparage those important efforts but, rather, 1989; Shoham-Salomon, 1990).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to highlight naturalistic process research in fam- We chose this schema to organize the review
ily therapy. for two reasons. First, this schema is particu-
To be included in the review, the study had to larly useful from the standpoint of practice.
focus on the actual in-session verbal behavior of Clinicians are best able to evaluate the conclu-
the participants or on their self-reported percep- sions of a given program of research when the
tions of in-session behaviors.1 Consequently, process unit is clearly specified. In other words,
we excluded retrospective studies of clients' the practical relevance of the finding that vari-
reactions (e.g., Beck & Jones, 1973; Greenberg, able A predicts variable B requires knowledge
James, & Conry, 1988; Kuehl et al., 1990; of the context (Greenberg, 1986; Heatherington,
Shapiro & Budman, 1973) as well as studies 1990). Consider the following hypothetical ex-
with only pre- and postmeasures of family be- ample. Speech act B (therapist confrontation) is
havior or other outside-of-therapy observations found to occur frequently in family therapy
(e.g., Seeman, Tittler, & Friedman, 1985), that sessions. It would be more useful to know,
is, those that typify outcome research. Studies however, that B follows A (therapist support)
of therapists' general or characteristic styles of significantly more often than chance. In addi-
behavior (e.g., Rice, Gurman, & Razin, 1974) tion, if the sequential relationship between A
were also excluded. Finally, while we recog- and B (support and confrontation) is found to
nize that family therapy is neither conceptually characterize specific, well-defined change epi-
nor practically limited to conjoint treatment sodes (C; e.g., challenging parents to work to-
(Gurman, 1988), we assume that in-session gether to set limits for their young children) in
processes are affected when more than a sin- the context of a supportive therapeutic relation-
gle client is present. Thus, for purposes of this ship (D), this finding is even more meaningful
review, we only included studies of conjoint for the practicing therapist. Indeed, in a recent
treatments involving two or more family survey of practitioners, Beutler, Williams, and
members. Wakefield (1993) found that the most strongly
In contrast to Pinsof's (1981) and Gurman et endorsed request was for "research that focuses
al.'s (1986) reviews of family therapy research, on therapist and/or client behaviors leading to
we excluded unpublished dissertations and con- important moments of change during psycho-
ference papers (e.g., Presser, Sigal, Mayero- therapy" (p. 56).
vitch, & Chayoga [cited in Pinsof, 1981]), as
well as articles reporting programs of instru-
ment development (e.g., Allred & Kersey, 1
In a case study of a single session, Scheflen
1977). Rather than organizing the review by (1973) developed an elaborate analysis of the coor-
investigator or by instrument, as have others dination of verbal and kinesic behaviors in a demon-
(e.g., Beutler & Crago, 1991; Greenberg & stration session with a mother and her daughter, who
Pinsof, 1986b; Pinsof, 1981), we focused on had schizophrenia. The therapists were Carl Whitaker
and Thomas Malone. While this is the sole study, to
three hierarchically ordered levels of in-session
our knowledge, in which nonverbal behaviors were
processes (Greenberg, 1986). We first reviewed analyzed in a family therapy context, the results are
investigations (a) at the speech act level, fol- not only exceptionally complex but also do not lend
lowed by studies of (b) therapeutic episodes themselves to drawing conclusions about the process
and, at the most global level, (c) the therapeutic of therapeutic change. As Pinsof (1981) noted in his
relationship. In this model of process research, review of Scheflen's work, the research was more
each unit or level is subsumed by successive ethnographic than psychological in nature.
392 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

Second, by organizing the review according Speech Acts


to this hierarchy of contexts, we are able to
present an overview of the entire literature base At the first, most microscopic level of the
from a perspective that is different from that of therapeutic process is the individual speech act,
earlier reviewers but which builds on their that is, a single statement or speaking turn
collective wisdom (e.g., Gurman et al., 1986; (Greenberg, 1986). In the studies reviewed be-
Pinsof, 1981, 1989; Wynne, 1988). In conduct- low, no higher order contexts (episodes or rela-
ing the review, for example, we rapidly became tionship) were taken into account. Although
aware that the majority of published research sequences of speech acts (i.e., reciprocal inter-
has been conducted at the speech act level, with actions) were observed in a few of the studies,
the variables of interest in the majority of these
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

relatively few studies of speech acts nested


investigations were frequencies or proportions
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

within either of the two higher order contexts.


of individual speech acts. Most often, the aim of
Because the nested approach to process research
the investigators was to describe some aspect of
has the greatest appeal for practitioners, orga-
the therapeutic process in single sessions or,
nizing our review in this manner seemed to be more rarely, over the course of treatment.
the best way to communicate the research find-
The following speech act variables have been
ings to practitioners as well as to underscore the
investigated: participation rates, interactional
need for coordinating future scientific efforts
frequencies, type of response, mode of expres-
(e.g., Pinsof, 1989). sion, relational control patterns, and interper-
On the other hand, despite having organized sonal complementarity. Investigators have ad-
the studies on the basis of research design, we dressed the following substantive questions:
chose not to highlight their methodological fea- Which therapist and family behaviors predict
tures. Many earlier reviewers of the family ther- premature termination? Which features of the
apy literature (e.g., Gurman et al., 1986; Pinsof, process are common across theoretical orienta-
1981,1988,1989; Wynne, 1988) have provided tions and which are distinctive, and are the
excellent, detailed critiques of research meth- distinctive features consistent with theory? How
ods, instruments, procedures, and analyses, with do participant characteristics (e.g., gender, role)
extensive recommendations for methodological and the treatment context affect in-session ther-
improvements. Our aim, by contrast, was to apeutic processes? What changes can be ob-
synthesize and integrate the substantive find- served over the course of family therapy, and
ings of the available empirical evidence—pre- which changes in speech acts, if any, predict
cisely what we do know about the process treatment outcome? The following review ad-
of family therapy. Of course, the findings of dresses each of these questions in turn.
individual studies and programs of research Premature termination. We located only
need to be considered in light of their method- three studies in which speech acts were used to
ological shortcomings. Rather than providing predict discontinuance of treatment. In a study
extensive critiques of each study, however, we of 21 families with "soft delinquent]" (p. 657)
youth, Alexander, Barton, Schiavo, and Parsons
indicate, within each section or subsection of
(1976) coded family members' modes of ex-
the review, the major limitations. We con-
pression. They found that the ratio of supportive
clude with general implications about what is to defensive speech acts was significantly lower
"known," followed by suggested directions for in prematurely terminated treatments as com-
future research. pared with successful cases. It is interesting that
these differences were observed only in the
latter phase of treatment, not in the initial ses-
What We Do Know sions. In another study of problem adolescents,
Chamberlain, Patterson, Reid, Kavanagh, and
In Table 1, the family therapy process studies Forgatch (1984) found that, on the basis of
reviewed below are listed, organized according speech acts, more highly resistant families had a
to speech acts, therapeutic episodes, and rela- greater dropout rate. In the third, more recent
tionship variables. Within each section and sub- study, Shields, Sprenkle, and Constantine
section the studies are grouped by their substan- (1991) hypothesized that joining, structuring,
tive focus or content. and solution focusing would discriminate be-
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 393

Table 1 tween "completer" and "noncompleter" cases


Family Therapy Process Investigations (p. 6). Families (N = 63) were seen by struc-
tural-strategic therapists for child-focused
Focus Reference problems. To compare families who discontin-
Speech act ued treatment prematurely with those who con-
Premature Alexander et al. (1976)a a tinued until an agreement about termination was
termination Chamberlain et al. (1984) reached with the therapist, Shields et al. coded
Shields et al. (1991) selected speech acts using their Therapeutic In-
Common, Friedlander & Highlen (1984)b,
distinctive Friedlander et al. (1985)"
teraction Coding Scheme. Results showed that
features Friedlander et al. (1987)b the noncompleter cases were characterized by
Friedlander et al. (1991)" more frequent attempts by family members to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Treatment Chamberlain et al. (1984)a structure the therapist and by more within-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

context Heatherington & Friedlander family disagreements. By contrast, the com-


(1990b)
Gender Mas et al. (1985) pleter cases were characterized by more thera-
Brown-Standridge & Piercy pist structuring in response to family disagree-
\lyoo) ments and by more family dialogues about
Heatherington & Friedlander problems.
(1990b)
Newberry et al. (1991) Although each investigation relied on a dif-
Shields & McDaniel (1992) ferent coding system, these results suggest, in
Participant Postner et al. (1971)a general, that defensiveness or resistance may be
role Dowling (1979)
Pinsof (1979, 1986) predictive of premature termination. As several
Friedlander et al. (1985)b reviewers of process research (e.g., Pinsof,
Mas et al. (1985) 1989; Shoham-Salomon, 1990) have pointed
Changes over Postner etal. (1971)a out, however, correlating speech acts with
treatment Winer (1971) global outcome in the absence of immediate
Chamberlain et al. (1984)a
Cline et al. (1984)a context can be problematic. We do not know,
Laird & Vande Kemp (1987)"-° for example, whether the defensive and resistant
Munton & Antaki (1988)* acts by family members observed by Alexander
Raymond et al. (1993) bc et al. (1976) and Chamberlain et al. (1984) were
Therapeutic episodes directed toward one another or toward the ther-
Important Zuk et al. (1963)b apist. Nor did we know how the therapists in the
moments De Chenne (1973)d more successful families handled defensiveness
Patterson & Forgatch (1985)d when it arose, as it most certainly must have
a
Patterson & Chamberlain (1988) from time to time.
Crits-Christoph et al. (1991) bc
Effective Johnson & Greenberg (1988)d Common and distinctive features. A series
sessions Holloway et al. (1990)b'c'd of studies by Friedlander and colleagues (Fried-
Gale & Newfield (1992) bd lander, Ellis, Raymond, Siegel, & Milford,
Change Heatherington & Friedlander 1987; Friedlander, Heatherington, & Wildman,
events (1990af-d
Greenberg et al. (1993) ad
1991; Friedlander & Highlen, 1984; Fried-
Friedlander et al. (1994)d lander, Highlen, & Lassiter, 1985) examined the
extent of similarity and dissimilarity in the in-
Relationship variables terventions of master theorists-therapists. In
Therapist Shapiro (1974) Friedlander and Highlen (1984) and Friedlander
responsiveness Sigal et al. (1967)a et al. (1985), the interventions of Nathan Ack-
Therapeutic Pinsof & Catherall (1986)
alliance Holtzworth-Munroe et al. erman, Murray Bowen, Don Jackson, and Carl
(1989)" Whitaker were compared in BirdwhistelFs
Bourgeois et al. (1990)" (1969) Hillcrest Family Series, four films of
Heatherington & Friedlander consultation interviews with one remarried fam-
(1990ba, 1990c)
ily (wife, husband, and 3 children). Like the
Note. References listed on the same line were based investigators of Carl Rogers, Albert Ellis, and
on the identical sample. Some references appear
more than once. Fritz Perls in the well-known film series Three
a
Client improvement was assessed objectively. Approaches to Psychotherapy (Shostrom,
b
Master therapists were involved. c Client im- 1966), Friedlander and colleagues sought to
provement was reported by therapists. d Speech identify the interventions that distinguished
acts were observed within a higher order process unit. therapists with notably different theoretical on-
394 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

entations. The assumption of this series of stud- the target" of the intervention (Friedlander et
ies was that a microanalysis of the work of al., 1985, p. 177). The authors suggested that
master therapists could provide an important indirectness may be an important strategy that
perspective on the common and diverse aspects cuts across approaches to family therapy.
of different approaches; by comparing experts' Friedlander et al. (1987) also observed the
work with a single family, one can attribute the extent of therapist variability across diverse
observed variations to differences in theoretical families. Despite the heterogeneity of families
approach rather than to the unique mix of family sampled, results showed remarkable consis-
and therapist. tency for both Whitaker and Minuchin. The
In a subsequent, more extensive investiga- greatest variability was in the therapists' activ-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion, Friedlander et al. (1987) compared the ity levels. Both therapists also varied across
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

work of Salvador Minuchin and Carl Whitaker, families in terms of the extent of here-and-now
master therapists known for their theoretical focus, the extent of directness and indirectness,
differences. Twelve representative sessions the amount of information provided to the fam-
were analyzed: six by Whitaker and six by ily, and the amount of focus on parental sub-
Minuchin. The interviews (most of which were systems. The authors speculated that the factor
consultations) were selected to maximize diver- accounting for these differences across families
sity in terms of family constellation, role and may be the therapist's judgment of the family's
age of identified patient, and presenting con- readiness for change.
cerns. In addition to identifying sources of con- Another, more systemically based perspective
vergence and divergence in the two approaches, on theoretical convergence was provided by
the authors assessed the extent of therapist vari- content-free analyses of the Hillcrest films and
ability across families. of the Minuchin and Whitaker consultations.
In two studies by Friedlander et al. (1985, Interactional frequencies (who-talks-to-whom)
1987), a series of linguistic content analyses were subjected to multidimensional scaling
were undertaken involving Pinsof's (1979, (MDS) to provide "a spatial representation of
1981, 1986) Family Therapist Coding System the 'hidden' structure in the communication
and a measure of therapist response modes that patterns" (Friedlander & Highlen, 1984, p.
is not specific to family treatment. Results 477). In contrast to the other, more traditional
showed, overall, remarkable similarities across comparative analyses of frequencies or propor-
approaches as well as differences congruent tions of process variables, MDS provides as
with theoretical differences. With respect to the output a geometric configuration—a spatial
similarities, in both studies the therapists tended map—in which distances between points are
to focus on individuals and dyads, particularly derived from the observed measure of proxim-
the parental subsystem, and to be more con- ity or relatedness. For each interview a proxim-
cerned with the present than either the future or ity matrix was generated, which consisted of the
past. Relatively few references were made to proportion of interactions between each pair of
the family as an undifferentiated group, to ex- participants in the session (e.g., mother-father,
tended family members, or to triangular rela- mother—therapist, father-therapist, and so on).
tions among family members. In terms of re- More frequent interactions were assumed to re-
sponse modes, the Hillcrest therapists were flect greater interpersonal closeness; less fre-
most similar in their use of requests and "infor- quent interactions, more distance.
mative directives" (Friedlander et al., 1985, In the Hillcrest study (Friediander & Highlen,
p. 171). They tended to avoid restatements, si- 1984), the four maps showed many similarities,
lences, self-revealing, empathic, or confronta- and correlations of the MDS solutions were
tive responses. In Friedlander et al. (1987), significantly positive, particularly the Acker-
Whitaker and Minuchin infrequently responded man and Whitaker sessions (.75) and the Bowen
with clarifications or reflections of feelings, en- and Jackson sessions (.99). Interpretation of the
couragement, or reassurance, tending to rely maps was based on the therapists' postsession
instead on requesting or providing information. impressions of the family. Comparison of two
It is interesting that in both investigations, while sessions by Minuchin and two by Whitaker
the therapists most often addressed themselves showed both therapists clearly in the midst of
to a single individual or subsystem, a sizable the family and most closely aligned with the
proportion of interventions were "indirectly problem clients and their parents (Friedlander et
routed, that is, addressed to someone other than al., 1987). Whereas the position of the therapists
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 395

in the two MDS studies differed somewhat, in Palazzoli). The focus was the relational control
both investigations the major determinants of dynamics between therapists and family mem-
interpersonal closeness or distance seemed to be bers. Relational control is an interactional vari-
the extent of psychological disturbance and the able, reflecting how verbal messages are deliv-
nature of the subsystem alliances. ered and responded to; and relational control
Many (but not all) of the distinctive features patterns, based on the Family Relational Con-
of the interviews tended to be congruent with trol Communication Coding System (FRCCCS;
theoretical differences. In his Hillcrest inter- Friedlander & Heatherington, 1989; Heather-
view, Bowen made relatively few references to ington & Friedlander, 1987), are assumed to
the session itself, concentrating more on the reflect the reciprocal nature of the participants'
past and on the families of origin than did the social relationship (Ericson & Rogers, 1973).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

other Hillcrest therapists. While these findings As hypothesized, in both approaches comple-
were consistent with Bowen's insight-oriented mentary relational control patterns predomi-
multigenerational emphasis, one unexpected nated, with therapists from both camps most
finding was the relative infrequency of interpre- often assuming a "one-up" position and family
tations. Don Jackson, in line with his interac- members assuming a "one-down" position.
tional, pragmatic approach to family problems, Other FRCCCS indices showed considerable
emphasized the parental subsystem and focused divergence, however. Consistent with the re-
on dyadic and triadic relations more so than did spective theories, the systemic therapists en-
Bowen, Ackerman, or Whitaker. Whereas the gaged in proportionately more complementarity
spatial maps of Bowen's and Jackson's sessions than did the structural therapists, probably be-
suggested therapist alignment with the parents cause of their greater use of question and an-
and distance from the children, the alliances in swer sequences. By contrast, the structural ther-
the MDS maps with Whitaker, Ackerman, and apists engaged in more competitive symmetry
Minuchin were notably more unclear. Consis- with family members. Systemic therapists en-
tent with his psychodynamic heritage, Acker- couraged or permitted significantly less within-
man focused more on the unique experiences of family interaction, and structural therapists
individuals than on dyadic or triadic relations. It intervened in more overtly controlling and un-
is of interest, however, that few of Ackerman's balancing ways by, for example, ordering, in-
responses were geared toward the past; rather,
structing, interrupting, praising, and supporting
Ackerman led the group of Hillcrest thera-
family members either directly or indirectly.
pists in the use of here-and-now interventions.
Systemic therapists, as expected, used more
Whitaker, observed in both investigations, also
neutral indirect communications.
tended to comment on the here-and-now. In
contrast to the other therapists and consistent The conclusions of the studies just reviewed
with his writings, Whitaker commented less on need to be considered in light of several limita-
the past, focusing more on the present situation, tions. First, because all of the sessions analyzed
on the therapeutic system and his own reac- in this line of research were published or tran-
tions, and on the unique experiences of individ- scribed from videotapes made available to pro-
ual family members. Finally, compared with fessional audiences, the representativeness of
Whitaker, Minuchin focused more on the paren- each master therapist's behavior can be ques-
tal subsystem and used more confrontation, in- tioned. Second, these data do not provide infor-
terpretation, direct guidance, and advice. These mation about change over time, either within or
differences were seen as reflecting Minuchin's across sessions, or about the ultimate (or imme-
more explicit focus on executive functioning diate) effectiveness of the distinctive or com-
and his well-known techniques of reframing, mon features of these therapists' behaviors. As
challenging the family's reality, and activating Alexander (1988) suggested, different interven-
within-session change. tions are likely to be powerful at different points
In a more recent study, Friedlander et al. in treatment. Third, each of these studies reports
(1991) compared the consultative work of three frequencies or proportions of speech acts. As
leading structural therapists (Charles Fishman, other reviewers have pointed out (e.g., Pinsof,
Ron Liebman, and Salvador Minuchin) with 1989; Shoham-Salomon, 1990), information
those of three Milan systemic therapists (Luigi about the aggregate of variable X can be mis-
Boscolo, Gianfranco Cecchin, and Mara Selvini leading in the absence of immediate context.
396 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

Treatment context. We located only two in the affective mode of experience than did
studies that compared speech act variables in their female counterparts (Mas, Alexander, &
different treatment contexts. First, Chamberlain Barton, 1985).
et al. (1984) examined differences in self-refer- These results should be juxtaposed against
ral versus agency referral in 18 families who other research, conducted in different settings
completed a behavioral treatment for child man- with more experienced therapists, in which the
agement problems. Agency-referred families findings were consistent with the traditionality
were observed to engage in more resistant ver- hypothesis. Shields and McDaniel (1992) found
bal behaviors than did self-referred families. No that, in structural-strategic therapy, male thera-
information was provided, however, about the pists tended to behave in a more instrumental,
processes of change in those agency-referred directive manner by talking more and providing
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

families who were able to complete treatment


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

more explanations when family members tried


successfully. to take charge or disagreed with them than did
Second, Heatherington and Friedlander female therapists. (No differences were ob-
(1990b) examined differences in relational con- served, however, in the retention rate of families
trol patterns using the FRCCCS in couple ver- seen by male versus female therapists.) In
sus family contexts. The therapists were expe- Heatherington and Friedlander's (1990b) early
rienced staff at an outpatient family therapy sessions of systemic—strategic therapy, experi-
center of a large teaching hospital. Results enced therapists of both genders tended to en-
showed no meaningful differences by treatment gage in therapist one-up and family member
modality. Complementarity (i.e., therapist one-down complementarity (on the FRCCCS)
one-up and family member one-down) was the somewhat more frequently with female clients
predominant control pattern in both contexts. It than with male clients. Shields and McDaniel
would not be wise to conclude, however, that did not, however, report differences in thera-
there were no meaningful therapist—client dif- pists' behaviors by client gender, and Heather-
ferences in the two treatment formats. Only a ington and Friedlander did not consider the gen-
single coding system was used, and it is likely der of the therapists, most of whom were men.
that there are differences in other, possibly more Nor did either research team analyze therapist
salient, indexes of the therapeutic process. behaviors directed to husbands versus those di-
Gender. Five investigations of the relation- rected to wives within a given family.
ship between participants' gender and therapeu- When family members are the focus, evi-
tic processes have considered the degree to dence from two studies suggests that early ses-
which, early in treatment, male and female sex- sions are characterized by interactions consis-
typed behaviors are observable in family treat- tent with traditional gender roles. First, Shields
ments. This question is important because if and McDaniel (1992) found that family mem-
stereotypically masculine and feminine behav- bers were more likely to disagree among them-
iors tend to be modeled by family therapists and selves with female therapists than with male
expected of their clients (Luepnitz, 1988), ther- therapists. With male therapists, family mem-
apy may reinforce the traditional views of fam- bers were more likely to make structuring state-
ily life that are particularly damaging to women. ments. The authors concluded that "[fjamily
Findings from two studies disconfirm this members may elicit different behavior from
traditionality hypothesis. First, with respect to male versus female therapists because of their
modes of expression, Newberry, Alexander, and own gender-based expectations" (Shields &
Turner (1991) reported that, in the first session McDaniel, 1992, p. 149).
of Functional Family Therapy, male and female Second, in a study of marital therapy, Brown-
student therapists did not differ in the extent of Standridge and Piercy (1988) examined hus-
overall structuring (a stereotypically masculine bands' and wives' responses to the therapist's
behavior) or supportiveness (a stereotypically use of reflections versus reframing. Thirteen
feminine behavior) toward family members. In- couples were seen by six experienced family
deed, female therapists in this study tended to therapists; two early sessions from each treat-
respond to client supportiveness with structur- ment were selected for coding. The Brown-
ing more so than did their male counterparts. Standridge Marital Therapy Interaction Scale
Second, in an earlier study of Functional Family was used to code client behaviors in the 20 s
Therapy, male therapists in training spoke more surrounding reframing (i.e., challenge) and re-
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 397

flection (i.e., understanding) identified as "ef- Only two studies contrasted the behaviors of
fective" by the senior author and subsequently therapists as a function of their role (i.e., ther-
confirmed by another observer. apist vs. cotherapist, trainee vs. supervisor).
Brown-Standridge and Piercy (1988) found First, Dowling (1979) analyzed the verbal be-
that when the husbands' behaviors were haviors of five therapists who interacted in 10
"closed" (p. 210), the therapists tended to use different cotherapist pairs at three points in
reflections; this was not the case, however, treatment. Results showed that therapists be-
when the wives' behaviors were closed. Follow- haved similarly with different cotherapists (and
ing therapist interventions, husbands tended to with different families). Second, Pinsof (1979,
respond more positively to reframing and wives 1986) compared the interventions of eight fam-
tended to respond more positively to reflecting. ily therapy trainees with those of their supervi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

However, qualitative analyses of the therapists' sors. The procedure was unique in that the su-
written responses to a series of questions about pervisor observed the trainee's session (either
their implicit decision rales for the use of re- an intake or a second interview) from behind a
flection versus reframing showed that 80% of one-way mirror for 20 min, then joined the
the therapists denied having behaved differently trainee and conducted the remainder of the in-
with husbands and wives. terview with the trainee present but silent. Us-
Caution is necessary, however, in generaliz- ing his Family Therapist Coding System, Pinsof
ing from these results because little information (1979, 1986) compared the interventions of
was provided about the authors' criteria for trainees and supervisors and concluded that
effective reframing and reflection. The study trainees worked more intrapersonally and that
was distinguished, on the other hand, in its supervisors worked more interpersonally. That
analysis of sequential behaviors and in its com- is, whereas the trainees focused more on the
bined use of qualitative and observational data. opinions and beliefs of individual family mem-
Participant role. Two investigations con- bers, the supervisors focused more on behav-
trasted the behaviors of parents and children in ioral sequences and family communication pat-
family therapy. In Mas et al.'s (1985) study of terns. The supervisors also tended to use a wider
gender effects as well as in Friedlander et al.'s range of responses: They were more active and
(1985) comparison of the Hillcrest sessions, explicit, more here-and-now focused, and more
parents tended to speak far more often than interpretive and supportive. These differences
children. The adolescents in Mas et al. ex- cannot uniquely be attributed to therapist role,
pressed themselves in terms of agreement or however, because of differences in the observa-
disagreement more so than did their parents, tion situation as well as in therapist experience
and they tended to be more verbal with male and skill (Pinsof, 1979).
therapists than with female therapists. In the In addition to comparing the behaviors of
Hillcrest interviews, the children's participa- trainees and supervisors, Pinsof (1986) ob-
tion rates varied widely across therapists, as served changes in behavior common to both
did the degree to which they spoke about groups over the course of one session. Trainees
parent-child, sibling, or marital relations in and supervisors alike increasingly emphasized
the family. Compared with their parents, the the clients' behaviors and focused on the future,
Hillcrest children made notably fewer refer- on the parents, and on the therapy itself; they
ences to the past. also used more indirect and more cyclical inter-
Only one of the studies that examined the ventions as the session progressed.
interaction of process variables and client role None of these investigations considered the
took into account treatment outcome. Postner, sequential responding of therapist-to-family
Guttman, Sigal, Epstein, and Rakoff (1971) re- member, or vice versa, or the specific patterns
ported that in their group with good outcomes, of interactions between and among family
fathers were addressed more often. In the group members. Nor did any of these studies take into
with poor outcomes, however, mothers tended account the functional role of family members
to be the primary targets of the therapists' in- (e.g., parental child, homemaker, or income
terventions. Overall, therapists (a) spoke signif- provider). It may be that family members' func-
icantly more often to parents than to children tional roles, which reflect differences in status
and (b) tended to consistently address one par- and power, are important predictors of family
ent over the other. therapy dynamics.
398 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

Changes over treatment. In five studies, cantly predictive of outcome, welfare state-
changes in the frequency of client (Chamberlain ments among family members tended to
et al., 1984; Munton & Antaki, 1988; Winer, increase over time, demonstrating changes in
1971) and/or therapist (Cline, Mejia, Coles, family members' ability to "express affection,
Klein, & Cline, 1984; Postner et al., 1971) re- sympathy, and pleasure" (Postner et al., 1971,
sponse were observed over time. Sequences of p. 463). Family members' speech also tended to
therapist-client interactions were only ob- be less neutral and to involve increasing levels
served in two case studies of successful struc- of emergency emotions. The most significant
tural family therapy (Laird & Vande Kemp, change in emotional expression tended to occur
1987; Raymond, Friedlander, Heatherington, between the second and sixth sessions.
Ellis, & Sargent, 1993). The studies of family Two investigations (Munton & Antaki, 1988;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior are reviewed first, followed by those


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Winer, 1971) were focused not on clients' emo-


involving therapist behavior and therapist- tional states but on their cognitive change.
client interactions. In the earlier study, Winer (1971) observed
First, Chamberlain et al. (1984) studied vari- changes in the frequency of / statements by
ability in client resistance and cooperativeness couples in a multiple family group treatment
in behavioral family therapy on the basis of led by Murray Bowen. The use of the / pro-
observations of six videotaped sessions at three noun was assumed, based on Bowen's theory
points in treatment. Resistance was defined as (Bowen, 1978), to reflect an increase in differ-
interrupting or challenging the therapist, dem- entiation of self, that is, the ability to separate
onstrating a negative attitude, pursuing an indi- and balance thinking and feeling and to tolerate
vidual agenda, disqualifying another, not pay- closeness and separateness from others. Winer
ing attention, and so forth. The authors found assumed that increases in / statements along
that early resistance predicted midtreatment with decreases in the use of we, our, and us
resistance. Indeed, the highest levels of resis- pronouns would "reflect less intense, symbiotic
tance tended to occur during the middle phase involvement in the family system" (p. 245).
of therapy. Furthermore, the more highly re- Speech acts in the first two or three available
sistant families had a greater dropout rate and recordings of 4 couples were compared with
tended to be perceived as less successful by those in three later sessions. As predicted, a
their therapists. greater proportion of / statements was observed
Postner et al. (1971) also examined family in the later sessions, with more we, our, and us
verbal behavior in relation to outcome. Twenty- statements in earlier sessions. Further support-
min excerpts were analyzed at four points in ing Bowen's theory, results indicated that in the
treatment for 11 families. In addition to partic- early sessions the ratios of / to we, our, and us
ipation rates, family members' speech acts were pronouns by husbands and wives were highly
coded with respect to three emotional states: similar, "showing almost a mirror pattern"
emergency (fear, anger, anxiety, sadness, and (Winer, 1971, p. 241), whereas in later sessions
dependence), welfare (happiness, joy, tender- there were wide discrepancies, suggesting
ness, love, sympathy, and support), and neutral greater differentiation between the spouses.
(information only). Outcome was identified as Munton and Antaki (1988) compared attribu-
"good" or "poor" on the basis of the indepen- tional changes in the speech acts of their most
dent perceptions of three judges who observed successful versus their least successful families
pre- and posttreatment interviews and rated (N = 10). The global outcome of brief therapy
each family for overall change, affective in- (M = 7.7 sessions) was assessed by therapists.
volvement, affective communication, and affec- Attributional statements were extracted and
tive expression. coded from the first and last sessions of each
Results showed that, over the course of treat- treatment. These attributions were then cate-
ment, family members tended to speak increas- gorized on five dimensions: stable-unstable,
ingly among themselves and less frequently to global-specific, internal-external, personal-
the therapist. Overall, the findings suggested universal, and controllable—uncontrollable.
that the process of family therapy involves None of the authors' hypotheses was supported,
enhanced affective involvement of family although two interesting observations were
members. Although variations in emotional re- noted. First, families with poor outcomes
sponses during the therapy were not signifi- tended to perceive their problems to be stable
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 399

early and late in treatment more so than did the groups. The process data were analyzed by
families with good outcomes. Second, regard- correlating the early-to-late change scores in
less of outcome, family members tended to the couples' behaviors (MICS) with therapist
make more internal and personal attributions of modes of expression and behaviors averaged
other family members over time. Munton and across the two time periods. Outcome was
Antaki speculated that attributional change does assessed posttherapy and at 3- and 6-month
not occur in "any straightforward way" (p. 96) follow-ups by means of several client reports of
in therapy and that averaging across family marital adjustment and satisfaction. At termina-
members or across families can obscure impor- tion therapists provided an evaluation of im-
tant individual changes in attributions. provement or deterioration in 12 areas of mar-
ital functioning.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

With respect to therapist behavior, Postner et


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

al. (1971) found that therapists rarely spoke to Results indicated notable differences between
more than one family member at a time and that the husbands and the wives within each SES
therapists did not vary significantly in their group. For couples in the middle SES group,
speech rate over the course of treatment, al- therapist directiveness was negatively corre-
though therapist variability across families was lated with outcome at each assessment point.
more pronounced. Therapists spoke increas- Furthermore, directiveness was inversely asso-
ingly more often in the group with good out- ciated with increases in positive behaviors (e.g.,
comes, and they tended to focus increasingly on acceptance, agreement, approval, and accepting
a single family member, generally the most responsibility) over time. Couples in the middle
verbal parent. With respect to type of response, SES group tended to increase their expressions
all interventions were categorized by observers of feelings when the therapists were less direc-
as either drive or interpretation on the basis of tive and when they used more reflections and
intent. Drive was defined as the therapist's aim probes for affect. This pattern was, however,
to stimulate interaction, obtain information, or notably more pronounced for husbands than for
provide support, and interpretation referred to wives.
the therapist's intent to clarify underlying mo- The couples in the low SES group seemed to
tives, further understanding, generalize, or sug- have experienced the process differently. Ther-
gest alternative behaviors. The authors observed apist directiveness was predictive of increased
a significant decrease in the drive to interpreta- positive behaviors on the part of these couples,
tion ratio by therapists overall; this change was and directiveness predicted successful out-
more marked in the group with good outcome come from the wives' perspective. For the hus-
than in the group with poor outcome. In other bands, "directive questions that explore affect
words, a change in the direction of fewer drive and reflecting statements related to affect"
responses relative to interpretation responses (Cline et al., 1984, p. 702) were most predictive
may be predictive of successful treatment. of outcome.
In a large-scale marital outcome study, Cline In summary, the major difference between the
et al. (1984) tested the degree to which changes two SES groups lay in how the husbands expe-
in couples' behaviors from early to late in ther- rienced the therapy, as the patterns were more
apy could be predicted from therapist behaviors. apparent for them than for their wives. Whereas
Seventy-seven couples were treated by 19 ex- the husbands in the low SES group were seem-
perienced male therapists, each of whom was ingly affected more by understanding the dy-
assigned 2 couples each from a low and a mid- namics in their marriage without changing their
dle socioeconomic status (SES) group. Using actual communication styles, husbands in the
Alexander et al.'s (1976) rating system for ther- middle SES group may have been helped by
apist modes of expression and a coding system expressing their feelings, so that gradually the
for client behavior that was modified from the couple's communication style improved (Cline
original Marital Interaction Coding System et al., 1984).
(MICS; Hops, Wills, Patterson, & Weiss, 1972 In Cline et al.'s (1984) investigation, process
[cited in Cline et al., 1984]), 10-min excerpts variables were correlated with one another or
were sampled from the first two and the last two used to predict outcome. Although therapist di-
sessions in each treatment. Comparisons were rectiveness was significantly correlated with
made of participants' behaviors (therapists, positive client behavior for some couples, this
wives, and husbands) within each of the SES finding does not mean that directiveness was
400 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

followed by positive client behavior. In only his SASB responses tended to be "affirming,"
two studies in the group that studied change "understanding," "nurturing," "helping," and
processes over time were sequences of behavior "protecting." It is interesting that one finding
observed. Both of these investigations (Laird & could be interpreted as supportive of the gender
Vande Kemp, 1987; Raymond et al, 1993) traditionality hypothesis (see earlier section on
were case studies in which well-known thera- Gender). Laird and Vande Kemp (1987) ob-
pists at the Philadelphia Child Guidance Clinic served that Minuchin responded in a non-
treated families with an adolescent daughter complementary manner to both the mother and
suffering from anorexia nervosa. the daughter with anorexia after these women
In the earlier research, Laird and Vande used either asserting or separating responses.
Kemp (1987) tested several hypotheses about In the second case study (Raymond et al.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

changes in Salvador Minuchin's responses to 1993), John Sargent, well-known for his inpa-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

family members over time and changes in their tient work with families who have members
responses to him. Responses were identified as with chronic anorexia, conducted (with a co-
either complementary or noncomplementary on therapist) 15 sessions of family treatment over a
the basis of observers' coding of reciprocal ver- 3-week period. Based on the FRCCCS (Fried-
bal exchanges between Minuchin and family lander & Heatherington, 1989), relational con-
members in 20-min segments of an early, a trol patterns between Sargent and each of 3 key
middle, and a late session. Interpersonal inter- family members (mother, daughter, and father)
actions were identified on the basis of the cod- and among the 3 clients were observed, and
ing of reciprocal speech acts involving the MDS spatial maps (cf. Friedlander & Highlen,
Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (SASB; 1984; Friedlander et al., 1987) of each session
Benjamin, 1974). Complementarity was defined were generated on the basis of all participants'
as (a) opposing behaviors with respect to focus interactional frequencies.
(one speaker focuses on self, the respondent Consistent with earlier research (Heathering-
focuses on other) and (b) similar behaviors with ton & Friedlander, 1990b) on relational control
respect to affiliation and interdependence. The- dynamics with the FRCCCS, therapist one-up
oretically, whereas complementary responses and family member one-down complementarity
are rewarding for both parties, noncomplemen- predominated throughout this case.2 With the
tary responses are considered to be challenging; identified patient, this pattern was observed
in order to build trust and rapport in the initial more frequently in the early and late phases of
stage of therapy, therapists should respond in a treatment than in the middle, or restructuring,
complementary fashion (Carson, 1969). How- stage. The MDS maps showed that Sargent
ever, to help clients modify their maladaptive maintained a central position in the system
interpersonal styles, therapists should adopt a throughout treatment, with the cotherapist being
noncomplementary style in the middle phase of more peripheral. Hypothesized changes in with-
treatment. in-family alliances were not evident in the
Results supported Laird and Vande Kemp's maps, however.
(1987) hypotheses that (a) Minuchin would re- Overall, Raymond et al.'s (1993) findings did
spond to family members with higher levels of not confirm the majority of their theory-based
complementarity in the early and late stages of hypotheses, particularly those concerning
treatment than in the middle stage and that (b) changes in family behavior over time. Further-
therapist complementarity would be negatively more, despite the structural emphasis on fam-
associated with immediate client change. Over- ily enactment (Minuchin, Rosman, & Baker,
all, these results indicated that Minuchin's non- 1978), there was surprisingly little within-
complementary behavior, particularly in the family interaction throughout; family members
middle stage of treatment, was predictive of addressed each other, on average, only 22% of
subsequent changes in family members' re- the time.
sponses toward him. (Family members' re-
sponses toward one another were not observed.) 2
Interpersonal complementarity and relational
Indeed, the descriptive results suggested that control complementarity are not identical. In Fried-
Minuchin was more variable in his responses in lander's (1993) review of complementarity in brief
the middle stage and used more "focus on self individual and family therapy, the similarities and
responses in the termination phase. Across time, differences are discussed at length.
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 401

A close inspection of the family's relational have little known validity. In the absence of
control patterns, however, suggested that some validity information, the assumption that, for
changes in the family's rigid communication example, increased usage of personal pronouns
style may have taken place (Raymond et al., (I, mine, etc.) reflects greater differentiation of
1993). Specifically, the parents were observed self (Winer, 1971) or that increased welfare
to engage in more competitive symmetry over statements among family members (Postner et
time, possibly reflecting a struggle for control al., 1971) signal greater closeness or better re-
between them. At the same time, the competi- lations should be questioned.
tive symmetry between the daughter with an- Both case studies of structural therapy did use
orexia and each parent decreased over time. valid, theoretically based measures, the SASB
Raymond et al. (1993) speculated that these in Laird and Vande Kemp (1987) and the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

shifts might reflect less detouring or conflict FRCCCS in Raymond et al. (1993). Each inves-
avoidance on the part of the parents and a con- tigation involved the treatment of a family who
sequent "'detriangling' of the symptomatic had a child with anorexia by an expert structural
child" (p. 323). therapist. The degree of generalizability of the
Raymond et al. (1993) also tested the hypoth- results of these studies to other families, thera-
esis that, because the goal of structural treat- pists, and theoretical orientations is, however,
ment is to disrupt communication patterns that unknown.
reflect boundary violations or cross-genera- Finally, in only one of the seven studies that
tional triangulation, the therapist and cother- tested changes over time (Raymond et al., 1993)
apist would intervene immediately by inter- was every session in the treatment analyzed in
preting or confronting these behaviors or by entirety. Drawing conclusions about shifts over
educating or coaching the family toward more time when short (10-20 min) segments are
functional communication. Based on the sampled at two (Cline et al., 1984; Munton &
FRCCCS, the following speech acts were iden- Antaki, 1988; Winer, 1971), three (Chamberlain
tified: (a) attempts by the daughter to intervene et al., 1984), or four (Postner et al., 1971) points
in her parents' conversations with each other, in time is questionable (Spector, Guttman,
(b) messages from the daughter in which the Sigal, Rakoff, & Epstein, 1970). The method of
daughter supported one parent while challeng- using proportions of therapist behaviors to pre-
ing the other, or (c) messages from either parent dict proportions of client behaviors (Cline et al.,
in which the parent supported the daughter 1984; Postner et al., 1971) is also problematic
while challenging the spouse. Surprisingly, only (Shoham-Salomon, 1990). Because this kind of
three such instances were observed over the analysis does not take into account the context
course of the entire 15-session treatment. On of these behaviors or their sequential patterns
each of these occasions, however, the therapists (i.e., therapist—client-therapist interactions), the
did respond immediately, as predicted. Their results can misleadingly suggest causation
interventions either "redirected the conversa- (Pinsof, 1981).
tion, . . . provided information, direct guidance
or advice, o r . . . sought information from family
members in a highly structuring, one-up man- Therapeutic Episodes
ner" (Raymond et al., 1993, p. 322).
The question addressed in these seven studies In research at the speech act level of the
(how does change occur over time in successful therapy process, all behaviors occurring during
treatment) has been studied in a number of the sample—be it a segment, a session, or an
ways. Unfortunately, however, because each in- entire treatment—are observed. Frequencies or
vestigator adopted a different perspective on the proportions of these behaviors are compared or
therapeutic process, there is little accumulated used to predict treatment outcome. In research
knowledge to answer this important question. at the episode level, by contrast, the assumption
Only one study (Cline et al., 1984) used more is that not all speech acts are of equal impor-
than a single coding system, but the categories tance. By limiting study to behaviors that
were global and atheoretical, and the interjudge surround clinically meaningful moments, the
reliabilities were at best fair ( < .60) for the researcher hopes to elucidate the actual mecha-
majority of variables under study. Furthermore, nisms of therapeutic change (Greenberg, 1986;
with a few exceptions, the coding systems used Greenberg & Pinsof, 1986a).
402 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

An early study of marital therapy demon- ment, conducted by Salvador Minuchin at the
strates the value of this approach. De Chenne Philadelphia Child Guidance Clinic, was geared
(1973) questioned whether clients experience toward increasing Suzie's control over her cry-
greater feeling in response to the therapist or in ing and her physical condition. Twenty-five
response to their spouse. Tape recorded, hour- 400-word segments (immediately before ,and
long sessions with 9 couples were sampled from immediately after a crying moment) were
a Catholic outpatient service. Trained raters ap- compared with 27 control segments. All vid-
plied the Experiencing Scale (Klein, Mathieu, eotaped segments were transcribed and rated
Kiesler, & Gendlin, 1969) to each client state- on a variety of individual and interpersonal
ment. When experiencing levels were averaged indicators.
across speech acts, no differences were ob- Compared with the control segments, in the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

served. However, a clinically and statistically


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

segments preceding Suzie's crying, the only


significant pattern was observed when each uniquely significant precipitant was a family
client's "peak" moments of experiencing were discussion about Suzie's difficulties, illness, or
compared. The latter results supported De shortcomings. Comparisons were also under-
Chenne's hypothesis. As predicted, greater taken of the pre- and postcrying moments. Re-
emotional experiencing followed therapist sults suggested that following her crying, Suzie
statements than spousal statements. asserted her opinion more, her mother spoke
In one variant on the events approach, behav- less, and the therapist tended to become more
iors that immediately precede or follow impor- active and assertive. Over the course of treat-
tant therapeutic moments are studied. In addi- ment, the level of family discussion about
tion to De Chenne (1973), one study involved Suzie's difficulties increased, only to drop off
frequent crying (Crits-Christoph et al., 1991), sharply at the end of treatment. At follow-up,
one involved inappropriate laughter (Zuk, Suzie was reportedly asymptomatic and demon-
Boszormenyi-Nagy, & Herman, 1963), one in- strated age appropriate maturity.
volved noncompliance (Patterson & Forgatch, Crits-Christoph et al. (1991) hypothesized
1985), and one involved family conflict (Patter- that by reinforcing the daughter in her role of a
son & Chamberlain, 1988). In a second variant weak, sickly baby, the parents were able to
on the events approach, effective sessions are avoid their own conflicts. According to the au-
intensively studied, with the aim of identifying thors, in many families "a difficult social con-
clinically meaningful change processes. We text leads to the identified patient feeling bad
located three studies of this variety (Gale and unable to cope, which then may lead to the
& Newfield, 1992; Holloway, Wampold, & symptom" (Crits-Christoph et al., 1991, p. 343).
Nelson, 1990; Johnson & Greenberg, 1988). In The fact that no intrapsychic variables uniquely
still another variant on the events approach, task predicted Suzie's crying moments was offered
analysis (Rice & Greenberg, 1984), the re- as support for the family systems perspective on
searcher operationally defines a therapeutic ep- individual symptomatology.
isode with a clinically identifiable task that In Zuk et al.'s (1963) case study, a 23-year-
is successfully resolved during the session old woman with schizophrenia frequently ex-
(Greenberg, 1984, p. 124). We located two such hibited bizarre laughter that appeared to be
research programs, one concerning the expres- "unrelated in meaning to the social situation"
sion of affect in emotionally focused therapy (p. 303). To determine whether the laughter was
(EFT; Greenberg & Johnson, 1988) for couples precipitated by the woman's "intrapersonal fan-
(Greenberg, Ford, Alden, & Johnson, 1993) and tasies" (p. 303) or by the ongoing therapy situ-
one concerning family engagement in problem ation, Zuk et al. (two of whom were the thera-
solving (Friedlander, Heatherington, Johnson, pists) calculated the frequency of laughter for
& Skowron, 1994; Heatherington & Fried- each family member along with the time inter-
lander, 1990a). val of the laughter, the "trigger person," and the
Important moments. In Crits-Christoph et recipient of the laughter.
al.'s (1991) case study of family therapy, Results showed that, over the course of 13
"Suzie," a 17-year-old with superlabile diabe- sessions, the amount of laughter varied system-
tes, exhibited a symptom of frequent, immature atically by person, by time interval, and by
crying that seemed related to her going into trigger. The mother and father tended to laugh
diabetic acidosis. The 10-session family treat- most in the first 15 min of the session, whereas
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 403

the daughter laughed most in the third 15-min Effective sessions. Three studies of marital
interval. Zuk et al. (1963) concluded that the therapy were located in which effective sessions
seemingly bizarre laughter of the daughter with were intensively analyzed. The earliest investi-
schizophrenia was not devoid of meaning and gation was an observational study of brief EFT
was, on some level, a reaction to the therapy with 6 couples. Johnson and Greenberg (1988)
session. (The daughter was least anxious in the examined change processes in the "best" ses-
initial 15-min segments, when structuring of the sions, as rated by therapists. Three couples re-
session took place, but she was most anxious in ported improvement in marital satisfaction post-
the last 15 min, when deeper thoughts and feel- treatment, whereas 3 couples reported minimal
ings tended to be explored.) Alternatively, the improvement. All verbal statements in the latter
reversal of patterns of laughter between the half of the six sessions were coded with respect
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to quality of interaction on the SASB (Ben-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

parents and their daughter suggested that


"laughter was a device by which the family jamin, 1974) and level of experiencing on the
members monitored each other's tension or Experiencing Scale (Klein et al., 1969).
anxiety states" (Zuk et al., 1963, p. 312). Comparisons showed proportionately more
In a series of six case studies of behavior affiliative and autonomous responses among the
therapy for serious child management problems, more successful couples, along with higher lev-
Patterson and Forgatch (1985) coded all in- els of emotional experiencing. Further analyses
stances of parental noncompliance (mothers showed that, in the successful group, the spouse
only). Results confirmed their hypotheses; In taking the "blaming position" (Johnson &
the majority of cases, the conditional probabil- Greenberg, 1988, p. 179) scored significantly
ity of noncompliance following therapist teach higher on the Experiencing Scale and used
or confront interventions was significantly proportionately more affiliative, autonomous
higher than was the base-rate probability. In responses. The latter results supported the au-
contrast, in all 6 dyads, therapist support or thors' theoretically based hypothesis that "soft-
facilitate interventions were followed by de- ening" (Johnson & Greenberg, 1988, p. 181) is
creases in noncompliance. Mixed results were an important change process in couples treat-
found for the therapist's use of reframe inter- ment, one that may distinguish successful from
ventions. These responses were followed by unsuccessful cases. In EFT, softening occurs
significantly increased noncompliance in 4 fam- "when a blaming dominant spouse accesses vul-
ilies but by significantly decreased noncompli- nerability and asks for closeness or comfort
ance in another family. from a previously distant partner" (Johnson &
In a larger sample from the same child man- Greenberg, 1988, p. 176).
agement project, Patterson and Chamberlain The two remaining studies of effective ses-
(1988) identified moments of "mother and fa- sions concerned systemic—strategic therapy.
ther within-session conflict" (p. 217). The re- First, Holloway et al. (1990) analyzed a suc-
sults of path analyses suggested that "forces cessful session with a paradoxical prescription
outside the therapy session such as parental for insomnia. The therapist, Gerald Weeks, is
stressors, marital conflict, and depression ap- well-known for his work with paradox. His in-
pear to increase the within-session conflict tervention involved refraining the wife's insom-
among family members" (Patterson & Cham- nia to reflect the couple's interpersonal dynam-
berlain, 1988, p. 216). This is the only study we ics. (An earlier prescription to change her
located in which outside-of-therapy behaviors sleeplessness to voluntary control had been un-
were used to predict in-session behaviors. Of successful.) Using a measure of "power and
more importance, perhaps, was that greater in- involvement" in the manifest and latent content
session conflict was directly associated with of verbal behavior (Penman, 1980), Holloway
higher antisocial scores for the children (a com- et al. coded all interactions during three phases
posite of teacher, parent, and child self-report of the session: before, during, and after the
ratings and home observation data). Patterson intervention. Base rates were obtained, and se-
and Chamberlain concluded that an essential quential analyses were conducted.
aspect in the treatment of such children is to Results indicated differences in the couple's
design interventions that focus on reducing pa- interactions before and after the intervention as
rental stressors and marital conflict outside the well as changes in their interactional patterns
session. with the therapist. Specifically, the couple's
404 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

negative symmetry was eliminated by the end (1993) were drawn from a previously reported
of the intervention, to be replaced by positive outcome study.
symmetrical behaviors. The apparent power In the first study, conflict events were iden-
struggle between Weeks and the husband "de- tified by specific markers—SASB (Benjamin,
escalated" (Holloway et al., 1990, p. 385) 1974) codes indicating that the couple was en-
following the intervention. Indeed, both part- gaging in negative complementarity or negative
ners engaged in more deferential complementa- reciprocity—followed by a therapist interven-
rity with the therapist, suggesting a willingness tion focusing on needs or emotions. In a quasi-
to collaborate with him. Holloway et al. con- experimental design, 20-min change events
cluded that the effectiveness of the intervention, were identified and coded in entirety beginning
as reflected in the observed interactional pat- with the operationally defined marker. Results
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

terns, was due to the therapist "playing to [the showed that couples' (N = 22) behaviors were
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

husband's] need for power . . . to elicit a col- significantly more supportive, affirming, and
laboration that facilitated rather than obstructed understanding in Session 7 than in Session 2,
the therapeutic endeavor" (p. 399). According with a significant reduction in "hostile control-
to Weeks, for 6 months following this session, ling" behaviors later in treatment (Greenberg et
the wife's insomnia did not recur, and the ther- al., 1993, p. 83).
apy shifted to the couple's underlying relation- In the second study, a within-couple (N = 16)
ship problems (Holloway et al., 1990). design was used to compare conflict events in
Second, in contrast to Weeks's problem fo- "peak" versus "poor" EFT sessions (Greenberg
cus, a successful session of solution-focused et al., 1993, p. 80). Sessions were selected on
therapy conducted by Bill O'Hanlon was ana- the basis of clients' postsession ratings. The
lyzed by Gale and Newfield (1992). Using con- marker for these conflict events was a sequence
versation analysis to identify "the paralinguistic of three "negative talk" speaking turns by the
features of the talk as well as the structural couple, followed by a therapist response aimed
sequencing of the various turn-takings in the toward exploring one partner's underlying af-
conversation" (p. 154), Gale and Newfield ex- fect. The event was defined as a 15-20-min
tracted themes to describe O'Hanlon's strate- period beginning with the therapist intervention.
gies, the interpersonal dynamics of the session, Results showed deeper experiencing levels in
and the participants' expressed agendas. From the peak sessions, along with proportionately
these themes the authors developed categories more affiliative and self-focused positive state-
that were then subjected to the scrutiny of other ments (e.g., "disclosing, expressing, approach-
researchers. Nine categories of therapist inter- ing, and enjoying"; Greenberg et al., 1993,
ventions, obtained by the use of this qualitative p. 83).
method, reflected O'Hanlon's ways of focusing In the third study of the series, Greenberg et
the session on solutions. Specifically, he con- al. (1993) isolated "intimate self-disclosure"
strained the couple's dialogue by (a) pursuing a (p. 82) change events in sessions that had been
response over many speaking turns, (b) clarify- rated moderately positive by the couple and the
ing unclear references, (c) modifying an asser- therapist. The event was defined as five speak-
tion until receiving the expected response, (d) ing turns by the spouse who was the recipient of
posing and answering questions himself, (e) ig- an intimate disclosure, preceded by a feeling-
noring misunderstandings, (f) interrupting, (g) based interchange between the therapist and
reformulating, (h) incorporating an answer in the disclosing spouse. Comparing the change
his question, and (i) using humor to switch from events with control segments that occurred prior
a problem theme to a solution theme. to the event, Greenberg et al. observed signifi-
Change events. In contrast to Johnson and cantly more affiliative responses in the change
Greenberg's (1988) research on effective ses- events than in the control episodes and signifi-
sions, in more recent research Greenberg et al. cantly more disaffiliative responses in the con-
(1993) sought to identify specific change events trol segments.
within sessions that could be linked to success- Taken together, the results of these three stud-
ful outcome in EFT couples treatment. This ies provide support for the major tenets of
approach focuses on the expression of affect EFT—that is, that change comes about through
and averages 8 to 10 sessions. Data for all three the expression of underlying needs and feel-
investigations reported in Greenberg et al. ings, which, in turn, allows spouses to gradually
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 405

become more responsive to one another tribution to the interpersonal impasse, commu-
behaviorally. nication of thoughts and feelings about the
Researchers interested in a different change impasse, validation of those feelings by other
event, one dealing not with affective expression family members, development of new construc-
but with interpersonal control, reached a similar tions of one another's behavior, and recognition
conclusion about the importance of emotional of the potential benefits of engagement. The
disclosures. To illustrate how task analysis authors described various strategies and inter-
could be used to study interpersonal behavior ventions that the therapists in the successful
change in structural therapy, Heatherington and sample used to facilitate sustained engagement
Friedlander (1990a) analyzed two consultation among family members.
Critique. In the 11 studies reviewed above,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

interviews by Salvador Minuchin. The authors


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

described the therapeutic task in both sessions intensive analyses were conducted either of en-
as "commitment to engage" (Heatherington & tire sessions that master therapists perceived to
Friedlander, 1990a, p. 40) because, in each case, be effective (Gale & Newfield, 1992; Holloway
the clinical work involved breaking an unpro- et al., 1990) or of theoretically important mo-
ductive pursue—distance cycle that was hinder- ments or episodes that occurred within sessions.
ing family members' ability to engage effec- The latter were selected by the investigators,
tively with one another in problem solving. In who believed that something clinically mean-
both change events, the interpersonal impasse ingful was occurring. Some of these moments
(the marker) was significantly modified such involved emotion—laughter (Zuk et al., 1963),
that the parents no longer pursued and the son crying (Crits-Christoph et al., 1991), or peak
no longer distanced. Rather, each boy began to experiencing (De Chenne, 1973)—on the part of
express his feelings to his parent(s) voluntarily a single family member. Other moments were
and deeply. Observational coding of the fami- considered meaningful because the therapists
ly's relational control dynamics based on the were working with problematic interpersonal
FRCCCS (Friedlander & Heatherington, 1989) behaviors—resistance (Patterson & Forgatch,
in the two events showed that interactional pat- 1985), conflict (Greenberg et al., 1993; Johnson
terns changed notably over time. In one session, & Greenberg, 1988; Patterson & Chamberlain,
for example, the initial competitive symmetry 1988), or disengagement (Friedlander et al.,
between father and son shifted to complemen- 1994; Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990a).
tarity, whereas the parents' interactions with The major issue to consider when critiquing
one another shifted from complementarity to this kind of research is whether the therapeutic
competitive symmetry. processes described in these studies are, indeed,
In a subsequent field study, Friedlander et al. meaningful (Pinsof, 1989). Only three of the
(1994) proposed that sustaining engagement is a studies in this group (Friedlander et al., 1994;
change event common to many approaches to Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990a; Greenberg
family therapy. An inductive method was used et al., 1993) could be considered change event
to operationally define family disengagement in research (Greenberg, 1986) in the sense that (a)
the marker phase of each session. Sustaining specific family behaviors were markers (Rice &
engagement was operationalized as "a sequence Greenberg, 1984) for the therapist's interven-
of speaking turns in which family members are tion and (b) these behaviors were observably
observably willing to disclose thoughts or feel- modified during the episode itself. To support
ings on [a] designated topic, to share or coop- their conclusion that meaningful change had
erate, to show interest and involvement in the taken place, Greenberg et al. (1993) contrasted
discussion, or to be responsive and attentive each successful disclosure episode with a con-
(i.e., emotionally present)" (Friedlander et al., trol segment. Friedlander et al. (1994) com-
1994, p. 442). Based on a qualitative compari- pared successful engagement events with un-
son of four successful and four unsuccessful successful events, that is, those that began
sustaining engagement events, a conceptual similarly but concluded with no observable
model of the process of change was created. change in behavior. With respect to outcome,
This model included a series of five interrelated only Greenberg et al. (1993) selected sessions
steps that seemed necessary for family members for analysis based on family members' views of
to move from disengagement to sustained en- their importance, and only Johnson and Green-
gagement: acknowledgment of one's own con- berg (1988) and Greenberg et al. (1993) ob-
406 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

jectively assessed the ultimate success of the process (Sluzki, 1975). Indeed, in the 1970s
treatments they observed. In the absence of prominent systemic theorists (e.g., Selvini-
information about immediate or ultimate out- Palazzoli, Boscolo, Cecchin, & Prata, 1978)
comes, we cannot be certain that the episodes were warning family therapists to behave in a
selected by the other investigators were indeed strictly neutral fashion and to remain unrespon-
meaningful for the family members or that they sive to family members' needs for approval and
characterize what goes on in successful family liking.
therapy (Pinsof, 1981). Despite the conclusions of Gurman and Kni-
One of the positive aspects of most of the skern in their 1978 review of literature that "the
episode-based studies, particularly the more re- ability of the therapist to establish a positive
cent ones, is that the theoretical approach of the relationship with his or her clients, long a cen-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

therapy was clearly identified. Results of these


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tral issue of individual therapy, receives the


studies not only provide evidence to support most consistent support as an important out-
theoretical tenets—EFT in Greenberg et al. come-related therapist factor to marital and
(1993), structural therapy in Heatherington and family therapy" (p. 875), there have been only
Friedlander (1990a), and strategic therapy in six published studies at the relationship level of
Holloway et al. (1990)—but also support more the therapeutic process that met our inclusion
general systems notions (e.g., Crits-Christoph et criteria.3 One investigation (Shapiro, 1974)
al., 1991; Patterson & Chamberlain, 1988; Zuk concerned therapist responsiveness, while the
et al., 1963) and provide information about others (Bourgeois, Sabourin, & Wright, 1990;
strategies to facilitate meaningful interpersonal Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990b, 1990c;
change in the therapy session itself (Friedlander Holtzworth-Munroe, Jacobson, DeKlyen, &
et al., 1994; Gale & Newfield, 1992; Greenberg Whisman, 1989; Pinsof & Catherall, 1986;
et al., 1993; Holloway et al., 1990). Sigal, Rakoff, & Epstein, 1967) concerned col-
Finally, for the most part, the coding systems laboration—or the therapeutic alliance—as per-
used in these studies were reliable, valid, and ceived by therapists, family members, or both.
theoretically meaningful, and two investigations Responsiveness. A study of therapist re-
(Friedlander et al., 1994; Gale & Newfield, sponsiveness (Shapiro, 1974) is included in this
1992) provide different models of conducting review because Shapiro investigated therapists'
clinically rich qualitative research. However, perceptions of their relationship with a particu-
since in all but two of the studies (Greenberg et lar family, not their perceptions of themselves
al., 1993; Patterson & Chamberlain, 1988) as therapists in general (cf. Rice et al., 1974).
fewer than 10 families were sampled, the au- That is, Shapiro asked therapists who evaluated
thors' claims should be considered cautiously. families (N = 27) to complete a questionnaire
indicating their emotional responses to the fam-
ily as a whole. As predicted, greater responsive-
Therapeutic Relationship ness on the part of the therapist predicted con-
tinuance beyond the initial evaluation phase.
At the third, most global level of the process Families who were viewed less positively by the
is the therapeutic relationship. Despite the as- therapists tended to reject treatment after this
sumption that the relationship between therapist phase.
and family system is an essential component of Therapeutic alliance. Shapiro's (1974) con-
the therapeutic process, it has been virtually cern with therapists' responsiveness to the fam-
ignored in the published empirical literature. ily is unique among the published studies at the
Furthermore, the ingredients of the relationship relationship level of the therapeutic process.
considered to be most predictive of outcome by The remaining studies in this section were con-
individual theorists and researchers—empathy,
acceptance, unconditional regard, and so on— 3
have been rarely mentioned in the clinical liter- Many of the studies referenced by Gurman and
ature. Rather than warm responsiveness, family Kniskern (1978) were unpublished dissertations,
which we excluded from the present review because
theorists have emphasized the systemic and of their relative inaccessibility. Others either did not
strategic aspects of the relationship, for exam- meet our inclusion criteria or were not at the rela-
ple, joining with the family (Minuchin & tionship level of Greenberg's (1986) model of the
Fishman, 1981) or attending to the coalitionary therapeutic process.
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 407

cerned with collaboration rather than with emo- levels. The dimensions refer to the nature of the
tional involvement. In the earliest study of this alliance as a strong emotional bond with mutu-
group, Sigal et al. (1967) sampled a heteroge- ally agreed upon goals and tasks. The tripartite
neous group of 19 families, all but one of whom aspect of the alliance refers to the following
were seeking help with a child. Therapists pro- levels: (a) whole system alliance (therapist al-
vided written perceptions of the patterns of col- lies with the family members as a group), (b)
laboration in the session after the 2nd, 6th, and subsystem alliance (therapist allies with various
12th interviews. Judges then used these descrip- subsystems, e.g., the marital dyad), and (c) in-
tions to score each family member's alliance dividual alliance (therapist allies with each in-
with every other family member and with the dividual in the family). Theoretically, no single
level of the alliance should be considered in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

therapist on a 6-point scale (0 = resistance, 3 =


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cooperation without engagement, and 5 = ac- isolation. When the three levels are attended to
tive engagement). A composite score was com- simultaneously, a determination can be made as
puted and used as a predictor of individual and to whether the alliance with the family is intact
family functioning 14 months after treatment (all family members feel positively toward
began. Results suggested that none of these the therapist and the therapy) or split (there
composite scores predicted outcome but that is notable variability in individual family
collaboration did tend to improve over time. members' feelings; Pinsof & Catherall, 1986,
While all of the "very high interactors" (Sigal et p. 139).
al., 1967, p. 225) in Session 2 had successful In preliminary work developing their alliance
outcomes, 80% of the families who terminated scale, which has parallel versions for individu-
prematurely had scores in Session 2 that fell als, couples, and families, Pinsof and Catherall
below the median. (1986) reported that clients, in general, tended
In a more recent study of marital therapy, to rate their therapists quite favorably. Citing
Holtzworth-Munroe et al. (1989) sought to de- unpublished research, the authors also reported
termine whether relational factors predicted (a) significantly positive correlations between
successful response to treatment. Thirty-two alliance ratings and therapist-rated outcome and
White couples who were treated with a social (b) little variation in couples' ratings over the
learning-based approach by 13 therapists who first eight sessions of treatment.
varied in experience level completed a rating Two subsequent investigations have used
form at the conclusion of each session and the Pinsof and CatheraH's (1986) alliance scales
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976) (Bourgeois et al., 1990; Heatherington & Fried-
pre- and posttreatment. Therapists completed a lander, 1990b, 1990c). Heatherington and
parallel version of the rating form after each Friedlander (1990b, 1990c) administered either
session. Results showed that, from the thera- the couple or the family version of the instru-
pists' perspective, successful outcome was re- ment to all family members over age 10 imme-
lated to their perceptions of having been effec- diately after an early session of systemic-
tive in creating a collaborative atmosphere and strategic therapy. Four empirical questions were
to their perceptions of the clients as "behaving addressed. First, results snowed that client rat-
in a facilitative manner" (Holtzworth-Munroe et ings were normally distributed, and no differ-
al., 1989, p. 661). Similar results were found ences were observed between couples and fam-
from the clients' perspective. For both husbands ilies or between male and female participants.
and wives, greater marital satisfaction posttreat- Second, patterns of association were observed
ment was predicted from greater participation in between the alliance measures and the Session
treatment and greater perceived collaboration Evaluation Questionnaire (Stiles, 1980), a well-
with therapeutic assignments. established measure of immediate therapeutic
The three remaining studies in this section impact. Among families, stronger bonds and
used a theoretically based instrument (Pinsof & greater agreement on tasks were correlated with
Catherall, 1986) to measure collaboration. Ex- smoothness and ease of the session. Among
trapolating from Bordin's (1979) view of the couples, greater agreement on tasks was asso-
therapeutic alliance in individual psychother- ciated with perceived depth or value of the
apy, Pinsof and Catherall (1986) conceptualized session. Third, the authors questioned how fre-
the therapeutic alliance in family therapy as quently split alliances, as described by Pinsof
multidimensional and occurring on multiple and Catherall (1986), occur in couples and fam-
408 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

ily therapy. On the basis of a 1.0 standard de- for them to feel understood and for the tasks and
viation difference between individual family objectives to be clearly stated and agreed on in
members' ratings, 43% of couples and 75% of the early sessions of the intervention program"
families were classified as experiencing a split (Bourgeois et al., 1990, p. 613).
alliance with the therapist. On the basis of a Critique. In the earliest studies of the rela-
more conservative decision rule (2.0 SDs), 14% tionship, Shapiro (1974) asked therapists to rate
of couples and 42% of families were so classi- their responsiveness to the family as a whole,
fied. Fourth, the authors tested associations be- and Sigal et al. (1967) combined each family
tween client-therapist verbal interactions—that member's collaboration score to create a single
is, complementarity and symmetry (as coded variable. The relational measures in these early
on the FRCCCS; Friedlander & Heatherington, studies were global and vague, with little psy-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1989)—and family members' perceptions of the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

chometric support. Furthermore, because evi-


therapeutic alliance. Their predictions were not dence from later studies (Bourgeois et al., 1990;
supported, although one interesting result ap- Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990c) in which a
proached significance. Clients tended to view more psychometrically sound measure was used
their personal alliance with the therapist some- (Pinsof & Catherall, 1986) suggests that the
what less favorably when they themselves were therapeutic alliance does not develop uniformly
more frequently in a complementary one-up for all members of the family, global family
relational control position with the therapist ratings and combined scores can be misleading.
one-down. Averaging scores across sessions, as did Holtz-
Heatherington and Friedlander (1990c) con- worth-Munroe et al. (1989), also obscures in-
sidered speech acts within the context of the formation about variability in the developing
alliance. By contrast, Bourgeois et al. (1990) alliance for different family members.
tested perceptions of the therapeutic alliance in Indeed, few investigators have assessed how
relation to client outcome. Assessing 63 mari- the relationship develops over time or how in-
tally distressed couples in time-limited group dividual client or therapist differences affect the
treatment with a social learning and humanistic developing therapeutic alliance. While some re-
focus, Bourgeois et al. (1990) reported that sults suggest that client gender may play a part
Pinsof and Catherall's (1986) couples alliance in this process, none of the investigators who
scale was significantly predictive of treatment considered client gender (Bourgeois et al.,
success as measured by the DAS and two au- 1990; Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990c;
thor-constructed questionnaires: an individual- Holtzworth-Munroe et al., 1989) took into ac-
ized measure of conflictual topics and a general count the therapist's gender. Consequently, lit-
measure of marital happiness. In addition to tle information exists of how effective therapists
confirming the predictive validity of the alli- develop same- and opposite-sex alliances si-
ance scale (administered to all couples after multaneously when working with a couple or
their third group session), Bourgeois et al. pro- family.
vided some important information about indi-
vidual client differences and therapists' percep-
tions of the alliance. Specifically, although Implications
pretreatment marital distress was not predictive
of the quality of the alliance, either for the Despite the abundant outcome research, sur-
couple or for the therapist, client gender was an prisingly few family therapy process studies
important predictor. Whereas for women alli- have been published. Compared with the hun-
ance scores contributed only moderately to one dreds—if not thousands—of process studies of
aspect of outcome, marital adjustment (i.e., the individual therapy, we located only 36 family
DAS), for men the perceived alliance signifi- studies that met our inclusion criteria. Of these
cantly predicted adjustment as well as two other investigations, which date from 1963, the ma-
indexes of marital harmony. The authors inter- jority (n = 19) are descriptive studies at the
preted their unexpected gender effect with re- speech act level of the therapeutic process. Only
spect to the impact of traditional sex role so- 5 of the speech act studies included an evalu-
cialization on men's willingness to engage in ation of client outcome; 9 of them examined
marital and group treatment. They concluded the work of master theorists or therapists in
that, for men to be successful, it is "important the absence of objective outcome information.
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 409

Studies designed to test theories of change in 1992). Men and women seem to experience the
family systems are only now emerging. Indeed, therapeutic process differently (Bourgeois et al.,
there are relatively fewer studies at the episode 1990; Cline et al., 1984).
or relationship level, levels that are arguably 5. Changes over the course of family therapy
more meaningful for the practicing family ther- as well as over time in a "good" session tend to
apist. In most of those investigations, speech be affective (Cline et al., 1984; Greenberg et al.,
acts were observed within specific therapeutic 1993; Johnson & Greenberg, 1988; Postner et
episodes or in relation to the therapeutic alliance al., 1971) and cognitive (Friedlander et al.,
(see Table 1). No studies were located, how- 1994; Winer, 1971) as well as behavioral (e.g.,
ever, in which all three levels in Greenberg's Sigal et al., 1967). These changes are observ-
(1986) process model were investigated simul- able not only in interpersonal relations (Green-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

taneously. berg et al., 1993; Johnson & Greenberg, 1988;


Although we organized the 36 process studies Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990a; Holloway
by contextual unit, our major aim in conducting et al., 1990; Laird & Vande Kemp, 1987;
this review was to synthesize and integrate the Raymond et al., 1993) but also within individ-
substantive findings. The following are some uals (Friedlander et al., 1994; Munton &
general conclusions about we do "know" about Antaki, 1988; Winer, 1971).
the process of conjoint family therapy. 6. Although early family theorists did not
1. Consistent with the systems perspective, focus on emotional states, evidence at all levels
there is some evidence to suggest (a) that when of the therapeutic process suggests that affec-
individuals' symptoms are observed during tive changes among family members are of cru-
family therapy sessions, they occur in the con- cial significance in family therapy (Cline et
text of predictable interpersonal events (e.g., al., 1984; Greenberg et al., 1993; Johnson &
family disagreements; Crits-Christoph et al., Greenberg, 1988; Postner et al., 1971).
1991; Zuk et al., 1963), and (b) that greater 7. Relatedly, family members' feelings about
stress outside of therapy is reflected in family the therapist (Bourgeois et al., 1990), and his or
members' communications during the session her responsiveness toward them (Shapiro,
(Patterson & Chamberlain, 1988). 1974), seem to be important, but often over-
2. For the most part, different family therapy looked, sources of gain in family therapy.
approaches are reflected in different kinds of 8. As in individual therapy, the family's level
therapeutic interventions and participant activ- of cooperativeness, willingness to engage in
ity rates, although there may be more common- problem solving, and sense of collaboration
alities across approaches than the clinical liter- with the therapist are predictive of session ef-
ature suggests (Friedlander & Highlen, 1984; fectiveness (Friedlander et al., 1994; Heather-
Friedlander et al., 1985, 1987, 1991). ington & Friedlander, 1990c; Holloway et al.,
3. Family therapists tend to be active and to 1990), continuation in treatment, and therapeu-
assume a dominant (Gale & Newfield, 1992; tic outcome (Alexander et al., 1976; Chamber-
Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990b), central lain et al., 1984; Holtzworth-Munroe et al.,
position in their work with families (Friedlander 1989; Shields et al., 1991). The strength of the
et al., 1987; Raymond et al., 1993). Many of therapeutic alliance is not, however, identical
their interventions are skillful indirect commu- for all family members (Bourgeois et al., 1990;
nications (Friedlander et al., 1985, 1987, 1991; Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990c; Pinsof &
Holloway et al., 1990; Pinsof, 1986). Family Catherall, 1986).
therapists tend to behave fairly consistently In our search for relevant research, we found
with different families (Friedlander et al., 1987) studies in a wide range of journals representing
and with different cotherapists (Dowling, various literature bases, from human communi-
1979). cations and counseling, clinical, and social psy-
4. While family therapists may not overtly chology to marital and family therapy proper.
behave in gender stereotypic ways, some of the While this diverse interest among scholars is
more subtle, interactive aspects of their com- heartening, researchers in one discipline seem
munication patterns with family members do not to be fully aware of the work of scholars in
reflect gender-based expectancies (Brown- other disciplines. The present review was un-
Standridge & Piercy, 1988; Heatherington & dertaken to identify, synthesize, and integrate
Friedlander, 1990b; Shields & McDaniel, the findings from all of these sources and, in so
410 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

doing, to offer guidelines for the advancement tasks in family therapy, so that family members'
of research on the process of change in family behaviors that are most facilitative of enduring
therapy. In the concluding section of this arti- change can be identified.
cle, we provide several recommendations, Relatedly, not much is known about effective
which are concerned not only with the sub- therapy from the family's perspective (Pinsof,
stance of future efforts but also with methods 1989; Wynne, 1988). Most research on the cli-
for conducting clinically relevant research on ent's role in the family therapy change process
family therapy. has been concerned with observable behavior
rather than with covert emotions and cognitions
What We Don't Know (Pinsof, 1988). With few exceptions, research-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ers have analyzed families and family therapy


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

In the process of integrating the results from interactions objectively—-from above—just as


the existing literature, we formulated several one might study, from some elevated platform,
generalizations about what there is yet to be the sequences of moves on a soccer field lead-
learned about effective family therapy. These ing to a goal. For the most part, observer ratings
generalizations are presented below, along with have been used to operationalize emotional
suggestions to redress the apparent gaps in the states (e.g., Johnson & Greenberg, 1988; Post-
literature. ner et al., 1971), personality changes (Winer,
1971), resistance (Chamberlain et al., 1984),
collaboration (Friedlander et al., 1994; Sigal et
What Do Family Members Do, Think, or al., 1967), and intimacy (Greenberg et al.,
Feel in Effective Family Therapy? 1993).
In virtually all of the major theories of family This preference for objective methods over
therapy, much more attention is paid to what phenomenological ones makes sense given the
the therapist "does" to the clients to promote early theoretical work on the pragmatics of fam-
change than to what family members "do"— ily communication (e.g., Watzlawick, Beavin,
behaviorally, cognitively, or emotionally & Jackson, 1967) and the emphasis on observ-
(Kuehl et al, 1990; Reiss, 1988). This trend is able behavior in the systemic, structural, and
mirrored in the available research. Although strategic therapies that evolved from these
results from several studies suggest that various theories. Certainly, observational studies have
therapeutic interventions affect family members provided much useful information about the
in predictable ways, few studies have been con- process of family therapy. Observer ratings,
cerned with describing the stages of successful however, provide only a single viewpoint. More
client performances. In the cognitive realm, for research is needed to discover how family mem-
example, several investigators have focused on bers themselves construe the effective ingredi-
strategies such as paradox (Holloway et al., ents of family therapy, that is, how they per-
1990), interpretation (Postner et al., 1971), or ceive various therapeutic events as helping or
refraining (Brown-Standridge & Piercy, 1988) hindering the process of change (Wynne, 1988).
for changing family members' beliefs about
their problems, but only one published study Indeed, there exist methodologies that can
has concerned itself with actual changes in provide answers to these questions. The method
clients' belief systems (Munton & Antaki, of interpersonal process recall, which has fruit-
1988), and no studies have concerned the in- fully been applied to individual therapy by
teractional process that leads to such cognitive Robert Elliott (1984), Clara E. Hill (Hill et al.,
changes. 1988), and Jack Martin (Martin, Martin, &
One promising method for the identification Slemon, 1987), among others, could be used to
of processes of change that involve clients' be- study family members' perspectives on the pro-
haviors as well as therapists' is the task analysis cess of therapy. Alternatively, less labor-inten-
paradigm (Rice & Greenberg, 1984), which has sive methods, such as postsession interviews or
recently been applied to couples (Greenberg et questionnaires, could be used to provide an un-
al., 1993) and family therapy (Friedlander et al., derstanding about just what does and does
1994; Heatherington & Friedlander, 1990a). not work in family therapy from the family's
Clearly, more research is needed on specific perspective.
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 411

How Does Change Occur in the 1988). These kinds of questions have tradition-
Nonbehavioral Approaches? ally lent themselves to outcome research, yet
they are increasingly—and convincingly—be-
As in individual therapy, the family therapy ing linked with process variables in individual
approaches that focus on overt behaviors have therapy (Sue & Zane, 1987). Recent process
been the most researched. Our review suggests research by Zane (personal communication,
that the bulk of research has been conducted April 1993), for example, suggests that the level
on the structural, strategic, and systemic thera- of early self-disclosure that is generally encour-
pies (cf. Gurman et al., 1986), with relatively aged by therapists may be appropriate for Anglo
less attention to the psychoanalytic, experien- clients but is excessively intimate for some
Asian and Asian-American clients, who, by vir-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tial, family-of-origin, or newer constructivist


tue of their culture, tend to be particularly con-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

approaches.
There is, nonetheless, fertile material for cerned with "face" in relationships. To expect
study in the nonbehavioral approaches (Gur- similar therapeutic processes with clients of dif-
man, 1988). The circular questioning tech- ferent cultural backgrounds may impede the
niques, for example, that are popular in the therapy, causing rifts in the alliance, premature
Milan and other constructivist therapies are eas- termination, or worse.
ily identified. Theoretically, these therapies are Many theorists are indeed concerned with
geared toward accomplishing certain tasks in issues of diversity in family therapy (e.g.,
the session. Is there any evidence to suggest that McGoldrick, Pearce, & Giordano, 1982;
they do so? Even constructivist theorists who Szapocznik & Kurtines, 1993), and their writ-
eschew simple, linear prescriptions for success- ings provide important sources from which to
ful therapy have made statements that can identify testable hypotheses. Researchers inter-
be operationalized and tested as hypotheses. ested in diversity would also do well to read and
Anderson and Goolishian (1992), for example, carefully critique family theories, especially
described "[t]he therapeutic conversation [as] a with regard to how theorists' assumptions and
two-way exchange, a criss-crossing of ideas . . . strategies may affect families from different
in which new meanings are continually evolv- cultures (cf. Tamura & Lau, 1992). One obvious
ing toward the 'dis-solving' of problems" (p. question is how different styles of family ther-
27). Indeed, is therapeutic discourse a "two-way apy (e.g., more or less active, directive, collab-
exchange, a criss-crossing of ideas"? Are "new orative, strategic, interpretive, etc.) intersect
meanings" expressed by family members over with the cultural or ethnic values that families
the course of therapy and, if so, how do these bring to the therapy room. It is reasonable to
meanings relate to the family's treatment goals? expect that cultural values differentially affect
Constructivist therapies are growing rapidly in family members' expectations for the thera-
popularity yet remain virtually unstudied. pist's behavior, their communication patterns in
the session, and their perceptions of the thera-
peutic alliance.
Diversity Issues in Family Therapy Regarding gender, the research reviewed here
suggests equivocal findings about bias in family
With the exception of the continuing interest therapy. It is interesting to note that the least
in gender effects, there has been little or no overt and the most interactional measures
research to date on issues of diversity in family of verbal behavior (in, for example, Brown-
therapy. In contrast to the relatively larger lit- Standridge & Piercy, 1988) were more likely to
erature base on diversity in individual therapy turn up evidence of gender differences in thera-
(e.g., Garfield, 1986), family therapists lack ba- pist-family communications. Indeed, it would
sic information about the demographics (race, be surprising if there were no such differences,
ethnicity, SES, or family structure) of family given evidence in sociolinguistics (Tannen,
therapy—who is referred for and who com- 1990), social psychology (Aries, 1987), and de-
pletes therapy (i.e., dropout rates), who is most velopmental psychology (e.g., Maccoby, 1990)
likely to benefit from or be harmed by family about the influence of gender on discourse pat-
therapy, and what family—therapist matchings terns in nontherapy contexts. Increased research
on demographic or cultural variables, if any, is needed in this area, including studies to ex-
facilitate progress in family therapy (Goldstein, amine the influence of gender on intrafamily
412 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

interactions (e.g., husband-wife, mother-chil- Yet more remains to be done. As one example,
dren, father-children, and sister-brother) dur- consider the recent work on the therapeutic al-
ing therapy, particularly as those family inter- liance. Despite recognition of its importance in
actions affect and are affected by the therapist's family therapy and the development of a self-
behavior. The politics of gender in cotherapy is report instrument for its measurement (Pinsof &
a second important area for study. Despite the Catherall, 1986), investigators have yet to dis-
popularity of cotherapy, the gender dynamics of cover what strategies are effective in creating a
this interpersonal relationship have yet to be strong alliance with a family group, whether
studied. Finally, studies of gender in family alliances with individual family members are
therapy should take into account interactions additive, or how therapists can effectively repair
with other salient variables like age (consider split alliances in a family (Heatherington &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

grandmothers) or family structure (consider les-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Friedlander, 1990c; Pinsof & Catherall, 1986).


bian mothers). Indeed, the very definition of Consistent with the recommendation to avoid
family is changing rapidly in U.S. culture, pos- isolating individual behavior from its interper-
sibly more rapidly than the definition is chang- sonal context is a second recommendation: that
ing in either family theory or family research. individual behaviors not be analyzed apart from
the ongoing stream of behaviors (Stanton,
1988). Snapshot studies in which a picture of
How Do Therapeutic Interventions Affect the process at one point in time is used to
Family Members Simultaneously? predict client outcome are less useful than run-
ning videotapes, in which entire sessions or,
In working conjointly with family members, preferably, all sessions in a given treatment are
therapists are challenged by complex demands examined to predict intermediate and global
that are not present in individual therapy, one of outcomes.
which is managing the multiperson conversa- This recommendation underscores the need
tion so as to keep it therapeutic rather than for the use of sequential analysis (e.g., Bakeman
destructive, another of which is establishing al- & Gottman, 1986; Wampold, 1989) to test hy-
liances with several individuals who are at odds potheses of the following variety: Given that the
with each other. As well as demands, the com- therapist (or client) does A behavior, is B be-
plexity of the family context also provides havior likely to follow? If so, does B facilitate C
unique opportunities for therapeutic interven- behavior in response, or D behavior by another
tion—assessing in vivo enactments of problem- party? The need for sequential analyses of com-
atic family interactions, intervening indirectly munication has been raised in many earlier cri-
so as to soften a message and minimize resis- tiques of family therapy research (e.g., Gurman,
tance, using family members as cotherapists, 1988; Piercy & Sprenkle, 1990; Pinsof, 1981).
and so forth. We raise it again here because of its importance:
Just as it takes beginning family therapists Questions of sequence and pattern go to the
some time to learn to handle the interpersonal heart of the interactional process of effective
challenges (Pinsof, 1979) and to take advantage family therapy.
of the opportunities afforded by conjoint treat- While the most popular sequential techniques
ment, so does it take time for researchers to are limited to the detection of simple reciprocity
appreciate the unique challenges of studying (A follows B), a careful qualitative analysis of
family therapy processes. Designing creative therapy sessions may reveal patterns of behav-
strategies for avoiding a singular focus on indi- ior that unfold over longer periods of time and
vidual behavior that is isolated from its social in more complicated ways (e.g., Friedlander et
context is perhaps the single most important al., 1994; Gale & Newfield, 1992). A therapist,
direction for future research on the family ther- for example, might listen to a mother's com-
apy process (e.g., Pinsof, 1988; Steier, 1988; plaints about her daughter and consistently re-
Wynne, 1988). frame them in positive terms, terms that create
Since Pinsof s 1981 review of family therapy bonds between mother and daughter ("I see that
process research, there has been notable there are two women in this family who speak
progress in testing theory-based questions, in their minds"). The cumulative effect of the se-
developing measures, and in using methods and ries of interventions might be to prepare mother
analyses that are truly interactional in nature. and daughter to comply with an assignment
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 413

requiring collaboration. Since patterns like S. McNamee & K. Gergen (Eds.), Therapy as
these unfold across sessions, entire therapies social construction (pp. 25-39). London: Sage.
need to be studied micro- and macroanalyti- Aries, E. (1987). Gender and communication. In P.
Shaver & C. Hendrick (Eds.), Sex and gender (pp.
cally. Detection of important patterns calls for
149-173). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
innovative methodologies (Stanton, 1988). The
Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Observing
method of interpersonal process recall (men- interaction. New York: Cambridge University
tioned earlier) might be used to complement Press.
sequential analyses of observer ratings of the Beck, D. F., & Jones, M. A. (1973). Progress on
process in order to locate significant episodes, family problems: A nationwide study of clients'
for example, those in which family members and counselors' views on family agency services.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

report experiencing a split in their alliance with New York: Family Service Association of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the therapist. America.


Benjamin, L. S. (1974). Structural analysis of social
behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 392-425.
Conclusion Beutler, L. E., & Crago, M. (Eds.). (1991). Psycho-
therapy research: An international review of pro-
The foregoing recommendations are not, of grammatic studies. Washington, DC: American
course, exhaustive. Many reviewers of family Psychological Association.
therapy research, referenced throughout this ar- Beutler, L. E., Williams, R. E., & Wakefield, P. J.
(1993). Obstacles to disseminating applied psycho-
ticle, have provided other excellent and detailed
logical science. Applied and Preventive Psychol-
recommendations for future efforts. Clearly, ogy, 2, 53-58.
such efforts are sorely needed. Birdwhistell, R. (Producer). (1969). Hillcrest family
But, despite the paucity of existing process series [Film]. Philadelphia: Eastern Pennsylvania
research, there is cause for optimism in the Psychiatric Institute.
quest to specify the key processes in effective Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the
family therapy. The development of interac- psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance.
tional instruments, the continued refinement Psychotherapy Theory, Research, and Practice,
and use of change event paradigms, the bur- 16, 252-260.
geoning interest in qualitative methods, and the Bourgeois, L., Sabourin, S., & Wright, J. (1990).
posing of systemically sensitive, creative re- Predictive validity of therapeutic alliance in group
marital therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
search questions all should prove helpful in
Psychology, 58, 608-613.
erasing the spurious distinction between clini- Bowen, M. B. (1978). Family therapy in clinical
cally meaningful versus methodologically rig- practice. Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
orous research. Brown-Standridge, M. D., & Piercy, F. P. (1988).
Reality creation versus reality confirmation: A pro-
cess study in marital therapy. American Journal of
References Family Therapy, 16, 195-215.
Carson, R. C. (1969). Interaction concepts of person-
Alexander, J. F. (1988). Phases of family therapy ality. Chicago: Aldine.
process: A framework for clinicians and research- Chamberlain, P., Patterson, G., Reid, J., Kavanagh,
ers. In L. C. Wynne (Ed.), The state of the art in K., & Forgatch, M. (1984). Observation of client
family therapy research: Controversies and recom- resistance. Behavior Therapy, 15, 144—155.
mendations (pp. 175-187). New York: Family Pro- Cline, V. B., Mejia, J., Coles, J., Klein, N., & Cline,
cess Press. R. A. (1984). The relationship between therapist
Alexander, J. F., Barton, C , Schiavo, R. S., & behaviors and outcome for middle- and lower-class
Parsons, B. V. (1976). Systems-behavioral inter- couples in marital therapy. Journal of Clinical Psy-
vention with families of delinquents: Therapist chology, 40, 691-704.
characteristics, family behavior, and outcome. Crits-Christoph, P., Luborsky, L., Gay, E., Todd, T.,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, Barber, J. P., & Luborsky, E. (1991). What makes
656-664. Susie cry? A symptom-context study of family
Allred, G. H., & Kersey, L. (1977). The AIAC, a therapy. Family Process, 30, 337-345.
design for systematically analyzing marriage and De Chenne, T. K. (1973). Experiential facilitation in
family counseling: A progress report. Journal of conjoint marriage counseling. Psychotherapy, 10,
Marriage and Family Counseling, 3, 17—25. 212-214.
Anderson, H., & Goolishian, H. (1992). The client is Dowling, E. (1979). Co-therapy: A clinical re-
the expert: A not-knowing approach to therapy. In searcher's view. In S. Walrond-Skinner (Ed.),
414 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

Family and marital therapy (pp. 173-198). Lon- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
don: Routledge & Kegan Paul. 4-9.
Elliott, R. (1984), A discovery-oriented approach to Greenberg, L. S., Ford, C. L., Alden, L., & Johnson,
significant events in psychotherapy: Interpersonal S. M. (1993). In-session change in Emotionally
process recall and comprehensive process analysis. Focused Therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clin-
In L. N. Rice & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Patterns of ical Psychology, 61, 78-84.
change (pp. 249-286). New York: Guilford Press. Greenberg, L. S., James, P. S., & Conry, R. F. (1988).
Ericson, P. M., & Rogers, L. E. (1973). New proce- Perceived change processes in emotionally focused
dures for analyzing relational communication. couples therapy. Journal of Family Psychology, 2,
Family Process, 12, 245-267. 5-23.
Friedlander, M. L. (1993). Does complementarity Greenberg, L. S., & Johnson, S. (1988). Emotionally
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

promote or hinder client change in brief therapy? focused therapy for couples. New York: Guilford
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The Counseling Psychologist, 21, 457-486. Press.


Friedlander, M. L., Ellis, M. V., Raymond, L., Siegel, Greenberg, L. S., & Pinsof, W. M. (1986a). Process
S. M., & Milford, D. (1987). Convergence and research: Current trends and future perspectives. In
divergence in the process of interviewing families. L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.), The psy-
Psychotherapy, 24, 570-583. chotherapeutic process: A research handbook (pp.
Friedlander, M. L., & Heatherington, L. (1989). An- 3-20). New York: Guilford Press.
alyzing relational control in family therapy inter- Greenberg, L. S., & Pinsof, W. M. (Eds.). (1986b).
views. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, The psychotherapeutic process: A research hand-
139-148. book. New York: Guilford Press.
Friedlander, M. L., Heatherington, L., Johnson, B., & Gurman, A. S. (1988). Issues in the specification of
Skowron, E. A. (1994). Sustaining engagement: A family therapy interventions. In L. C. Wynne (Ed.),
change event in family therapy. Journal of Coun- The state of the art in family therapy research:
seling Psychology, 41, 438-448. Controversies and recommendations (pp. 125-
Friedlander, M. L., Heatherington, L., & Wildman, J. 138). New York: Family Process Press.
(1991). Interpersonal control in structural and Gurman, A. S., & Kniskern, D. P. (1978). Research
Milan systemic family therapy. Journal of Marital on marital and family therapy: Progress, perspec-
and Family Therapy, 17, 395-408. tive and prospect. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin
Friedlander, M. L., & Highlen, P. S. (1984). A spatial (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
view of the interpersonal structure of family inter- change: An empirical analysis (2nd ed., pp. 817-
views: Similarities and differences across counsel- 901). New York: Wiley.
ors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, All— Gurman, A. S., Kniskern, D. P., & Pinsof, W. M.
487. (1986). Research on the process and outcome of
Friedlander, M. L., Highlen, P. S., & Lassiter, W. L. marital and family therapy. In S. L. Garfield & A.
(1985). Content analytic comparison of four ex- E. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and
pert counselors' approaches to family treatment: behavior change: An empirical analysis (3rd ed.,
Ackerman, Bowen, Jackson, and Whitaker. Jour- pp. 565-624). New York: Wiley.
nal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 171-180. Heatherington, L. (1990). Toward more meaningful
Gale, J., & Newfield, N. (1992). A conversation clinical research: Taking context into account in
analysis of a solution-focused marital therapy ses- coding psychotherapy interaction. Psychotherapy,
sion. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 18, 26, 436-447.
153-165. Heatherington, L., & Friedlander, M. L. (1987). The
Garfield, S. L. (1986). Research on client variables in Family Communication Control Coding System:
psychotherapy. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin Coding manual. Unpublished manual. (Available
(Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior from L. Heatherington, Department of Psychology,
change: An empirical analysis (3rd ed., pp. 213— Williams College, Williamstown, MA 01267)
257). New York: Wiley. Heatherington, L., & Friedlander, M. L. (1990a).
Goldstein, M. J. (1988). Patient status, family com- Applying task analysis to structural family therapy.
position and other structural variables in family Journal of Family Psychology, 4, 36-48.
therapy research. In L. C. Wynne (Ed.), The state Heatherington, L., & Friedlander, M. L. (1990b).
of the art in family therapy research: Controver- Complementarity and symmetry in family therapy
sies and recommendations (pp. 109-115). New communication. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
York: Family Process Press. ogy, 37, 261-268.
Greenberg, L. S. (1984). Task analysis: The general Heatherington, L., & Friedlander, M. L. (1990c).
approach. In L. N. Rice & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Couple and family therapy alliance scales: Empir-
Patterns of change (pp. 124-148). New York: ical considerations. Journal of Marital and Family
Guilford Press. Therapy, 16, 299-306.
Greenberg, L. S. (1986). Change process research. Hill, C. E., Helms, J., Tichenor, V., Spiegel, S. B.,
PROCESS OF FAMILY THERAPY 415
O'Grady, K. E., & Perry, E. S. (1988). The effects Patterson, G. R., & Chamberlain, P. (1988). Treat-
of therapist response modes in brief psychother- ment process: A problem at three levels. In L. C.
apy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 27-36. Wynne (Ed.), The state of the art in family therapy
Holloway, E. L., Wampold, B. E., & Nelson, M. L. research: Controversies and recommendations
(1990). Use of a paradoxical intervention with a (pp. 189-223). New York: Family Process Press.
couple: An interactional analysis. Journal of Fam- Patterson, G. R., & Forgatch, M. S. (1985). Therapist
ily Psychology, 3, 385-402. behavior as a determinant for client noncompli-
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Jacobson, N. S., DeKlyen, ance: A paradox for the behavior modifier. Journal
M , & Whisman, M. A. (1989). Relationship be- of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 846-
tween behavioral marital therapy outcome and pro- 851.
cess variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Penman, R. (1980). Communication processes and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Psychology, 57, 658-662. relationships. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Jacobson, N. (1984). A component analysis of be- Piercy, F. P., & Sprenkle, D. H. (1990). Marriage and
havioral marital therapy: The relative effectiveness family therapy: A decade review. Journal of Mar-
of behavior exchange and communication/problem riage and the Family, 52, 1116-1126.
solving training. Journal of Consulting and Clini- Pinsof, W. M. (1979). The Family Therapist Behav-
cal Psychology, 52, 295-305. ior Scale (FTBS): Development and evaluation of a
Johnson, S. M., & Greenberg, L. S. (1988). Relating coding system. Family Process, 18, 451-461.
process to outcome in marital therapy. Journal of Pinsof, W. M. (1981). Family therapy process re-
Marital and Family Therapy, 14, 175-183. search. In A. S. Gurman & D. P. Kniskern (Eds.),
Klein, M., Mathieu, P., Kiesler, D., & Gendlin, E. T. Handbook of family therapy (Vol. 1, pp. 699-741).
(1969). The Experiencing Scale. Madison: Wiscon- New York: Brunner/Mazel.
sin Psychiatric Institute. Pinsof, W. M. (1986). The process of family therapy:
Kuehl, B. P., Newfield, N. A., & Joanning, H. (1990). The development of the Family Therapist Coding
A client-based description of family therapy. Jour- System. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.),
nal of Family Psychology, 3, 310—321. The psychotherapeutic process: A research hand-
Laird, H., & Vande Kemp, H. (1987). Complemen- book (pp. 201-284). New York: Guilford Press.
tarity as a function of stage in therapy: An analysis Pinsof, W. M. (1988). Strategies for the study of
of Minuchin's structural family therapy. Journal of family therapy research. In L. C. Wynne (Ed.), The
Marital and Family Therapy, 13, 127-137. state of the art in family therapy research: Con-
Luepnitz, D. A. (1988). The family interpreted. New troversies and recommendations (pp. 159-174).
York: Basic Books. New York: Family Process Press.
Maccoby, E. (1990). Gender and relationships. Amer- Pinsof, W. M. (1989). A conceptual framework and
ican Psychologist, 45, 513-520. methodological criteria for family therapy process
Martin, J., Martin, W., & Slemon, A. G. (1987). research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
Cognitive mediation in person-centered and ra- chology, 57, 53-59.
tional-emotive therapy. Journal of Counseling Pinsof, W. M., & Catherall, D. R. (1986). The inte-
Psychology, 34, 251-260. grative psychotherapy alliance: Family, couple and
Mas, C. H., Alexander, J. F., & Barton, C. (1985). individual therapy scales. Journal of Marital and
Modes of expression in family therapy: A process Family Therapy, 12, 137-151.
study of roles and gender. Journal of Marital and Postner, R. S., Guttman, H. A., Sigal, J. J., Epstein,
Family Therapy, 11, 411—415. N. B., & Rakoff, V. M. (1971). Process and out-
McGoldrick, M., Pearce, J., & Giordano, J. (1982). come in conjoint family therapy. Family Process,
Ethnicity and family therapy. New York: Guilford 10, 451-474.
Press. Raymond, L., Friedlander, M. L., Heatherington, L.,
Minuchin, S., & Fishman, C. (1981). Family therapy Ellis, M. V., & Sargent, J. (1993). Communication
techniques. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University processes in structural family therapy: Case study
Press. of an anorexic family. Journal of Family Psychol-
Minuchin, S., Rosman, B. L., & Baker, L. (1978). ogy, 6, 308-326.
Psychosomatic families: Anorexia nervosa in con- Reiss, D. (1988). Theoretical versus tactical infer-
text. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press ences: Or, how to do family psychotherapy re-
Munton, A. G., & Antaki, C. (1988). Causal beliefs search without dying of boredom. In L. C. Wynne
amongst families in therapy: Attributions at the (Ed.), The state of the art in family therapy re-
group level. British Journal of Clinical Psychol- search: Controversies and recommendations (pp.
ogy, 27, 91-97. 33-45). New York: Family Process Press.
Newberry, A. M., Alexander, J. F., & Turner, C. W. Rice, L. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (1984). The new
(1991). Gender as a process variable in family research paradigm. In L. N. Rice & L. S. Green-
therapy. Journal of Family Psychology, 5, 158- berg (Eds.), Patterns of change (pp. 7-25). New
175. York: Guilford Press.
416 FRIEDLANDER, WILDMAN, HEATHERINGTON, AND SKOWRON

Rice, D. G., Gurman, A. S., & Razin, A. M. (1974). Stanton, M. D. (1988). The lobster quadrille: Issues
Therapist sex, "style" and theoretical orientation. and dilemmas for family therapy research. In L. C.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 159, Wynne (Ed.), The state of the art in family therapy
413-421. research: Controversies and recommendations
Scheflen, A. E. (1973). Communication structure: (pp. 5-31). New York: Family Process Press.
Analysis of a psychotherapy transaction. Bloom- Steier, F. (1988). Toward a coherent methodology for
ington: Indiana University Press. the study of family therapy. In L. C. Wynne (Ed.),
Seeman, L., Tittler, B. I., & Friedman, S. (1985). The state of the art in family therapy research:
Early interactional change and its relationship to Controversies and recommendations (pp. 227-
family therapy outcome. Family Process, 24, 234). New York: Family Process Press.
59-68. Stiles, W. B. (1980). Measurement of the impact of
Selvini-Palazzoli, M., Boscolo, L., Cecchin, G., &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

psychotherapy sessions. Journal of Consulting and


Prata, G. (1978). Paradox and counterparadox: A
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Clinical Psychology, 48, 176-185.


new model in the therapy of the family in schizo- Sue, S., & Zane, N. (1987). The role of culture and
phrenic transaction (E. V. Burt, Trans.). North- cultural techniques in psychotherapy: A critique
vale, NJ: Aronson. and reformulation. American Psychologist, 42,
Shapiro, R. J., (1974). Therapist attitudes and prema- 37-45.
ture termination in family and individual therapy. Szapocznik, J., & Kurtines, W. M. (1993). Family
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 159, 101- psychology and cultural diversity. American Psy-
107. chologist, 48, 400-414.
Shapiro, R. J., & Budman, S. H. (1973). Defection, Tamura, T., & Lau, A. (1992). Connectedness versus
termination, and continuation in family and indi- separateness: Applicability of family therapy to
vidual therapy. Family Process, 12, 55-67. Japanese families. Family Process, 31, 319-340.
Shields, C. G., & McDaniel, S. H. (1992). Process Tannen, D. (1990). You just don't understand:
differences between male and female therapists in Women and men in conversation. New York:
a first family interview. Journal of Marital and Ballantine.
Family Therapy, 18, 143-151. Wampold, B. E. (1989). Kappa as a measure of
Shields, C. G., Sprenkle, D. H., & Constantine, J. A. pattern in sequential categorical data. Quality and
(1991). Anatomy of an initial interview: The im- Quantity, 23, 171-187.
portance of joining and structuring skills. Ameri- Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J. H., & Jackson, D. D.
can Journal of Family Therapy, 19, 3-18. (1967). Pragmatics of human communication. New
Shoham-Salomon, V. (1990). Interrelating research York: Norton.
processes of process research. Journal of Consult- Winer, L. R. (1971). The qualified pronoun count as
ing and Clinical Psychology, 58, 295-303. a measure of change in family psychotherapy.
Shostrom, E. L. (Producer). (1966). Three ap- Family Process, 10, 233-248.
proaches to psychotherapy [Film]. Santa Ana, CA: Wynne, L. C. (1988). An overview of the state of the
Psychological Films. art: What should be expected in current family
Sigal, J. J., Rakoff, V., & Epstein, N. B. (1967). therapy research. In L. C. Wynne (Ed.), The state
Indicators of therapeutic outcome in conjoint fam- of the art in family therapy research: Controver-
ily therapy. Family Process, 6, 215-226. sies and recommendations (pp. 249—266). New
Sluzki, C. (1975). The coalitionary process in initi- York: Family Process Press.
ating family therapy. Family Process, 14, 67—77. Zuk, G. H., Boszormenyi-Nagy, I., & Heiman, E.
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: (1963). Some dynamics of laughter during family
New scales for assessing the quality of marriage therapy. Family Process, 2, 302-314.
and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 38, 15-28.
Spector, R., Guttman, H., Sigal, J., Rakoff, V., & Received June 28, 1993
Epstein, N. (1970). Time sampling in family ther- Revision received December 14, 1993
apy sessions. Psychotherapy, 7, 37-40. Accepted December 19, 1993 •

You might also like