You are on page 1of 6

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (1996), 2, 565-570.

Copyright © 1996 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA.

CASE STUDY

Recovery of memory and executive function following


anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture

MARK D'ESPOSITO, 1 MICHAEL P. ALEXANDER,2 RICHARD FISCHER,3


REGINA McGLINCHEY-BERROTH,4'5 AND MARGARET O'CONNOR 5
'Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania Medical Center, Philadelphia, PA 19104
department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine and Stroke Rehabilitation Program,
Braintree Hospital, Braintree, MA 02185
3
Department of Neuropsychology, New England Sinai Hospital, Stoughton, MA and Department of Neurology,
Tufts University School of Medicine, Boston, MA 02702
4
GRECC, West Roxbury VA Medical Center, West Roxbury, MA and Department of Psychiatry,
Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02132
5
Memory Disorders Research Center, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA 02215
(RECEIVED July 5, 1995; REVISED December 19, 1995; ACCEPTED January 4, 1996)

Abstract
We studied the recovery of memory and executive function in 10 patients following anterior communicating artery
aneurysm (ACoA) rupture and repair. Patients were tested at 2 consecutive points in time following surgery
(approximately at 2 and 3 months). At the first testing, the patients divided into 2 groups based on the severity of
impairment on executive measures. Both groups had severe anterograde amnesia, but only patients with severe
executive impairments had retrograde amnesia with a temporal gradient. At second testing, both groups had
persistent severe anterograde amnesia. The dysexecutive group showed significant improvement in executive
deficits and in retrograde amnesia, with attenuation of the temporal gradient. Patients with more severe executive
impairments had more extensive bilateral frontal lesions than other patients. These results suggest that the cognitive
profile following ACoA rupture changes with time. Time postonset following aneurysm rupture and lesion site are
both critical for defining the neuropsychological profile, and determining the underlying cognitive mechanisms in
this neurological disorder. (JINS, 1996, 2, 565-570.)
Keywords: Anterior communicating artery aneurysm, Amnesia, Executive function, Recovery

INTRODUCTION retrograde amnesia in ACoA patients as a group that was


similar, to Korsakoff's syndrome. They observed a temporal
The profile of cognitive impairment following ACoA rup- gradient of remote recall with information from earlier de-
ture and repair consists of a variable degree of amnesia, ex- cades being retrieved better than information from more re-
ecutive function impairment, and confabulation (Alexander cent decades. The executive function impairments commonly
& Freedman, 1984). Several investigators have explored spe- present following ACoA rupture have only been character-
cific aspects of this cognitive impairment in detail in small ized in a few patients (Shoqeirat et alM 1990; Parkin et al.,
groups of patients. For example, Irle et al. (1992) described 1994), and a clear understanding has not emerged. Many,
a spectrum of anterograde amnesia in ACoA patients rang- but not all, patients with ACoA rupture confabulate, but in
ing from a severe deficit comparable to that seen in global those who do, confabulation ranges from spontaneous and
amnesics (i.e., Korsakoff's syndrome) to no memory deficit fantastic to confabulations that are only obtained when pro-
at all. Likewise, Gade et al. (1990) described a pattern of voked (Fischer et al., 1995).
Most investigations of the "ACoA syndrome" have stud-
ied patients who were selected because they exhibited cog-
Reprint requests to: Mark D'Esposito, Cognitive Neurology Section,
Department of Neurology, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, 3400 nitive deficits. Studies of unselected ACoA rupture patients
Spruce Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104-4283. have, however, found persistent cognitive deficits present
565
566 M. D'Esposito et al.

in a large percentage of patients (Laiaconaet al., 1989; Sten- 3. Anterograde amnesia: supraspan list learning test [first
houseetal., 1991). All studies have found a variable neuro- testing, California Verbal Learning Test (Delis et al.,
psychological profile without a clear relationship between 1987); second testing, Rcy Auditory Verbal Learning Test
the cognitive profile and the site of brain damage. Thus, the (Lczak, 1983)]
effects of vasospasm or a "toxic" etiology have been put 4. Retrograde amnesia: Famous Faces Test (Albert ct al.,
forward as explanations for the cognitive impairments, but 1979).
these potential etiologies are unlikely. For example, 3 of 6
patients in one study with patients who exhibited global cog- All scores obtained in patients were compared to pub-
nitive impairments had no or mild vasospasm (Stenhouse lished normative values (Sprecn & Strauss, 1991) except
et al., 1991). Moreover, the exact nature of a possible "toxin" for the Famous Faces Test in which normative values were
has not been elucidated (Laiacona et al., 1989). obtained from 10 age- (t = .28, df = 18) and education-
Several factors contribute to the difficulty in uncovering matched (/ = .21, df = 18) control subjects. Each patient
clinicoanatomic relations in ACoA patients. First, most stud- was tested at two consecutive points in time after onset. Ini-
ies have not grouped patients according to lesion site, in- tial testing was performed at an average of 59 days (range
cluding patients with a mixture of lesions, or even no lesions 32-85) postonset and later testing was performed at an av-
at all (Stenhouse et al., 1991). Second, time postonset of erage of 89 days (range 60-110), resulting in a mean of 30
aneurysm rupture is not controlled; patients have been tested days between early and later testing.
at widely varying points during their recovery (Laiacona All patients had a postoperative CT brain scan, on aver-
et al., 1989; Stenhouse et al., 1991; Irle et al., 1992). Fi- age 52 days (range 19-76 days) after surgery. Analysis was
nally, lesion site may not have an unvarying relationship done by two raters (MD and MPA), who were blind to each
with deficit. Different lesion sites may produce different def- other's rating and to the neuropsychological results. Le-
icits that also have differing potentials for recovery. We have sions were scored along three dimensions: (1) latcralky; (2)
demonstrated such an interaction for lesion site, confabula- regional involvement of basal forebrain, striatal, medial fron-
tion, and time postonset in ACoA patient (Fischer et al., tal, orbitofrontal or dorsolateral frontal structures by the cri-
1995). In this study, it was our hypothesis that there are sim- teria of Irle et al. (1992); and (3) relative extent of frontal
ilar interactions for executive function impairment, lesion lesions (0%, <25%, 25-75%, or >75%). There was 98%
site and time postonset; and for retrograde amnesia, execu- agreement between raters, and when there was not com-
tive function impairments and time postonset in these pa- plete agreement, a consensus between raters was obtained,
tients. Only longitudinal assessment of ACoA patients will
characterize the cognitive profile of this neurological dis-
order and the relationship of cognitive deficits to lesion sites.
RESULTS
To our knowledge, there have been no reports that evaluate Analysis of patients1 performance on tasks of executive func-
the effect of recovery of specific cognitive functions in this tion during early testing revealed two distinct groups. Pa-
patient group. tients in Group 1 (n ~ 5) were severely impaired ( > 2 SDs
from published normative means) on at least three of four
executive measures (dysexecutive group). In contrast, pa-
METHODS tients in Group 2 (n = 5), performed within normal limits
on at least three of four measures (see Table 1). There were
Research Participants no significant differences on the remaining measures be-
Ten consecutive patients admitted to Braintree Rehabilita- tween these patient groups in age (t = 1.49, df = 8), edu-
tion Hospital who had had rupture and repair of an anterior cation (/ = .88, df = 8), general intellectual functioning
communicating aneurysm were studied. There were 4 men [VIQ: (f = .66); PIQ: (t = 1.93, df = 8)], or the number of
and 6 women with a mean age of 47 years (range 27-66) days between initial and later testing (/ = .77, df = 8).
and 14 years of education (range 9-20). All patients were
right-handed. First Evaluation (Table 1)
Both groups demonstrated severe deficits in new learning
Evaluation as evidenced by their supraspan list learning. There was no
Each patient underwent a comprehensive neuropsycholog- difference between groups in the percentage of items re-
ical evaluation: called during Trial 5 (t = .83, df = 8), during delayed free
recall after distraction (/ = 1.73, df — 8), or the percentage
1. Genera! intellectual functioning: Wechsler Adult Intel- lost from Trial 5 to the delay (t = .23, df = 8). Only Group
ligence Scale-Revised (Wcchsler, 1981) 1 showed evidence of retrograde amnesia during recall on
2. Executive functions: backward digit span of the Wech- the Famous Faces Test as compared to controls (r = 2.11,
sler Memory Scale-Revised (Wcchsler, 1987), FAS test df - \3,p< .002). Regression analyses of the percentage
(Bcnton, 1968), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton, of faces correctly recalled across decades from the 1950s
1981), and the Trail Making Test part B (Rcitan, 1958) through the 1980s was conducted. A significant negative
ACoA syndrome 567

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and ncuropsychological performance between two groups of ACoA patients

Patient group
Group 1 (dysexecutive) (3roup 2 (normal)
Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2
Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD

Age 52.4 7.7 — 42.4 12.9 -


Education 12.6 2.0 - 14.4 4.2 -
WAIS-R VIQ 96.6 10.1 - 102.8 17.8 -
WAIS-R PIQ 89.6 5.9 - 110.5 23.7 -
Digits backward 2.6 1.7 4.0 1.6 5.0 0.8 5.5 0.6
WCST (categories) 1.2 2.2 3.5 2.8 4.8 1.5 5.3 1.5
Verbal fluency (words) 13.8 3.3 26.2 10.8 28 10.2 33.8 12.8
Trails B (s) 324.8 105.7 153.6 55.1 83.4 11.4 83.3 37.0
Supraspan Trial 5 44.0% 12.1 51.1% 30.0 45.3% 17.3 64.9% 21.5
Supraspan delayed recall 0.0% 0.0 10.77o 12.1 17.8% 23.0 20.0% 15.6
Supraspan Trial 5—delay 44.0% 12.1 40.4% 6.7 27.5% 11.9 44.9% 24.6
Famous faces total recall 44.8% 21.0 61.4% 21.5 74.1% 29.9 82.4% 13.2

linear relationship was revealed for Group 1 (r = - . 7 1 , erage of 84.1%, whereas the improvement in Group 2 was
p < .01) but not for Group 2. The negative correlation in only 10.2%. Performance in Group 1 remained impaired on
Group 1 is consistent with the existence of a temporal gra- executive measures compared to Group 2, but this reached
dient (Figure 1). significance for only the Trail Making test (/ = 2.37, df -
8, p < .05). The degree of anterograde amnesia remained
severe in both groups. There was no significant improve-
Second Evaluation (Table 1) ment noted between testing during Trial 5 learning (Group
1: / = .31, df = 4; Group 2: / = 1.90, df = 4); delayed free
In Group 1 (dysexecutive group), performance improved on
recall (Group I: / = 2.38, df = 4; Group 2: / = .58, df= 4);
all executive measures from the first to the second evalua-
or the number of items lost from Trial 5 to the delay (Group
tion. On two of these measures, the extent of improvement
I: t = 1.33, df = 4); Group 2: / = 1.37, df = 4). In Group 1,
was statistically significant (FAS: / = 2.68, df = 4,/? < .05;
improvement was noted in the severity of retrograde amne-
Trails B: / = 2.86, df= A,p < .05). No significant change
sia (t = 3.21, df = 4, p = .06) but performance was still
was seen on these measures in Group 2. The amount of im-
impaired compared to controls (/ = 2.11, df = 13, p < .05).
provement across executive measures between the first and
Regression analyses revealed that the pattern of retrograde
second evaluation was calculated. Group I improved an av-
loss in this group no longer showed a significant linear re-
lationship (r = .31). This finding is consistent with an at-
tenuation of the temporal gradient in the dysexecutive group
100-, (Figure 1).
Lesion analysis revealed that Group I patients had bilat-
eral medial frontal lesions including the anterior cingulate
** so
o and corpus callosum, as well as striatal (caudate) and basal
o forebrain damage (Figure 2). Patients in Group 2 had le-
o 60- sions limited to basal forebrain (Figure 3) except for two
01
O> patients who also had small orbital frontal lesions. The le-
re sion profile for each patient is presented in Table 2.
c 40-
0)
u
l_
0)
Q. 20-
DISCUSSION
Following rupture and repair of anterior communicating an-
1950 1960 1970 1980 eurysms, our patients showed varying degrees of cognitive
Decade dysfunction in the domains of memory and executive func-
Fig. 1. Pattern of recovery of retrograde amnesia in Group (dys- tions. All patients exhibited significant anterograde amne-
executive group) on the Famous Faces Test. sia that persisted over time. In the early stage following
568 M. D'Esposito et til.

Fig. 2. MRI scan of a patient in Group I demonstrating lesions in the Fig. 3. MRI scan of a patient in Group 2 demonstrating a lesion in
medial frontal lobes, basal forebrain (' septum) and striatum {caudate). the basal forebrain (septum).

surgery (1-2 months), two distinct groups could be identi- sions; patients who performed relatively normally on exec-
fied, based on the magnitude of executive function im- utive measures did not have significant frontal lesions. Only
pairment. Further analysis indicated that this executive patients with executive function impairments had signifi-
impairment correlated with extent of frontal lobe damage. cant difficulty recalling remote information, and a signifi-
Dyscxecutivc patients had extensive bilateral frontal le- cant temporal gradient was observed in their pattern of recall
(poorer recall in more recent decades). Dysexecutive pa-
tients (Group 1) showed significant improvement in exec-
utive function within the next month. Some improvement
might have been due to practice effect (two testings, 1 month
Table 2. Lesion sites for all patients apart). Group 2, however, had only modest improvement,
suggesting practice effects are small. Furthermore, the dys-
Frontal Stnatal Basal
executive patients were and remained densely amnesic, so
Group Patient (L/R) (L/R) forebrain
thai no explicit benefit of practice could be exploited. These
1 (dysexecutive) 1 f + f / 1 + t,. + /() patients also showed improvement in recalling information
-) t f + / f ()/ + from more recent decades, thereby flattening the slope of
3 + t / + * 0/ • their retrograde deficit. This change could not be due to
4 • / t +•/() practice.
5 0 WO
Studying the recovery of cognitive function after an acute
2 (normal) 6 • / • 0 s
brain insult provides a unique opportunity to gam insight
7 t /() 0
8
regarding interactions among different cognitive processes,
0 «) i

0 0
F'or example, the temporally graded retrograde amnesia that
10 0 0 0
was observed only in the dysexecutive group supports the
hypothesis that this pattern of retrograde amnesia may he
related to the combination of executive and memory im-
l"i»i" t r o n t a l l e s i o n s . 0 no lesion. • • 25*4, • • IS IS'",
• * <• • 7V£ . F;or striatal and basal I'orebrain lesions, 0 absent. * pairments. 'This finding is consistent with the common
present observation of a temporally graded retrograde amnesia in
ACoA syndrome 569

Korsakoff's syndrome (Albert et al., 1979), another disor- can remain quite impaired. Although the number of patients
der in which patients have both executive and memory im- in our study is small, there appears to be a relationship be-
pairments (Butters & Stuss, 1989). Improvement in our tween lesion site, time of testing after injury, and the pattern
patients' retrograde amnesia was concurrent with improve- of cognitive deficits. It will be crucial for future neuropsy-
ment in executive function, despite a persistent anterogradc chological studies of ACoA patients to account for both re-
memory deficit. Impairment in strategies for retrieving re- covery and lesion site when investigating the cognitive and
mote information may contribute to their impaired perfor- behavioral profiles of the ACoA syndrome.
mance. Gade (Gade & Mortensen, 1990) also found a
temporal gradient pattern in remote recall in a large group
of ACoA patients. Their patients had significant antero-
grade memory impairments, but they were not tested on ex- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
ecutive measures assessing frontal lobe function. Since lesion
site was not reported either, it also cannot be determined Supported in part by NIH grants NS 26985 (Memory Disorders
whether their group of patients showed a similar relation- Research Center, Boston University School of Medicine) and NS
ship between retrograde amnesia, executive function im- 01762 (M.D.). This study was presented in part at the Inter-
pairment, and lesion site. Most of their patients were tested national Neuropsychological Society meeting in Cincinnati, OH,
February, 1994. The authors wish to thank Drs. Marie Walbridge
approximately 2 years after injury, suggesting that retro-
and Randall Otto for performing the neuropsychological testing
grade amnesia can persist. The mechanisms underlying per- on many of these patients.
sistent retrograde amnesia in ACoA patients remain unclear,
although persistent impairment in executive function would
seem the likely cause.
All of our patients had severe anterograde amnesia that REFERENCES
persisted up to 4 months following aneurysm rupture. In a
study by Irle (1992), anterogradc amnesia was investigated Albert, M., Butters, N., & Levin, J. (1979). Temporal gradients in
in a large group of ACoA patients who were selected be- the retrograde amnesia of patients with alcoholic Korsakoff's
cause of lesions identified in the ventromedial frontal lobe disease. Archives of Neurology, 36, 211-216.
or striatum. They found a clear relationship between lesion Alexander, M. & Freedman, M. (1984). Amnesia after anterior com-
site and severity of amnesia: Patients with combined le- municating artery aneurysm. Neurology, 34, 752-757.
sions in the basal forebrain and striatum, or basal forebrain, Benton, A. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe
disease. Neuropsychologia, 28, 171-179.
striatum and frontal lobes, had a severe memory deficit,
Butters, N. & Stuss, D. (1989). Dienccphalic amnesia. In F. Boiler
whereas patients with lesions in the basal forebrain or stri-
& J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 3,
atum alone showed essentially no deficit. Our findings are pp. 107-148). New York: Elsevier Science Publishers.
not consistent with these data since several of our patients Delis, D., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E.. & Ober, B. (1987). California
had lesions restricted to the basal forebrain, yet had severe Verbal Learning Test manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
anterograde amnesia. Since the patients in Irle et al.'s study Corporation.
were evaluated at times much later postonset (1-5 years af- Fischer, R., Alexander, M., D'Esposito, M., & Otto, R. (1995).
ter surgery) than our patients, it seems possible that antero- Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of confab-
grade amnesia continues to recover, especially in patients ulation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol-
with less extensive lesions. ogy, 77,20-28.
Executive function has been investigated in detail in only Gade, A. & Mortensen, E. {1990). Temporal gradient in the re-
mote memory impairment of amnesic patients with lesions in
a few studies of ACoA patients. When assessed, patients have
the basal forebrain. Neuropsychologia, 28, 985-1001.
shown impairments on tests thought to be sensitive to fron-
Heaton, R. (1981). Manual for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
tal lobe function. In one study, a single patient was studied Odessa. FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
3 years after aneurysm rupture (Parkin et al., 1994) and in Irle, E., Wowra. B., Kuncrt. H., Hampl. J., & Kunze, S. (1992).
another, the time of testing was not reported (Shoqeirat Memory disturbance following anterior communicating artery
ct al., 1990). Neither study reported details about the lesion rupture. Annals of Neurology, 31, 473-480.
site in their patients. Similar to the studies of memory func- Laiacona, M., Dc Santis, A., Barbarotto, R., Basso, A., Spagnoli,
tioning in ACoA patients, it is apparent that impairments in D., & Capitani, E. (1989). Ncuropsychological follow-up of
executive function can also persist. The factors that lead to patients operated for aneurysms of anterior communicating ar-
poor recovery of executive function in ACoA patients have tery. Cortex, 25, 261-273.
not been elucidated, but large frontal lesions are the likely Lczak, M. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New
cause in most patients. York: Oxford University Press.
Parkin. A., Ycomans. J., & Bindschaedler. C. (1994). Further char-
Our study demonstrates that the cognitive profile follow- acterization of the executive memory impairment following fron-
ing ACoA rupture changes with time. During the first 3 tal lobe lesions. Brain and Cognition, 26, 23-42.
months following aneurysm rupture, there can be signifi- Rcitan, R. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indica-
cant recovery in specific cognitive functions such as exec- tion of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, S,
utive function, whereas others, such as anterogradc memory, 271-276.
570 M. D'Esposito et ah

Shoqeirat, M.f Mayes, A., MacDonald, C , Meudell, P., & Picker- Stenhouse, L., Knight, R., Longmore, B., & Bishara, S. (1991).
ing, A. (1990). Performance on tests sensitive to frontal lobe Long-term cognitive deficits in patients after surgery on aneu-
lesions by patients with organic amnesia: Leng & Parkin revis- rysms of the anterior communicating artery. Journal of Neu-
ited. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29, 401-408. rology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 54, 909-914.
Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1991). A compendium of neuropsycho- Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised
logical tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. New manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
York: Oxford University Press. Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised manual. New
York: Psychological Corporation.

You might also like