You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/7505634

An evaluation of evaluative personality terms: A comparison of the Big Seven


and Five-Factor Model in predicting psychopathology

Article  in  Psychological Assessment · October 2005


DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.17.3.359 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
47 268

2 authors:

Christine Durrett Timothy J Trull


Flint Hills Neuropsychology University of Missouri
11 PUBLICATIONS   1,010 CITATIONS    253 PUBLICATIONS   14,004 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Elevated rate of alcohol consumption in borderline personality disorder patients in daily life View project

Event-Level Predictors of Alcohol-Impaired Driving Intentions View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Timothy J Trull on 20 October 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Assessment Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association
2005, Vol. 17, No. 3, 359 –368 1040-3590/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.17.3.359

An Evaluation of Evaluative Personality Terms: A Comparison of the Big


Seven and Five-Factor Model in Predicting Psychopathology
Christine Durrett and Timothy J. Trull
University of Missouri—Columbia

Two personality models are compared regarding their relationship with personality disorder (PD)
symptom counts and with lifetime Axis I diagnoses. These models share 5 similar domains, and the Big
7 model also includes 2 domains assessing self-evaluation: positive and negative valence. The Big 7
model accounted for more variance in PDs than the 5-factor model, primarily because of the association
of negative valence with most PDs. Although low-positive valence was associated with most Axis I
diagnoses, the 5-factor model generally accounted for more variance in Axis I diagnoses than the Big 7
model. Some predicted associations between self-evaluation and psychopathology were not found, and
unanticipated associations emerged. These findings are discussed regarding the utility of evaluative terms
in clinical assessment.

Keywords: Big Seven, five-factor model, positive valence, negative valence, psychopathology

The five-factor model (FFM) is a model of personality based on are and, instead, when endorsed, may reflect a more global indi-
the lexical hypothesis, the assumption that important individual cation of positive or negative self-esteem. In other words, positive
differences are represented in language. Several researchers have and negative self-evaluation terms may not reflect specific person-
based empirical investigations of the structure of personality on the ality traits but rather the person’s opinion of the merits of his or her
lexical hypothesis by subjecting lists of adjectives culled from personality.
dictionaries to factor analysis in an effort to find underlying Tellegen and Waller (1987) have argued that such evaluative
dimensions of personality. Results of these analyses have generally descriptors should have been included in studies of the structure of
converged on five broad dimensions, which, according to FFM personality. Tellegen (1993) has written that self-evaluation should
terminology, are known as Neuroticism (or Emotional Stability), be considered a personality trait because of its relationship with
Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to self-esteem, “a core area of individual differences,” and that it was
Experience (or Intellect/Culture) (Fiske, 1949; Goldberg, 1990; an error to leave out purely evaluative terms in the creation of the
Norman, 1963; Tupes & Christal, 1961). FFM. He has further stated that self-evaluation is a critical diag-
In all the previous studies, exclusion rules were applied in the nostic factor in personality disorder (PD). Therefore, Tellegen and
selection of the adjectives used in the factor analyses in order to Waller repeated the factor analytic method used to create the FFM,
retain only those words that were descriptive of personality. For including evaluative terms in the initial list of adjectives. The
instance, some words were ruled out for describing physical char- resulting Big Seven domains include five dimensions that roughly
acteristics or for being too obscure or ambiguous. Significantly, correspond with the FFM—Positive and Negative Emotionality,
evaluative terms like evil or worthy were also excluded because Openness/Conventionality, Agreeableness, and Conscientious-
they do not describe specific personality characteristics. For ex- ness—as well as two unique dimensions, Positive and Negative
ample, Saucier and Goldberg (1998) differentiated “personality Valence. Positive Valence is defined by terms such as excellent,
descriptors” and “person descriptors.” Personality descriptors re-
superior, and impressive (vs. ordinary), whereas Negative Valence
fer to stable behavioral characteristics. In contrast, evaluative
is defined by terms such as evil, awful, and cruel (vs. decent).
terms fall under the larger category of person descriptors because
The Big Seven model of personality has been replicated in
ratings would not necessarily be consistent between self and others
research with Americans as well as in cross-cultural research with
or because they contain pure valence and less content than other
British, Israeli, and Spanish individuals (Almagor, Tellegen, &
adjectives. Similarly, Ashton and Lee (2001) argued that evalua-
Waller, 1995; Benet & Waller, 1995; Benet-Martinez & Waller
tive terms are not descriptive of persons like personality trait terms
1997; Blumberg, 2001; Waller & Zavala, 1993). In all these
studies, factor analyses of trait adjectives, including evaluative
terms, resulted in a seven-factor solution with dimensions closely
Christine Durrett and Timothy J. Trull, Department of Psychological resembling those originally delineated by Tellegen and Waller
Sciences, University of Missouri—Columbia. (1987). However, other research has shown support for the Neg-
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health
ative Valence factor but not for the Positive Valence factor. For
Grant R01 MH52695 awarded to Timothy J. Trull. We thank Tom Widiger
for his comments on an earlier version of this article. example, results from a study of Filipinos indicated that when
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Timothy evaluative terms were included in a factor analysis of adjectives, a
J. Trull, Department of Psychological Sciences, 210 McAlester Hall, Negative Valence factor did emerge, but positively valenced items
University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211. E-mail: trullt@missouri.edu tended to load on the Intellect (or Openness) factor (Church,
359
360 DURRETT AND TRULL

Katigbak, & Reyes, 1998). In fact, in all factor solutions (ranging socially inept, personally unappealing, or inferior to others” (APA,
from 5 to 10 factors), a Positive Valence factor never emerged. 1994). Furthermore, individuals with borderline PD “usually have
However, the Negative Valence factor was one of the most repli- a self-image that is based on being bad or evil” (APA, 1994). For
cable dimensions. Similar results were obtained in a study of this reason, Waller and Zavala (1993) posited that the Big Seven
Americans by Saucier (1997), in which a Negative Valence factor domains of Positive and Negative Valence would likely add pre-
was identified, yet positively valenced items were split between dictive value above and beyond the FFM in predicting personality
two factors labeled Intellect and Attractiveness. Finally, Schiller, pathology. However, as of yet, there are no empirical studies of
Tellegen, and Evens (1995) conducted another study using a this hypothesis. The formal evaluation and documentation of in-
different method in which participants generated their own lists of cremental validity is a necessary first step in the justification of
adjectives to describe other people. When the resulting items from new clinical assessment measures or scales (Haynes & Lench,
each participant were submitted for factor analysis, a Negative 2003).
Valence dimension sometimes appeared, but, in other cases, such Not only are PDs related to personality traits, but Axis I diag-
descriptors loaded on the opposite pole of Agreeableness. A Pos- noses are also related to such traits. Trull and Sher (1994) inves-
itive Valence dimension never emerged; this class of adjectives tigated the relationship between the FFM and Axis I diagnoses of
generally loaded on the Intellect factor. mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders in a nonclinical sample
Some researchers have argued that the FFM already contains of young adults. They found that FFM scores distinguished indi-
positive and negative self-evaluation, and therefore separate di- viduals with and without Axis I disorders and were, to some
mensions to capture valence are unnecessary. For example, Mc- extent, sensitive to diagnosis. Axis I disorders were associated
Crae and Costa (1995) found that high scores on the FFM facets of with higher than average Neuroticism and Openness to Experience
competence and assertiveness are associated with Big Seven Pos- and lower than average Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Consci-
itive Valence, whereas low scores on warmth and dutifulness and entiousness. These results have been replicated in community and
high scores on depression and vulnerability are associated with Big clinical samples for a wide range of Axis I disorders (Bagby et al.,
Seven Negative Valence. Therefore, McCrae and Costa concluded 1997; Bienvenu et al., 2001). Given these findings of an associa-
that “positive and negative valence are not separate factors of tion between FFM domains and Axis I pathology, it seems rea-
personality, but reflections of the social and personal value of sonable to propose that the Big Seven personality domains of
objective features of personality encompassed by the five-factor self-evaluation may also be related to Axis I pathology. For in-
model” (p. 455). In another empirical test of this argument, Saucier stance, one symptom of depression is strong feelings of guilt or
and Goldberg (1998) investigated 53 potential dimensions of worthlessness, which presumably reflects negative self-evaluation.
person-descriptive content in an effort to find which were most In the present study, we investigated the overlap between the
strongly independent of the FFM. Negative Valence was reliable FFM and the Big Seven model of personality, giving special
and substantially distinct from the FFM, but Positive Valence did attention to the extent to which Positive and Negative Valence are
not account for significant variance in personality independent of independent of the FFM. We also assessed whether the Big Seven
the FFM factors. model (and particularly the domains of Positive and Negative
Valence) contributed to the prediction of maladaptive personality
Personality and Psychopathology traits and DSM–IV (APA, 1994) Axis I disorders above and be-
yond the FFM. Another factor that warrants consideration in these
Previous research has uncovered significant relationships be- analyses is the extensive comorbidity of Axis I and Axis II disor-
tween constellations of FFM personality traits and PDs. In a recent ders (Dolan-Sewell, Krueger, & Shea, 2001). If both Axis I and II
review, Widiger and Costa (2002) concluded that a substantial disorders prove to be related to self-evaluation, then the meaning
proportion of this research supports the conceptualization of PD and significance of the finding may be obscured by high rates of
symptomatology as maladaptive variants of FFM traits. Several comorbidity between Axis I and II. By controlling for this comor-
empirical studies have confirmed the predicted linkages between bidity, it is possible to determine what significant and unique
the FFM and PDs in clinical and nonclinical participants (Trull, relations exist between dimensions of self-evaluation and various
1992; Wiggins & Pincus, 1989), with FFM traits generally ac- forms of psychopathology.
counting for a moderate proportion of variance in PD symptom In summary, our study compared the strength of association
counts. Furthermore, FFM traits in many cases discriminate be- between both the FFM and the Big Seven model and measures of
tween different types of personality pathology. For example, psychopathology. We expected that both models of personality
avoidant PD corresponds with high Neuroticism and low Extra- would show significant, clinically meaningful relationships with
version, whereas borderline PD can be represented as high Neu- personality pathology as well as with Axis I disorders. We further
roticism and low Conscientiousness (Widiger & Costa, 2002). hypothesized that the domains of Positive and Negative Valence
Despite the evidence supporting the conceptualization of PDs may contribute significant predictive value to PD symptoms and
within the FFM, others have argued that self-evaluation is an Axis I diagnoses above and beyond the FFM. We also have a
important component of personality pathology that is missing from number of specific hypotheses. First, we expect that Negative
the FFM (Waller & Zavala, 1993). Some of the Diagnostic and Valence will significantly predict borderline, avoidant, and depen-
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM–IV; dent PDs, all of which share the core feature of low self-esteem. If
American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994) criteria seem to negative evaluation does indeed primarily tap low-self-esteem,
imply that self-evaluation may be a component of certain PDs, then it is not likely to be related to PDs that are not characterized
such as the criterion for narcissistic PD “has a grandiose sense of by low self-worth (e.g., schizotypal or antisocial PD). Also, we
self-importance” or the criterion for avoidant PD “views self as expect that narcissistic PD will be significantly associated with
FFM AND THE BIG SEVEN 361

Positive Valence, given that narcissists are characterized by overly time or were given research credit. Written consent was obtained from each
generous perceptions of their own self-worth. With regard to the participant as well as permission to have all interviews videotaped for
Axis I disorders, we expect that mood disorders and eating disor- reliability purposes. Furthermore, all participants were informed that the
ders will be significantly associated with Negative Valence be- principal investigator had secured a certificate of confidentiality for the
study from the federal government. This added protection was obtained
cause both of these diagnoses include self-image distortion, shame
because some of the interview and inventory questions in the larger study
and guilt, and feelings of inadequacy (Hayaki, Friedman, &
concerned illegal behaviors (e.g., antisocial behaviors). A certificate of
Brownell, 2002; O’Connor, Berry, Weiss, & Gilbert, 2002), confidentiality protects participants’ data from subpoena to the fullest
respectively. extent possible allowed by law regarding the anonymity of their individual
responses.
Method Three interviewers with backgrounds in clinical psychology underwent
2 months of training before gathering data for the study. Training was
Screening Procedure supervised by Timothy J. Trull and involved didactic instruction, review of
manuals and any training videotapes that were available for these inter-
Data for this analysis were obtained from a larger study of the devel- views, review of previously taped interviews, and at least 10 practice
opment of borderline PD features in young adults (see Trull, 2001). interviews with each measure.
Approximately 71% (N ⫽ 4,927) of all 18-year-old freshmen at the All interviewers were unaware of the borderline features status (above or
University of Missouri—Columbia during two consecutive academic years below threshold) of the participants, and interviews and inventories were
took part in the initial screening procedure. Participants were contacted administered in randomized sequences to control for possible order effects.
through mailings, classes, telephone calls, and electronic messages (e- All interviews were videotaped, and 80 participants’ interviews were
mails) and completed the screening battery during supervised sessions held randomly selected for reliability checks. For each of these selected inter-
in dormitories, fraternities and sororities, and classrooms. Students were views, one of the other two interviewers reviewed the relevant portion(s) of
scheduled up to 10 times to complete the screening battery or until they the videotape and made independent ratings.
indicated that they were no longer interested in participating. Screening
participants provided written consent to take part in the study. Those who
completed the screening received either $5 or research credit.
Measures
The screening battery included items from the Personality Assessment Self-reports. The primary measures for this analysis include two self-
Inventory—Borderline Features scale (PAI-BOR; Morey, 1991). The PAI- report measures of personality, the Revised NEO Personality Inventory
BOR is a 24-item self-report measure that taps features of severe person- (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992), a 240-item measure that assesses the
ality pathology associated with borderline and other PDs (with subscales FFM of personality, and the Inventory of Personality Characteristics #7-
including Affective Instability, Identity Problems, Negative Relationships, short form (IPC7; Tellegen, Grove, & Waller, 1991), a 70-item measure
and Self-Harm). The PAI-BOR scale has demonstrated reliability and that assesses the Big Seven personality domains. The two measures do not
construct validity in predicting problems associated with borderline pathol- share any items in common. The NEO-PI-R includes six facet scales
ogy such as psychiatric diagnoses (Morey, 1991; Trull, 1995). (subscales) for each domain; however, the IPC7-short form does not
From the screening pool, individuals who scored ⱖ 38 on the PAI-BOR include subscales. Reliability coefficients (internal consistency alphas) for
(above threshold; two standard deviations above the mean score for com- the FFM domains of Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience,
munity participants) and those who scored below threshold (⬍ 38) were Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness were .94, .91, .89, .91, and .93,
identified. From these lists of above- and below-threshold scorers, indi- respectively. For the Big Seven domains of Negative Emotionality, Posi-
viduals were randomly chosen to participate in the laboratory phase of the tive Emotionality, Conventionality, Positive Valence, Negative Valence,
study. Above-threshold individuals were oversampled to increase the dis- Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness, reliability coefficients were .86,
tribution of borderline personality disorder features (for the purposes of the .88, .82, .83, .86, .80, and .86.
larger study, although not of this particular analysis), and attempts were Interviews. Participants also completed two interview measures of
made to sample an equal number of men and women from each threshold psychopathology. The Structured Interview for DSM–IV Personality
group. (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1997) is a semistructured inter-
view that assesses DSM–IV (APA, 1994) PD criteria. Interrater reliabilities
Participants (intraclass correlations) for the number of criteria met for each PD were as
follows: paranoid PD, .77; schizoid PD, .92; schizotypal PD, .93; antisocial
Each person who agreed to participate in the laboratory phase of the PD, .97; borderline PD, .94; histrionic PD, .89; narcissistic PD, .93;
study first completed the PAI-BOR again in order to ensure that he or she avoidant PD, .99; dependent PD, .89; and obsessive– compulsive, .91. At
scored in the same range (i.e., above or below threshold) at retest. There Wave 2, 34 individuals (9.6%) met diagnostic criteria for at least one
were two waves of laboratory data collection, Wave 1 at approximately age DSM–IV PD; the most prevalent PDs were antisocial (7.0%), avoidant
18 and Wave 2 at age 20, 2 years later. Of the participants, 421 individuals (2.0%), and borderline (1.1%).
participated at Wave 1 and 361 at Wave 2. The present analysis focused on Also, the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders/
data from Wave 2, at which time both personality trait measures were Nonpatient Version (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995) was admin-
administered (see below). At that time, 169 above-threshold (borderline istered to assess the presence and severity of Axis I disorders. For the
features positive) and 192 below-threshold (borderline features negative) purposes of this study, we included variables indicating the lifetime pres-
participants took part in the study. Of those individuals, 56.4% were ence of any mood disorder (major depression, dysthymia, and bipolar I and
women, 84.1% were Caucasian, 8.4% were African American, and 3.9% II disorders), of any anxiety disorder, of any substance use disorder (abuse
were Asian American. or dependence on alcohol or any other drug), and of any eating disorder.
Interrater reliabilities (kappas) were 1.0 for all of those diagnostic classes.
Laboratory Procedure Of the full sample at Wave 2, 241 (66.7%) were diagnosed with a lifetime
history of any Axis I disorder, 137 (38.0%) were diagnosed with any mood
The laboratory session required approximately 5 hr and involved com- disorder, 57 (15.8%) were diagnosed with any anxiety disorder, 174
pletion of a number of self-report measures as well as the administration of (48.2%) were diagnosed with any substance use disorder, and 13 (3.6%)
several structured interviews. Participants were paid $10 per hour for their were diagnosed with any eating disorder.
362 DURRETT AND TRULL

Table 1
Correlations Between Five-Factor Model and Big Seven Domains

Five-factor model

Openness to
Big Seven Neuroticism Extraversion Conscientiousness Agreeableness Experience

Negative Emotionality .79*** ⫺.16** ⫺.28*** ⫺.28*** .03


Positive Emotionality ⫺.24*** .79*** .12* .17** .12*
Conscientiousness ⫺.16** ⫺.01 .70*** .24*** ⫺.38***
Agreeableness ⫺.26*** .11* .10 .61*** ⫺.05
Conventionality ⫺.23*** .13* .44*** .26*** ⫺.50***
Negative Valence .45*** ⫺.31*** ⫺.37*** ⫺.39*** .05
Positive Valence ⫺.26*** .40*** .23*** ⫺.13* .10

Note. n ⫽ 354.
* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

Statistical Analyses results indicate good convergent and discriminant validity in that
convergent correlations were greater than discriminant correla-
First, bivariate correlations between the FFM and Big Seven domains
tions. However, in some cases, nonparallel domains were also
were calculated to assess overlap and similarity between domains. Further-
more, regressions of Positive and Negative Valence on the FFM domains associated. For example, both Neuroticism and Agreeableness
as well as bivariate correlations between FFM facet scores and the Big from the FFM were at least minimally correlated with every
Seven domains of Positive and Negative Valence were calculated to domain from the Big Seven. Therefore, although there are five
determine the extent to which the self-evaluation domains were indepen- pairs of similar domains between the two models, these pairs are
dent of the FFM (similar to the analysis conducted by McCrae & Costa, far from identical.
1995). Finally, a factor analysis of the FFM facets and Big Seven domains Positive and Negative Valence scores from the Big Seven also
was carried out to investigate the relationship between the scales of each showed significant relationships with scores from most domains of
model of personality.
the FFM. Negative Valence was positively related to Neuroticism
Next, to determine the association between the FFM and Big Seven
and negatively related to Extraversion, Conscientiousness, and
models of personality and Axis I and II pathology, three series of hierar-
chical multiple regression analyses were carried out. In the first series, sex Agreeableness. Positive Valence was moderately associated with
and then the personality domain scores from both models were entered in Extraversion and to a lesser extent correlated with Conscientious-
blocks to predict PD symptom counts. Two models were constructed for ness and inversely associated with Neuroticism. Both were inde-
each criterion: one in which the FFM domains were entered first, followed pendent of Openness to Experience.1
by the Big Seven domains, and the other in which the Big Seven domains Next, Positive and Negative Valence scores, separately, were
were entered first, followed by the FFM domains. In the full model, sex and regressed on the FFM domain scores to determine how much
both the FFM and Big Seven domains were entered. In the second series of variance in the domains of self-evaluation could be accounted for
regressions, lifetime Axis I diagnoses were predicted from sex and the two
by the FFM. FFM domains accounted for 29% of the variance in
models of personality in logistic models. Again, two models were tested for
each criterion, entering the FFM and then the Big Seven domains (and vice
Positive Valence and 30% of the variance in Negative Valence.
versa) to determine the incremental contribution of each model of person- Positive Valence was most strongly related to high Extraversion
ality in the prediction of lifetime Axis I diagnoses above and beyond the and Agreeableness and low Neuroticism, whereas Negative Va-
other model. lence was most strongly related to high Neuroticism and low
To address the alternate hypothesis that any observed associations be- Agreeableness.
tween PD symptoms and personality traits (especially self-evaluation) Last, we conducted a factor analysis of the 30 FFM facets and
result from the comorbidity between Axis I and II disorders, we also seven Big Seven domains. A factor analysis of all items from each
carried out one further parallel series of hierarchical regression models. In
instrument would have been ideal, but a much larger sample would
this series, we tested hierarchical regression models predicting PD symp-
be required for a factor analysis of 310 items. Factors were
tom counts from sex, the two models of personality, and lifetime Axis I
diagnoses to determine the contributions of both models of personality in generated with principal-components extraction and Promax rota-
the prediction of PD symptoms above and beyond Axis I diagnoses.
1
Not only were the FFM domains related to self-evaluation, but Positive
Results and Negative Valence showed differential relationships with the facet
scales of the FFM. In the bivariate case, Positive Valence was most highly
Correlations between the FFM and Big Seven domains are
correlated with the facets of Depression (r ⫽ ⫺.34), Self-Consciousness
presented in Table 1. Associations between parallel domains
(r ⫽ ⫺.25), Assertiveness (r ⫽ .40), Positive Emotions (r ⫽ .36), Modesty
across the two models of personality were substantial and signif- (r ⫽ ⫺.52), Competence (r ⫽ .31), and Achievement Striving (r ⫽ .32).
icant, as expected (i.e., Negative Emotionality and Neuroticism, Negative Valence was most highly correlated with the facets of Angry
Positive Emotionality and Extraversion, Conscientiousness and Hostility (r ⫽ .42), Depression (r ⫽ .49), Vulnerability (r ⫽ .34), Warmth
Agreeableness in both, and Conventionality and Openness to Ex- (r ⫽ ⫺.36), Trust (r ⫽ ⫺.40), Altruism (r ⫽ ⫺.33), Competence (r ⫽
perience, which have an inverse relationship). In general, these ⫺.34), and Self-Discipline (r ⫽ ⫺.40).
FFM AND THE BIG SEVEN 363

tion. Seven factors had eigenvalues greater than one, and a five- from ⫺.35 (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) to ⫺.04 (Agree-
factor solution was retained after examining the scree plot and the ableness and Openness to Experience); the average factor corre-
coherency of the factor solutions. There was a clear break in the lation was 兩.17兩.
scree plot after five factors, and the sixth and seventh factors
consisted of only two scales, each loading at greater than 0.35 Hierarchical Regressions on Axis II Symptom Counts
(Agreeableness-Modesty and the reverse of Positive Valence and
Extraversion-Excitement Seeking and Openness to Values, respec- A series of hierarchical regressions was next conducted to
tively). The pattern matrix for the five-factor solution is shown in determine the relative contributions of the FFM and Big Seven
Table 2. The solution is largely consistent with the FFM, with domains in predicting PD symptom counts (see Table 3). In the
factors reflecting Conscientiousness, Neuroticism/Negative Emo- first step for Model 1, sex and the FFM domains were entered,
tionality, Extraversion/Positive Emotionality, Agreeableness, and followed by the Big Seven domains entered in the second step. The
Openness to Experience/Conventionality (reversed coded), respec- combination of sex and the FFM domains accounted for a signif-
tively. Positive Valence loads on the Extraversion/Positive Emo- icant increment in variance for all symptom counts. When the Big
tionality factor, and both Positive and Negative Valence load Seven domains were next entered in Model 1, they accounted for
inversely on the Agreeableness factor. Factor correlations ranged small but significant increments in variance above and beyond the

Table 2
Factor Analysis Pattern Matrix of Five-Factor Model (FFM) and Big Seven Domains

Component

Facet and domain 1 2 3 4 5

FFM Neuroticism
Anxiety .84
Angry Hostility .69 ⫺.49
Depression .75
Self-Consciousness .74
Impulsiveness ⫺.35 .56
Vulnerability .69
FFM Extraversion
Warmth .70 .44
Gregariousness .79
Assertiveness .65 ⫺.37
Activity .32 .69
Excitement Seeking .62
Positive Emotions .70 .30
FFM Openness
Fantasy ⫺.33 .42
Aesthetics .78
Feelings .44 .44 .48
Actions ⫺.31 .42
Ideas .82
Values .64
FFM Agreeableness
Trust .31 .54
Straightforwardness .69
Altruism .71
Compliance .77
Modesty ⫺.42 .63
Tender Mindedness .59 .33
FFM Conscientiousness
Competence .72
Order .76
Dutifulness .74
Achievement Striving .82
Self-Discipline .81
Deliberation .71
Big Seven Negative Emotionality .87
Big Seven Positive Emotionality .87
Big Seven Positive Valence .47 ⫺.33
Big Seven Negative Valence ⫺.32
Big Seven Agreeableness .76
Big Seven Conscientiousness .78
Big Seven Conventionality .34 ⫺.60

Note. Loadings above .30 are displayed.


364 DURRETT AND TRULL

Table 3
Regression Results Predicting Personality Disorder Symptom Counts From Five-Factor Model (FFM) and Big Seven Scores

Model 1 Model 2
Significant predictors
Predictor Sex FFM R 2
Big Seven added ⌬R 2
Sex Big Seven R2 FFM added ⌬R2 ␤ at last step

Cluster A
Paranoid 0.07*** 0.03 0.08*** 0.02 .22** Negative Valence
Schizoid 0.04* 0.01 0.04* 0.01 (none)
Schizotypal 0.05** 0.05* (.22) 0.09*** 0.01 .23** Negative Valence
⫺.13* Positive Valence
Cluster B
Antisocial 0.22*** 0.05*** (.22) 0.21** 0.06*** (.24) .12* Sex
.15* Negative Valence
⫺.23** Conventionality
.19* Neuroticism
⫺.17* Conscientiousness (FFM)
⫺.15* Agreeableness (FFM)
Borderline 0.23*** 0.08*** (.28) 0.29*** 0.02* (.14) .30*** Negative Valence
.23* Neuroticism
Histrionic 0.24*** 0.03* (.17) 0.24*** 0.03* (.17) .20* Extraversion
Narcissistic 0.15*** 0.02 0.13*** 0.04** (.20) ⫺.22** Agreeableness (FFM)
Cluster C
Avoidant 0.14*** 0.05** (.22) 0.17*** 0.02 .22** Negative Valence
Dependent 0.11*** 0.04* (.20) 0.12*** 0.03* (.17) .20** Negative Valence
.26** Neuroticism
Obsessive–compulsive 0.10*** 0.07*** (.26) 0.16*** 0.01 .19** Negative Valence
⫺.22** Agreeableness (Big 7)

Note. Effect sizes appear in parentheses.


* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

effects of sex and the FFM for all individual PD symptom counts, at each step. For Model 1, Step 1, with sex and the FFM domains
with the exception of paranoid, schizoid, and narcissistic PDs. The entered, the FFM significantly predicted all diagnostic classes (any
effect sizes of the significant increment of prediction afforded by Axis I diagnosis and any mood, substance, anxiety, and eating
the Big Seven domains were in the small to medium range (0.17– disorder). When the Big Seven domains were added in Model 1,
0.28; Cohen, 1992). Step 2, no significant improvement in model fit was observed for
Model 2 was identical to Model 1 except that the order of entry any diagnostic class. However, the addition of the FFM domains
of the personality domains in Steps 1 and 2 were reversed. When significantly improved model fit in Model 2, Step 2 for all Axis I
sex and the Big Seven domains were entered in Step 1, they diagnoses except substance use disorders. Effect sizes were in the
accounted for a significant increment in variance for all symptom small to moderate range (0.20 – 0.33).
counts. In Step 2, the FFM domains were entered, and these Although the Big Seven domains as a group did not predict Axis
accounted for significant increments in variance above and beyond I diagnoses over and above the FFM, self-evaluation proved to be
the effects of sex and the Big Seven for all four Cluster B PDs and an important predictor for several diagnostic classes. Low Positive
for dependent PD. The effect sizes of the significant increment of Valence was a significant predictor in the full model for any Axis
prediction afforded by the FFM scales were in the small to medium I disorder, any substance use disorder, and any eating disorder.
range (0.14 – 0.24; Cohen, 1992).
An examination of the significant individual predictors of Hierarchical Regressions on Axis II Symptom Counts
PDsymptom counts once all variables were entered into the re- Controlling for Axis I
gression models (see last column of Table 3) revealed that Nega-
tive Valence was a significant, positive predictor in the full model Finally, a third series of regressions was conducted to determine
for all symptom counts, with the exception of schizoid, histrionic, the relative contributions of the FFM and Big Seven domains in
and narcissistic PDs. However, Positive Valence only accounted predicting Axis II symptom counts while controlling for lifetime
for significant variance in schizotypal PD symptom counts in the Axis I diagnoses (see Table 5). These models were identical to the
full model, above and beyond both the Big Seven and FFM. first series of regressions on PD symptom counts, except that in
Step 1 of each model, the presence or absence of any Axis I
diagnosis was entered as well as sex and the first set of personality
Hierarchical Regressions on Axis I Diagnoses
domains. Results from this analysis were similar to the results from
A similar series of regressions was conducted next to determine the first series of regressions. Whereas the Big Seven model
the contribution of self-evaluation to the prediction of lifetime accounted for significant variance in schizotypal, antisocial, bor-
Axis I diagnoses (see Table 4). The diagnoses were coded as derline, avoidant, dependent, and obsessive– compulsive symptom
dichotomous variables (present vs. absent), so logistic regressions counts above and beyond sex, Axis I diagnosis, and the FFM, the
were used with chi-square difference tests to determine model fit FFM only accounted for significant variance in antisocial and
FFM AND THE BIG SEVEN 365

Table 4
Logistic Regression Results Predicting Axis I Lifetime Diagnoses From Five-Factor Model (FFM) and Big Seven Scores

Model 1 Model 2
Significant predictors
Predictor Sex FFM R 2
Big Seven added ⌬R 2
Sex Big Seven R2 FFM added ⌬R2 Odds ratio at last step

Any Axis I disorder 0.27*** 0.04 0.27*** 0.04* (.20) 0.94* Positive Valence
1.03** Neuroticism
Mood disorder 0.29*** 0.03 0.26*** 0.06** (.24) 1.04*** Neuroticism
Anxiety disorder 0.20*** 0.03 0.16*** 0.07** (.26) 1.04** Neuroticism
Substance use disorder 0.17*** 0.04 0.18*** 0.03 0.94* Positive Valence
0.94* Conventionality
Eating disorder 0.24* 0.13 0.26* 0.11* (.33) 0.85* Positive Valence
1.06* Neuroticism

Note. Effect sizes appear in parentheses. Nagelkerke R2 is used.


* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

narcissistic symptom counts above and beyond sex, Axis I diag- schizotypal, paranoid, antisocial, or obsessive– compulsive PD
nosis, and the Big Seven. symptoms. Also contrary to hypotheses, Positive Valence was not
Negative Valence remained a significant predictor of all PD a significant predictor of narcissistic PD symptoms. Positive Va-
symptom counts except for schizoid, histrionic, and narcissistic lence scores did show a weak, negative relationship with schizo-
PDs in the full model, including sex, Axis I diagnosis, the FFM typal symptoms. It is somewhat surprising that Positive Valence
and the Big Seven. Positive Valence was a negative, significant was not also positively associated with narcissistic PD symptoms,
predictor in the full model for only the schizotypal PD symptom given that relatively high-positive self-evaluation is implied in the
count. In the full model, Axis I diagnostic status was a significant criteria set. Perhaps this is because of the possibility that the
predictor only for antisocial PD symptom count.2 characteristics of positive self-evaluation in the context of those
PDs may be somewhat different than those captured by the Big
Discussion Seven domain of Positive Valence. Having a sense of entitlement
or requiring excessive admiration from others may not be ade-
Positive and Negative Valence of the Big Seven share a signif-
quately tapped by a domain assessing whether an individual feels
icant proportion of variance with the domains of the FFM, as
special and unique.
indicated by the results of the correlational and factor analyses.
For lifetime Axis I diagnoses, the results were different from
These findings suggest that there is some redundancy of informa-
those for Axis II symptom counts. In this case, the FFM accounted
tion between the FFM and the self-evaluation domains. Though
for variance above and beyond the Big Seven domains for each
there is a moderate degree of overlap between the FFM and
diagnosis, with the exception of substance use disorders, whereas
Positive and Negative Valence, these correlations were no higher
the Big Seven model did not account for variance above and
than the correlations between some FFM domains in the present
beyond the FFM in any case. Therefore, in general, the FFM
sample (for instance, between Neuroticism and Conscientiousness,
appeared to be a better predictor of Axis I pathology than is the Big
r ⫽ ⫺.48 and between Conscientiousness and Agreeableness, r ⫽
.27). McCrae and Costa’s (1995) argument that the FFM already Seven model. We had anticipated that high Negative Valence
contains self-evaluation is to some extent true; however, Positive would be associated with Axis I pathology, but low scores on
and Negative Valence are about as independent of the FFM do- Positive Valence were a consistent predictor of several forms of
mains as the FFM domains are of each other. Axis I pathology in the full model, including both the FFM and the
Both the FFM and the Big Seven models of personality ac-
counted for similar proportions of variance in PD symptom counts. 2
Given that some of the domains in the FFM and Big Seven are quite
However, the Big Seven model more consistently accounted for similar, multicollinearity among predictors in the full model was also of
variance in PD symptoms above and beyond that accounted for by possible concern. Therefore, tolerance statistics were examined to assess
the FFM. Negative Valence was a significant predictor of all this potential problem. Tolerance ranges from 0 to 1, with a value of 1
individual PD symptom counts in the full regression model, with indicating complete independence from the other predictors in the model
the exception of narcissistic and histrionic PDs. The inclusion of and values near 0 indicating unacceptably high multicollinearity among the
this domain in the Big Seven is likely the reason that the Big Seven predictors. For the models in the present analysis, tolerance never dropped
outperformed the FFM in accounting for variance in PD below a cutoff of 0.10 for any predictor.
symptoms. All of the above regression models were then tested again using
weighted data (to control the effects of oversampling participants on
Therefore, it appears that Negative Valence accounts for unique
borderline PD features) to ascertain whether these results would generalize
variance in personality pathology above and beyond the FFM to the larger population of young adults (see Trull, 2001). The regression
despite its moderately high correlations with some domains of the results were generally consistent with those from the unweighted data in
FFM. Although we predicted that Negative Valence would be terms of what personality domains predicted each symptom count or
associated with borderline, avoidant, and dependent PD symptoms, diagnosis and the incremental contributions of each model of personality in
we did not anticipate that Negative Valence would also predict predicting psychopathology.
366 DURRETT AND TRULL

Table 5
Regression Results Predicting Personality Disorder Symptom Counts From Five-Factor Model (FFM) and Big Seven Scores,
Controlling for Axis I Disorder

Model 1 Model 2

Sex Axis I diagnosis Sex Axis I diagnosis Significant predictors at


Predictor FFM R2 Big Seven added ⌬R2 Big Seven R2 FFM added ⌬R2 ␤ last step

Cluster A
Paranoid 0.09*** 0.03 0.10*** 0.02 .23** Negative Valence
Schizoid 0.04* 0.01 0.04* 0.01
Schizotypal 0.05** 0.05* (.22) 0.09*** 0.01 .23** Negative Valence
⫺.13* Positive Valence
Cluster B
Antisocial 0.33*** 0.04** (.20) 0.34*** 0.03** (.17) .12* Sex
.37*** Any Axis I diagnosis
.16** Negative Valence
⫺.16* Negative Emotionality
⫺.19** Conventionality
⫺.18* Conscientiousness (FFM)
⫺.13* Agreeableness (FFM)
Borderline 0.23*** 0.08*** (.28) 0.29*** 0.02 .31*** Negative Valence
.20* Neuroticism
Histrionic 0.24*** 0.03 0.24*** 0.02 .20* Extraversion
Narcissistic 0.15*** 0.02 0.13*** 0.04* (.20) ⫺.22** Agreeableness (FFM)
Cluster C
Avoidant 0.14*** 0.05** (.20) 0.17*** 0.02 .22** Negative Valence
Dependent 0.11*** 0.04* (.20) 0.12*** 0.03 .20** Negative Valence
.25* Neuroticism
Obsessive–compulsive 0.10*** 0.07*** (.26) 0.16*** 0.01 .19** Negative Valence
⫺.22** Agreeableness (Big 7)

Note. Effect sizes appear in parentheses.


* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

Big Seven, whereas Negative Valence did not emerge as a predic- were not significantly associated with narcissistic symptom counts,
tor of any form of Axis I pathology. This suggests a possible role but these scores were significantly, negatively associated with
of self-evaluation in Axis I disorders, but further research is schizotypal symptoms as well as with substance use and eating
necessary to confirm these findings. disorders. The lack of predictable, differential associations with
One may argue that these findings appear to support a distinc- personality or Axis I psychopathology that are theoretically coher-
tion between Axis I and II disorders from the perspective of ent call into question the utility of these evaluative terms in clinical
positive and negative evaluation. Lifetime Axis I disorders are assessment.3
associated with the personality trait of low-positive self-evaluation These findings also highlight that there are different aspects or
(feeling ordinary or plain) but not negative self-evaluation, dimensions of incremental validity (Haynes & Lench, 2003; Hun-
whereas PDs are associated with the personality trait of high- sley & Meyer, 2003). In the present case, Big Seven scores do
negative self-evaluation (feeling evil or awful) but not positive account for significant increments of variance in several psycho-
self-evaluation. One possible explanation for this finding is that pathology scores, meeting the general requirement for “incremen-
PD symptoms, which are by definition more chronic and pervasive tal validity.” However, because of a lack of predicted differential
in nature, may be viewed by patients as ego-syntonic—patients, in association between Negative Valence scores (or Positive Valence
a sense, see their symptoms or deficits as part of their own scores) and psychopathology scores, it is difficult to advocate for
personality (APA, 2000). Thus, those with PD symptoms may be the use of these evaluative dimensions in clinical assessment (see
more inclined to endorse negative, evaluative terms in describing
Haynes & Lench, 2003, for an extended discussion of these and
their own personality (e.g., vicious, cruel, mentally disturbed) than
related issues).
those who suffer from episodic Axis I disorders. These latter
Focusing on Negative Valence, these findings call into question
disorders are more likely to be seen as ego-dystonic and as foreign
the interpretation of this domain as an indicator of low self-esteem.
to one’s “typical” self or personality style.
Perhaps more important, this finding supports the argument that
However, these results also suggest that negative and positive
evaluative terms do not reflect specific aspects of one’s personality
evaluation scores are limited in their diagnostic utility. These
scores are associated with a wide range of personality pathology
and Axis I pathology, respectively. Contrary to predictions, Neg- 3
Although beyond the scope of this article, which primarily focused on
ative Valence scores were significantly associated with paranoid, FFM domain scores, it is important to note that a number of studies have
schizotypal, antisocial, and obsessive– compulsive PD symptoms found that facet scores from the NEO-PI-R do show differential correla-
and not with mood or anxiety disorders. Positive Valence scores tions with the DSM PDs (Trull & Durrett, 2005).
FFM AND THE BIG SEVEN 367

but rather an evaluation of one’s personality traits. Those with oversampled relative to other PDs, and, as a result, there was less
personality features may endorse Negative Valence items because variation in symptoms of Clusters A and C PD than in Cluster B
they feel bad about their personality traits, traits that create dis- PDs. By using weighted data, we did find evidence to suggest that
tress, as well as social and occupational impairment. However, our findings would generalize to a nonclinical young adult popu-
these terms will not aid in the differentiation between maladaptive lation of similar age (see Footnote 2). However, it will be impor-
personality styles, a differentiation that is critical for clinical tant to replicate these results in clinical samples that are more
assessment and one that is afforded by FFM scores (Widiger & diverse in age and background and exhibit, on average, more
Costa, 2002). It will be important to attempt to replicate these extreme PD pathology.
findings in clinical samples as well as to determine whether
clinical and nonclinical participants may have different views of
their own PD symptoms. References
Another potential limitation of these evaluative scales in clinical
Almagor, M., Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The Big Seven model:
assessment is that one may not expect to obtain the same findings
A cross-cultural replication and further exploration of the basic dimen-
for evaluative terms if peer or collateral ratings were used. Other sions of natural language trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and
persons would not likely label a dependent, avoidant, or borderline Social Psychology, 69, 300 –307.
individual as evil, awful, or cruel (although, perhaps, one could American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-
make a case for the possibility of a peer labeling an antisocial ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
inmate this way). Thus, self- versus other-reports of evaluative American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
terms may not converge. However, this is in contrast to ratings of ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
personality traits, provided by self- versus other formats of per- Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2001). A theoretical basis for the major
sonality items. For example, self- versus other-ratings of the NEO- dimensions of personality. European Journal of Personality, 15, 327–
PI-R personality facets and domains have been shown to converge 353.
Bagby, R. M., Bindseil, K. D., Schuller, D. R., Rector, N. A., Young, N. L.,
in several studies (McCrae & Costa, 2003). In these cases, what is
Cooke, R. G., et al. (1997). Relationship between the five-factor model
being rated are specific personality features not the respondent’s of personality and unipolar, bipolar and schizophrenic patients. Psychi-
evaluation of the target’s personality. atry Research, 70, 83–94.
Another interesting finding from this study is that although Benet, V., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The Big Seven factor model of
Neuroticism and Negative Emotionality are correlated at .79, only personality description: Evidence for its cross-cultural generality in a
Neuroticism proved a significant predictor of Axis I pathology. Spanish sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
One possible contributing factor may be the difference in the 701–718.
reliability of the two scales, which differ in length. However, this Benet-Martinez, V., & Waller, N. G. (1997). Further evidence for the
alone seems insufficient to account for this finding. We conducted cross-cultural generality of the Big Seven factor model: Indigenous and
further analyses to explore this result, focusing on the facet level imported Spanish personality constructs. Journal of Personality, 65,
567–598.
scales of the FFM. NEO-PI-R Neuroticism has six facet scales:
Bienvenu, O. J., Nestadt, G., Samuels, J. F., Costa, P. T., Howard, W. T.,
Anxiety, Angry Hostility, Depression, Self-Consciousness, Vul- & Eaton, W. W.. (2001). Phobic, panic, and major depressive disorders
nerability, and Impulsiveness. Negative Emotionality scores were and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Nervous & Mental
correlated with the first five facet scale scores between .57 and .71 Disease, 189, 154 –161.
but were correlated less with Impulsiveness scores (.46). In hier- Blumberg, H. H. (2001). The common ground of natural language and
archical logistic regressions predicting each of the Axis I diagnos- social interaction in personality description. Journal of Research in
tic variables from the FFM Neuroticism facets and Big Seven Personality, 35, 289 –312.
Negative Emotionality scores, Negative Emotionality did not ac- Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., & Reyes, J. A. S. (1998). Further explo-
count for variance above and beyond the Neuroticism facets in any ration of Filipino personality structure using the lexical approach: Do the
case. However, the Impulsiveness facet accounted for significant Big-Five or Big-Seven dimensions emerge? European Journal of Per-
sonality, 12, 249 –269.
variance in mood, substance use, and eating disorder diagnoses.
Cohen, J. A. (1992). Power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
Therefore, Impulsiveness is significantly and independently pre-
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality
dictive of Axis I pathology but is not fully captured in the Negative Inventory (NEO-PIR) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Pro-
Emotionality scale. This evidence again suggests some limitation fessional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
of predictive utility in using the IPC7-short form as a clinical Dolan-Sewell, R. T., Krueger, R. F., & Shea, M. T. (2001). Co-occurrence
assessment tool. with syndrome disorders. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of person-
In summary, Positive and Negative Valence were significantly ality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 84 –104). New
related to personality and Axis I and II psychopathology. Although York: Guilford Press.
these evaluative scales were significantly and uniquely related to First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1995).
several forms of psychopathology, these relations were, at times, Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM–IV Disorders, nonpatient
edition (SCID-I/NP, Version 2.0). New York: New York State Psychi-
inconsistent with theoretical predictions. Therefore, based on these
atric Institute.
results, their utility in clinical assessment seems questionable. It is
Fiske, D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality
important to note that this study had several limitations that could ratings from different sources. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology,
be fruitfully addressed with additional research. The sample con- 44, 329 –344.
sisted of 20-year-old young adults, of whom few fully met criteria Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The
for a PD, although a substantial proportion had personality pathol- Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
ogy at a subdiagnostic level. Also, borderline PD features were 59, 1216 –1229.
368 DURRETT AND TRULL

Hayaki, J., Friedman, M. A., & Brownell, K. D. (2002). Shame and Characteristics #7-short form (IPC7). Minneapolis: Department of Psy-
severity of bulimic symptoms. Eating Behaviors, 3, 73– 83. chology, University of Minnesota.
Haynes, S. N., & Lench, H. C. (2003). Incremental validity of new clinical Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1987). Reexamining basic dimensions of
assessment measures. Psychological Assessment, 15, 456 – 466. natural language trait descriptors. Paper presented at the 95th Annual
Hunsley, J., & Meyer, G. J. (2003). The incremental validity of psycho- Convention of the American Psychological Association, New York, NY.
logical testing and assessment: Conceptual, methodological, and statis- Trull, T. J. (1992). DSM–III–R personality disorders and the five-factor
tical issues. Psychological Assessment, 15, 446 – 455. model of personality: An empirical comparison. Journal of Abnormal
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1995). Positive and negative valence Psychology, 101, 553–560.
within the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, Trull, T. J. (1995). Borderline personality disorder features in nonclinical
443– 460. young adults: I. Identification and validation. Psychological Assessment,
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A 7, 33– 41.
five-factor theory perspective (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Trull, T. J. (2001). Structural relations between borderline personality
Morey, L. C. (1991). The Personality Assessment Inventory professional disorder features and putative etiological correlates. Journal of Abnor-
mal Psychology, 110, 471– 481.
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Trull, T. J., & Durrett, C. A. (2005). Categorical and dimensional models
Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality
of personality disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1,
attributes: Replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality
355–380.
ratings. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 66, 574 –583.
Trull, T. J., & Sher, K. J. (1994). Relationship between the five-factor
O’Connor, L. E., Berry, J. W., Weiss, J., & Gilbert, P. (2002). Guilt, fear,
model of personality and Axis I disorders in a nonclinical sample.
submission, and empathy in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 350 –360.
71, 19 –27. Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. C. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based
Pfohl, B., Blum, N., & Zimmerman, M. (1997). Structured Interview for on trait ratings (USAF WADC Tech. Note No. 61–97). Lacklund Air
DSM–IV Personality. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Force Base, TX: United States Air Force.
Publishing. Waller, N. G., & Zavala, J. D. (1993). Evaluating the Big Five. Psycho-
Saucier, G. (1997). Effects of variable selection on the factor structure of logical Inquiry, 4, 131–135.
person descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, Widiger, T. A., & Costa, P. T. (2002). Five-factor model personality
1296 –1312. disorder research. In P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1998). What is beyond the Big Five? disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 59 – 87).
Journal of Personality, 66, 495–524. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
Schiller, R., Tellegen, A., & Evens, J. (1995). An idiographic and nomo- Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1989). Conceptions of personality disor-
thetic study of personality description. In J. N. Butcher & C. D. Spiel- ders and dimensions of personality. Psychological Assessment: A Jour-
berger (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 10, pp. 1–23). nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 305–316.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tellegen, A. (1993). Folk concepts and psychological concepts of person- Received September 17, 2004
ality and personality disorder. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 122–130. Revision received February 3, 2005
Tellegen, A., Grove, W. M., & Waller, N. G. (1991). Inventory of Personal Accepted April 12, 2005 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like