You are on page 1of 12

Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Linguistics and Education


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/linged

Modes and meaning in the classroom – The role of different semiotic


resources to convey meaning in science classrooms
Kristina Danielsson
Department of Language Education, Stockholm University, 106 91 Stockholm, Sweden

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study is framed within social semiotic perspectives on multimodality, and it has a twofold aim. The
Received 5 July 2014 primary aim is to analyze the ways in which teachers draw on different semiotic resources when intro-
Received in revised form 29 June 2016 ducing a new scientific concept in secondary school science classrooms, and to link the results to modal
Accepted 13 July 2016
affordance. A secondary aim is to try out parallel analyses of different modes in multimodal meaning
making using the ideational meta-function of the SFL framework. Analyses are based on instructional
Keywords:
episodes when chemistry teachers introduced the atom as a scientific phenomenon. The main focus of
Multimodality
the analyses is on processes used in different modes and how these depict the atom as either static or
Disciplinary literacy
Semiotic affordance
dynamic. The framework proved fruitful, and analyses revealed important patterns as to what aspects of
Systemic functional linguistics the atom were given through what mode(s), something which could partly be linked to modal affordance.
Science teaching The results are discussed in relation to its implications for research and education.
Classroom communication © 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc.

1. Introduction Lemke’s (1998) case study, focusing on one student in secondary


school during a single day in physics and chemistry classrooms
Over the last decades, the role of language in meaning making is elucidating as to the complex demands for students to “juggle”
has been emphasized by numerous scholars (Halliday & Martin, between and integrate, for instance, verbal, chemical-symbolic, and
1993; Lemke, 1990; Norris & Phillips, 2003; Schleppegrell, 2004; visual–graphical symbolic systems for different aspects of scientific
Shanahan & Shanahan, 2008; Wellington & Osborne, 2001). How- content. Yet, Lemke claims that the same information needs to be
ever, multimodality is ubiquitous in all communication, and a accessible through as many types of media or channels as possi-
specialized use of modes for representation (e.g. writing, speech, ble, not only since exactly the same content cannot be expressed
gesture, and image) is central in every content area (e.g. Danielsson through any mode, but also since there needs to be a level of redun-
& Selander, 2014, 2016; Kress et al., 2005; Kress, Jewitt, Ogborn, & dancy in the learning situation. Another example is the study by
Tsatsarelis, 2001). Tang and colleagues (Tang et al., 2011), where ninth-grade physics
In this article, mode is defined as a meaning-making resource students were followed during a teaching and learning cycle on the
system which is shaped and developed over time in a commu- concept of work-energy. Here students’ understanding of multiple
nity. Examples of modes are speech, writing, gesture, and image. representations in different modes was focused. The students had
Multimodality deals with representations or communicative events difficulties in constructing a scientific understanding of the content,
involving more than one mode, for instance a drawing consisting of something which was discussed in regard to differences in the cat-
both image and writing, or spoken words combined with gestures egorical, quantitative, and spatial meanings of the representations.
(see, Jewitt, 2009a, for definitions and further discussions of these The findings in both these studies gave rise to reflections about the
concepts). Meaning making in science classrooms, which is the importance of meta-discussions in classrooms as to how content is
focus of the present study, has repeatedly been described as highly expressed.
multimodal, involving action, speech, writing, subject specific sym- Since each representation of a concept (in the form of a gesture,
bols, graphs, and diagrams, et cetera (e.g. Axelsson & Danielsson, an image, words, etc.) has its specific potential for meaning making
2012; Jakobson et al., forthcoming; Kress et al., 2001; Lemke, 1998; (see affordance, below), certain aspects will be toned down while
Simpson & Walsh, 2010; Tang, Tan, & Yeo, 2011). others will be emphasized depending on the representation used.
Thus, to be able to “juggle” with representations, or to make mean-
ing from the teacher’s juggling, is a demanding task for the novice,
E-mail address: kristina.danielsson@isd.su.se who is about to learn something new.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.linged.2016.07.005
0898-5898/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc.
K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99 89

Regarding the role of multiple representations in knowledge with ways of expressing that knowledge through different semiotic
building in science, Tang and colleagues (Tang, Delgado, & Moje, choices.
2014) have noted two main foci. Firstly, research on multiple rep- Studies within social semiotics are connected to Halliday’s sys-
resentations concerning how multiple representations of scientific temic functional grammar (SFG) (Halliday & Matthiessen, 2004), the
concepts in classroom practices affect students’ understanding; theoretical foundation of systemic functional linguistics (SFL); the
secondly, research on multimodality dealing with students’ under- latter term is often used for applications of the SFG system on com-
standing of scientific concepts through the simultaneous use of munication in different modes, such as speech, writing, images, and
various modes within and across representations. The present gestures.
study can be seen as yet another alternative, through its focus on Social-semiotic theory rests on three meta-functions: the
the ways in which various semiotic resources used for describing a ideational (representing meanings about actions, states and events
specific scientific concept convey different meanings of the particu- in the world), the interpersonal (representing meanings about the
lar concept; in this case, the extent to which one curricular entity in social relations of those engaged in the communication) and the
the science classroom, namely the atom, is represented as dynamic textual meta-function (representing the communicative channel
or static in teacher introductions of the concept. and textual organization). These meta-functions reflect different
A key notion for the study is that of affordance (Gibson, 1977; van aspects of meaning making (see Halliday & Matthiessen, 2004, for
Leeuwen, 2005; Kress, 2010), here defined as the ‘meaning poten- a thorough presentation of the framework). The analyses in the
tial’ (van Leeuwen, 2005) or ‘potentials and limitations’ of modes present study are done in relation to the ideational meta-function
(e.g. Kress, 2010:84). This notion relates to the fact that different (sometimes also referred to as experiential meaning), that is, the
modes, as they are realized in social contexts, are governed by dif- field of communication, or what content is in focus in the commu-
ferent logics. Examples are speech, which is governed by the logic nicative situation.
of time (speech sounds happen across time) and image, which is A foundation of the ideational meta-function is the transitivity
governed by the logic of space, with still images also being gov- system in SFG (see Halliday & Matthiessen, 2004:302, for a model).
erned by the logic of simultaneity, and moving images by the logic Transitivity analyses involve three major features in a clause,
of time. This is one reason why speech is regarded as especially namely the type of process (i.e. that something happens, is said, is
apt for describing temporal aspects, or issues of cause and effect. or is perceived: ‘the electrons move in a high speed’), what partici-
Images, on the other hand, are considered to be more apt for spa- pants are involved in the process (i.e. who/what does/is/says/owns
tial properties, such as size and relative position between parts and what, etc.: ‘the electrons move in a high speed’), and the circum-
wholes. stances around the process (where, how, when, etc., the participant
Also, aspects such as valued modes (see Jewitt, 2009b or Kress, did/thought/said: ‘the electrons move in a high speed’).
2009 for discussions) need to be taken into consideration. In This article focuses on process types in the various resources
schools, writing has long been valued, and the mode which students for meaning making that teachers use when introducing the atom
normally are supposed to use when their learning is assessed, that as a phenomenon (see Section 4). Previously, ideational analyses
is, for their learning to be recognized (Kress, 2010; Kress & Selander, connected to different disciplines have mainly been performed on
2012). The present study uses data from a larger project (see Data, writing (e.g. Halliday & Martin, 1993; Schleppegrell, 2004). When
below). One finding in that project was that teachers’ multimodal the SFL framework has been used for other modes, such as images
board notes appeared to be highly valued in the classroom prac- (Kress & van Leeuwen, 2006; Martin & Rose, 2008), other aspects
tices. What was written in the students’ notebooks was considered of the ideational meta-function have been highlighted. In their
decidedly important for passing the final written assessment of the ideational analysis of images in scientific texts, Martin and Rose
course, and with few exceptions these notes were direct trans- focus on aspects such as whether the scientific phenomenon is an
formations of the teachers’ board-notes. Thus, information given entity or an activity or if the categories are explicit or implicit. Even
through other resources, such as speech or gestures, was generally though Martin and Rose (just like Unsworth, e.g. 2001) discuss
disregarded (Danielsson, 2011a). relations between image and writing, the type of detailed analy-
The primary aim of the present study is to reveal patterns in ses to compare modes which is performed in the present study,
chemistry classrooms regarding what aspects of a specific scientific are scarce. Here Martinec’s (2004) model for gesture analyses, is
phenomenon (in this case, the atom) is given through what mode, interesting from a methodological point of view. In his data, Mar-
or combinations of modes in multimodal ensembles (e.g. Jewitt, tinec analyzed both gestures and linguistic choice with respect
2009b; Kress et al., 2001), and to link these results to the notion to experiential meaning (the ideational meta-function), and found
of affordance. The main focus is whether the atom is depicted that process types were often the same in both modes. The fact
as static or dynamic through the different modes. A secondary that Martinec analyzed gestures and verbal language as equivalent
aim is to contribute with methodological insights regarding multi- is relevant for the present study. However, in the present study,
modal analyses, by using the ideational meta-function in systemic a number of modes (speech, writing, images, gestures, etc.), at
functional grammar (Halliday & Matthiessen, 2004) for parallel anal- times appearing in multimodal ensembles, are analyzed through
yses of different modes (see below). With this twofold perspective, the same framework, not only speech and gesture (cf. Martinec,
the present study can contribute with theoretical and analytical 2004), or writing in connection to images (cf. Martin & Rose, 2008;
insights as well as knowledge with relevance for classroom prac- Unsworth, 2001).
tices.
To accommodate for these aims, the study is theoretically
and methodologically framed within social semiotics (Halliday, 1.1. The atomic model
1978), and specifically on multimodality (van Leeuwen, 2005;
Jewitt, 2009a; Kress, 2010). A key notion in social semiotics is Since the atomic model is in focus for they study, some com-
that semiotic choice both reflects and shapes content. Further- ments on the nature of the atom need to be given for the sake
more, choice of semiotic resource or mode for representation is of clarity for readers who might not be familiar with this subject
seen as a result of social, cultural and situational factors in the content.
context in which the communication takes place, including par- At the beginning of the 20th century, Niels Bohr presented his
ticipants and available modes and resources. Hence, from such model of the atom, a model which is widely used even today, not
perspectives, learning something new is intrinsically interwoven the least in schools. A typical image of the model is the third from
90 K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99

Fig. 1. Different models of the atom during the 20th century (Wikipedia 2014: “The evolution of the atomic model in the 20th century”). (The image is free to re-use: http://
en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Evolution of atomic models infographic.svg.)

the left in Fig. 1, with a positively charged nucleus1 surrounded by linguistically and culturally relatively homogenous, with a major-
negatively charged electrons, which, in Bohr’s model, are found in ity of the pupils speaking the language of instruction as their first
orbits (or “shells”) around the nucleus. These orbits are supposed language (Finland-Swedish and Swedish, respectively), though the
to correspond to the approximate distance from the nucleus that teacher and some of the students in the suburban Finland-Swedish
the electrons can be presumed to be, according to their level of classroom were bilingual in Finland-Swedish and Finnish. In all
energy. Note that all these models are to some extent multimodal, classrooms, some students (2–4) spoke an additional language,
with symbols, such as plus and minus signs, integrated in the visual though none of them were newly arrived in the country. The teach-
depiction. ers were all qualified science teachers. For the sake of anonymity, all
The orbits, in combination with the number of valence electrons participants have been assigned pseudonyms. The Finland-Swedish
(the number of electrons in the outer orbit), are the basis for the teachers were given names starting with an F (Fia and Fred), while
position of the chemical substance in the periodic table. the Swedish teachers were given names starting with an S (Sören
Today we also know that electrons appear in pairs (see and Stig).
the rightmost model in Fig. 1, depicting the model of Heisen- The data used in the present study consist of video record-
berg/Schrödinger), and that these pairs have different levels of ings and digital photographs collected during whole class teaching
energy. Simple models of the atom often neglect these pairs, and episodes when the teachers introduced the atom as a scientific phe-
a common simplification is to describe the electrons as particles nomenon. The video recordings were made with one video camera
spinning around a nucleus in a fashion similar to that of the planets’ placed at the back of the classroom, focusing on the teacher. Digital
orbits around the sun. However, it is more correct to view the elec- photographs of the teachers’ notes on the whiteboard or black-
tronic orbits as “electronic clouds”, where the density of the cloud board were taken continuously by the author of the present study.
describes the probability to find an electron at a certain position. In all classrooms, these teaching episodes were a starting point for
A previous article (Danielsson, 2011b) describes how atoms the overall content area, and the introductions were given through
are depicted through different semiotic resources in the chemistry “chalk talk” (Artemeva & Fox, 2011), that is, a spoken exposition
classrooms focused on in the present study. That analysis revealed combined with notes on the board. The introductions were typ-
that the content that students came across varied greatly as to the ically around 8–12 min in length. Stig’s introduction was longer,
qualities of the atom, sometimes with partly contradicting infor- mainly because it contained more social talk besides the actual
mation, such as viewing the atom as either static or a dynamic. content. Such talk was excluded from the analyses. Thus, only the
The present study aims at looking deeper into the role of different actual communication around the atom as a scientific phenomenon
modes for representation in relation to that aspect. is presented in the results section. All in all, around 40 min of video
recordings were used in the present study.
2. Data
3. Methods of analysis
The data used in the present study was collected within the
multidisciplinary project Chemistry text as tools for scientific learn-
The video sequences were transcribed and analyzed using the
ing (Eriksson, 2011), financed by the Swedish Research Council,
software Multimodal Analysis VideoTM , allowing for parallel anal-
though the present study is independent of that project. The over-
yses of various meaning making resources (O’Halloran, Podlasov,
all project followed all lessons during a content area dealing with
Chua, & K.L.E., 2012). In the teachers’ introductions, speech (here
the atomic model, the periodic table, and chemical bonds. Between
defined as spoken words) was accompanied by gestures, and to
20 and 40 lessons, normally around 40 min long, in altogether 7
highlight certain aspects of the content, the teachers made board
Finland-Swedish (in Finland) and Swedish (in Sweden) mainstream
notes, consisting of images (non-verbal visuals), writing (written
secondary school classrooms were studied, with students aged
words), and written chemical or mathematical symbols.2 Also, one
between 14 and 15 years.
model (a wooden ball normally used to represent a coal atom) was
In the present study, data from 4 classrooms have been used,
used once in one of the classrooms. These modes (italicized) were
with one teacher and around 20 students in each classroom; two
the basis for the analyses. However, since the teachers used writ-
Finland-Swedish and two Swedish. In both countries, one class-
ten mathematical or chemical symbols only to a small extent, these
room was in a suburban school outside a main city, and one
modes were merged with writing. Also, the sole use of a model was
was in a school in a mid-sized, rural town. The schools were
merged with image. Thus, four main categories are presented in

1
The positive charge of the nucleus is due to the positively charged proton(s) in
2
the nucleus. Apart from protons, the nucleus can also contain neutrons, which are Chemical or mathematical symbols are considered separate modes from writing,
neutral. since they are governed by specific culturally and socially developed “grammars”.
K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99 91

the overviews in the Results section: speech, gestures, image, and Table 1
Overview of process type in different modes in teacher introductions of the atom.
writing.
Frequencies and percentages for each mode.
As a first step, spoken language was transcribed. In parallel with
this, tags indicating different types of action connected to the con- Relational Material Verbal Mental Total
tent were made in the transcripts. Examples of actions that were Gesture 33 (40%) 51 (60%) 0 0 84
tagged are gesturing or the use of artefacts, such as concrete mod- Image 87 (86%) 14 (14%) 0 0 101
els, or writing and drawing on the board. In this article, “drawing” Speech 261 (56%) 109 (23%) 13 (3%) 84 (18%) 467
Writing 1 0 0 0 1
refers to teachers’ notes on the board when these consisted of an
Total 382 174 13 84 653
image or a combination of image and writing and/or symbols. Thus,
in the following, a drawing can be a multimodal ensemble, while
an image is not. Table 2
Relational and material processes in different modes in teacher introductions of the
As a second step, the various meaning making resources in
atom. Observed and expected frequencies (EF).
the different modes were analyzed through the ideational meta-
function. As was mentioned above, in this article only the results Relational EF Material EF Total
as regards processes will be presented. Gesture 33 58 51 26 84
Halliday and Matthiessen (2004) present six processes: material Image 87 69 14 32 101
(e.g. ‘draw’, ‘take apart’), behavioural (e.g. ‘yawn’, ‘stretch’), mental Speech 261 254 109 116 370
Writing 1 1 0 0 1
(e.g. ‘think’, ‘believe’), verbal (e.g. ‘said’, ‘declared’), relational (e.g. 381 174 556
‘is’), and existential (e.g. ‘live’, ‘exist’). However, in the analyses of
p < 0.001.
the present data, behavioural processes were merged with material,
since the former can be seen as a subgroup of the latter (Andersen,
Petersen, & Smedegaard, 2001; Holmberg & Karlsson, 2006). For the Table 3
Relational and material processes in gestures and images in teacher introductions
same reason, existential processes were merged with relational pro-
of the atom. Observed and expected frequencies (EF).
cesses. Thus, four process types are used in the following: material,
mental, verbal, and relational processes. Here, material processes Relational EF Material EF Total

can be expected to depict the atom as dynamic, while mental, ver- Gesture 33 54 51 30 84
bal and relational processes can be expected to depict the atom as Image 87 66 14 35 101
120 65 185
a static phenomenon.
The unit of analysis for the spoken language was the clause, and p < 0.001.
processes in both independent and dependent clauses were ana-
lyzed: “before, the scientists thought (mental process) that the atom Table 4
was (relational process) the smallest existing particle”. Relational and material processes in gestures and speech in teacher introductions
Only gestures with a clear relation to the subject content were of the atom. Observed and expected frequencies (EF).

analyzed, for instance if a teacher made a circular arm movement Relational EF Material EF Total
when talking about the way electrons move in orbits. The process Gesture 33 54 51 30 84
in such a gesture was coded as material (cf. “the electrons move Speech 261 240 109 130 370
in circles”). For still gestures, as when a teacher shaped his or her 294 160 454
hands spherically when talking about an atomic nucleus, the pro- p < 0.001.
cess was coded as relational (cf. “the nucleus is shaped as a ball”).
These two types of gestures (material and relational) are partic-
Table 5
ularly important in regard to depictions of the atom as a static Relational and material processes in images and speech in teacher introductions of
or dynamic concept, and they have a direct relation to Martinec’s the atom. Observed and expected frequencies (EF).
(2004) analyses of gestures in static or action processes.
Relational EF Material EF Total
In analysing images, two-dimensional images showing particles
Image 87 75 14 26 101
of the atom in static positions (as in Fig. 5) were coded as depict-
Speech 261 273 109 97 370
ing relational processes. If containing an arrow, or if in other ways 348 123 471
indicating a movement, the image was coded as depicting a mate-
p < 0.002.
rial process. The images were analyzed as to their final design (see
e.g. Fig. 7), but repeated codings were made during the teachers’
expositions, when images were built up gradually through “chalk- significant, chi square analyses were used to compare observed
talk”. In these cases, different aspects of the image was commented frequencies with the expected statistical frequencies. The statisti-
on through speech, thus being part of multimodal ensembles (for cal significances are indicated by p-values for the results presented
example the different stages of the image in Fig. 6). Images were in each table. p-values below 0.05 denote statistically significant
usually present for some time after they were drawn, or in the case differences between occurrences of process types in the differ-
of the chart with the periodic table, pointed at. If the teacher related ent modes. For readers unfamiliar with basic statistics such as chi
to the image again, for instance by connecting to it explicitly in square analyses, a brief explanation is given in Bryman (2012).
spoken language (e.g. “as you can see in the drawing”), pointing, or
clearly looking at it, the coding was repeated. The rationale for this 4. Results
decision was that when the teacher brought an image to the fore
again, the image was part of a multimodal ensemble, complement- In this section, to start with, quantifications regarding pro-
ing the spoken words or gestures. cess types expressed through the different modes are presented in
As a next step, basic calculations regarding the process types Tables 1–5. Then the results for each mode are discussed regarding
in different modes were made to give an overview of the data, modal affordance, with examples given from the teaching episodes.
and to reveal patterns of the extent to which the various modes Finally, patterns in multimodal ensembles are presented.
were used in specialized ways regarding process types (Tables 2–5). A total of 653 processes occurred in the data, and these are the
To ensure that differences noted in this regard were statistically basis for the quantifications in Tables 1–5. Table 1 gives an overview
92 K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99

of the frequencies of processes given through the different modes involving mental processes were scientific content, like in the fol-
used during the teachers’ expositions: writing (including mathe- lowing anthropomorphic metaphors (e.g. Taber & Watts, 1996) in
matical and chemical symbols), speech, image (including one use of which particles are humanized, with desires: “negatively charged
a concrete model), and gesture. As can be seen in the overview, a particles do not want to be close to each other”, “negatively charged
majority of the processes were expressed through speech (467 out particles and positively charged particles want to be drawn towards
of 653 instances). Fewer were expressed through image (101) or each other” (the use of metaphors in these classrooms is analyzed
gesture (84). Even though writing includes mathematical and chem- in more detail in Danielsson et al., forthcoming).
ical symbols in the overviews, only one process was given through
this mode. This is due to the fact that teachers mostly wrote sin-
4.1. Different modes for different aspects of the atom – the role of
gle words on the board, usually to label different particles in an
affordance
image depicting the atom. In some cases they also used symbols of
chemical substances, such as H (for ‘Hydrogen’) or Li (for ‘Lithium’).
All images, gestures and instances of writing analyzed were
The single written process in the data was the mathematical symbol
directly connected to the atom as such. As regards speech this was
“=”, analyzed as a relational process (“equals”). Some other symbols
not always the case. Some processes were of the kind exemplified
also imply relational processes (see Multimodal ensembles, below).
around mental processes, above, with the students or scientists as
The concrete model (a wooden ball) gave a static depiction, with
the main participant. Others were related to what the teachers did,
the process coded as relational.
or what they wanted the students to do, such as “Here I have drawn a
Table 1 also reveals that the vast majority of processes in the
simplified model of the atom” (as mentioned in Section 3, social talk
data were relational. Around half as many were material; fewer
was already excluded from the analyses). With this said, interest-
were mental, while verbal processes were even more scarce. Rela-
ing patterns in the data can be discerned, which can be connected
tional and material processes occurred in all modes, while mental
to affordance. Since only one process was expressed in writing (see
and verbal processes only occurred in speech. As was mentioned
Table 1), only gestures, images, speech, and multimodal ensembles
in the Methods section, mainly material processes have the poten-
will be discussed in the following.
tial to depict the atom as a dynamic phenomenon. Therefore, the
overview in Table 1 implies that the atom is primarily depicted as
a static phenomenon in these classrooms. 4.1.1. Gestures
Since verbal and mental processes only occurred in speech, and With regard to their materiality, gestures are governed both by
because they were seldom directly connected to the atom as a the logics of space and time. Therefore a gesture can be used as a
concept (see below), a separate comparison was made between kind of visualization, and by using gestures involving hands and
relational and material processes (556 processes in total) in the dif- arms, it is possible to mimic a movement or a shape. The gestures
ferent modes (Table 2). Especially gestures and images appear to be used in relation to the atom as a scientific concept were all con-
used in certain patterns, with gestures depicting material processes sistent with the affordance of the mode in that respect. Examples
more often than expected from their statistical relative frequencies of gestures in the teacher expositions are shown in Figs. 2 and 3.
(EF in Tables 2–5), while the opposite holds for images. The same In the left photograph in Fig. 2, the teacher moves her arms in a
pattern persists when comparing only these two modes (Table 3). circular manner to depict the ways in which electrons swirl around
From the overview in Table 1, it is evident that speech and the nucleus. Another type of gesture is shown to the right in Fig. 2,
gestures are important for expressing material processes in the and in the two photographs in Fig. 3. Here the teachers hold their
classrooms (160 of the 174 material processes were given through hands still to depict relative sizes or positions of particles in the
these two modes). However, when comparing these two modes atom. Both these types of gestures could be regarded as iconic in
with respect to relational and material processes, gestures were that they to some extent are used to resemble what they represent:
used for material processes significantly more often than expected moving electrons and relative sizes or positions of particles (see
from their relative frequencies, while the opposite was the case for e.g. Martinec, 2004 or Kress, 2010 for discussions around Peirce’
speech (see Table 4). classical classification of gesture types into iconic, indexical and
A comparison was also made between image and speech symbolic).
(Table 5). Here, image is used significantly more often than Regarding the key issue concerning the atom as a static or
expected to express relational processes compared to speech, with dynamic phenomenon, the quantifications above revealed that
an opposite result regarding material processes. even though processes expressed through gestures were coded as
To connect to the key question about the extent to which differ- both material and relational, there was an over-representation of
ent modes appear to depict the atom as static or dynamic, gestures material processes. Thus gestures appear to be used for highlight-
and images stand out as being used in opposing manners. Gestures ing the dynamic aspects of the atom. One such typical gesture is
were often coded as material, thus giving a dynamic depiction, shown to the left in Fig. 2, where the teacher talked about elec-
while images were often coded as relational, thus giving a static trons swirling around the nucleus. All four teachers used similar
depiction. Processes in speech, finally, were more often coded as gestures for this aspect of the atom. Gestures that were coded as
relational than material. depicting relational processes were used to demonstrate relations
Before discussing the use of the various modes in relation to between particles in the atom, such as the gestures in Fig. 3 and in
affordances, the relatively high frequency of mental processes in the right photograph in Fig. 2. Fig. 3 shows how Sören first depicted
speech (see Table 1) needs to be commented on. Many of these the atomic nucleus with his hands pulled together as a sphere,
were of the following types: “nowadays we know that atoms can and shortly afterwards held his hands apart, depicting an empty
be divided into parts” or “before, the scientists thought that. . .” space between the nucleus and the electrons. The gesture shown
or “now you think about planet orbits and things like that”. In to the right in Fig. 2 shows how Fia made a gesture when talking
fact, most main participants (see Halliday & Matthiessen, 2004) about “Penny Proton” who was really big (compared to “Elvis the
in clauses with mental processes were either “scientists” or “you” Electron”). Here we can note that Sören and Fia used quite simi-
(the students in the classroom), and often these clauses were a lar gestures for different aspects of the atom: empty space around
starting point for saying something about the atom in a dependent nucleus, and size of particle in relation to other particles, respec-
clause, such as “before, the scientists thought that the atom was the tively. Thus, the meaning of a gesture in itself might not be possible
smallest existing particle”. However, some participants in clauses to interpret without further enhancement from other resources,
K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99 93

Fig. 2. Teacher gestures: electronic movements (left) and “big proton” (right), Fia’s classroom.

Fig. 3. Teacher gestures: nucleus (left) and “empty space” (right), Sören’s classroom.

such as a spoken exposition, hence being a part of a multimodal In all classrooms, the teachers made drawings of the atomic
ensemble (see Section 4.1.4). model in line with Bohr’s model (see Fig. 1, Section 1.1), with a
nucleus surrounded by electrons in clearly defined orbits. Typical
4.1.2. Images examples of such drawings are shown in Figs. 4–8 (these drawings
During these teaching episodes, only still images were used. are also commented on below concerning their multimodality).
Since the materiality of still images is governed by the logics of All images in these drawings were regarded as depicting rela-
space and simultaneity, they are especially apt for spatial aspects tional processes, and they focus on the particles that build up the
of the atom, for instance relative size and position of particles. Thus, atom. They also highlight spatial relations, such as where in the
according to the modal affordance of still images, we could expect atom the particles are found, as well as their relative sizes (though
them to be used mainly for relational processes (cf. ‘the atom con- in a very crude manner, which is sometimes pointed out by the
sists of. . .’). This was also the case, with an over-representation of teachers).
relational processes in the images in comparison to other modes. The instances when images in drawings were coded as depict-
However, here it is important to note that processes in images ing material processes were all connected to the drawing in Fig. 9,
were analyzed each time they were brought to the fore in the which was built up in two steps. It was made when Stig told the
classroom. Therefore the same image appears more than once students about a university lecturer who used drawings and ges-
in the analysis. Yet, since an image depicting the atom as either tures to explain that electrons in fact do not move in clearly defined
static or dynamic was brought to the fore a number of times, this orbits. The first stage (left in Fig. 9) depicts the electronic movement
meant that this way of viewing the atom was also recurrent in the in the inner orbit, and the second step (right in Fig. 9) depicts the
classroom. electronic movement in the second orbit.

Fig. 4. Teacher’s drawing of the atomic model, first stages (Fia).


94 K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99

Fig. 5. Final drawing of atomic model (Fia).

Fig. 6. Image of the atomic model built up in stages (Sören).

However, it is important to mention that the extent to which an that happened, such reasoning was never expressed through pro-
image is actually depicting the atom as static or dynamic could be cesses.
discussed. Images containing circles illustrating orbits imply that The affordance of speech might not affect whether the atom
something is moving along an imagined line. In that sense, images was depicted as a static or dynamic concept. As was shown in
such as the one in Fig. 4 could to some extent be regarded as depic- the overviews above, processes in speech depicted the atom as
tions of a movement, even though the dynamic aspect is toned both dynamic and static. However, speech was over-represented
down in comparison to images like the one in Fig. 9. in regard to relational processes when compared with gestures
(Table 4), while speech was over-represented in regard to material
4.1.3. Speech processes when compared with images (Table 5), which indicates
Due to its sequentiality, the materiality of speech is gov- that gesture and image were used in more specialized ways than
erned by the logic of time. In regard to affordance, it is often speech.
claimed that speech (and writing) is especially apt for describ-
ing temporality or for logic reasoning (see Section 1) rather than, 4.1.4. Multimodal ensembles
for instance, spatiality. However, even though information given Multimodal ensembles were in many ways the “state of the art”
through an image or a gesture can be taken in by the viewer in the teachers’ expositions, where typical combinations of modes
more or less as a whole, in many cases a similar content could were the following:
be expressed in words, even though quite a lengthy explanation
might be required. Regarding temporality and logic reasoning (such • Multimodal drawings (involving image, writing and/or mathe-
as cause and effect), where speech (and writing) is expected to matical or chemical symbols)
work especially well, there were very few instances of such rea- • Speech and gestures
soning in the teachers’ introductions to the atom. Also, when • Speech and images
K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99 95

Fig. 7. Final drawing of the atomic model (Sören).

Fig. 8. Image of lithium in two stages (Stig).

Fig. 9. Image of swirling electrons, drawing in two stages (Stig).

• Speech and multimodal drawings drawings imply relational processes (cf. “the proton is posi-
• Speech, gestures and images or multimodal drawings. tively charged”), further strengthening a static depiction of the
atom.
Most drawings could be regarded as multimodal ensembles, Altogether, almost all drawings (including image, writing and
combining images with writing and/or mathematical or chem- symbols) depict the atom as static. The only image clearly depicting
ical symbols (the only exception being the drawing in Fig. 9, the atom as a dynamic concept is the one shown in Fig. 9. We can
which consists of an image only). Figs. 5 and 7 illustrate how also note that this image is the only one dissimilar to the classic
writing typically is used in the board notes, with headings, and models in Fig. 1. The image, which in itself contains no writing or
then single words labelling particles. Fig. 5 shows the sole pro- symbols, functioned as part of a multimodal ensemble where Stig
cess expressed in writing: the mathematical symbol “equals”. used his whole body: while making the drawing he gave spoken
In that drawing, plus and minus signs in direct relation to the comments on what his lecturer had done, and imitated him, moving
name of the various particles are also used. Even though no pro- his arms round and round in circles, almost jumping while drawing
cess is explicitly expressed, the plus and minus signs in these (see also the excerpt in Table 7).
96 K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99

Table 6
Excerpt from multimodal exposition, Fia’s classroom. Processes in italics; process type indicated for processes related to the atom in teacher’s communication.

Speech Action (blackboard notes and gestures)

001 Teacher: in the nucleus there are (RP) two of those particles/. . ./do you
remember charges?
002 Student: the one in the middle is neutral
003 T: and what is the neutral particle called? (RP) starts drawing a small circle on the blackboard beside the model (I:
RP), equivalent to one of the particles in the nucleus (Fig. 5)
004 S: nucleus
005 T: no. but is it a neutron proton or electron? (RP)
006 S: neutron
007 T: writes on blackboard: “neutron (neutral)” (W: RP [implied])
008 T: it’s easy to remember since it starts neutron. neutral. points at the blackboard at the beginning of the words `neutron' and
`neutral'
009 T: it has no charge (RP) points at the blackboard at the small circle in the model, depicting a
neutron (I: RP)
010 T: Do you remember the copy we distributed?
011 S: “Elvis the electron” (in English)
012 T: it was (RP) Penny Proton who was (RP) so big and round and really gestures with full arms forming round sphere (G: RP) (right, Fig. 2)
positive charge
013 T:. and the proton was. it is positive (RP) draws a plus sign in one of the small circles depicting the proton in
the model; writes a plus sign and a circle around to the right of the
model, depicting proton (I: RP; W: RP [implied]) (Fig. 5)
014 T: And it’s easy to remember since it starts with a P like in positive writes on blackboard, beside the small circle: “proton (positive)”
(Fig. 5) (W: RP [implied])
015 T: And neutrons and protons are (RP) the same size. approximately the points at the blackboard at the circle depicting a nucleus (I: RP; W: RP
same size. they are (RP) in the nucleus [implied])
writes on blackboard: “nucleus”
016 T: And the little tiny particles. that were called (RP) points at the blackboard at the circle depicting an electron (I: RP)
017 S: electrons
018 T: yes. that was (RP) “Elvis the electron” (in English) writes a minus sign in the circle depicting an electron (W: RP
[implied])
019 T: yes which swirls (MP) around there all the time. and just rolls (MP) gestures with full arms making moving circles (G: MP) (left Fig. 2)
020 writes on blackboard: circle with minus sign and “electron” (Fig. 5)

I: image; G: gesture; W: writing; RP: relational process; MP: material process.

Table 7
Excerpt 1 from multimodal exposition, Stig’s classroom. Processes in italics; process type indicated for processes related to the atom in teacher’s communication.

Speech Action (whiteboard notes and gestures)

001 Teacher: then I write two writes “2” on the electronic orbit (see Fig. 8, left image)
002 T: there are (RP) two electrons there. I will show you later how they really move (MP)
003 Student: are they at the same place then?
004 T: no holding (MP) handsa .. no.. give me two minutes and I will explain how it really is (RP)
005 T: you know. they are (RP) there in approximately the same orbit Makes circular gesture (G: MP)

G: gesture; RP: relational process; MP: material process.


a
The teacher has a humorous style and uses uncommon metaphors. In this case, “no holding hands” is a way of expressing the fact that the electrons are never at the same
place.

The drawings, which were the main board notes made during above, Fia and Sören used similar gestures for different aspects of
the teachers’ expositions, were typically made in combination with the atom (the relative size of the proton, and empty space between
speech, and, at times, with gestures. While the drawings in the nucleus and electrons). Here the information needed for the inter-
teachers’ notes usually depicted the atom as static (e.g. Figs. 5–8), pretation of the gesture was given through speech (line 012).
both similar and dissimilar patterns were found in the accompa- In the multimodal ensembles during Fia’s exposition (combining
nying speech and gestures. Examples are given in the excerpts in speech, gestures and multimodal drawings), the blackboard draw-
Tables 6 and 7, with transcriptions of speech in the left column ing (image and writing) depicted the atom as a static concept, while
and comments on actions like drawing and gesturing in the right speech and gestures either enhanced this static depiction, or added
column. a dynamic component to it. Similar to Martinec’s (2004) study,
After a short introduction, in which all spoken processes related when speech was combined with gestures, both modes boosted
to the atom were relational, Fia started making the drawing shown each other to give the same notion of the atom in Fia’s exposition.
in Figs. 4 and 5. When the communication shown in Table 6 took This pattern was common in all classrooms. However, an excerpt
place, she had finished the first stages of drawing the model (see from another classroom (Table 7), reveals a somewhat different
Fig. 4) and she went on to comment on the charges of the different pattern.
particles. Just before the short dialogue shown in Table 7 took place, Stig
Similar to the introductory spoken text, most of the processes had started drawing an image of Lithium (Fig. 8, left).
directly linked to the atom as a concept were relational, mainly in In this excerpt, the gesture is the only resource offering the
the form of are or has. This was also the case when Fia talked about dynamic aspect of the atom. We can also note that the teacher com-
“Penny Proton” who was big and positive, making the static (rela- bines this material gesture with a relational process in speech: “[the
tional) gesture shown to the right in Fig. 2. The two spoken material electrons] are in approximately the same orbit”. Thus, instead of
processes, swirls and rolls (line 019), were combined with the enhancing each other, gesture and speech give opposing, or com-
dynamic (material) gesture shown to the left in Fig. 2. As was noted plementary, representations. After a discussion about where and
K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99 97

Table 8
Excerpt 2 from multimodal exposition, Stig’s classroom. Processes in italics; process type indicated for processes related to the atom in teacher’s communication.

Speech Action (whiteboard notes and gestures)

001 T: he was going to illustrate how they really move (MP)


002 T: I asked the same question as you.. how do they really move (MP)?
003 T: how. where are (RP) they? gesturing in circular manner with right arm (G: MP)
004 T: the first little orbit, I grasped that
005 T: because it goes (MP) like . . . this drawing/gesturing in circular manner (Fig. 9, left image) (I: MP) (G: MP)
006 T: but the electron. or electrons. could just as well be (RP) here.. or here drawing/gesturing in circular manner (Fig. 9, left image) (I: MP) (G: MP)
007 T: they can be (RP) out here drawing/gesturing in circular manner (Fig. 9, left image) (I: MP) (G: MP)
008 T: but usually the are (RP) here drawing/gesturing in circular manner (Fig. 9, left image) (I: MP) (G: MP)
009 T: so. this orbit. is where it usually is (RP) pointing/gesturing (Fig. 8, right image) (I: RP) (G: MP)

I: image; G: gesture; RP: relational process; MP: material process.

how to draw electrons in orbits, the drawing was finished (Fig. 8, an image is still there, and can be recalled again by pointing or
right). clearly looking at it, or by referring to it in speech). Without such
Later during the same teaching episode, Stig came back to how repeated analyses, images would have been left out from analyses
the electrons “really move”, now telling the anecdote from his own of multimodal ensembles when the teachers commented on them
university years, mentioned above. He described how his univer- again, through speech (and gesture), for instance in the examples
sity teacher had explained how electrons move, drawing the image given in Section 4.1.4.
shown in Fig. 9 accompanied by speech and action (Table 8). Bearing the abovementioned downsides in mind, to make par-
Here, most spoken processes associated with the atom were allel process analyses proved to be fruitful, enabling systematic
relational (except goes, line 005), while the processes given in the comparisons between modes, which was the basis for the primary
accompanying image or gestures were material. Once (line 009) a aim to reveal patterns as to the use of different modes to depict
dynamic gesture was combined with relational processes in draw- the atom as static or dynamic. These analyses revealed that differ-
ing and speech. In the excerpt presented in Table 8, the same types ent modes appeared to be used in specialized ways, which could
of processes were usually given through image and gestures. How- be related to modal affordance. This was particularly true for ges-
ever, as opposed to the pattern in Fia’s classroom, processes given tures and images. In most cases, these modes were used to depict
through gestures quite often depicted the atom differently than the atom in opposing manners. Gestures mainly highlighted the
those given through speech, again giving opposing, or complemen- dynamic dimension of the atom, with swirling electrons (process
tary, representations, allowing the two modes of communication type coded as material). Images, on the other hand, usually depicted
to convey separate meanings. the atom as static, highlighting the various particles of the atom,
and their relative positions (process type coded as relational). How-
5. Conclusions and discussion ever, even though gestures and image in particular appeared to be
used in specialized ways, all modes were to some extent used to
The primary aim, to analyze to what extent the atom was give both dynamic and static depictions of the atom.
depicted as dynamic or static through different modes, is depen- When analysing processes in gesture and speech in multimodal
dent on the secondary aim. Therefore, the secondary aim is ensembles, these often boosted each other, parallel to Martinec’s
discussed initially. (2004) finding that the “same” content was given in both modes.
The secondary aim concerned the potentials of parallel analyses However, this was not always the case in the present study. In some
of processes in different modes. A challenge here is the fact that the cases, such as in the excerpts from Stig’s classroom in Section 4.1.4,
SFL framework was developed in relation to writing (and speech). a relational process was expressed through speech while the ges-
Therefore, any attempt to use the framework for other modes, such ture was dynamic, and vice versa. This might be unproblematic for
as gesture or image, must involve some kind of “translation”. students who do not struggle with content or language. Yet, when
In the present study, gestures were only analyzed when they two resources used in parallel give opposing information, this could
had a clear connection to the atom as a phenomenon, and here be a potential obstacle.
the codings of the data as relational or material processes were An important result is that it was through the teachers’ use of
straightforward. Most gestures were used to depict electronic multiple modes that the students were given a relatively compre-
movement (coded as material), while some depicted aspects such hensive illustration of the atom, including the various particles
as relative size of particles, or “empty space” (coded as relational). and the dynamic aspect (though commonly, the descriptions of
These codings were parallel to Martinec’s (2004) analyses of ges- the atom were in line with Bohrs’ simplified model, see Fig. 1,
tures in static or action processes. Section 1.1). However, as mentioned in the introduction, results
The coding of processes in images was less clear-cut. As was from the main project revealed that the students in these class-
mentioned in Section 4.1.2, images with circles representing orbits rooms transformed the teachers’ board notes into their notebooks
could perhaps have been analyzed as depicting a movement. Even without adding information given orally or through gestures
so, such images were coded as relational, since a circle in itself does (Danielsson, 2011a). Since the content given through gestures
not imply a movement, unless a clear indication of movement, like or speech was seldom integrated into the students’ notes, the
an arrow, is added.3 Another analytical choice regarding images dynamic aspects of the atom were toned-down (see also Kress’
was to repeat analyses of images when they were brought to the discussion about ‘ontological weight’ as regards different modes,
fore again. Such repeated analyses could be seen as a parallel to 2010:86).
wordings being repeated during the expositions (a word “disap- Regarding the use of multiple resources used for meaning mak-
pears” when it has been uttered, and needs to be repeated, while ing, the results are in line with previous studies from science
classrooms. The teachers in the present study used a variety of
modes to explain different aspects of the atom: writing, speech,
3
Arrows were not added in the images of the atom during these introductions, images, gestures, and combinations of these, in multimodal ensem-
though such use was noted in one of classrooms further into the content area. bles. Even though the present study differs from Lemke’s (1998), in
98 K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99

which one student was focused on, the classroom practices were Artemeva, N., & Fox, J. (2011). The writing on the board: The global and the local in
similar, with information given through a variety of modes more teaching undergraduate mathematics through chalk talk. Written Communica-
tion, 28(4), 345–379.
or less simultaneously. There was indeed redundancy during the Axelsson, M., & Danielsson, K. (2012). Multimodality in the science classroom. In
teaching episodes analyzed in the present study, and thus students A. Pitkänen-Huhta, & L. Holm (Eds.), Literacy practices in transition: Perspectives
were thus given the “same” information through different channels from the Nordic countries (pp. 142–166). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Bryman, A. (2012). Social research methods. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(i.e. modes). However, due to their different potentials for meaning Danielsson, K. (2011a). Att bli del av en naturvetenskaplig diskurs. In I. Eriksson (Ed.),
making, different modes were used for slightly different aspects Kemiundervisning, text och textbruk i finlandssvenska och svenska skolor – En kom-
of the content. Consequently, an important conclusion is that these parativ tvärvetenskaplig studie (pp. 161–237). Stockholm: Stockholm University
Press.
students needed to combine the information given through the var-
Danielsson, K. (2011b). Då blir de fulla och glada. Multimodala representationer av
ious semiotic resources in order to pick up what particles the atom atommodellen i kemiklassrum. In J. Smidt, E. S. Tønnessen, & B. Aamotsbakken
consist of, what the they are called, their electric charge, and their (Eds.), Tekst og tegn. Lesing, skriving og multimodalitet i skole og samfunn (pp.
121–143). Trondheim, Norway: Tapir Akademisk Forlag.
relative positions in relation to each other, at the same time as they
Danielsson, K., Löfgren, R., & Pettersson, J. A. (forthcoming). Gains and losses.
need to recognize that the atom is dynamic. Metaphors in chemistry classrooms. In K.-S. Tang, & K. Danielsson (Eds.),
As both Lemke (1998) and Tang and colleagues (Tang et al., Global developments in literacy research for science education. Dordrecht, NL:
2011), argue, to “juggle” between modes and representations in Springer.
Danielsson, K., & Selander, S. (2014). (Multimodal texts in education) Se Texten! Mul-
science, and to make meaning from them is a demanding activ- timodala texter i ämnesdidaktiskt arbete. Malmö: Gleerups.
ity. Similar to their studies, no discussions were noted in these Danielsson, K., & Selander, S. (2016). Reading multimodal texts for learning. A model
classrooms as to the affordance of modes, or how modes need for cultivating multimodal literacy. Designs for Learning, 8
Eriksson, I. (Ed.). (2011). Kemiundervisning, text och textbruk i finlandssvenska och
to be combined to give a comprehensive representation of the svenska skolor – En komparativ tvärvetenskaplig studie (Chemistry teaching, text
content. When such discussions are connected to content, they and text practices in Finland-Swedish and Swedish schools – A comparative
have the potential to enhance both content knowledge and a multidisciplinary study). Stockholm: Stockholm University Press.
Gibson, J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw, & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiv-
more general awareness of the ways in which different modes ing, acting and knowing. Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 67–82). Hillsdale,
are often used in specialized ways. As Lemke argues in relation NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
to the need to teach students about multi-literacies and hybrid Halliday, M. A. K. (1978). Language as social semiotic. London: Edward Arnold Ltd.
Halliday, M. A. K., & Martin, J. R. (1993). Writing science. Literacy and discursive power.
genres, this is not a matter of adding new content to the cur-
London: University of Pittsburgh Press.
riculum but instead about “foregrounding and thematising what Halliday, M. A. K., & Matthiessen, C. M. I. M. (2004). An introduction to functional
is already in the curriculum, getting teachers and students to grammar (3rd edition). London: Arnold.
Holmberg, P., & Karlsson, A.-M. (2006). (Grammar with meaning) Grammatik med
pay attention to what was formerly taken for granted” (Lemke,
betydelse. Uppsala: Hallgren & Fallgren.
1998:269). Jakobson, B., Danielsson, K., Axelsson, M., & Uddling, J. (forthcoming). Measuring
To sum up, the parallel analyses of processes proved to be a fruit- time. Multi-lingual elementary school students’ meaning-making in physics. In
ful way of revealing how different modes were used for different K.-S. Tang, & K. Danielsson (Eds.), Global developments in literacy research for
science education. Dordrecht, NL: Springer.
aspects of the atom. These results could be related to modal affor- Jewitt, C. (Ed.). (2009). The Routledge handbook of multimodal analysis. London: Rout-
dance. Also, the analyses revealed that the teachers in this study ledge.
gave a relatively comprehensive picture of the atom, assuming that Jewitt, C. (2009). An introduction to multimodality. In C. Jewitt (Ed.), The Routledge
handbook of multimodal analysis (pp. 14–28). London: Routledge.
they consider Bohr’s simplified model of the atom adequate enough Kress, G. (2009). What is a mode? In C. Jewitt (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of
for students of this age. However, since there were no classroom multimodal analysis (pp. 54–67). London: Routledge.
discussions about the affordance of modes, or what aspects of the Kress, G. (2010). Multimodality. A social semiotic approach to contemporary commu-
nication. London: Routledge.
atom were highlighted through different representations, it was Kress, G., Jewitt, C., Bourne, J., Franks, A., Hardcastle, J., Jones, K., & Reid, E. (2005).
left to the students to make meaning from the combined resources. English in urban classrooms: A multimodal perspective on teaching and learning.
Also, since board notes appear to be highly valued in these class- New York, NY: RoutledgeFalmer.
Kress, G., Jewitt, C., Ogborn, J., & Tsatsarelis, C. (2001). Multimodal teaching and
rooms, students might miss important aspects of the content given
learning. The rhetorics of the science classroom. London: Continuum.
through resources like speech and gestures. As regards implications Kress, G., & van Leeuwen, T. (2006). Reading images. The grammar of visual design
for education, the results imply that teachers need to become aware (2nd edition). London: Routledge.
Kress, G., & Selander, S. (2012). Multimodal design, learning and cultures of recog-
of the ways they themselves, as well as other learning resources
nition. The Internet and Higher Education, 15(4), 265–268.
used, employ different modes or combination of modes in teach- Lemke, J. L. (1990). Talking science. Language, learning and values. Norwood: Ablex
ing. Also, they need to make the students aware of this. If focusing Publishing.
both on content and on the ways in which the content is expressed Lemke, J. L. (1998). Multimedia literacy demands of the scientific curriculum. Lin-
guistics and Education, 10(3), 247–271.
through different semiotic resources, students will get the oppor- Martin, J. R., & Rose, D. (2008). Genre relations. Mapping culture. London:
tunity to enhance both their content knowledge and a more general Equinox.
awareness of how modes are used in specialized ways. Martinec, R. (2004). Gestures that co-occur with speech as a systematic resource. The
realization of experiential meanings in indexes. Social Semiotics, 14(2), 193–213.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1035033042000238259
Norris, S., & Phillips, L. M. (2003). How literacy in its fundamental sense is central to
Acknowledgements scientific literacy. Science Education, 87(2), 224–240.
O’Halloran, K., Podlasov, A., Chua, A., & K.L.E., M. (2012). Interactive software for
The author wishes to express her appreciation to the partici- multimodal analysis. Visual Communication, 11(3), 363–381.
Schleppegrell, M. (2004). The language of schooling: A functional linguistics perspective.
pants in the interdisciplinary project Chemistry texts as tools for Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
scientific learning, funded by the Swedish Research Council, from Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, C. (2008). Teaching disciplinary literacy to adoles-
which the data was collected. The author also wishes to thank the cents. Rethinking content-area literacy. Harvard Educational Review, 78(1),
40–59.
anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on earlier versions Simpson, A., & Walsh, M. (2010). Multiple literacies: Implications for a changed
of the article. pedagogy. In F. Christie, & A. Simpson (Eds.), Literacy and social responsibility.
Multiple perspectives (pp. 24–39). London: Equinox.
Taber, K. S., & Watts, M. (1996). The secret life of the chemical bond. Students’ anthro-
pomorphic and animistic reference to bonding. International Journal of Science
References
Education, 18(5), 557–568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0950069960180505
Andersen, T., Petersen, U. H., & Smedegaard, F. (2001). (The language as a resource. Tang, K.-S., Delgado, C., & Moje, E. B. (2014). An integrative framework for the
Danish systemic functional linguistics in theory and practice) Sproget som resource. analysis of multiple and multimodal representations for meaning-making in
Dansk systemisk funktionel lingvistik i teori og praksis. Odense: Odense Univer- science education. Science Education, 98(2), 305–326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
sitetsforlag. sce.21099
K. Danielsson / Linguistics and Education 35 (2016) 88–99 99

Tang, K.-S., Tan, S. C., & Yeo, J. (2011). Students’ multimodal constructs of the work- Wellington, J., & Osborne, J. (2001). Language and literacy in science education. Buck-
energy concept. International Journal of Science Education, 33(13), 1775–1804. ingham: Open University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2010.508899
Unsworth, L. (2001). Teaching multiliteracies across the curriculum: Changing con- Kristina Danielsson, PhD, has a position as professor in reading and writing devel-
texts of text and image in classroom practice. Buckingham: Open University opment at Stockholm University and in Swedish at the Linnaeus University. Her
Press. main research area is disciplinary literacy with a special focus on multimodality
van Leeuwen, T. (2005). Introducing social semiotics. New York: Routledge. and learning in the science disciplines.

You might also like