You are on page 1of 6

Article

Childhood Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Traits


in Adult Women With Eating Disorders:
Defining a Broader Eating Disorder Phenotype

Marija Brecelj Anderluh, M.D. Objective: The authors retrospectively ex- value for development of eating disorders,
amined a spectrum of childhood traits that with the estimated odds ratio for eating
reflect obsessive-compulsive personality in disorders increasing by a factor of 6.9 for
Kate Tchanturia, Ph.D.
adult women with eating disorders and as- every additional trait present. Subjects
sessed the predictive value of the traits for with eating disorders who reported perfec-
Sophia Rabe-Hesketh, Ph.D. the development of eating disorders.
tionism and rigidity in childhood had sig-
Janet Treasure, Ph.D., F.R.C.P., Method: In a case-control design, 44 nificantly higher rates of obsessive-com-
women with anorexia nervosa, 28 women pulsive personality disorder and OCD
F.R.C.Psych. with bulimia nervosa, and 28 healthy fe- comorbidity later in life, compared with
male comparison subjects were assessed eating disorder subjects who did not re-
with an interview instrument that asked
port those traits.
them to recall whether they had experi-
enced various types of childhood behavior Conclusions: Childhood traits reflecting
suggesting traits associated with obsessive- obsessive-compulsive personality appear
compulsive personality. The subjects also to be important risk factors for the devel-
completed a self-report inventory of obses- opment of eating disorders and may rep-
sive-compulsive disorder (OCD) symptoms. resent markers of a broader phenotype
Results: Childhood obsessive-compulsive for a specific subgroup of patients with
personality traits showed a high predictive anorexia nervosa.

(Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:242–247)

T he etiology of complex disorders such as anorexia


nervosa and bulimia nervosa is likely to involve a dynamic
of perfectionism are overrepresented among healthy first-
degree relatives of patients with eating disorders, suggest-
interplay of environmental and genetic factors. New ap- ing that obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, or its
proaches in genetic research provide an opportunity to traits, represent a heritable factor contributing to vulnera-
study associations between a disorder and specific genes bility for both types of eating disorders (13, 15, 16).
but face a well-recognized problem of phenotypic defini- The aim of this study was to determine whether retro-
tion. It is likely that diagnostic categories as presented in spectively measured childhood personality traits reflect-
the DSM-IV do not fully correspond to underlying biologi- ing obsessive-compulsive personality predicted develop-
cal factors. ment of eating disorders. To minimize biases related to
One approach to specifying phenotypes is to use both retrospective reporting, the study used a newly developed
axis I and axis II dimensions. The restricting subtype of an- interview instrument with items based on examples of
orexia nervosa is typically linked to personality traits such childhood behavior. The study hypothesis was that child-
as perfectionism, sense of ineffectiveness, preoccupation hood traits linked to obsessive-compulsive personality are
important risk factors for the development of eating disor-
with orderliness, and excessive persistence and compliance
ders. The subsidiary hypothesis was that people with an
(1–7). Bulimia nervosa is often linked with impulsive per-
eating disorder who had a high number of childhood traits
sonality traits (8, 9), but the majority of people with bulimia
reflecting obsessive-compulsive personality have a higher
nervosa are also found to be perfectionistic (4). In a study of
prevalence of this type of personality disorder and have
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) symptoms in
more compulsive symptoms later in life.
women with bulimia, 39% of the subjects with current bu-
limia or a history of bulimia were found to have obsessions
related to symmetry and exactness (10). Perfectionism and Method
preoccupation with orderliness have been found to persist Participants
after recovery from eating disorders (10–13). Perfectionism
The participants consisted of three groups: 44 female patients
in childhood is one of the risk factors for both anorexia and with a DSM-IV diagnosis of anorexia nervosa (26 with the restrict-
bulimia (6, 14). In family studies, persons with a high level ing type, 18 with the binge-eating/purging type), 28 female pa-

242 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 160:2, February 2003


ANDERLUH, TCHANTURIA, RABE-HESKETH, ET AL.

tients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of bulimia nervosa, and 28 healthy TABLE 1. Childhood Traits Reflecting Obsessive-Compulsive
female comparison subjects. The clinical participants were re- Personality and Main Areas of the Child’s Life in Which the
cruited from the inpatient and outpatient programs of the Eating Traits Are Assessed in the EATATE Interview
Disorders Unit of the Maudsley Hospital, London, a tertiary refer- Assessed Trait Areas of the Child’s Life
ral center. To avoid possible reporting biases related to an acute Perfectionism: Perfectionism is Schoolwork
physical state, patients who fulfilled the diagnostic criteria and assessed separately in four Self-care
were willing to participate were entered in the study 3–4 weeks af- areas of child’s life; the trait is Looking after her room
ter the beginning of treatment. Exclusion criteria consisted of a regarded as present if reported Hobbies, caring for pets,
history of psychotic disorder or bipolar disorder assessed accord- to have influenced markedly part-time job, or housework
ing to the ICD-10 criteria. The comparison subjects were admin- the child’s life in at least two of
istrative staff and students from two universities in different the assessed areas.
Inflexibility: Inflexibility and Difficulties in adjusting to changes
towns in the United Kingdom. They were matched by age, sex,
rule-bound trait both measure linked to house moves, school
and educational level with the patients and were blind to the rigidity; rigidity is regarded as changes, changes in family
study hypothesis. The comparison subjects were of normal present if at least one of the schedule or structure, changes
weight, had no personal or family history of an eating disorder or two traits was present and in planned daily activities
any other psychiatric disorder, and had no history of bingeing, markedly influenced the Presence of activities to
vomiting, or abuse of laxatives. All subjects except those recruited child’s life. compensate for these
as inpatients were reimbursed for their time. All participants were difficulties, including written
native English speakers. The study was approved by the South plans, making contingency plans
London and Maudsley National Health Service Trust and Institute Rule-bound trait: Inflexibility Excessive persistence
and rule-bound trait both High degree of compliance with
of Psychiatry ethics committee. After complete description of the
measure rigidity; rigidity is rules set by parents or teachers
study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained. regarded as present if at least
one of the two traits was
Procedure present and markedly
Specialists in the Eating Disorders Unit who were blind to the influenced the child’s life.
study hypothesis made the initial screening diagnoses of eating Excessive doubt and Excessive doubt about actions
disorders using the DSM-IV criteria. Study subjects were inter- cautiousness: The trait is Excessive cautiousness about
viewed by a trained researcher (one of six postgraduate psycholo- regarded as present if both making a mistake
excessive doubt and
gists and psychiatrists) using a semistructured interview (de-
cautiousness were present and
scribed in the next section). On the day of the interview, data on markedly influenced the
demographic characteristics were collected, and the subject’s child’s life.
weight and height were measured. Thirty interviews were re- Drive for order and symmetry: Looking after her room
corded on audiotapes and were rerated by one of two assessors The trait is regarded as present Housework
who were blind both to the previous scoring and to the subject’s if reported to have influenced Appearance (dress, hair style)
diagnosis. markedly the child’s life in at
least two of the assessed areas.
Measures
The instruments used in the study included the semistructured “anankastic,” and “childhood.” A focus group of patients with an-
EATATE interview and the self-report Maudsley Obsessive-Com- orexia nervosa was held to identify behavioral examples of child-
pulsive Inventory. The National Adult Reading Test was used to hood traits reflecting obsessive-compulsive personality. The be-
match patients and comparison subjects in intellectual ability. havioral examples were used in designing the EATATE interview
The EATATE interview. The EATATE interview was developed to minimize the problems associated with retrospective reporting
for the European Healthy Eating Project, which examined genetic and to reduce rationalization. The final instrument is used to as-
and environmental risk factors for eating disorders and obesity. sess five childhood traits that reflect obsessive-compulsive per-
(The instrument is available from the first author.) The first part, sonality (Table 1): perfectionism (e.g., Did you spend a long time
which consists of an adaptation of the Eating Disorder Examina- doing or redoing your hair to make sure it was straight without
tion (17), is used to identify a lifetime eating disorder diagnosis in bumps?), inflexibility (e.g., To what extent were you the sort of
subjects and first-degree family members. The first part also is person who liked to make written plans/notes or have intricate
used to assess whether the subject meets the criteria for an ICD- details about the time ahead?), rule driven (e.g., Were you the kind
10 lifetime diagnosis of OCD. Food and body-related obsessions of person who felt she always had to follow rules? For example,
are excluded as symptoms of OCD. The second part of the EATATE how far did you bend or break rules that were set by your parents
interview, which provided the data that are the focus of this paper, or teachers?), drive-for-order and symmetry (e.g., While trying to
is used to assess whether the subject meets the criteria for current get your room tidy and organized, were you particularly con-
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, according to the ICD- cerned about making sure that everything was “just so” and in its
10 International Personality Disorder Examination (18) module proper place?), and excessive doubt and cautiousness (e.g., Were
for anankastic personality disorder, and to collect data on child- your frightened to make a mistake as a child? Can you give an ex-
hood traits reflecting obsessive-compulsive personality. (An ICD- ample?). The interviewer used a scoring manual to rate the re-
10 diagnosis of anankastic personality disorder corresponds to a sponses according to predefined criteria. The interviewers were
DSM-IV diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder). trained in the use of the manual by scoring videotaped interviews
The prototype of the assessment of childhood traits was devel- and by participating in consensus meetings where ratings were
oped in an expert meeting by collaborators on the European discussed. The presence of each trait was assessed together with
Healthy Eating project and other experts with extensive clinical its effect on the child’s life in terms of the child’s relationship with
and research experience in the field. After this session, the MED- the world and with others. Traits were rated 0 for absent, 1 for
LINE and PsycINFO databases (1965–2001) were searched for pa- present but not influencing the child’s life, or 2 for impinging on
pers describing current and past personality traits linked with the child’s life or her relationship with the world or with others.
eating disorders. The key words used in the search were “person- Assessment of interrater reliability was based on the full 3-point
ality,” “perfectionism,” “rigidity,” “inflexibility,” “obsessionality,” scale. In all other analyses, the values 0 and 1 were not discrimi-

Am J Psychiatry 160:2, February 2003 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 243


CHILDHOOD TRAITS AND ADULT EATING DISORDERS

TABLE 2. Characteristics of Subjects With Eating Disorders and Healthy Comparison Subjects in a Study of Childhood Traits
Reflecting Obsessive-Compulsive Personalitya
Subjects With Anorexia Subjects With Bulimia Healthy Comparison
Characteristic Nervosa (N=44) Nervosa (N=28) Subjects (N=28) Analysis
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD F df p

Current age (years)b 27.9 9.1 26.7 9.5 25.1 5.1 0.92 2, 97 n.s.
Body mass index (kg/m2)
Currentc 15.9 2.8 21.6 2.0 22.1 2.4 74.05 2, 97 <0.001
Lowest ever 12.2 1.8 17.1 2.6 — 87.51 1, 70 <0.001
Highest ever 20.7 2.6 24.2 2.4 — 33.58 1, 70 <0.001
National Adult Reading Test scored 16.4 5.6 17.8 4.0 15.1 5.4 0.80 2, 41 n.s.
Age at onset of eating disorder (years) 16.3 3.6 16.8 2.8 — 0.51 1, 70 n.s.
Maudsley Obsessive-Compulsive
Inventory scoree,f 12.6 6.1 11.0 6.1 3.8 3.1 22.70 2, 72 <0.001

N % N % N % χ2 df p
ICD-10 diagnosis
Anankastic (obsessive-compulsive)
personality disorderf 27 61 13 46 1 4 24.10 2 <0.001
Obsessive-compulsive disorderf,g 18 45 5 20 0 0 18.33 2 <0.001
a Patients with eating disorders were recruited from the inpatient and outpatient programs of the Eating Disorders Unit of the Maudsley
Hospital, London. Comparison subjects were recruited among students and administrative staff at two universities in the United Kingdom.
b Age range=15–52 years for the subjects with anorexia nervosa, 15–61 for the subjects with bulimia nervosa, and 18–45 for the healthy
comparison subjects.
c Significant difference between subjects with anorexia nervosa and subjects with bulimia nervosa (p<0.001, Tukey).
d N=18 for patients with anorexia nervosa, N=9 for patients with bulimia nervosa, N=16 for comparison subjects.
e N=26 for patients with anorexia nervosa, N=19 for patients with bulimia nervosa, N=20 for comparison subjects.
f No significant difference between subjects with anorexia nervosa and subjects with bulimia nervosa (Tukey).
g N=40 for patients with anorexia nervosa, N=25 for patients with bulimia nervosa, N=28 for comparison subjects.

nated, and only traits that were reported to impinge on the child’s subjects with eating disorders who did and did not report child-
life (a rating of 2) were regarded as present. Any problems with the hood perfectionism and rigidity. Two-tailed tests with a 5% level
ratings were discussed at consensus meetings. of significance were used throughout the analyses. No significant
differences were found between the subjects with the restricting
Maudsley Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory. The Maudsley
type of anorexia nervosa and those with the binge-eating/purging
Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory (19) is a 30-item self-report
type. Thus, the data for those subgroups were analyzed together.
scale that assesses four types of obsessive-compulsive com-
All analysis were carried out by using SPSS-PC (21).
plaints: checking, washing, doubting, and slowness. Alpha coeffi-
cients of 0.7, 0.8, 0.7, and 0.7 for the four subscales, respectively,
were found. Good test-retest reliability has been reported for the Results
instrument (Kendall’s tau=0.8) (19).
The National Adult Reading Test. The National Adult Read- Subject Characteristics
ing Test (20) was used to match the participant groups in terms Table 2 summarizes the subjects’ demographic and clin-
of approximate intellectual ability. The National Adult Reading
Test requires the participant to read aloud 50 phonetically ir-
ical characteristics. The groups did not differ significantly
regular words, and the number of mistakes is recorded. The test in age, and the eating disorder groups had similar mean
results have been found to correlate significantly with level of ages at onset of the illness. As expected, the body mass in-
education (20). dex at assessment differed significantly across the groups
(F=74.05, df=2, 97, p<0.001). The two clinical groups dif-
Analysis
fered significantly on the lowest past body mass index (F=
The clinical and demographic data were compared across 87.51, df=1, 70, p<0.001). National Adult Reading Test
groups by using chi-square tests and one-way analysis of vari-
ance, with the Tukey test for post hoc comparisons of group dif-
scores were assessed in about half of the subjects, and the
ferences. Interrater reliability for the childhood trait scale of the availability of data was consistent across the groups. No
EATATE interview was assessed with weighted kappa coefficients significant differences were observed in National Adult
calculated for individual items (rated as 0–2) and an unweighted Reading Test score across the groups. The three groups dif-
kappa coefficient for the diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive per- fered significantly in the prevalence of obsessive-compul-
sonality disorder. Cronbach’s alpha was calculated to measure the
internal consistency of the childhood trait scale. Linear regres-
sive personality disorder (χ2=24.10, df=2, p<0.001), but the
sion analysis was used to examine the relationship of childhood difference in prevalence between the two patients groups
and adult obsessive-compulsive personality traits within the eat- was not significant. The prevalence of OCD comorbidity
ing disorder and the comparison groups. Logistic regression was was higher in the bulimia nervosa group than in the anor-
used to measure the association of childhood obsessive-compul- exic group, although the difference did not reach signifi-
sive personality traits with the presence of an eating disorder and
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder later in life. T tests for
cance. Although every effort was made to collect complete
independent samples (with Bonferroni corrections) and chi- data for all subjects, data from the Maudsley Obsessive-
square tests (with Yates’s corrections) were used to compare the Compulsive Inventory were available for only 62% of the

244 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 160:2, February 2003


ANDERLUH, TCHANTURIA, RABE-HESKETH, ET AL.

TABLE 3. Prevalence of Childhood Traits Reflecting Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Among Subjects With Eating Disor-
ders and Healthy Comparison Subjects
Subjects With Anorexia Subjects With Anorexia
Nervosa, Restricting Nervosa, Binge-Eating/ Subjects With Bulimia All Eating Disorder Healthy Comparison
Type (N=26) Purging Type (N=18) Nervosa (N=28) Subjects (N=72) Subjects (N=28)
Traita N % N % N % N % N %
Perfectionism 17 65.4 13 72.2 14 50.0 44 61.1 0 0.0
Inflexibility 20 76.9 11 61.1 7 25.0 38 52.8 0 0.0
Rule-bound trait 16 61.5 14 77.8 14 50.0 44 61.1 5 17.9
Doubt and cautiousnessb 6 27.3 7 46.7 6 21.4 19 29.2 0 0
Drive for order and
symmetryc 10 38.5 5 31.3 3 10.7 18 25.7 1 3.6
a Kappa coefficients measuring interrater reliability were 0.79 for perfectionism, 0.89 for inflexibility, 0.76 for rule-bound trait, 0.73 for doubt
and cautiousness, and 0.65 for drive for order and symmetry.
b N=22 for subjects with anorexia nervosa, restricting type; N=15 for subjects with anorexia nervosa, binge-eating/purging type; N=65 for all
eating disorder subjects.
c N=16 for subjects with anorexia nervosa, binge-eating/purging type; N=70 for all eating disorder subjects.

subjects. The loss of data was consistent across the groups, formed with the presence of an eating disorder as the de-
and subjects with a completed Maudsley Obsessive-Com- pendent variable and the number of childhood traits as the
pulsive Inventory did not differ from those without a com- independent variable. With every additional trait reported,
pleted inventory on any of the clinical parameters. The the estimated odds ratio for development of an eating dis-
eating disorder groups reported higher total Maudsley Ob- order increased by 6.9 (95% CI=2.9–16.4, p<0.001), suggest-
sessive-Compulsive Inventory scores than the compari- ing a strong dose-response relationship.
son group (F=22.70, df=2, 72, p<0.001), but no significant
difference was found between the patients with anorexia Childhood Perfectionism and Rigidity
nervosa and those with bulimia. and Adult Clinical Variables
The patients with eating disorders were grouped ac-
Interrater Reliability and Consistency cording to whether childhood perfectionism and rigidity
of the Childhood Trait Scale traits were reported. (These dimensions showed the high-
The weighted kappa was estimated from 30 interviews est difference in prevalence between the clinical and the
that were recorded on audiotapes and rerated by an asses- comparison groups). Two-thirds of the patients with anor-
sor who was blind to the previous scorings. Good inter- exia nervosa and approximately one-third of the patients
rater reliability of the instrument was observed, with with bulimia nervosa reported perfectionism and at least
mean weighted kappa coefficients of 0.74 and 0.80 for the one of the two traits reflecting rigidity in childhood; the
ratings of childhood traits and for the diagnosis of adult difference in the prevalence of the traits between the two
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, respectively. groups was significant (χ2=7.1, df=2, p<0.03). People with
The weighted kappas for the individual items are shown in eating disorders who reported perfectionism and rigidity
Table 3. The childhood scale also showed good internal in childhood did not differ significantly in their age at on-
consistency (Cronbach’s alpha=0.74). set of illness or in their lowest-ever body mass index from
those who did not report these traits, but they did have sig-
Childhood Traits
nificantly higher dimensional scores on the assessment of
Robust differences between groups were observed in obsessive-compulsive personality disorder in adulthood
the frequency of the reported childhood traits reflecting (t=4.67, df=70, p<0.001). However, the prevalence of life-
obsessive-compulsive personality (Table 3). In an analysis time OCD comorbidity and the total Maudsley Obsessive-
that controlled for eating disorder diagnoses, adult obses- Compulsive Inventory score at assessment did not differ
sive-compulsive personality disorder traits were signifi- significantly between the two groups (Table 4).
cantly related to childhood traits reflecting obsessive-
compulsive personality (regression coefficient=0.73, 95%
Discussion
confidence interval [CI]=0.47–0.99, p<0.001). In a logistic
regression analysis with adult obsessive-compulsive per- The results of this study confirm our hypothesis that
sonality disorder diagnosis as the dependent variable and women with eating disorders show high levels of obses-
childhood traits as the independent variables, every addi- sive-compulsive personality traits in childhood, relative to
tional reported trait increased the estimated odds ratio for healthy comparison subjects. There was a strong increas-
development of adult obsessive-compulsive personality ing relationship between the number of reported child-
disorder by 2.1 (95% CI=1.4–3.2, p<0.001). hood traits and the odds of developing an eating disorder,
To test the predictive value of the childhood obsessive- with each extra reported trait increasing the odds of devel-
compulsive personality traits for the development of an oping an eating disorder nearly sevenfold. We also con-
eating disorder, a logistic regression analysis was per- firmed our subsidiary hypothesis of developmental conti-

Am J Psychiatry 160:2, February 2003 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 245


CHILDHOOD TRAITS AND ADULT EATING DISORDERS

TABLE 4. Characteristics of Subjects With Anorexia Nervosa or Bulimia Nervosa With and Without Childhood Traits of
Perfectionism and Rigiditya
Childhood Perfectionism and Rigidity
Characteristic Yes (N=37) No (N=35) Analysis
Mean SD Mean SD t df p

Age at onset (years) 16.8 3.7 16.1 2.9 0.83 70 n.s.


Lowest-ever body mass index (kg/m2) 13.6 2.9 14.7 3.4 1.60 70 n.s.
EATATE dimensional score for obsessive-compulsive
personality disorder 10.8 3.0 7.3 3.4 4.67 70 <0.01b
Maudsley Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory scorec 13.5 6.1 10.0 5.6 2.06 45 n.s.

N % N % χ2 (Yates’s Correction) df p
ICD-10 diagnosis
Anankastic (obsessive-compulsive) personality disorder 28 75.7 12 34.3 10.86 1 <0.001
Obsessive-compulsive disorderd 11 33.3 12 37.5 0.01 1 n.s.
a Rigidity was considered present if either inflexibility or the rule-bound trait was present and markedly influenced the child’s life.
b With Bonferroni correction.
c N=26 for subjects with childhood perfectionism and rigidity; N=21 for subjects without childhood perfectionism and rigidity.
d N=33 for subjects with childhood perfectionism and rigidity; N=32 for subjects without childhood perfectionism and rigidity.

nuity in that the presence of childhood perfectionism and interviewer-based ratings (according to predefined crite-
rigidity identified a subgroup of people with eating disor- ria) to optimize the precision of the assessment, further
ders with a significantly higher prevalence of obsessive- studies with other informants or with a prospective design
compulsive personality disorder later in life. are needed to confirm the findings. It was impossible for
Our results confirm and extend findings from previous the assessor to be blind to the primary diagnosis in assess-
studies. Fairburn et al. (14) assessed perfectionism in ing severely emaciated subjects with anorexia nervosa.
childhood with a single item (high personal standards) This source of bias was minimized by rerating audiotaped
and identified it as a risk factor for anorexia and bulimia in interviews. Indeed, for the interviews that were audio-
eating disorder patients, compared with healthy subjects taped, we found a high level of agreement between the two
(odds ratio=3.9, 95% CI=2.1–7.4, and odds ratio=2.6, 95% raters, one of whom scored the taped interview and was
CI=1.5–4.6, respectively). The instrument we developed blind to the subject’s appearance. The subjects with eating
retrospectively examined a broader spectrum of obses- disorders were recruited from a tertiary referral center and
sive-compulsive personality traits, including perfection- were taking medication while participating in the study.
ism, by using a range of behavioral examples. Thus, their medication status may have influenced report-
Approximately two-thirds of the subjects with anorexia ing accuracy.
nervosa reported perfectionism and rigidity in childhood. The findings suggest that childhood obsessive-compul-
This prevalence is consistent with that reported by Rastam sive personality traits are important risk factors for later
(22) for premorbid obsessive-compulsive (or anankastic) development of eating disorders, particularly anorexia
personality disorder in anorexia nervosa. We are not aware nervosa. Furthermore, the findings suggest that childhood
of any similar studies conducted for patients with bulimia perfectionism and rigidity may offer a more specific and
nervosa. However, previous findings that perfectionism, homogenous phenotypic determination for genetic stud-
obsessionality, excessive concern about mistakes, and ies. Further studies are needed to determine whether
doubt about actions persist after recovery from both disor- these traits are specific for eating disorders or are also
ders suggest that they represent persistent traits (10, 13, 16). linked to other psychiatric disorders, such as depression
The prevalence of comorbidity of anorexia and bulimia or OCD. Personality traits may also act as maintaining fac-
with OCD in this study was congruent with the published tors and as such may have an important influence on the
rates of 25%–69% in women with anorexia nervosa (23–25) prognosis of the disorder. Studies of people who have re-
and 25%–36% in women with bulimia nervosa (8, 26). covered from an eating disorder would be needed to ex-
The prevalence of cluster C personality disorders, which plore the influence of childhood obsessive-compulsive-
include obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, has personality traits on the length of illness and its severity.
been reported to range between 5% and 80% in people Female subjects were included in this study because the
with anorexia nervosa (4, 27–29) and to be as high as 25% prevalence of eating disorders is approximately nine times
in people with bulimia nervosa (8, 28). These figures are higher in women than in men. However, further studies
congruent with our findings. that include male subjects are needed to better under-
This study had several limitations. The retrospective as- stand the role of the assessed traits. To our knowledge, the
sessment was subject to inaccuracies of memory and sub- interview scale described here is the first to measure these
jectivity in reporting. Although the instrument used in this personality trait risk factors in a broad and comprehensive
study was based on simple behavioral examples and used way. The finding that perfectionism and rigidity represent

246 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 160:2, February 2003


ANDERLUH, TCHANTURIA, RABE-HESKETH, ET AL.

strong risk factors suggests that these items might also be 12. Srinivasagam NM, Kaye WH, Plotnicov KH, Greeno C, Weltzin
also used to identify people at high risk for developing an TE, Rao R: Persistent perfectionism, symmetry, and exactness
after long-term recovery from anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychia-
eating disorder later in life. Prospective studies are needed
try 1995; 152:1630–1634
to replicate these findings. 13. Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Moss H, Fernstrom J, Fernstrom M, Lilen-
feld LR, Weltzin TE, Mann JJ: Alterations in serotonin activity
Received Sept. 4, 2001; revisions received March 6 and Aug. 6, and psychiatric symptoms after recovery from bulimia ner-
2002; accepted Aug. 27, 2002. From the Institute of Psychiatry, King vosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:927–935
College London, De Crespigny Park; and the University Psychiatric 14. Fairburn CG, Welch SL, Doll HA, Davies BA, O’Connor ME: Risk
Hospital Ljubljana, Ljubljana-Polje, Slovenia. Address reprint re- factors for bulimia nervosa: a community-based case-control
quests to Dr. Anderluh, Eating Disorders Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:509–517
De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AD; marija.brecelj@guest.arnes.si
15. Lilenfeld LR, Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Merikangas KR, Plotnicov K,
(e-mail).
Pollice C, Rao R, Strober M, Bulik CM, Nagy L: A controlled fam-
Supported by grant QLK1-1999-916 from the European Commis-
ily study of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: psychiatric
sion Framework V program.
disorders in first-degree relatives and effects of proband co-
The authors thank Prof. C. Fairburn, Prof. P. McGuffin, Prof. R. Plo-
min, Dr. I. Hayman, Dr. D. Nicholls, and the European Consortium for morbidity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:603–610
advice provided at the expert meeting to develop the EATATE inter- 16. Lilenfeld LR, Stein D, Bulik CM, Strober M, Plotnicov K, Pollice C,
view and Prof. I. Campbell and Dr. N. Troop for comments on the Rao R, Merikangas KR, Nagy L, Kaye WH: Personality traits
manuscript. among currently eating disordered, recovered and never ill
first-degree female relatives of bulimic and control women.
Psychol Med 2000; 30:1399–1410
References 17. Cooper Z, Cooper PJ, Fairburn CG: The validity of the Eating Dis-
order Examination and its subscales. Br J Psychiatry 1989; 154:
1. Lasègue C: De l’anorexie hysterique (1873), in Evolution of Psy- 807–812
chosomatic Concepts: Anorexia Nervosa: A Paradigm. Edited 18. Loranger AW, Janca A, Sartorius N (eds): Assessment and Diag-
by Kaufman RM, Heiman M. New York, International Universi- nosis of Personality Disorders: The ICD-10 International Per-
ties Press, 1964, pp 141–155 sonality Disorder Examination (IPDE). New York, Cambridge
2. Bruch H: Perceptual and conceptual disturbances in anorexia University Press, 1997
nervosa. Psychosom Med 1962; 24:187–194 19. Hodgson RJ, Rachman S: Obsessional-compulsive complaints.
3. Strober M: Personality and symptomatological features in Behav Res Ther 1977; 15:389–395
young, nonchronic anorexia nervosa patients. J Psychosom Res 20. Nelson HE, O’Connell A: Dementia: the estimation of premor-
1980; 24:353–359 bid intelligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex
4. Vitousek K, Manke F: Personality variables and disorders in an- 1987; 14:234–244
orexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 21. Norusis MJ: SPSS for Windows: Base System User’s Guide and
103:137–147 Advanced Statistics, Release 6.1. Chicago, SPSS, 1994
5. Bastiani AM, Rao R, Weltzin T, Kaye WH: Perfectionism in anor- 22. Rastam M: Anorexia nervosa in 51 Swedish adolescents: pre-
exia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1995; 17:147–152 morbid problems and comorbidity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1992; 31:819–829
6. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Doll HA, Welch SL: Risk factors for anor-
23. Rothenberg A: Eating disorder as a modern obsessive-compul-
exia nervosa: three integrated case-control comparisons. Arch
sive syndrome. Psychiatry 1986; 49:45–53
Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:468–476
24. Hudson JI, Pope HG Jr, Jonas JM, Yurgelun-Todd D: Phenome-
7. Halmi KA, Sunday SR, Strober M, Kaplan A, Woodside DB, Fich-
nologic relationship of eating disorders to major affective dis-
ter M, Treasure J, Berrettini WH, Kaye WH: Perfectionism in an-
order. Psychiatry Res 1983; 9:345–354
orexia nervosa: variation by clinical subtype, obsessionality,
25. Halmi KA, Eckert E, Marchi P, Sampugnaro V, Apple R, Cohen J:
and pathological eating behavior. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:
Comorbidity of psychiatric diagnoses in anorexia nervosa. Arch
1799–1805
Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:712–718
8. Braun DL, Sunday SR, Halmi KA: Psychiatric comorbidity in pa- 26. Hudson JI, Pope HG Jr, Jonas JM, Yurgelun-Todd D, Frankenburg
tients with eating disorders. Psychol Med 1994; 24:859–867 FR: A controlled family history study of bulimia. Psychol Med
9. Skodol AE, Oldham JM, Hyler SE, Kellman HD, Doidge N, Davies 1987; 17:883–890
M: Comorbidity of DSM-III-R eating disorders and personality 27. Nilsson EW, Gillberg C, Gillberg IC, Rastam M: Ten-year follow-
disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 14:403–416 up of adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa: personality disor-
10. von Ranson KM, Kaye WH, Weltzin TE, Rao R, Matsunaga H: Ob- ders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:1389–1395
sessive-compulsive disorder symptoms before and after recov- 28. Gartner AF, Marcus RN, Halmi K, Loranger AW: DSM-III-R per-
ery from bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1703– sonality disorders in patients with eating disorders. Am J Psy-
1708 chiatry 1989; 146:1585–1591
11. Casper RC: Personality features of women with good outcome 29. Wonderlich SA, Swift WJ, Slotnick HB, Goodman S: DSM-III-R
from restricting anorexia nervosa. Psychosom Med 1990; 52: personality disorders in eating-disorder subtypes. Int J Eat Dis-
156–170 ord 1990; 9:607–616

Am J Psychiatry 160:2, February 2003 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 247

You might also like