You are on page 1of 25

Chapter 12

Emotion and Sport


Performance: Stress, Anxiety,
Arousal, and Choking
Mark C. Frame and Sydney Reichin
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

According to Tom Hanks’s character in the 1992 DEFINING CONCEPTS


film A League of Their Own, “There’s no crying in
Most athletes understand what it means to experi-
baseball!” (Abbott, Greenhut, & Marshall, 1992).
ence an emotion, whether the feeling is the joy
However, everyone who has ever taken part in
resulting from a successful performance, supporting
sports as a child, played as an amateur athlete, or
a teammate, or disappointment about letting others
participated professionally in sports knows that
down. However, although most people understand
there are many emotions involved in sports compe-
what an emotional experience is, researchers and
tition. Much has been written about emotions
scholars have yet to agree on an accepted defini-
such as stress, arousal, anxiety, and choking in tion of emotion (Derouesné, 2011), especially as it
the realm of sports and sport psychology, but our relates to sport psychology (Hanin, 2007; Vallerand
level of understanding of these phenomena is & Blanchard, 2000). Rather than define emotion,
far from complete. This chapter presents current many researchers seem to accept that no definition
perspectives, theories, and models relating to the of emotion will be agreed upon and have focused
role of emotion in sports performance. Models instead on the facets, categories, and subcomponents
and theories relating to the role of stress, arousal, of emotion (Hanin, 2007; Vallerand & Blanchard,
and anxiety in athletic performance are reviewed. 2000). Providing a universally accepted definition
Of particular emphasis is the phenomenon of of emotion is challenging, but doing so is critical to
choking, as well as the inverse phenomenon of understanding the impact that emotions play in sport
clutch performance. We conclude the chapter by and all forms of performance; it provides a foundation
highlighting practical considerations and the appli- for understanding the various dimensions, categories,
cation of these topics by discussing future research facets, and aspects of emotion about which sport
directions. psychologists have theorized and researched.
A consistent challenge in the study of emotion
in athletic performance is that related concepts Emotion
such as stress, arousal, and anxiety have often Emotions, much like personality, memory, language,
been used interchangeably, which is incorrect. The intelligence, and other higher order mental func-
concepts are not synonymous and do not accurately tions, are dynamic and complex. Human emotions,
reflect how they are measured or conceptualized in particular, are abstract hypothetical constructs
in theoretical models. Thus, it is necessary to first that influence behavior and are adaptive, temporary,
define these concepts. and psychophysiological reactions to interactions

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0000123-012
APA Handbook of Sport and Exercise Psychology: Vol. 1. Sport Psychology, M. H. Anshel (Editor-in-Chief)
Copyright © 2019 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.

219
APA Handbook of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Volume 1: Sport Psychology,
edited by M. H. Anshel, T. A. Petrie, and J. A. Steinfeldt
Copyright © 2019 American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Frame and Reichin

with the environment. Emotions are mediated by the the cause of the anxiety (Ekman, 1999). In this situ-
personal significance one associates with the situation ation, regulating or reducing the emotion of anxiety
(Derouesné, 2011; Lazarus, 1991, 2000). Emotions could lead to improved physical performance, but
involve cognitive and biological processes and if an athlete is experiencing a substantially anxious
multiple functional regions of the brain, which result mood, there likely is no antecedent that is discernible
in conscious sensations and feelings (Derouesné, to the athlete. In addition to the emotional interven-
2011). Emotions are not unidimensional phenomena; tion, an effective strategy for regulating or improving
they may be experienced in response to a real or the athlete’s mood might include having the athlete
imagined environmental stimulus or event (see also engage in physical exercise, listen to music, or some
Chapter 13, this volume). Although there is consid- other cognitive or behavioral strategy (Thayer, 1996).
erable communality in emotions, two athletes may Although the concepts of mood and emotion are
experience similar emotions in different situations distinct, the two may in fact influence each other.
and for different reasons. Similarly, the same game, For instance, an athlete who feels a negative mood
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

sporting event, or situation may result in different is more likely to recall previous failures and other
emotional experiences in different athletes. Human negative or disappointing experiences. The cogni-
emotions such as happiness, sadness, anger, and tive availability of negative recollections could result
fear can be characterized as beginning rapidly, being in the athlete experiencing negative emotions when
short or limited in duration, experienced as specific faced with performance demands. In some cases, the
feelings, and behaviorally manifested in a common focus on negative experiences and failures can become
manner (e.g., facial expressions; Uphill, McCarthy, & continual and repetitive—a process called rumination.
Jones 2009). Rumination is a maladaptive cognitive process
in which a person constantly and persistently thinks
Mood
about her or his failures and the causes and conse-
Moods are related to emotions and may exhibit
quences of those failures (Valderrama, Miranda, &
similar feelings and sensations. Moods are enduring
Jeglic, 2016). Rumination that involves ascertaining
and general hypothetical constructs that are not
the reason for the negative mood in an effort to solve
associated with particular objects or events. Moods
problems and improve one’s mood is termed reflection;
are the result of psychological and physiological
processes and states that may ultimately influence it is considered less maladaptive than brooding, which
an athlete’s cognitive processes (Beedie, Terry, Lane, is rumination that dwells on feelings of distress, nega-
& Devonport, 2011). A clear distinction between tive mood, and negative thoughts, and is not focused
emotion and mood is fundamental in understanding on helpful problem-solving (Valderrama, Miranda,
both the research and the applied implications of & Jeglic, 2016). Athletes’ moods play an important
research on sports performance, emotion, and mood. role in the emotional experiences they have prior to,
A mood is a diffuse and enduring feeling that may during, and after competition. A positive mood can
influence an athlete’s cognitive process but is not enhance feelings of exhilaration and mitigate feelings
focused on or caused by a specific event or object. of disappointment, whereas a negative mood can tamp
Emotions and moods may be experienced simulta- down feelings of excitement and can compound and
neously, but an athlete may be unable to pinpoint exacerbate feelings of failure.
the reason for a given mood. For this reason, mood
detection is a necessary component of strategies and Stress
interventions to improve athletic performance by Athletes encounter different types of stress before
regulating emotions and changing mood states. and during sports competition. Stress is a psycho­
For example, because emotions influence behaviors logical and physiological response to an environmental
(e.g., sport skills) and have specific antecedents, one demand often referred to as a stressor (Thomas,
approach to assist an athlete challenged by anxious Mellalieu, & Hanton, 2009). Stressors can be inter-
emotions would be to consciously regulate selected preted by an athlete as positive or negative, and
emotions and to reappraise both the situation and as such, a stress response can also be experienced

220
Emotion and Sport Performance

as positive or negative. The stress experienced in cardiovascular disease, increased blood pressure,
response to a given stressor depends upon the dura- anxiety, muscle pain, and even cancer (American
tion and intensity of the environmental demand. In Psychological Association, 2010; Dhabhar, 2012).
some cases, an athlete reacts to these stressful events The health concerns related to stress are significant
in the short term, while other—chronic—stressors and can affect the behaviors of individuals who are
can last for weeks, months, or years. A short-term engaged in athletic pursuits.
stressor, also known as an acute stressor, is an Researchers have long recognized that stress
unexpected, different, and short-duration event can result in negative health outcomes. Physical
or stimuli that interrupts or stymies behavior and ailments such as cardiovascular disease (e.g., Dyck
necessitates an immediate response (Salas, Driskell, & Roithmayr, 2002) and diabetes (Quick, Quick,
& Hughes, 1996). Acute stressors, regardless of the Nelson, & Hurrell, 1997) have been linked to chronic
magnitude, are events or situations that occur for a stress. Although the negative effects of stress are more
short duration and may occur unexpectedly. Acute familiar to most people, stress may also yield positive
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

stress abounds in sport. Basketball players receiving outcomes, particularly in competitive sport. When
criticisms from a coach experience acute stress an athlete is cognitively and emotionally involved in
(Doron & Bourbousson, 2017) as do runners who the demands of the task at hand, that athlete is more
lose their footing during a race or cyclists who get likely to focus on performing that task than an athlete
bumped or knocked down during competition. who is not as involved in the current activity. Elevated
A chronic stressor occurs over a relatively long amounts of stress demand that an individual’s atten-
period of time. Chronic stress may be caused by a tion be focused on the present task (Tetrick & Quick,
single stressor that requires an individual to respond 2003). While some researchers (e.g., Lazarus, 2000)
over a long duration, or it may result from repeated have argued that stress and emotion should be inves-
exposure to several short-term stressors in series tigated as one complex issue, much of the research
(Furrer, Moen, & Firing, 2015). Months of excessive on stress and emotions has examined the topics sepa-
training and insufficient recovery time can result in rately; thus, they will be considered as two separate
chronic stress, as can performance demands from topics in this chapter.
coaches, persistent interpersonal conflicts with team
members, and long-term injuries. In the literature, Anxiety
these have also been referred to as work overload, Sport anxiety is a learned negative emotional reaction
social overload, social tensions, pressure to perform, and to a perceived demand or threat that has not actually
chronic worrying (Sallen, Hirschmann, & Herrmann, occurred. In the context of competitive sport, anxiety
2018). Thus, acute stress experienced with high is an athlete’s reaction to a situation that may or may
frequency or successively over time may lead to the not happen in the future. Anxiety can be categorized
development of chronic stress if the athlete is not able as trait anxiety and state anxiety (Spielberger, 1966,
to return to a resting state between acute stressors 1972). Trait anxiety is an individual’s relatively stable
(Dhabhar, 2012). Ignoring or failing to cope effec- tendency to perceive a wide range of circumstances
tively with acute stressors may also lead to chronic and situations as demanding or threatening. State
stress (American Psychological Association, 2010). anxiety, on the other hand, is a temporary response
Although acute stress may positively affect people to a specific pending situation that is perceived as
in some situations, chronic stress generally does threatening (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo,
not have the same positive affect. Chronic stress 2007). A kicker would likely feel state anxiety when
has become a significant issue that results in a preparing for a field goal that could win the game. A
myriad of negative health, social, and economic batter would feel state anxiety going up to bat against
outcomes (C. Smith, Hancock, Blake-Mortimer, & a pitcher who has a history of striking the batter out.
Eckert, 2007). Some health problems associated with In sport psychology research, state anxiety is often
chronic stress are impairment in brain functioning, a variable of interest, whereas trait anxiety is often
psychological depression, immunosuppression, examined as a measure of an athlete’s inclination

221
Frame and Reichin

to experience state anxiety in competition. Many muscle activity, and results from electroencepha-
athletes experience some level of anxiety before a lograph are often reported as physiological indica-
competition, but for most, the anxious feelings start tions of arousal, as are analyses of chemicals such
just prior to the competition, may continue during as cortisol, epinephrine, and norepinephrine. The
the competition, and diminish after the competi- cognitive aspects of arousal have been assessed using
tion is over. This would be an example an athlete self-report methods such as the Modified Somatic
experiencing state anxiety, but trait anxiety impacts Perception Questionnaire and the Activation–
how strong the feeling of anxiety is in different Deactivation Adjective Check List (Gould, Greenleaf,
athletes when they are anticipating a competition. & Krane, 2002; Hackfort & Schwenkmezger, 1993).
One athlete might feel slightly nervous leading up to Researchers have examined both forms of arousal
a tournament, whereas another might feel overcome in terms of its intensity and have suggested that an
with anxiety and get nauseous or faint. An athlete athlete may have an optimal level of arousal, as well
with a high degree of trait anxiety is more likely to as nonoptimal levels.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

have anxious feelings in more situations then an An athlete experiencing a low level of arousal
athlete with lower levels of trait anxiety (Wolf, Eys, is typically motivated to engage in behaviors that
& Kleinert, 2015). are oriented toward a goal or competition that will
Anxiety is multifaceted, but some also contend likely increase the athlete’s arousal. Optimal arousal
that it is multidimensional, arguing that anxiety occurs when an athlete is aroused at a level that
manifests in somatic and cognitive response (Cox, enhances performance by stimulating the athlete
Martens, & Russell, 2003; Edwards, Hardy, Kingston, to perform well by focusing energy and effort on
& Gould, 2002). Somatic anxiety is physiological the required tasks (Ribble, 2010; Schmidt, Mussel,
response (e.g., sweating, upset stomach, increased & Hewig, 2013). Too much arousal can be detri-
heart rate) to an athletic event, tournament, compe- mental to the athlete’s performance, as can too little
tition, or other situation that is perceived as threat- arousal (Berlyne, 1960; Ribble, 2010). An athlete’s
ening. Cognitive anxiety, on the other hand, is a optimal arousal level is unique to the individual
mental response to a perceived threat that mani- athlete and is a function of the level of arousal the
fests as negative concerns, unpleasant thoughts, athlete experiences at rest and the situation and
and worries and concerns regarding performance circumstances the athlete experiences when not
(Janelle, 2002). Cognitive anxiety can manifest as a at rest (Ribble, 2010; Schmidt, Mussel, & Hewig,
loss of confidence in skills, an inability to concen- 2013; Van der Lei, Tenenbaum, & Land, 2016). For
trate on the tasks at hand, difficulty making deci- example, a well-practiced and typically calm golfer
sions during the game, or detrimental thoughts or may engage in preshot routines before a critical shot
concerns about one’s athletic ability or performance in an effort to increase her level of arousal prior to
(Martiny et al., 2015). making the shot. When making the shot, the golfer
has ideally reached an optimal degree of arousal and
Arousal is focused on the shot she is making. If however, she
Arousal is a multidimensional, nondirectional mix of is gets into a verbal altercation with a member of the
psychological and physiological activation (Landers, gallery, the golfer may experience too much arousal
Arent, & Lutz, 2001) that can vary in intensity along and her ability to make the shot could be impaired.
a continuum ranging from unconsciousness or sleep
to excitement and extreme exhilaration. Arousal is Choking
responsible for harnessing the body’s resources for Choking, in the context of sports performance, relates
activity (Arent & Landers, 2003). The physiological to situations in which events appraised as stressful
aspects of arousal have been measured using elec- produce heightened anxiety that results in signifi-
trophysiological, biochemical, and cardiovascular/ cantly impaired motor performance (Marquardt,
respiratory methods. Blood pressure, resting heart 2009). For example, during a final field goal attempt
rate, rate of respiration, galvanic skin response, during a 2006 playoff game for the National Football

222
Emotion and Sport Performance

League, Tony Romo—serving as holder—bobbled the were originally presented. Stress, anxiety, arousal,
ball, which resulted in the kicker (Martín Gramática) and choking in the context of sport performance
being unable to make a relatively routine 19-yard have been examined with both qualitative and
field goal (Corbett, 2007). Choking, however, is more quantitative research. As with other applied fields
than an instance of poor performance; it is perfor- of psychology, research examining the relationship
mance at a level significantly poorer than expecta- between these emotional factors and performance
tions. Choking can be attributed to the importance can have an experimental design, quasi-experimental
or criticality of a situation rather than random fluc- design, cross-sectional or survey design, time-series
tuations in skill or performance (Beilock & Gray, design, longitudinal design, or case study design.
2007). Choking represents an acute, momentous, An experimental design is used to determine if
and dramatic performance decline that is directly an independent variable has an effect upon a chosen
and clearly a response to some perceived pressure dependent variable. In an experimental design, the
(Hill, Hanton, Matthews, & Fleming, 2010). While researcher may opt to manipulate the level of stress,
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

an athlete may choke under pressure on more than anxiety, or arousal experienced (independent vari-
one occasion, the unexpected nature of choking ables) by an athlete and determine the effect that the
distinguishes it from longer term performance change in stress, anxiety, or arousal has on athletic
declines such as yips and slumps. Whereas choking performance (the dependent variable). However, a
is unexpected poor performance under pressure, researcher could ask athletes to participate in an
yips are longer term movement or motor-skill prob- intervention or training (the independent variable)
lems that repeatedly and systematically interfere with and seek to determine the effect of the intervention
the execution of controlled behaviors and perfor- on the dependent variables of the athletes’ stress,
mance (McDaniel, Cummings, & Shain, 1989; Weiss anxiety, or arousal. Many of these experimental
& Reber, 2012). Similarly, a slump is a prolonged time studies occur in controlled laboratory settings.
in which an athlete continuously performs below the Researchers may employ a quasi-experimental
level that one might expect but the decrease in perfor- design when working in a natural environment in
mance is unexplainable and occurs across many, if not which the conditions required (random selection
all, situations and cannot be attributed to high-stakes and random assignment) for an experiment cannot
situations (Ball, 2013; Gray, 2004). be met. A cross-sectional or survey design is very
common and involves collecting data from interviews
or questionnaires. A time-series design measures the
RESEARCH METHODS RELATED TO
dependent variable across a specified time period to
EMOTION AND SPORT PERFORMANCE
ascertain the effect of the independent variable on
Stress, anxiety, arousal, and choking are both the dependent variable. Time-series designs should
psychological and biological phenomena. As not, however, be confused with longitudinal studies
such, the research examining these concepts as in which data are collected from a person or group
they relate to sport performance utilizes a variety over an extended period of time. Case study research
of methodologies. Research into these related is when the researchers examine multiple aspects of
constructs is often conducted as primary research a specific situation or scenario (case) to determine
in which original data has been collected and the environment and context in which a particular
analyzed. However, researchers may use data from phenomenon occurred. Although case studies are
archival sources such as an athletic organization’s useful in gaining an understanding of some issues and
annual reports, governmental databases or publi- relationships, they are anecdotal and often are used to
cations, or previously published research articles. develop testable theories and future empirical inves-
When research is conducted using existing data tigations. As noted earlier, in regards to the assess-
sources, it is said to be secondary research and it ment of both the subjective and objective measures of
typically involves reanalyzing data or analyzing athletic performance, the use of multiple methods for
data in a way that is different from the way the data collecting data to better understand a phenomenon

223
Frame and Reichin

will allow for more conclusive and robust findings. Long, 1995; Solomonov, Avugos, & Bar-Eli, 2015).
Triangulation refers to the practice of using multiple Objective measures of performance can also be
methods and sources to collect data in an attempt to tallied for more complex tasks, groups of tasks, or for
examine a variable of interest. Triangulation increases competition contest outcomes in which a series of
the validity of a study by providing corroborating tasks and behaviors are required. Examples include
findings between qualitative and quantitative data. the number of bouts won by fencers (Doron &
Much as the emotion of athletes can be assessed, Gaudreau, 2014), golfing scores (Gaudreau, Nicholls,
both using objective assessments (i.e., physiol­ & Levy, 2010), performance on indoor rowing
ogical, biochemical, and cardiovascular/respiratory tasks (Hatzigeorgiadis, 2006), and bowling scores
measures) and subjective measures (i.e., self-report (Woodman, Albinson, & Hardy, 1997). All of these
measures of anxiety, stress, and arousal, the Trier measures have been used in research to operationalize
Inventory for Chronic Stress; Sallen, Hirschmann, performance. Although objective performance data
& Herrmann, 2018), the outcome of athletic perfor- are typically reliable and relatively straightforward to
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

mance can be measured using both objective and collect and interpret, they do not provide insight into
subjective methods. Athletic performance is often a the subjective aspects of athletic performance, which
key variable of interest in research relating to stress, are often critical to investigations of stress, anxiety,
anxiety, and arousal in sport psychology. Similarly, the arousal, and choking in sport performance.
degree to which an athlete does or does not perform
well is critical when discussing choking. Thus, it Subjective Measures of Sport Performance
is important to explain the manner in which sport Athletic performance is subjectively measured by
performance can be operationally defined in research having individuals rate or report on their percep-
examining stress, anxiety, arousal, and choking. tions or feelings about the quality of their athletic
performance, their ability to achieve their goals, or
Objective Measures of Sport Performance their satisfaction with their athletic performance
Athletic performance by its very nature lends (Laborde, Dosseville, & Kinrade, 2014; Nicholls,
itself to objective measures of success. The athlete Polman, & Levy, 2010, 2012). Essentially, the idea
either hits the ball or does not, sinks the basket behind these ratings is that athletes’ satisfaction
or misses, blocks the shot or does not. Counting with their effort and goal attainment can be used as
the occurrences of discrete behaviors is often used subjective measures of performance. This is because
in research to assess the performance of an indi- athletes generally have some reference point for
vidual or group (i.e., team). Researchers have used their performance satisfaction, and they typically
a myriad of objective performance data, from the have some criteria by which they gauge their actions
ability of participants to hit a virtual golf ball, from (Gaudreau & Blondin, 2004).
a virtual tee, into a virtual hole (Witt, Linkenauger, When measuring athletes’ levels of fulfilment
& Proffitt, 2012); the batting averages of baseball or gratification with their sport or their sport
players (R. E. Smith & Christensen, 1995); the performance, many researchers use scales with
U.S. football kicker’s field goal percentages (i.e., the known reliability and validity, such as the Sport
number of successful field goals made divided by Satisfaction Instrument (Duda & Nicholls, 1992;
the number of field goals attempted for a season by Granero-Gallegos, Baena-Extremera, Gómez-
a kicker); the number of field goal attempts made López, & Abraldes, 2014) or the Satisfaction with
by basketball players and the number of assists, Sport Scale (Baudin, Blanch, Rolland, Martí-Guiu, &
foul drawings, and free-throw attempts of basket- Aluja, 2014; Diener & Emmons, 1984). Researchers
ball players, as well as the free-throw percentage may also use nonsport psychological measures
(i.e., the number of free throws made divided by adapted for sport to assess athlete satisfaction
the number of free throws attempted for a season); (Gaudreau & Braaten, 2016; Gaudreau, Gunnell,
and the number of turnovers, steals, rebounds, Hoar, Thompson, & Lelièvre, 2015). Similarly, when
and points scored by basketball players (Haney & assessing athletes’ perceptions relating to achieving

224
Emotion and Sport Performance

their performance goals, there are scales such as the of affect is a circular model of emotion first proposed
Sport Achievement Goal Scale (Amiot, Gaudreau, & by James A. Russell (1980). Specifically, he proposed
Blanchard, 2004; Gaudreau & Antl, 2008; Nicolas, “affective states falling in a circular order in a two-
Martinent, & Campo, 2014). These scales gener- dimensional bipolar space, the axes of which are
ally provide a sound psychometric foundation for interpretable as pleasure-displeasure and degree of
measuring sports satisfaction and satisfaction with arousal” (p. 1171).
their sport performance. In contrast, researchers who
have examined subjective ratings of performance Prototypical emotional episodes.   Russell and Barett
have used single item measures on a 10- or 100-point (1999) described a prototypical emotional episode as
Likert rating scale (Laborde et al., 2014; Nicholls some overt behavior in relation to a person, condition,
et al., 2010; Nicholls, Polman, & Levy, 2012). event or thing (e.g., striking someone out of anger).
As one might expect, some researchers operation- They also noted the subevents of these episodes as
ally define athletic performance using both objec- including an antecedent, an appraisal, an attribu-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

tive and subjective methods. Performance–goal tion, core affect, cognitive processes, behaviors, and
discrepancy is one such method that assesses both physiological processes. Russell and Barett suggested
objective and subjective performance indicators. This that these episodes can be structured in dimensions
method involves asking athletes to set attainable goals (i.e., variations in amount, gradation of pleasure and
for a given athletic performance or event and then amount of activation) and hierarchies (i.e., higher
creating a difference score by subtracting the goal from order emotions with various facets). These proto-
their actual performance (Gaudreau & Blondin, 2004; typical emotional episodes may result from a sudden
Gaudreau, Blondin, & Lapierre, 2002). The mecha- change in core affect. An intense prototypical emo-
nism of combining objective and subjective sport tional episode might be winning first place in a race,
performance is not as important as the methodological scoring the winning goal for one’s team, successfully
improvements and theoretical advancements that making a defensive play that decides the game in one’s
could be gained by continued attempts to combine favor, or missing a kick that would have won the game
these measures of performance outcomes. for one’s team. Less intense prototypical emotional
episodes might involve coaching a teammate to help
THEORIES AND MODELS him perform better or avoiding a teammate because
OF EMOTION AND SPORT he is a rival for one’s position on the team.

After we establish a frame of reference for emotion, Core affect.   Russell and Barrett (1999) noted that
mood, and the specific emotions to be discussed in these emotional episodes can result from a change in
this chapter, we will review selected theories regarding core affect, or any consciously accessible feeling, not
emotions and how they may impact performance. necessarily directed at any person, condition, event,
Although not all of these theories and models were or thing. This is much like the situation with mood.
developed to explain the role of stress, anxiety, This component of a prototypical emotional episode
and arousal as they relate to sport, they each will is activated, which results in the subsequent episode.
be examined and discussed within the context of For example, the core affect of displeasure would
emotion and athletic performance. (See also first be activated, which in turn would lead a person
Chapter 13, this volume.) to avoid her teammate. Russell and Barett noted that
core affect can be described in terms of the degree
Circumplex Model of Affect or amount of pleasantness and activation. The next
Russell and Barrett (1999) attempted to break emotion theory of emotion explores how and through what
down into various facets. Two of these facets include mechanisms emotion can affect performance.
prototypical emotional episodes and core affect. The
following section describes these two components of Relational Meaning
the circumplex model of affect and how the model Lazarus (2000) offered a theory, called relational
relates to emotion and sport. The circumplex model meaning, detailing what specific emotions are

225
Frame and Reichin

aroused in competition, as well as why and how 1995; G. Jones & Swain, 1992, 1995; G. Jones, Swain,
these emotions may affect performance. The crux & Harwood, 1996). A key component to the idea
of the theory revolves around the relational meaning of directional competitive anxiety was the concept
that individuals place on various situations, stem- of control (G. Jones, 1995). In essence, the degree
ming from the perception of a particular situation to which an athlete perceives an ability to control
as harmful, threatening, challenging, or beneficial. a given stressor governs the extent to which the
Further, Lazarus (2000) noted that there are various stressor will be interpreted as facilitative or debilita-
psychological functions influencing performance. tive to successful athletic performance.
These include motivation, attention and concentra-
Guilt/shame.   Lazarus (2000) emphasized the dif-
tion. The emotions presented include anger, anxiety,
ference between guilt and shame. To Lazarus, guilt
guilt/shame, hope, relief, happiness, and pride. Below
was a result of a transgression to morals (e.g., win-
are brief descriptions of how Lazarus (2000) believed
ning against someone whose career is in trouble),
these emotions are provoked, how to cope with these
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

and shame resulted from failing to live up to an


emotions, and the result of these coping efforts.
ideal (e.g., losing a competition). To cope with this
Anger.   Lazarus (2000) suggested that a precursor to emotion, athletes may need to convince themselves
anger is being demeaned or slighted. This makes com- that those feelings are unnecessary, or they will try
petitors feel as if they need to counterattack for revenge to re-create the event that made them feel guilty or
or repair any harm to their self-esteem. He noted that shameful to make it seem unimportant. Athletes
anger can be a result of various sources, including may even go as far as to try and blame others.
spectators, competitors, coaches, and even oneself.
Hope.   Lazarus (2000) stressed the importance of
Anger can impact performance through motivation
hope in competition and defined it as believing that
by decreasing the motivation to perform. However,
better is possible. This is an important construct in
Lazarus (2000) noted that anger can also motivate
relation to competitive sport, because a lack of hope
individuals to perform better and concentrate harder
may lead to a combination of anger, anxiety, guilt,
on competition in an “I’ll show them” type of way.
shame, sadness, and ultimately, poor sport perfor-
Anxiety.   Lazarus (2000) suggested that anxiety mance. However, feeling hopeful can allow athletes to
results from having important values and goals threat- use psychological functions that impact performance,
ened and, particularly, being involved in a situation such as motivation, attention and concentration.
with an ambiguous outcome. Anxiety can actually
Relief.   Relief is relevant to sport performance
intensify during performance. This may happen after
because it consists of athletes’ feelings follow-
a failed effort or a series of failed efforts because an
ing threat or anxiety. Lazarus (2000) noted that,
athlete may start to worry about performing poorly or
as with hope, feeling relief can lead to facilitative
losing. Lazarus (2000) noted that, like anger, anxiety
psychol­ogical functions of motivation, attention,
can also sometimes facilitate performance.
and concentration. However, as with anxiety and
The notion that anxiety can hinder athletic perfor-
anger, being too relaxed may not benefit sport per-
mance in some situations and assist sport perfor-
formance. As noted earlier, athletes that feel under-
mance in other situations was given credence by
arousal may search for and engage in activities with
research conducted by G. Jones and associates in the
the goal of returning to optimal arousal (Ribble,
1990s. G. Jones and colleagues proposed the notion
2010; Schmidt, Mussel, & Hewig, 2013).
of directional interpretations of competitive anxiety
(G. Jones, 1995; G. Jones & Swain, 1992, 1995; Happiness.   Happiness may be produced through
G. Jones, Swain, & Harwood, 1996). Directional triumph or victory, enabling progress to be made
interpretations of competitive anxiety were the degree toward a goal, or achievement of honors, fame, or
to which athletes considered the anxiety as something recognition. Happiness may not always last for very
that would be debilitative (harmful) or facilitative long periods of time, but Lazarus (2000) noted that
(helpful) in their athletic performance (G. Jones, happiness can help maintain motivation. Similarly,

226
Emotion and Sport Performance

positive affect has been found to be associated with theory (Hanin, 1995). Specifically, the IZOF theory
increased energy, improved aspects of cognitive accounts for individual differences in the degree
functioning, enhanced information processing, and to which athletes perceive a given level of arousal
improved visual perception (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, as optimal or nonoptimal. The inverted-U theory
1999; Isen, 1987; Zadra & Clore, 2011). More recent assumes that the bell curve is accurate for all indi-
research suggests that the emotional benefits of posi- viduals; however, the IZOF theory acknowledges that
tive affect may be enhanced when the positive experi- not all athletes are going to respond in the same way
ences are shared with others, for example, teammates to anxiety, stress, and arousal. Instead, IZOF theory
(Lambert et al., 2013). evaluates each athlete’s zone of optimal functioning
(Ruiz, Raglin, & Hanin, 2017). For some athletes,
Pride.  Finally, Lazarus (2000) juxtaposed anger
their optimal zone might be brought about by low
and shame with pride and said that pride results
anxiety. For others, perhaps very high anxiety is related
from an enhancement of one’s social and self-esteem.
with optimal performance. Both models acknowledge
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

As with happiness, relief, and hope, pride can be


that anxiety, stress, and arousal may not always be
motivating. One caveat, however, is that being too
detrimental to performance, but the IZOF model
prideful (e.g., boasting and being cocky) can be
recognizes that the optimal level of these constructs
harmful to social relationships.
varies at the individual level (Ruiz et al., 2017).
All in all, Lazarus (2000) helped us to understand
how these discrete emotions can impact performance
through drawing on relational meanings. Further, he THEORIES AND MODELS
stressed the importance of motivation, attention, and OF STRESS AND SPORT
concentration in sport performance in response to A definition of stress was provided earlier; there are
positive emotional states. Finally, he noted how having theories that may further help one understand the
the ability to cope with these discrete emotions can construct of stress as it relates to sport and athletic
help us maintain the required motivation, attention, performance. Both IZOF and the inverted-U theory
and concentration needed to perform consistently well. have been applied to stress, yet other theories and
modes of stress are of interest to sport psychology
The Inverted-U Theory researchers and practitioners.
The theories discussed so far have focused on anxiety,
stress, and arousal; the inverted-U theory applies to Two-Dimensional Model of Stress
all three of these concepts (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908). Hans Selye (1985) originated the idea that we
The premise of this theory is that while these three should differentiate between good (eustress) and
emotions may have negative undertones, moderate bad (distress) forms of psychosocial stress. He
amounts of these may be beneficial for performance argued that the two types of stress cause different
(Martens & Landers, 1970). Specifically, this theory outcomes because we adapt to the two types differ-
takes the form of a bell curve on a plot. The begin- ently. More recently, researchers defined stress in
ning of the curve would represent low anxiety, this two-dimensional manner. For example, Lepine,
arousal, or stress, resulting in poor performance. Podsakoff, and Lepine (2005) differentiated the
The middle of the curve (at the top) would represent two dimensions as challenge (i.e., good stress) and
how a moderate amount of anxiety, stress, or arousal hindrance (i.e., bad stress). Further, they relied on
results in optimal performance. Finally, the end of concepts from Vroom’s (1964) expectancy theory to
the curve suggests that too much anxiety, stress, or explain their model.
arousal results in poor performance. The expectancy theory framework is used to
explain how hindrance and challenge stressors may
Individual Zones of Optimal Functioning be related to motivation differently and thus result
The individual zones of optimal functioning (IZOF) in different levels of athletic performance. The
theory attempts to improve upon the inverted-U idea is that hindrance and challenge stressors can

227
Frame and Reichin

become associated with perceptions explained in by showing that heart rate (somatic stress) increased
expectancy theory. A stressor may be associated more following threat stress, but the heart rate was
with thoughts or feelings about the relationship unchanged following challenge stress (Williams,
between the effort an athlete will spend dealing Veldhuijzen van Zanten, Trotman, Quinton, &
with the stressor and the likelihood that the athlete Ginty, 2017). In sport, a large increase in heart rate
will experience success (expectancy). There is may be detrimental to performance.
likely a belief regarding the probability of reaching
a desired goal or outcome given the athlete’s Cognitive–Phenomenological Model
successful performance (instrumentality). Finally, Historically, stress research focused on stress from a
the goal or outcome likely has some degree of value physiological or biological viewpoint, that is, until
to the athlete (valance). A challenge stressor is seen the introduction of the cognitive–phenomenological
as a situation in which athletes are able to meet the view of stress (Lazarus & Launier, 1978). The
demand with adequate resources and produce a cognitive–phenomenological approach to stress
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

desired performance goal. Hindrance stressors are suggests that before stress can occur, the individual
those in which athletes believe that no amount of must cognitively appraise a potential stressor as
coping will enable them to meet the demands and threatening or challenging. To encounter stress, the
reach a desired level of performance or goal. individual must perceive an imbalance between
For example, a baseball player at bat, poised to the demands required to deal with the stressor
make a game-winning home run, may perceive the and her or his ability to deal with the stressor
situation as a challenge stressor and be more moti- effectively (Sulsky & Smith, 2005). The cognitive–
vated to conquer the challenge stressors if he feels phenomenological approach suggests that a partic-
(a) that he is likely to hit a home run, (b) that he ular situation could be perceived as stressful to
understands that by hitting a home run, his team some people but viewed as not stressful to others,
will win the game, and (c) that winning the game is depending upon the person’s cognitive appraisal of
a desirable outcome. However, the player might view the scenario and perceived intensity of the stressor.
the same scenario as a hindrance stressor if he is not It has been hypothesized that the degree of stress
confident that he can hit a home run. Likewise, if he that may be endured or the stress threshold may vary
is confident that he can hit a home run but knows among athletes. In other words, some athletes have a
that a home run will not help his team win the game, need for high degree of pressure and others perform
he will view the situation as more of a hindrance. better in less stressful circumstances. This associa-
Finally, even if the player were to hit a home run and tion between stress arousal and performance is often
his team win the game, if he sees no value in winning described as the Yerkes–Dodson law. This model,
the game (because the team’s season is over regard- illustrated by an arch-shaped inverted U, suggests
less of what he does), the player may see the stress of that as stress increases, performance increases up
the situation as a hindrance. to a point. At that point, a person’s performance
In addition to Lepine and colleagues’ (2005) begins to decrease as stress continues to increase
conceptualization of stress, Lazarus and Folkman (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908).
(1984) noted that two types of stress might be
threat and challenge. Threat-type stressors impair Managing and Coping With Stress
performance, whereas challenge-type stressors Although stress can have devastating effects on
facilitate performance. The theory behind the threat– athletic performance, research has uncovered various
challenge conceptualization of stress is that threat interventions and processes that have been successful
stressors trigger negative emotions, which result in at attenuating the stress–performance relationship.
passive or emotional (i.e., ineffective) coping, but One determinant of how stress affects a person is how
challenge stressors trigger positive emotions, which the individual chooses to cope with the stress. As with
promote problem-solving (i.e., effective) coping many of the concepts mentioned, coping is multi­
strategies. Recent research has supported this idea dimensional. Coping is influenced by the situation

228
Emotion and Sport Performance

and individual difference factors. An inability to anxiety (Cunningham & Ashley, 2002), an athlete
effectively cope with stress may result in short-term who learns to perceive anxiety as facilitative may
athletic performance problems and longer term perfor- use less avoidance-focused coping strategies and
mance issues such as burnout. Coping is a conscious have more adaptive behavioral and cognitive results
process that requires both psychological and physical during and after competition (Baghurst, 2012;
efforts that serve to increase the resources one needs Hatzigeorgiadis & Chroni, 2007). However, it may
to deal with stress or to decrease the demands of the be that the perceived level of excitement the athlete
situation (Anshel, Kang, & Miesner, 2010; Carver, experiences during competition plays a role in
Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Krohne, 1993; Lazarus, whether anxiety is perceived as facilitative (Nicholls,
1991). Some have argued that coping can become Polman, Levy, & Hulleman, 2012). Inversely,
an automatic response because of overlearning of a debilitative anxiety was more likely to result in
particular coping strategy (Hardy, Jones, & Gould burnout, a reduced sense of athletic accomplish-
1996), but others contend that if an athlete learns ment, and diminished evaluation of the sport overall
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

to effortlessly eliminate situational stressors from (Gustafsson & Skoog, 2012).


conscious awareness, then he or she is no longer
coping with the stress because the athlete is using Multidimensional Anxiety Theory
a stress management technique (Aldwin, 1994; Martens, Vealey, Burton, and Martens (1990)
Compas, Connor, Osowiecki, & Welch, 1997). proposed a two-dimensional theory of anxiety:
the multi­dimensional anxiety theory (MAT). The
two components are the cognitive component
THEORIES AND MODELS OF ANXIETY
(e.g., fear of losing) and the somatic component
Various theories further help explain the anxiety (i.e., physiological responses). Specifically,
construct. As stated previously, one theory differenti- Martens et al. (1990) noted that the two compo-
ates state versus trait anxiety, but many other theo- nents are independent of each other and there-
ries should be kept in mind when reading the anxiety fore can be stimulated separately. This means
performance literature, including the inverted-U that they can increase and decrease at different
theory and the IZOF theory. parts of competition. For example, physiological
responses to anxiety may increase during perfor-
Directionality Hypothesis mance, but cognitive aspects of anxiety may
Although theories have distinguished between state remain constant. They hypothesized that cogni-
and trait anxiety, some theories have focused on the tive anxiety is the dimension which exerts the
facets of each. For example, the directionality hypo­ greatest influence on performance (Martens
thesis evaluates not only intensity but also direction et al., 1990).
(G. Jones, Swain, & Hardy, 1993). Specifically, Critics note that this theory attempts to describe
researchers were interested in discovering if state a three-dimensional relationship—between cogni-
anxiety could be perceived as both facilitative and tive and somatic anxiety and performance—in a
debilitative. Results of previous studies have indi- two-dimensional manner. In other words, Martens
cated that high-quality performance had a stronger and colleagues (1990) are really exploring two two-
association with those who perceived anxiety as dimensional relationships examining cognitive and
facilitative versus debilitative (G. Jones et al., somatic forms of state anxiety on sport performance.
1993). With respect to competitive sport, athletes The results of studies have supported these MAT
who perceived anxiety as being facilitative were hypotheses with athletes (Chamberlain & Hale,
more likely to earn a degree, develop mastery in 2007), although some research has found that an
their sport, and gain additional confidence in their athlete’s personality may impact the athlete’s physio­
sport (Kamusoko & Pemberton, 2013). Whereas logical and cognitive responses during competition
the skill level of the athlete may play a role in the (Balyan, Tok, Tatar, Binboga, & Balyan, 2016).
performance impact of facilitative interpretations of Researchers have attempted to close this gap by

229
Frame and Reichin

truly exploring the three-dimensional relationship THEORIES AND MODELS OF CHOKING


between cognitive and somatic anxiety.
Choking as a Self-Defeating Behavior
The theory of choking revolves around the fact that
Catastrophe Model
attempts to prevent choking are self-defeating or have
The catastrophe model attempts to improve upon
the opposite effect of the intended one. Baumeister
the MAT. The catastrophe model notes that the
(1997) argued that performance becomes unconscious
level of cognitive anxiety determines the size of the
and automatic to professional athletes, so any attempt
effect of physiological arousal (Hardy & Parfitt,
to interfere with this automatic process undermines
1991). Specifically, the model hypothesizes that
performance. In other words, any conscious effort to
when cognitive anxiety is low, physiological arousal
perform especially well in competition may result in
acts as an inverted U (i.e., moderate amounts of
physiological arousal are beneficial for performance). choking (Baumeister, 1997). In sport performance,
However, when cognitive anxiety is high, an this may happen in instances where it is especially
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

increase in physiological arousal is catastrophic to important to perform well because of high pres-
performance. Once physiological arousal reaches sure and the expectations of others, including the
a certain level, performance doesn’t just taper off, athletes. Choking is more likely to happen during
it drops quite suddenly. This effect is detrimental a tournament or a championship game because the
to performance because in order for performance added pressure of this type of competition requires
to improve after this sudden drop, physiological the athlete to rely on the automatic processes that
arousal would need to decrease dramatically can be susceptible to error because of the athlete’s
(Hardy, Beattie, & Woodman, 2007). conscious attempts to not choke.

Choking Because of Distraction


DRIVE REDUCTION THEORY OF AROUSAL One of the earliest explanations for choking revolves
The inverted-U theory and the IZOF theory apply to around the idea that the anxiety and stress created
arousal as well. The theories of arousal subsequently from performing under pressure becomes a distrac-
discussed, however, will examine arousal from a tion to the athlete. The distraction then prevents
different perspective. the athlete from performing the task or action at the
Hull (1943) developed the drive reduction theory level expected or at a level that reflects her or his
of motivation. which focuses on the primary drives training and nonpressure performance (Wine, 1971).
that humans need. These primary drives include Distraction causes an athlete to experience divided
hunger, thirst, sleep, sex, among others. When indi- attention; choking is a result of the performer’s divided
viduals fulfill these drives (e.g., when they eat when attention (Englert, Bertrams, Purley, & Oudejans,
hungry and sleep when tired), the drive is reduced. 2015). One of the more prevalent theories tested in
This “reduction” acts as a learning mechanism, the research that has examined choking as a result
whereby humans learn once they have these drives, of distraction is the attentional control theory (ACT;
they will be acted upon, and therefore, the drives Eysenck et al., 2007).
will continue. This theory assumes a linear relation- ACT evolved from the processing efficiency theory
ship between arousal and performance, such that, as (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992); it suggests that anxiety can
arousal increases, performance increases. Research on be both distracting and motivating (Wilson, 2008).
drive theory, however, has failed to produce consis- In the ACT paradigm, anxiety serves to inform the
tent support for the theory (Martens, 1971, 1974). athlete that the situation is important and thus trig-
Moreover, drive theory fails to account for cognitive gers the use of additional effort to maintain perfor-
appraisal (Gill, 1994) and is unable to accurately mance while experiencing distractions. The anxiety,
explain the performance of complex tasks often however, may directly interfere with those functions
associated with athletic performance (Fisher, 1976; (e.g., working memory and executive functioning)
Martens, 1971; Neiss, 1988; Weinberg, 1979). critical to performance. Specifically, according to

230
Emotion and Sport Performance

ACT, anxiety impairs the person’s ability to attend of the mediators and moderators related to anxiety
to the task (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992; Eysenck et al., have been examined. The impact of these concepts
2007). The ability to control one’s attention is on athletic performance is also discussed.
reduced when threat-related cues are attended to
Criticality of the situation.   One of the concepts
more so than goal-directed aspects of the situation
related to anxiety is the degree to which the athlete
(Kinrade, Jackson, & Ashford, 2010).
is under substantial pressure. Researchers have
found that in conditions appraised as low pressure,
Skill-Focus Theories
precompetitive state anxiety was not related to sport
In contrast to distraction theories, skill-focus theo-
performance. Under high-pressure situations, how-
ries suggest that pressure causes the athlete to focus
ever, precompetitive state anxiety was negatively
attention on the technical execution of the skill. In
related to performance (Geukes, Harvey, Trezise, &
essence, an athlete’s anxiety impairs performance by
Mesagno, 2017). That is, critical or high-pressure
causing the athlete to become self-conscious about
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

situations produced circumstances in which athletes’


the execution of a task that is normally performed
level of anxiety impacted performance, whereas in
outside of conscious awareness (Masters & Maxwell,
low-pressure situations, athletes were neither posi-
2008). Skill-focused theories of choking are founded
tively nor negatively influenced by anxiety. Similarly,
on the idea that “learning progresses from a declara-
Neil, Wilson, Mellalieu, Hanton, and Taylor (2012)
tive knowledge stage in which performance is
found that only under critical performance situa-
consciously controlled and requires much attention
tions did athletes consider their anxiety debilitative.
to a procedural knowledge stage in which perfor-
The researchers distinguished between how athletes
mance is automatic and requires little attention”
interpreted their anxiety as either debilitative or
(Masters & Maxwell, 2008, p. 161). In a stressful
facilitative because they found that facilitative anxi-
situation, however, a process referred to as deautoma-
ety can be positively related to sport performance
tization occurs. Deautomatization is the process by
(Neil et al., 2012). Neil et al. (2012) attributed
which an athlete’s previously automatic behavior
this finding to the athletes’ level of self-confidence.
or skill is reversed and becomes a behavior or skill
These findings taken together highlight a potential
that requires effortful, declarative attention by the
limitation of the existing theories and models
athlete. In essence, what was once implicit—almost
researched. Specifically, it seems that in extreme
unconscious—behavior becomes behavior that
situations, the high-pressure environment experi-
requires full attention and explicit rules. It results
enced by athletes may be so overwhelming that all
in the athlete attempting to coordinate movements
anxiety is perceived by the athlete as debilitative and
involved in a task by using explicit rules instead of
has a negative impact on sport performance.
using procedural memory. The process by which the
automatic process becomes cognitively effortful is Intensity and direction of anxiety.   Not only does
mediated by a “de-chunking” process that slows and the athlete’s perceived criticality of the situation
impairs performance and introduces error between affect the relationship between anxiety and perfor-
steps, or chunks, making the athlete perform more mance, but the intensity and direction of anxiety is
like a novice (Land & Tenenbaum, 2012). also important. The MAT theory of anxiety suggests
that anxiety can be perceived at various levels of
intensity (e.g., high intensity, moderate intensity,
EMPIRICAL RESEARCH FINDINGS
low intensity). Recent research has shown that
AND LIMITATIONS
heightened intensity of anxiety is related to poorer
Effect of Anxiety on Performance sport performance (Neil et al., 2012). Further, as
As with stress, anxiety can significantly impact sport discussed previously in relation to the directionality
performance. The mechanisms for this process, hypothesis, anxiety can be perceived by the athlete
however, is a function of various moderators and as debilitative or facilitative. Recent research find-
mediators. The next section describes how some ings suggest that cognitive anxiety that is perceived

231
Frame and Reichin

as facilitative is associated with better performance affect their performance. Further, recent research has
(Chamberlain & Hale, 2007). suggested that the degree to which an athlete main-
Additionally, researchers have examined what tains self-control may give him or her an enhanced
types of individuals may be more likely to experi- capability to translate intentions into action under tax-
ence intense feelings of anxiety and in what direc- ing situations (Landman, Nieuwenhuys, & Oudejans,
tion. Weinberger, Schwartz, and Davidson (1979) 2016). Landman et al. (2016) found that under high-
suggested that athletes can be grouped based on pressure situations, athletes with higher action orien-
self-reported trait anxiety and scores of defensive- tation (AOD) had higher levels of performance
ness on the Marlowe–Crowne Scale. The first group (i.e., accuracy in a shooting task). However, whereas
are repressors, who self-report low trait anxiety but AOD (self-control strength) may have reduced the
score high on defensiveness. These individuals show effect of anxiety on performance that stems from high-
physiological symptoms of high anxiety, implying pressure situations, AOD did not lessen the perception
they tend to avoid or ignore their anxiety symptoms of anxiety (Landman et al., 2016). In summary, it may
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

or feelings. The next group are those who self-report be that the focus of interventions and training in the
low trait anxiety and are low on defensiveness. realm of emotion and sport should be on assisting
These individuals report low trait anxiety. athletes to maintain and strengthen their self-control
Weinberger et al. (1979) recognized those who and compose in a variety of settings rather than
self-report high anxiety but low defensiveness. These attempting to have them perceive stressful situations
individuals acknowledge their anxiety and are better as unthreatening and or positive.
at coping than repressors. There are also athletes
who self-report high anxiety and score high on
defensiveness. Franklin, Smith, and Holmes (2015) SPORT STRESS
wanted to investigate if these groups of athletes differ Stress, an aspect of everyday life for athletes
in their perceived intensity and direction of anxiety. (Mellalieu, Neil, Hanton, & Fletcher, 2009), is
Results showed that repressors reported less intense defined as the “process by which certain environ-
and more facilitative state anxiety than those in the mental demands evoke an appraisal process in which
high-anxiety groups (Franklin et al., 2015). Further, perceived demand exceeds resources and results
the researchers reported no significant differences in undesirable, physiological, behavioral, or social
in reported intensity or direction for the repressor outcomes” (Salas, Driskell, & Hughes, 1996, p. 6).
and low-anxiety, low-defensive group. Finally, while
Stress is inevitable, and most athletes experience
researchers suggest that both intensity and direc-
various degrees of stress on a regular basis. Stressful
tion of anxiety can affect performance, direction
situations can lead to poor performance (Liao &
is apparently a stronger predictor of performance
Masters, 2002; Whitehead, Butz, Kozar, & Vaughn,
(Chamberlain & Hale, 2007).
1996). Still, in other situations, stress may actually
Self-control.   In addition to the perceived critical enhance performance (Lindahl, Theorell, & Lindblad,
level of the situation and the intensity and direction 2005). As noted earlier, stress has psychophysiol­
of state anxiety, an athlete’s self-control strength can ogical properties (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004;
moderate the effect of anxiety on sport performance. Lazarus, 2000; Tetrick & Quick, 2003) and can
Researchers found that there is a negative relation- be chronic or acute (Dhabhar, 2012; Furrer et al.,
ship between anxiety and motor performance when 2015). Thus, research into sport stress has examined
self-control strength is low (Englert & Bertrams, both the physical and psychological components.
2012, 2013). Englert and Bertrams (2012) defined low
self-control as “the ability to resist immediate urges or Sources of Stress
to not give into automatic impulses in specific situ- When athletes experience environmental stimuli
ations” (p. 581). Because of this ability, athletes are (e.g., threat, time constraint, high-demand task, group
likely to keep their anxiety from dictating their behav- pressure), they must determine the importance
ior, and therefore, they do not allow their anxiety to of the stimuli and ascertain the degree to which

232
Emotion and Sport Performance

they have the skills, abilities, equipment, and other be to career development, to performance, or to
resources to meet the demand of the situation. the organization’s image. An example of a threat to
Eventually, those athletes will determine if the current career development that the authors noted was new
situation presents a significant threat or a challenge athletes coming in and more veteran athletes feeling
that can be handled easily or is categorized as stressful. threatened. Finally, the third stressor the authors
Depending upon the athletes’ appraisal of the stim- mentioned was challenges. These can be challenges
ulus, the resulting stress may elicit changes in their that an individual is determined to overcome or a
physiology, cognitions, emotions, behaviors, and sport challenge that makes the individual excited about
performance quality (Lazarus, 2000; Salas et al., 1996). the future.
In this review we divide the categories of stressors
as external and internal. Stress and Performance
Generally speaking, stress shows a negative relation-
External stressors.   There are an array of (external)
ship with performance (Otter, Brink, Van der Does,
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

stressors that athletes experience in every contest.


& Lemmink, 2016). Stress, however, can at times
External stressors can be acute or chronic; they
be interpreted as positive and can be instrumental
might include the sight of or interaction with an
in an athlete’s ability to perform well in competition
opponent, bad weather, a bad call by a game official,
(Gillham & Gillham, 2014; Mellalieu et al., 2009;
rude spectators, or disagreeable coaches (Suinn,
Neil, Mellalieu, Hanton, 2006). Stress can manifest
2005). Further, Mellalieu et al. (2009) noted various
in different ways. Stress can manifest physiologically
performance stressors, including physical, technical,
(e.g., heightened arousal or distress), behaviorally
and tactical preparation; injury; goals; performance
(e.g., muscle tightness or coordination deficiencies),
problems; self-presentation; and aspects relating to
and cognitively (e.g., negative thoughts or problems
one’s opponents. Mellalieu et al. also identified orga-
concentrating; Suinn, 2005). These stressors can
nizational stressors, including stress from coaches,
affect an individual’s performance, which can be
teammates, competitive environment, time demands,
problematic. One of the potential consequences of
and the media. The challenging and competitive
stress includes overreaching (OR), which manifests
nature of sports means that these external stressors
in physiological ways, both behaviorally and cogni-
are unavoidable in the day-to-day life of athletes and
tively. Overreaching can be functional (FOR) and
in competition. Well-trained and skilled athletes,
nonfunctional OR (NFOR). Brink, Visscher, Coutts,
however, effectively use appropriate strategies for
and Lemmink (2012) noted that an individual who
coping with these external stressors.
exhibits symptoms of OR may show a decrease
Internal stressors.   Internal stressors can be acute in performance, fatigue, poor concentration, and
or chronic stressors. In the sport performance lit- changes in sleeping and eating patterns. FOR is
erature, internal stressors include thoughts about short term, with long-term benefits, but NFOR can
upcoming performance, the awareness of fatigue, or be severe, coupled with more extreme symptoms
fear of failure (Suinn, 2005). Further, Mellalieu et al. that continue for months (Brink et al., 2012).
(2009) noted additional internal stressors, such as Recovery is an important aspect to consider
reflecting on previous accomplishments and pres- when one is examining the stress performance rela-
sure to do well. Additionally, Hanton, Wagstaff, tionship. If an individual spends more time under
and Fletcher (2012) found that cognitive stressors stress than recovery, than performance can decrease
fall into one of three categories. The first stressors (Van der Does et al., 2015). However, when stress
are those that make individuals feel as if they have is balanced with recovery, there can be positive
been personally harmed or that a goal of theirs relation­ships with performance (Otter et al., 2016).
has been harmed. An example of this would be a Researchers have divided stress into challenge
teammate insulting the way you look. The second and hindrance (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lepine
stressor that Hanton et al. described is one in which et al., 2005). Challenge can be appraised as “positive
the individual feels threatened. This threat could stress.” A degree of stress that challenges a person

233
Frame and Reichin

can improve sport performance (Crane & Searle, Others, however, have argued that the construct
2016; Prem, Ohly, Kubicek, & Korunka, 2017). of arousal is of limited use and have suggested that
Hindrance stress, however, inhibits the athlete studying arousal contributes little to the under-
from performing at a level commensurate with her standing of performance (Neiss, 1990). Specifically, it
or his competence. Results of meta-analytic results has been suggested that it would be more beneficial to
have found that challenging stress is positively understand the psychobiological processes and their
associated with performance when compared with relationship to performance, as opposed to studying
the negative association with hindrance (Lepine the construct of arousal (Neiss, 1990). Contemporary
et al., 2005). In the workplace, Wallace, Edwards, definitions of arousal in sport psychology research
Arnold, Frazier, and Finch (2009) found that tend to operationalize arousal as a physiological and
manageable or challenging stressors were positively psychological phenomenon that ranges along an exci-
associated with performance, whereas unmanage- tation continuum from unconsciousness or sleep to
able or hindrance stressors were negatively associ- highly excited (Adam et al., 1997; Gould & Krane,
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

ated with performance. 1992; Gould & Udry, 1994; Koob, 1991; Parfitt et al.,
1995; Wann, 1997; Zaichkowsky & Baltzell, 2001).
Arousal is a multidimensional construct that consists
AROUSAL
of a cognitive interpretation—an appraisal compo-
Arousal in terms of sport psychology can be chal- nent and a complex physiological component.
lenging to define because of the complexity of The cognitive interpretation involves an indi-
the construct (Gould & Krane, 1992; Venables, vidual’s appraisal of the physiological component of
1983). For sport psychology, however, the concept arousal. The appraisal can be favorable, thus resulting
of arousal and the regulation of arousal is often in a positive effect, or it can be an unfavorable assess-
mentioned and discussed (Gould & Udry, 1994; ment, which would result in a negative affect (Gould
Zaichkowsky & Baltzell, 2001). Yet, despite the & Udry, 1994). The physiological component of
interest, the term arousal has many varying opera- arousal is thought to begin in the reticular activating
tional definitions in sport psychology research. system, which interacts with the hypothalamus, the
Also confusing is that the term arousal is often used cerebral cortex, and other components of the nervous
synonymously in relation to other constructs, such system (Gould & Udry, 1994; Hardy et al., 1996;
as anxiety, emotion, excitement, and stress (Arent & LeDoux, 1993; Zaichkowsky & Baltzell, 2001). The
Landers, 2003). Early definitions of arousal consid- physiological component of arousal begins with brain
ered the construct unidimensional and conceptual- mechanisms that produce various biological indica-
ized it as a physiological response and mobilization tors such as increased muscle tension, increased
of energy and an effort in response to a physical cardiovascular activity, increased cortical activity,
demand (Zaichkowsky & Baltzell, 2001). Some of increased physiological markers (e.g., electrodermal
these early definitions drew on motivation theories activity), and changes in biochemistry (e.g., changes
and considered arousal as behavior that reflects the in cortisol levels, changes in epinephrine levels, and
intensity and the direction of behavior toward a goal changes in norepinephrine levels; Zaichkowsky &
(Brown & McGill, 1989; Duffy, 1962; Sage, 1984). Baltzell, 2001).
These early definitions were criticized, and multi­ Arousal has been measured with a variety of
dimensional operational definitions of arousal were methods, including physiological measurements,
put forth (Gould & Krane, 1992; Gould & Udry, self-report inventories, and behavioral observations
1994; Hockey & Hamilton, 1983; J. G. Jones & (A. M. Smith, Sim, Smith, Stuart, & Laskowski,
Hardy, 1989; Wann, 1997). The multi­dimensional 1998). Physiological measurements include changes
perspective on arousal has also been supported by in physiology (e.g., changes in respiration, blood
research (Adam, Teeken, Ypelaar, Verstappen, & pressure, galvanic skin response, muscle tension
Paas, 1997; Koob, 1991; Parfitt, Hardy, & Pates, [measured with an electromyogram], heart rate
1995; Wann, 1997). [measured with an electrocardiogram], and brain

234
Emotion and Sport Performance

waves [measured with an electroencephalogram]) their success to a temporary yet optimal mental
and changes in biochemistry (e.g., changes in state in which they were fully engaged in the task
epinephrine, norepinephrine, and cortisol). Whereas or activity.
self-report inventories are often used in research
to measure the cognitive interpretation—appraisal Research in Choking
component of arousal, it is worth noting that most Much of the research in the domain of choking
of these scales confound arousal with anxiety. It is mirrored the research that we have described thus
possible that the confounding of arousal and anxiety far in other realms of stress, anxiety, and arousal.
is one reason that researchers often find low corre- The research designs typically involved measuring
lations between the self-report skill measurement performance before a pressure-filled scenario
and measures of physiological, behavioral, and and then measuring performance after a pressure
biochemical indicators of arousal. scenario. Research that used experimental designs
generally involved a baseline level of performance
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

(under practice or controlled conditions), manipu-


CHOKING
lating the level of state anxiety by altering the
In the academic literature, the phrase choking under perceived pressure of the situation, and assessing
pressure was introduced in reference to the “occur- the sub­sequent performance as compared with
rence of inferior performance despite individual the previously demonstrated standard (Mesagno,
striving and situational demands for superior perfor- Harvey, & Janelle, 2012). Nonexperimental obser-
mance” (Baumeister, 1984, p. 610). A more succinct vational or case studies examined the effects of
definition of choking as “suboptimal performance pressure inherent in high-stakes real-world situ-
under pressure conditions” has been the basis for ations such as championship games and title
much of the research on choking (Baumeister & matches. These naturalistic studies compare
Showers, 1986, p. 362). As with some of the other previous performance in low-pressure settings
topics discussed in this chapter, the definitions cited (e.g., preseason games, regular matches, practice)
have not been universally accepted. Some researchers with performance in high-pressure situations.
contend that the current operational conceptual- Qualitative studies that have examined choking
ization does not adequately reflect the dramatic or make use of reflective self-reports of those who
drastic performance decline typically associated have choked in a high-pressure situation.
with the use of “choke” in athletic performance Experimental, quasi-experimental, and natural-
(Gucciardi, Longbottom, Jackson, & Dimmock, 2010; istic studies have produced empirical support for the
Hill et al., 2010; Mesagno, Marchant, & Morris, idea that choking exists as a distinct and observable
2008). It is argued that choking represents a substan- phenomenon. Regardless of how one defines choking,
tial decline in performance that significantly exceeds research suggests—as does the anecdotal experiences
what might be expected because of chance or other of many people—that when required to perform
factors, and that choking is clearly and directly attrib- under pressure, people may perform more poorly
utable to a “specific negative response to perceived than expected.
pressure” (Hill et al., 2010, p. 24). Whereas choking
is performance significantly below expectations in a
LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH
high-pressure situation, clutch performance is “any
performance increment or superior performance that Several areas warrant future research on emotion
occurs under pressure” (Otten, 2009, p. 584). Thus, in competitive sport. For arousal, it is critical for
clutch performance may be viewed as the opposite researchers to develop better measures of the cogni-
of choking. Clutch performance involves perfor- tive interpretation—appraisal component of arousal.
mance in which the perceived criticality of the situ- While the studies in the area of arousal that have
ation creates heightened tension and anticipation. used measures of anxiety are informative, they do
Successful athletes in such situations often attribute not provide a full understanding of the cognitive

235
Frame and Reichin

interpretation—appraisal component of arousal References


because of a lack of reliable measures to assess the Abbot, E. (Producer), Greenhut, R. (Producer), &
construct. (See also Chapter 13, this volume.) Marshall, P. (Director). (1992). A league of their own
[Motion picture]. United States: Sony Pictures.
Adam, J., Teeken, J., Ypelaar, P., Verstappen, F., & Paas, F.
CONCLUSION
(1997). Exercise-induced arousal and information
The primary purpose of this chapter was to processing. International Journal of Sport Psychology,
28, 217–226.
review the literature on emotion, specifically
stress, arousal, anxiety, and choking, as they relate Aldwin, C. M. (1994). Stress, coping, and development: An
integrative perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
to sports performance. We have reviewed some
of the defining constructs within this area of American Psychological Association. (2010). Stress won’t
go away? Maybe you are suffering from chronic stress.
sport psychology, as well as how emotion affects Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/
athletic performance. Research evidence suggests chronic-stress.aspx
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

that negative stress and debilitative state anxiety, Amiot, C., Gaudreau, P., & Blanchard, C. (2004). Self-
unlike positive stress and facilitative anxiety, determination, coping, and goal attainment in sport.
respectively, may inhibit sport performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 396–411.
Previous studies have also suggested, however, http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.26.3.396
that certain forms and intensities of anxiety, Anshel, M. H., Kang, M., & Miesner, M. (2010). The
arousal, and stress may be beneficial for athletic approach-avoidance framework for identifying
athletes’ coping style as a function of gender and race.
performance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51, 341–349.
We also discussed the phenomenon of choking http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00796.x
in sport. Although much research in this area Arent, S. M., & Landers, D. M. (2003). Arousal,
has been conducted as choking relates to athletic anxiety, and performance: A reexamination of
performance, a full understanding of the phenom- the Inverted-U hypothesis. Research Quarterly for
enon in terms of its causes and cures is still Exercise and Sport, 74, 436–444. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/02701367.2003.10609113
lacking.
Research in the area of stress, anxiety, arousal, Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuro-
psychological theory of positive affect and its influence
and choking as it relates to sports performance has on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.
direct applied implications for athletes and sport http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.529
psychologists. Competitive athletes are typically Baghurst, T. (2012). The psychological components of
under pressure to meet performance expectations. elite cycling. Athletic Insight: The Online Journal of
It is incumbent upon them to determine the strate- Sport Psychology, 4, 13–29.
gies, routines, and thought processes that can aid Ball, C. (2013). Unexplained sporting slumps and causal
them in using a pressure situation to enhance their attributions. Journal of Sport Behavior, 36, 233–242.
performance. Balyan, K. Y., Tok, S., Tatar, A., Binboga, E., & Balyan, M.
Throughout this chapter we have attempted (2016). The relationship among personality, cogni-
tive anxiety, somatic anxiety, physiological arousal,
to explain the research findings related to perfor-
and performance in male athletes. Journal of Clinical
mance and stress, anxiety, and arousal. Future Sport Psychology, 10, 48–58. http://dx.doi.org/
research in this domain should continue to focus 10.1123/jcsp.2015-0013
on the sources of undesirable emotions and ways Baudin, N., Blanch, A., Rolland, J., Martí-Guiu, M., &
to provide effective interventions that results in Aluja, A. (2014). Core self-evaluations, life satisfac-
favorable and consistent performance in competi- tion, and sport satisfaction. Escritos de Psicologia, 7,
19–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.5231/psy.writ.2014.2507
tive sport. Similarly, when investigating the ante-
cedents of sports performance future research Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-
consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives
should use both physiological and psychological
on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and
methods to determine the athletes’ levels of stress, Social Psychology, 46, 610–620. http://dx.doi.org/
arousal, and anxiety. 10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.610

236
Emotion and Sport Performance

Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Esteem threat, self-regulatory Psychology, 25, 519–533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/
breakdown, and emotional distress as factors in self- jsep.25.4.519
defeating behavior. Review of General Psychology, 1, Crane, M. F., & Searle, B. J. (2016). Building resilience
145–174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.1.2.145 through exposure to stressors: The effects of chal-
Baumeister, R. F., & Showers, C. (1986). A review of para- lenges versus hindrances. Journal of Occupational
doxical performance effects: Choking under pressure Health Psychology, 21, 468–479. http://dx.doi.org/
in sports and mental tests. European Journal of Social 10.1037/a0040064
Psychology, 16, 361–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ Cunningham, G. B., & Ashley, F. B. (2002). Debilitative and
ejsp.2420160405 facilitative perceptions of trait anxiety among students
Beedie, C. J., Terry, P. C., Lane, A. M., & Devonport, T. J. in a college golf class. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94,
(2011). Differential assessment of emotions and 739–742. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.94.3.739
moods: Development and validation of the Emotion Derouesné, C. (2011). Qu’est-ce qu’une émotion? Une
and Mood Components of Anxiety Questionnaire. introduction à l’étude des émotions [What is an
Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 228–233. emotion? An introduction to the study of emotions].
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.034 Gériatrie et Psychologie Neuropsychiatrie du
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Beilock, S. L., & Gray, R. (2007). Why do athletes choke Vieillissement, 9, 69–81.
under pressure? In G. Tenenbaum &, R. C. Eklund Dhabhar, F. (2012, December). Good stress, bad
(Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 425–444). stress. Stanford Medicine. Retrieved from https://
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. med.stanford.edu/news/all-news/2012/12/
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. good-stress-bad-stress.html
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. http://dx.doi. Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors
org/10.1037/11164-000 and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration
Brink, M. S., Visscher, C., Coutts, A. J., & Lemmink, K. A. and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological
(2012). Changes in perceived stress and recovery in Bulletin, 130, 355–391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
overreached young elite soccer players. Scandinavian 0033-2909.130.3.355
Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 22, 285–292. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01237.x positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and
Brown, J. D., & McGill, K. L. (1989). The cost of good Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117. http://dx.doi.org/
fortune: When positive life events produce negative 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105
health consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Doron, J., & Bourbousson, J. (2017). How stressors are
Psychology, 57, 1103–1110. http://dx.doi.org/ dynamically appraised within a team during a game:
10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1103 An exploratory study in basketball. Scandinavian
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 27, 2080–2090.
Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sms.12796
based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Doron, J., & Gaudreau, P. (2014). A point-by-point
Psychol­ogy, 56, 267–283. http://dx.doi.org/ analysis of performance in a fencing match: Psychol­
10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 ogical processes associated with winning and losing
Chamberlain, S. T., & Hale, B. D. (2007). Competitive streaks. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 36,
state anxiety and self-confidence: Intensity and 3–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2013-0043
direction as relative predictors of performance on Duda, J. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1992). Dimensions of
a golf putting task. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: An achievement motivation in schoolwork and sport.
International Journal, 20, 197–207. http://dx.doi.org/ Journal of Educational Psychology, 5, 84290–84299.
10.1080/10615800701288572
Duffy, E. (1962). Activation and behavior. Oxford, England:
Compas, B. E., Connor, J., Osowiecki, D., & Welch, A. Wiley.
(1997). Effortful and involuntary responses to
Dyck, D., & Roithmayr, T. (2002). Organizational
stress: Implications for coping with chronic stress.
stressors and health: How occupational health
In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.), Coping with chronic stress
nurses can help break the cycle. Workplace
(pp. 105–130). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Safety & Health, 50, 212–219. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9862-3_4 10.1177/216507990205000507
Corbett, J. (2007, January 8). Magical season bobbled Edwards, T., Hardy, L., Kingston, K., & Gould, D. (2002).
away. USA Today, p. 5C. A qualitative analysis of catastrophic performances
Cox, R., Martens, M., & Russell, W. (2003). Measuring and the associated thoughts, feelings, and emotions.
anxiety in athletics: The revised Competitive State The Sport Psychologist, 16, 1–19. http://dx.doi.org/
Anxiety Inventory-2. Journal of Sport & Exercise 10.1123/tsp.16.1.1

237
Frame and Reichin

Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & sport participants pursue their goals? Psychologica
M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion Belgica, 56, 244–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.5334/pb.266
(pp. 45–60). New York, NY: Wiley. Gaudreau, P., Gunnell, K. E., Hoar, S. D., Thompson, A., &
Englert, C., & Bertrams, A. (2012). Anxiety, ego deple- Lelièvre, J. (2015). Optimism, pessimism, and coping
tion, and sports performance. Journal of Sport & in a dual-domain model of sport and school satisfac-
Exercise Psychology, 34, 580–599. http://dx.doi.org/ tion. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 4,
10.1123/jsep.34.5.580 140–152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spy0000032
Englert, C., & Bertrams, A. (2013). Too exhausted Gaudreau, P., Nicholls, A., & Levy, A. R. (2010). The ups
for operation? Anxiety, depleted self-control strength, and downs of coping and sport achievement: An
and perceptual–motor performance. Self and episodic process analysis of within-person associa-
Identity, 12, 650–662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ tions. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32,
15298868.2012.718865 298–311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.32.3.298
Englert, C., Bertrams, A., Purley, P., & Oudejans, R. R. D. Geukes, K., Harvey, J. T., Trezise, A., & Mesagno, C.
(2015). Is ego depletion associated with increased (2017). Personality and performance in real-world
distractibility? Results from a basketball free throw competitions: Testing trait activation of fear of nega-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

task. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 18, 26–31. tive evaluation, dispositional reinvestment, and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.12.001 athletic identity in the field. Psychology of Sport and
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and Exercise, 30, 101–109. http://dx.doi.org.ezaccess.
performance: The processing efficiency theory. libraries.psu.edu/10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.02.008
Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434. http://dx.doi.org/ Gill, D. L. (1994). A sport and exercise psychology
10.1080/02699939208409696 perspective on stress. Quest, 46, 20–27. http://
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, dx.doi.org/10.1080/00336297.1994.10484108
M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance:
Gillham, E., & Gillham, A. D. (2014). Identifying athletes’
Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.
sources of competitive state anxiety. Journal of Sport
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
Behavior, 37, 1–16.
Fisher, A. C. (1976). Psychology of sport. Palo Alto, CA:
Mayfield. Gould, D., Greenleaf, C., & Krane, V. (2002). Arousal-
anxiety and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.),
Franklin, Z., Smith, N., & Holmes, P. (2015). Anxiety Advances in sport psychology (pp. 207–236).
symptom interpretation and performance expecta- Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
tions in high-anxious, low-anxious, defensive high-
anxious and repressor individuals. Personality and Gould, D., & Krane, V. (1992). The arousal–athletic perfor-
Individual Differences, 77, 27–32. mance relationship: Current status and future direc-
tions. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology
Furrer, P., Moen, F., & Firing, K. (2015). How mindful- (pp. 119–142). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
ness training may mediate stress, performance and
burnout. The Sport Journal, 20, 1. Gould, D., & Udry, E. (1994). Psychological skills
for enhancing performance: Arousal regulation
Gaudreau, P., & Antl, S. (2008). Athletes’ broad dimen- strategies. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise,
sions of dispositional perfectionism: Examining 26, 478–485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/
changes in life satisfaction and the mediating role of
00005768-199404000-00013
sport-related motivation and coping. Journal of Sport
& Exercise Psychology, 30, 356–382. http://dx.doi.org/ Granero-Gallegos, A., Baena-Extremera, A., Gómez-
10.1123/jsep.30.3.356 López, M., & Abraldes, J. A. (2014). Psychometric
Gaudreau, P., & Blondin, J. (2004). Differential associa- properties of the “Sport Satisfaction Instrument
tions of dispositional optimism and pessimism with (SSI)” in female athletes: Predictive model of sport
coping, goal attainment, and emotional adjustment commitment. Psychological Reports, 115, 148–164.
during sport competition. International Journal of http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/08.06.PR0.115c14z1
Stress Management, 11, 245–269. http://dx.doi.org/ Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex
10.1037/1072-5245.11.3.245 sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking,
Gaudreau, P., Blondin, J.-P., & Lapierre, A.-M. (2002). and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Athletes’ coping during a competition: Relationship Applied, 10, 42–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
of coping strategies with positive affect, negative 1076-898X.10.1.42
affect, and performance–goal discrepancy. Psychology Gucciardi, D. F., Longbottom, J.-L., Jackson, B., &
of Sport and Exercise, 3, 125–150. http://dx.doi.org/ Dimmock, J. A. (2010). Experienced golfers’
10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00015-2 perspectives on choking under pressure. Journal
Gaudreau, P., & Braaten, A. (2016). Achievement goals and of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 61–83.
their underlying goal motivation: Does it matter why http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.32.1.61

238
Emotion and Sport Performance

Gustafsson, H., & Skoog, T. (2012). The mediational role Hockey, G., & Hamilton, P. (1983). The cognitive
of perceived stress in the relation between optimism patterning of stress states. In G. Hockey (Ed.), Stress
and burnout in competitive athletes. Anxiety, Stress, and fatigue in human performance (pp. 331–362).
& Coping: An International Journal, 25, 183–199. Chichester, England: Wiley.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2011.594045 Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to
Hackfort, D., & Schwenkmezger, P. (1993). Anxiety. behavior theory. Oxford, England: Appleton-Century.
In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and
(Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.; pp. 203–253),
(pp. 328–364). New York, NY: Macmillan. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York,
Haney, C., & Long, B. (1995). Coping effectiveness: A NY: Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
path analysis of self-efficacy, control, coping, and S0065-2601(08)60415-3
performance in sport competitions. Journal of Applied Janelle, C. M. (2002). Anxiety, arousal and visual
Social Psychology, 25, 1726–1746. http://dx.doi.org/ attention: A mechanistic account of performance
10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb01815.x variability. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 237–251.
Hanin, Y. L. (1995). Individual zones of optimal func- http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026404102317284790
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

tioning (IZOF) model: An idiographic approach to Jones, G. (1995). More than just a game: Research devel-
performance anxiety. In K. Henschen & W. Straub opments and issues in competitive anxiety in sport.
(Eds.), Sport psychology: An analysis of athlete behavior British Journal of Psychology, 86, 449–478. http://
(pp. 103–119). Longmeadow, MA: Movement. dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02565.x
Hanin, Y. L. (2007). Emotions in sport: Current issues Jones, G., & Swain, A. (1992). Intensity and direction
and perspectives. In G. Tenenbaum, R. C. Eklund, as dimensions of competitive state anxiety and
G. Tenenbaum, & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of relationships with competitiveness. Perceptual and
sport psychology (pp. 31–58). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Motor Skills, 74, 467–472. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781118270011.ch2 10.2466/pms.1992.74.2.467
Hanton, S., Wagstaff, C. R., & Fletcher, D. (2012). Jones, G., & Swain, A. (1995). Predispositions to expe-
Cognitive appraisals of stressors encountered in rience debilitative and facilitative anxiety in elite
sport organizations. International Journal of Sport and and nonelite performers. The Sport Psychologist, 9,
Exercise Psychology, 10, 276–289. http://dx.doi.org/ 201–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.9.2.201
10.1080/1612197X.2012.682376
Jones, G., Swain, A., & Hardy, L. (1993). Intensity and
Hardy, L., Beattie, S., & Woodman, T. (2007). Anxiety- direction dimensions of competitive state anxiety
induced performance catastrophes: Investigating and relationships with performance. Journal of Sports
effort required as an asymmetry factor. British Sciences, 11, 525–532. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Journal of Psychology, 98, 15–31. http://dx.doi.org/ 02640419308730023
10.1348/000712606X103428
Jones, G., Swain, A., & Harwood, C. (1996). Positive and
Hardy, L., Jones, J. G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding negative affect as predictors of competitive anxiety.
psychological preparation for sport: Theory and prac- Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 109–114.
tice of elite performers. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00140-2
Hardy, L., & Parfitt, G. (1991). A catastrophe model Jones, J. G., & Hardy, L. (1989). Stress and cognitive func-
of anxiety and performance. British Journal of tioning in sport. Journal of Sports Sciences, 7, 41–63.
Psychology, 82, 163–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640418908729821
j.2044-8295.1991.tb02391.x
Kamusoko, S. D., & Pemberton, C. A. (2013). Student-
Hatzigeorgiadis, A. (2006). Approach and avoidance athlete wellbeing and persistence: An in-depth look
coping during task performance in young men: at student-athlete perceptions. Journal for the Study
The role of goal attainment expectancies. Journal of Sports and Athletes in Education, 7, 41–61. http://
of Sports Sciences, 24, 299–307. http://dx.doi.org/ dx.doi.org/10.1179/1935739713Z.0000000003
10.1080/17461390500188645
Kinrade, N. P., Jackson, R. C., & Ashford, K. J. (2010).
Hatzigeorgiadis, A., & Chroni, S. (2007). Pre-competition Dispositional reinvestment and skill failure in
anxiety and in-competition coping in experienced cognitive and motor tasks. Psychology of Sport and
male swimmers. International Journal of Sports Exercise, 11, 312–319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Science & Coaching, 2, 181–189. http://dx.doi.org/ j.psychsport.2010.02.005
10.1260/174795407781394310
Koob, G. F. (1991). Arousal, stress, and inverted U-shaped
Hill, D., Hanton, S., Matthews, N., & Fleming, S. (2010). curves: Implications for cognitive function. In R. G.
Choking in sport: A review. International Review of Lister & H. J. Weingartner (Eds.), Perspectives on
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3, 24–39. http:// cognitive neuroscience (pp. 300–313). New York, NY:
dx.doi.org/10.1080/17509840903301199 Oxford University Press.

239
Frame and Reichin

Krohne, H. W. (1993). Attention and avoidance: Strategies Journal, 48, 764–775. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/
in coping with aversiveness. Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & amj.2005.18803921
Huber. Liao, C.-M., & Masters, R. S. W. (2002). Self-focused
Laborde, S., Dosseville, F., & Kinrade, N. (2014). attention and performance failure under psycholog-
Decision-specific reinvestment scale: An explora- ical stress. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24,
tion of its construct validity, and association with 289–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.24.3.289
stress and coping appraisals. Psychology of Sport and Lindahl, M., Theorell, T., & Lindblad, F. (2005). Test
Exercise, 15, 238–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ performance and self-esteem in relation to experi-
j.psychsport.2014.01.004
enced stress in Swedish sixth and ninth graders—
Lambert, N., Gwinn, A., Baumeister, R., Fincham, F., Saliva cortisol levels and psychological reactions
Strachman, A., Washburn, I., Gable, S., & Fincham, F. to demands. Acta Paediatrica, 94, 489–495. http://
(2013). A boost of positive affect: The perks of dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2005.tb01922.x
sharing positive experiences. Journal of Social and
Marquardt, C. (2009). The vicious circle involved in
Personal Relationships, 30, 24–43. http://dx.doi.org/
the development of the yips. International Journal of
10.1177/0265407512449400
Sports Science & Coaching, 3, 67–88. http://dx.doi.org/
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Land, W., & Tenenbaum, G. (2012). An outcome- 10.1260/174795409789577506


and process-oriented examination of a golf-
Martens, R. (1971). Internal–external control and
specific secondary task strategy to prevent
social reinforcement effects on motor performance.
choking under pressure. Journal of Applied Sport
Research Quarterly, 42, 307–313.
Psychology, 24, 303–322. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/10413200.2011.642458 Martens, R. (1974). Arousal and motor performance.
Exercise and sport sciences review, 2, 155–188.
Landers, D. M., Arent, S. M., & Lutz, R. S. (2001). Affect
and cognitive performance in high school wrestlers Martens, R., & Landers, D. M. (1970). Motor performance
undergoing rapid weight loss. Journal of Sport & under stress: A test of the inverted-U hypothesis.
Exercise Psychology, 23, 307–316. http://dx.doi.org/ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 29–37.
10.1123/jsep.23.4.307 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029787
Landman, A., Nieuwenhuys, A., & Oudejans, R. R. D. Martens, R., Vealey, R. S., Burton, D., & Martens, R. (1990).
(2016). Decision-related action orientation predicts Competitive anxiety in sport. Champaign, IL: Human
police officers’ shooting performance under pres- Kinetics.
sure. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: An International Martiny, S. E., Gleibs, I. H., Parks-Stamm, E. J., Martiny-
Journal, 29, 570–579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Huenger, T., Froehlich, L., Harter, A. L., & Roth, J.
10615806.2015.1070834 (2015). Dealing with negative stereotypes in sports:
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, The role of cognitive anxiety when multiple identi-
NY: Oxford University Press. ties are activated in sensorimotor tasks. Journal of
Sport & Exercise Psychology, 37, 379–392. http://
Lazarus, R. S. (2000). How emotions influence perfor-
dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2014-0284
mance in competitive sports. The Sport Psychologist,
14, 229–252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.14.3.229 Masters, R., & Maxwell, J. (2008). The theory of
reinvestment. International Review of Sport and
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal,
Exercise Psychology, 1, 160–183. http://dx.doi.org/
and coping. New York, NY: Springer.
10.1080/17509840802287218
Lazarus, R. S., & Launier, R. (1978). Stress-related
McDaniel, K. D., Cummings, J. L., & Shain, S. (1989).
transactions between person and environment.
The “yips”: A focal dystonia of golfers. Neurology, 39,
In L. A. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds.), Perspectives in
192–195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.39.2.192
interactional psychology (pp. 287–327). New York,
NY: Plenum. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ Mellalieu, S. D., Neil, R., Hanton, S., & Fletcher, D.
978-1-4613-3997-7_12 (2009). Competition stress in sport performers:
Stressors experienced in the competition environ-
LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional networks in the brain.
ment. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 729–744.
In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640410902889834
emotions (pp. 109–118). New York, NY: Guilford
Press. Mesagno, C., Harvey, J. T., & Janelle, C. M. (2012).
Choking under pressure: The role of fear of negative
Lepine, J. A., Podsakoff, N. P., & Lepine, M. A. (2005).
evaluation. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 60–68.
A meta-analytic test of the challenge stressor–
hindrance stressor framework: An explanation http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.07.007
for inconsistent relationships among stressors Mesagno, C., Marchant, D., & Morris, T. (2008). A
and performance. Academy of Management pre-performance routine to alleviate choking in

240
Emotion and Sport Performance

“choking-susceptible” athletes. The Sport Psychol­ Prem, R., Ohly, S., Kubicek, B., & Korunka, C. (2017).
ogist, 22, 439–457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/ Thriving on challenge stressors? Exploring time
tsp.22.4.439 pressure and learning demands as antecedents
Neil, R., Mellalieu, S. D., & Hanton, S. (2006). Psycho­ of thriving at work. Journal of Organizational
logical skills usage and the competitive anxiety Behavior, 38, 108–123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
response as a function of skill level in rugby union. job.2115
Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, 5, 415–423. Quick, J. C., Quick, J. D., Nelson, D. L., & Hurrell, J. J.
Neil, R., Wilson, K., Mellalieu, S. D., Hanton, S., & (1997). Preventative stress management in organiza-
Taylor, J. (2012). Competitive anxiety intensity and tions. Washington, DC: American Psychological
interpretation: A two-study investigation into their Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10238-000
relationship with performance. International Journal Ribble, R. G. (2010). Optimal arousal theory. Pasadena,
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 10, 96–111. CA: Salem Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2012.645134
Ruiz, M. C., Raglin, J. S., & Hanin, Y. L. (2017). The indi-
Neiss, R. (1988). Reconceptualizing arousal: Psycho­ vidual zones of optimal functioning (IZOF) model
biological states in motor performance. Psychological
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

(1978–2014): Historical overview of its development


Bulletin, 103, 345–366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ and use. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
0033-2909.103.3.345 Psychology, 15, 41–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Neiss, R. (1990). Ending arousal’s reign of error: A reply 1612197X.2015.1041545
to Anderson. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 101–105. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.101 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39,
Nicholls, A., Polman, R., & Levy, A. (2010). Coping 1161–1178.
self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect,
performance among athletes. European Journal of prototypical emotional episodes, and other things
Sport Science, 10, 97–102. http://dx.doi.org/ called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal
10.1080/17461390903271592 of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819.
Nicholls, A., Polman, R., & Levy, A. (2012). A path http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805
analysis of stress appraisals, emotions, coping, and Sage, G. H. (1984). Motor learning and control: A neuropsy-
performance satisfaction among athletes. Psychology chological approach. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown.
of Sport and Exercise, 13, 263–270. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.12.003 Salas, E., Driskell, J. E., & Hughes, S. (1996). Introduction:
The study of stress and human performance.
Nicholls, A., Polman, R., Levy, A., & Hulleman, J. (2012). In J. E. Driskell & E. Salas (Eds.), Stress and human
An explanation for the fallacy of facilitative anxiety: performance (pp. 1–47). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stress, emotions, coping, and subjective performance
in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 4, Sallen, J., Hirschmann, F., & Herrmann, C. (2018).
273–293. Evaluation and adaption of the trier inventory for
chronic stress (TICS) for assessment in competitive
Nicolas, M., Martinent, G., & Campo, M. (2014). Evalua­ sports. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 308. http://dx.doi.org/
tion of the psychometric properties of a modified 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00308
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule including a
direction scale (PANAS-D) among French athletes. Schmidt, B., Mussel, P., & Hewig, J. (2013). I’m too calm—
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15, 227–237. Let’s take a risk! On the impact of state and trait
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.01.005 arousal on risk taking. Psychophysiology, 50, 498–503.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12032
Otten, M. (2009). Choking vs. clutch performance:
A study of sport performance under pressure. Selye, H. (1985). The nature of stress. Basal Facts, 7, 3–11.
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 583–601. Smith, A. M., Sim, F. H., Smith, H. C., Stuart, M. J., &
http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.31.5.583 Laskowski, E. R. (1998). Psychologic, situational,
Otter, R. T., Brink, M. S., Van der Does, H. T., & and physiologic variables and on-ice perfor-
Lemmink, K. A. (2016). Monitoring perceived stress mance of youth hockey goalkeepers. Mayo Clinic
and recovery in relation to cycling performance in Proceedings, 73, 17–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
female athletes. International Journal of Sports Medicine, S0025-6196(11)63614-8
37, 12–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0035-1555779 Smith, C., Hancock, H., Blake-Mortimer, J., & Eckert, K.
Parfitt, G., Hardy, L., & Pates, J. (1995). Somatic anxiety (2007). A randomised comparative trial of yoga
and physiological arousal: Their effects upon a high and relaxation to reduce stress and anxiety.
anaerobic, low memory demand task. International Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 15, 77–83.
Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 196–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2006.05.001

241
Frame and Reichin

Smith, R. E., & Christensen, D. S. (1995). Psychological The effect of stress and recovery on field-test
skills as predictors of performance and survival in performance in floorball. International Journal of
professional baseball. Journal of Sport & Exercise Sports Medicine, 36, 460–465. http://dx.doi.org/
Psychology, 17, 399–415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/ 10.1055/s-0034-1398581
jsep.17.4.399
Van der Lei, H., Tenenbaum, G., & Land, W. (2016).
Solomonov, Y., Avugos, S., & Bar-Eli, M. (2015). Do Individual arousal-related performance zones effect
clutch players win the game? Testing the validity on temporal and behavioral patterns in golf routines.
of the clutch player’s reputation in basketball. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 26, 52–60. http://
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 130–138. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.06.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.10.004 Venables, P. H. (1983). Cerebral mechanisms, autonomic
Spielberger, C. D. (1966). Anxiety and behavior. Oxford, responsiveness, and attention in schizophrenia.
England: Academic Press. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 31, 47–91.
Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Anxiety: Current trends in theory Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York, NY:
and research. Oxford, England: Academic Press. Wiley.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Suinn, R. (2005). Behavioral intervention for stress Wallace, J. C., Edwards, B. D., Arnold, T., Frazier, M. L.,
management in sports. International Journal of Stress & Finch, D. M. (2009). Work stressors, role-based
Management, 12, 343–362. http://dx.doi.org/ performance, and the moderating influence of orga-
10.1037/1072-5245.12.4.343 nizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94,
254–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013090
Sulsky, L., & Smith, C. (2005). Work stress. Belmont, CA:
Thomson Wadsworth. Wann, D. L. (1997). Sport psychology. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Tetrick, L. E., & Quick, J. C. (2003). Prevention at work:
Public health in occupational settings. In J. C. Quick, Weinberg, R. S. (1979). Anxiety and motor performance:
& L. E. Tetrick (Eds.), Handbook of occupational Drive theory versus cognitive theory. International
health psychology; handbook of occupational health Journal of Sport Psychology, 10, 112–121.
psychology (pp. 3–17). Washington, DC: American Weinberger, D. A., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, R. J.
Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org.ezaccess. (1979). Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive
libraries.psu.edu/10.1037/10474-001 coping styles: Psychometric patterns and behavioral
and physiological responses to stress. Journal of
Thayer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods:
Abnormal Psychology, 88, 369–380. http://dx.doi.org/
Managing energy, tension, and stress. New York, NY:
10.1037/0021-843X.88.4.369
Oxford University Press.
Weiss, S. M., & Reber, A. S. (2012). Curing the dreaded
Thomas, O., Mellalieu, S., & Hanton, S. (2009). Stress “Steve Blass disease.” Journal of Sport Psychology in
management in applied sport psychology. In Action, 3, 171–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
S. Hanton & S. D. Mellalieu (Eds.), Advances in 21520704.2012.682702
applied sport psychology: A review (pp. 124–161).
London, England: Routledge. Whitehead, R., Butz, J. W., Kozar, B., & Vaughn, R. E.
(1996). Stress and performance: An application of
Uphill, M., McCarthy, P. J., & Jones, M. (2009). Getting Gray’s three-factor arousal theory to basketball free-
a grip on emotion regulation in sport: Conceptual throw shooting. Journal of Sports Sciences, 14, 393–401.
foundations and practical application. In S. Hanton http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640419608727726
& S. D. Mellalieu (Eds.), Advances in applied sport
psychology: A review (pp. 162–194). London, Williams, S. E., Veldhuijzen van Zanten, J. J. C. S.,
England: Routledge. Trotman, G. P., Quinton, M. L., & Ginty, A. T. (2017).
Challenge and threat imagery manipulates heart rate
Valderrama, J., Miranda, R., & Jeglic, E. (2016). and anxiety responses to stress. International Journal
Ruminative subtypes and impulsivity in risk for of Psychophysiology, 117, 111–118. http://dx.doi.org/
suicidal behavior. Psychiatry Research, 236, 15–21. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.04.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.008
Wilson, M. (2008). From processing efficiency to atten-
Vallerand, R. J., & Blanchard, C. M. (2000). The study tional control: A mechanistic account of the anxiety–
of emotion in sport and exercise: Historical, defini- performance relationship. International Review
tional, and conceptual perspectives. In Y. L. Hanin of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 184–201.
(Ed.), Emotions in sport (pp. 3–37). Champaign, IL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17509840802400787
Human Kinetics.
Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention.
Van der Does, H. T., Brink, M. S., Visscher, C., Huijgen, Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104. http://dx.doi.org/
B. C., Frencken, W. G., & Lemmink, K. A. (2015). 10.1037/h0031332

242
Emotion and Sport Performance

Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., & Proffitt, D. R. (2012). Get Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation
me out of this slump! Visual illusions improve sports of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-
performance. Psychological Science, 23, 397–399. formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611428810 and Psychology, 18, 459–482. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/cne.920180503
Wolf, S. A., Eys, M. A., & Kleinert, J. (2015). Predictors of
the precompetitive anxiety response: Relative impact Zadra, J. R., & Clore, G. L. (2011). Emotion and percep-
and prospects for anxiety regulation. International tion: The role of affective information. WIREs
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 344–358. Cognitive Science, 2, 676–685. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2014.982676 10.1002/wcs.147
Woodman, T., Albinson, J., & Hardy, L. (1997). An inves- Zaichkowsky, L., & Baltzell, A. (2001). Arousal and
tigation of the zones of optimal functioning hypoth- performance in sport. In R. Singer, Hausenblas, H. A.,
esis within a multidimensional framework. Journal & Janelle, C. (Ed.), The handbook of research in
of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 19, 131–141. http:// sport psychology (2nd ed., 319–339). New York, NY:
dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.19.2.131 Wiley.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

243

You might also like