You are on page 1of 9

Sex Differences in Social Behavior

Are the Social Role and Evolutionary Explanations Compatible?


John Archer
University of Central Lancashire

The competing claims of two explanations of sex differ- sures on men and women during the course of human
ences in social behavior, social role theory, and evolution- and prehuman evolution.
ary psychology are examined. The origin and scope of
research on sex differences in social behavior are outlined, Historical Background of Sex
and the application of social role theory is discussed. Re- Differences in Social Behavior
search that is based on modern sexual selection theory is The investigation of sex differences in psychological at-
described, and whether its findings can be explained by tributes and behavior arose from the tradition of individ-
social role theory is considered. Findings associated with
ual differences research. It originally concentrated on
social role theory are weighed against evolutionary ex- cognitive and perceptual attributes for which there was a
planations. It is concluded that evolutionary theory ac- considerable amount of descriptive data. In earlier reviews
counts much betterfor the overall pattern of sex differences (e.g., Anastasi, 1958; Garai & Scheinfeld, 1968; Maccoby,
and for their origins. A coevolutionary approach is pro- 1967), the relative skills and abilities of the two sexes
posed to explain cross-cultural consistency in socialization formed the main agenda. Nevertheless, even here there
patterns. was some consideration of the social realm, in the form
of interests and attitudes and emotion and aggressiveness.
The extensive summary by Maccoby and Jacklin (1974)
likewise considered both cognitive and social sex
The purpose of this article is to examine whether s e x differences.
differences in social attributes are better explained Essentially, this research tradition was atheoretical
by the dominant social science view, social role the- in origin. Later, explanations were provided for these sex
ory, or by a Darwinian perspective and to discuss whether differences, but they were largely an exercise in the na-
they are alternative or complementary explanations. I be- ture-nurture debate, with traditional socialization views
gin by outlining the historical background of research on involving reinforcement and imitation (Kagan & Moss,
sex differences in social behavior, which led to the devel- 1962; Mischel, 1967) vying with physiological explana-
opment of social role theory. The scope of the sex differ- tions involving brain development or hormonal differ-
ences covered by this largely laboratory based research ences (e.g., Gray & Buffery, 1971 ; Hutt, 1972). They were
can be widened by examining some other areas in which mainly applied to cognitive abilities, and the physiological
findings are consistent with social role theory. I then con- explanations were often regarded as implying that the
sider the Darwinian principle of sexual selection, which two sexes possessed natural abilities fitting them for tra-
has informed research on differences between men and ditional roles (Archer, 1976), whereas the content of so-
women in sexuality, mate choice, and aggression. The cialization was viewed as arising from sociohistorical
issues of whether these findings can be explained by social forces.
role theory and research associated with social role theory,
or by evolutionary theory, are then considered. The Application of Meta-Analysis
I conclude that the Darwinian perspective provides
Since this time, several important developments have oc-
a more plausible account of the origins of sex differences
curred in the methodology and theory that have been
in social behavior, in that the specific content of sex dif- brought to bear on sex differences in psychological attri-
ferences fits the different fitness requirements, or repro- butes and behavior. One was the introduction of meta-
ductive strategies, of males and females, and the inter-
analytic techniques to the review of research evidence
action between them. However, this does not mean that (see Eagly, 1995). There have been several consequences
cultural influences play no part in the generation of these
differences. Socialization, one of the major processes
identified by social role theory, can be viewed as reflecting 1 thank David Buss, Anne Campbell, Alice Eagly, and Barry
McCarthy for helpful discussion and comments on this article.
the adaptive requirements of men and women. Gendered
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John
socialization practices can be understood in terms of co- Archer, Department of Psychology, University of Central Lancashire,
evolutionary processes, originating not from historically Preston PRI 2HE, Lancashire, England. Electronic mail may be sent
developed societal roles but from different selection pres- via Internet to j.archer@uclan.ac.uk.

September 1996 • American Psychologist 909


Copyright 1996 by the American PsychologicalAssociation, Inc. 00034)66X/96/$2.00
Vol. 51, No. 9,909-917
ology or of social learning. This was, however, an over-
simplification even for cognitive attributes. For example,
if the same task is presented to boys and girls as either a
measure of needlework or of electronics, the effect of the
labeling is to reverse the direction of sex difference in
performance (Davies, 1986; Hargreaves, Bates, & Foot,
1985). Thus, social context is important even for sup-
posedly internalized skills. For social behavior, it is es-
sential to consider the context in which it is manifest,
and this led to changes of emphasis in the way that sex
differences were conceptualized. Instead of being viewed
as residing in the individual, what Deaux (1984) called
"sex as a subject variable" (p. 105), they became viewed
as the product of an interaction between people in a wider
social setting. Consequently, explanations of such sex dif-
ferences became more concerned with social processes.
John Archer For example in their model, Deaux and Major (1987)
Copyright by University identified first the gender-related beliefs that people
of Central Lancashire. brought to an interaction, and second the extent to which
a specific situation activated these beliefs: Sex differences
arose from the extent to which these beliefs influenced
of the increased precision this entailed. The decision about the specific interactions of the people concerned. The
which sex differences are "real" was much less a matter emphasis had therefore shifted to processes such as gen-
of impression and personal judgment, as it was when der-stereotypic beliefs and the salience of these beliefs for
Maccoby and Jacklin's (1974) review was published. In- those people in a given situation. Sex differences were
creased quantification also brought the realization that consequently viewed as flexible, context-dependent out-
many of the sex differences that had been extensively dis- comes of social processes.
cussed and argued over were relatively small in terms of
overall effect size: For example, that for spatial perfor- Societal Roles
mance showed a point biserial correlation of.22 and that Deaux and Major (1987) concentrated on sex differences
for verbal skills showed. 10 (Hyde, 1981). One practical in terms of dyadic interactions. In her social role theory,
implication of such quantification was to question the Eagly (1987) was concerned with a different level of anal-
logic of regarding these sex differences as strong influences ysis, that of societal roles. This is both a theory of origins
on the numbers of women entering traditionally mas- and of process. Sex differences in social behavior are
culine occupational roles such as science and engineering viewed as having arisen historically from the societal po-
(Hyde, 1981; but see Feingold, 1995, for an analysis that sition of women and m e n - - t h e i r division of labor into
questions this conclusion). homemakers and full-time paid employees. The different
A second development, beginning with Hall (1978) roles evoke expectancies about the characteristics asso-
but later associated with the work of Eagly and her col- ciated with these roles. These have been summarized as
leagues, was the extension of the meta-analytic approach communal (i.e., features such as nurturance and yielding)
to social behavior. Eagly and Carli (1981) examined in- in the case of the homemaker role, and agentic (i.e., fea-
fluenceability, concluding that women were more per- tures such as assertive and instrumental) in the case of
suadable than men, but with the proviso that the effect the paid employee role. Such features are also viewed as
size was small and that there may have been (male) bias forming the basis of gender stereotypes (e.g., Williams &
in the selection of studies that were published. Hall (1984) Best, 1982).
analyzed 125 studies of nonverbal communication and According to social role theory, expectancies are
found comparatively large differences between men and transformed into sex differences in social behavior
women. Smaller differences were found for helping be- through two processes: The first is the learning of sex-
havior (Eagly, 1987) and for aggression (Eagly & Steffen, typed skills and beliefs (i.e., through socialization pro-
1986), although in the second case the data came exclu- cesses) and the second is a more direct influence of ex-
sively from experimental social psychological studies, pectancies associated with gender roles on people's be-
which had a number of limitations (Archer, 1989). The havior and dispositions. The second process was empha-
issue of the magnitude of such differences and their im-
sized in the earlier research on influenceability (Eagly,
plications is the continued subject of debate (Eagly, 1995;
1983), thus making the social role view similar to that of
Hyde & Plant, 1995). Henley (1977), who explained sex differences in nonverbal
The Importance of Social Context behavior as directly stemming from societal power rela-
tions. Nevertheless, in her subsequent book, Eagly (1987)
The older reviews of cognitive differences tended to view characterized this as an oppression theory, and distanced
them as individual attributes--either the product of bi- the social role view from it, leaning more toward an over-

910 September 1996 • American Psychologist


lap with socialization theory. Essentially, the distinction case, the findings can be explained in terms of the agency-
between the two processes depends on whether internal communion distinction identified by social role theory.
dispositions or situational contingencies control behavior Feingoid found that men were more assertive and women
(House, 1981). Internal dispositions will have been built more tenderminded, trusting, and anxious. It is again
up over a period of time through learning and could only worth noting that some of these, such as assertiveness,
be changed gradually, whereas for situational contingen- anxiety, and well-being, might be the direct consequence
cies, a change in behavior would follow fairly closely any of status inequalities, and others, such as well-being,
change in societal roles. emotion, and anxiety, might result from socialization
Although social role theory incorporates socializa- specifically concerned with male inexpressiveness.
tion as an important process in generating sex differences, Other important sex differences, which did not form
it differs from traditional socialization accounts of sex part of analyses that were based on experimental social
differences (e.g., Mischel, 1967) in that it specifies (or, psychological studies, include same-sex friendship pat-
more accurately, implies) the form this socialization will terns, the meanings attached to material possessions, at-
take. From the perspective of social role theory, we should tachment styles in close relationships, and coping with
expect socialization to involve learning communal and the loss of a relationship. These are also consistent with
agentic traits. the overall pattern predicted by social role theory of
Another important aspect of any approach that women being more communal and men more agentic.
views societal structures and roles as primary is that it But, again, the more specific pattern of male inexpres-
necessarily includes the concept of power. A power ex- siveness can be identified.
planation could be viewed as an alternative to one that Rubin (1985) studied men's and women's friendship
emphasizes the specific content of roles, and Henley's patterns in North America and found that, compared
explanation of nonverbal behavior (see above) provides with women, men tended to form less disclosing rela-
an example of this. However, Eagly (1987) incorporated tionships, which were based on shared activities. Most of
the higher status of men in social role theory through the men in Rubin's sample could not name a best friend.
evidence that status is associated with agentic character- Those who did have close friends avoided speaking about
istics. Power therefore provides an additional reason for personal matters, so as not to appear vulnerable. Women
men to be agentic. The reader should keep in mind that formed closer, more intimate friendships that were based
this is distinct from explanations, such as Henley's, which on sharing feelings and experiences, and most of the
involve a more direct influence of power on people's women in the sample (single, ages 25 to 55 years) were
behavior. able to name a best friend.
One important aspect of the social role view of sex Many other studies (e.g., Wright, 1988) have con-
differences is its inclusiveness. All aspects of men's and firmed these differences between men's and women's
women's social behavior can be characterized as arising friendships in Western samples, and the different patterns
from their agency and communion. However, as we have have been characterized as agentic and communal, re-
just noted, there could be a more direct influence of status spectively, clearly fitting them to social role theory. Nev-
differences on some characteristics, for example, asser- ertheless, in contrast to the meta-analytic syntheses, these
tiveness. Other sex differences might be attributed to as- conclusions arise from impressions gained from a variety
pects of socialization that are better characterized in terms of studies using a variety of methods, and as with all sex
other than agency and communion: Male emotional differences, they hide within-sex variations (Duck &
inexpressiveness, which arises from the emphasis on Wright, 1993; Wright, 1988). There is also a controversy
toughness and avoidance of femininity in boys' peer over whether the difference lies in the underlying structure
groups (Archer, 1992a), is one possibility. of friendship, as Rubin suggested, or in the method of
communication used (Dosser, Balswick, & Halverson,
The Scope of Social Role Theory 1986; Duck & Wright, 1993). If it is the second of these,
Eagly's (1987) application of social role theory was in- the sex difference would be more specifically connected
formed by the available experimental social psychological to male inexpressiveness rather than the overall agentic-
literature and therefore covered topics such as helping, communal distinction.
influenceability, nonverbal behavior, and behavior in Dittmar (1989) found that the reasons women and
small groups. However, the scope of the sex differences men gave for valuing material possessions tended to be
in social behavior that can be characterized in terms of different, and these could be characterized in terms of a
agency and communion is wider than Eagly's analysis symbolic-functional dimension. Women viewed posses-
suggested. This is partly because further data have become sions more in terms of emotional and relational conno-
available but also because her meta-analyses mainly cov- tations, whereas men viewed them more instrumentally.
ered experimental social psychological evidence. Again, the distinction fits the more general c o m m u n a l -
More recent studies include an analysis of women's agentic dimension.
lower physical and psychological well-being (Helgeson, Research on adult attachment has shown that more
1994), women's greater emotional sensitivity and respon- men fit the dismissing sO;le (Bartholomew & Horowitz,
siveness (Grossman & Wood, t 993), and a recta-analysis 1991 ; Brennan, Shaver, & Tobey, 1991) that involves not
of sex differences in personality (Feingold, 1994). In each wanting to depend on others, whereas more women fit

September 1996 • American Psychologist 911


the preoccupied style, which involves a heightened desire words, the past is also seen as explaining present-day psy-
for intimacy with others (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; chological mechanisms. Like social role theory, this ex-
Brennan et al., 1991). These differences can also be viewed planation therefore concerns both origins and current
in terms of agency and communion, or alternatively as psychological processes.
resulting from male inexpressiveness (Dosser et al., 1986). Darwin ( 1871 ) described the differences in form and
Stroebe and Schut (1995) have characterized the behavior of the two sexes in the animal world in terms
typical ways in which women and men cope with be- of sexual selection, which he viewed as a result of com-
reavement as loss-oriented (i.e., facing the loss) and res- petition for access to preferred mates among the males
toration-oriented (i.e., avoiding or denying it). According and choice between available mates by females. Why this
to their Dual Process model (DPM) of coping with grief, should be so was not realized until the article by Trivers
both coping styles are usually necessary for successful (1972), which formed one of the foundations of modern
resolution, but there are sex differences in the degree to Darwinian thinking. The sex (usually the female) that
which the styles are adopted. This view is supported by contributes most to the future offspring becomes a re-
several findings, including those of Schut, Stroebe, van source sought after by the sex (usually the male) contrib-
den Bout, and de Keijser (in press) that widows benefit uting less. Another way of putting this is that the female
more from behaviorally oriented therapy, whereas wid- is limited in the number of potential offspring she can
owers benefit more from therapy emphasizing emotional produce as a result of her larger contribution to each one.
expression. These different coping styles are perhaps more This leads to different selection pressures on the two sexes:
marked in the case of death of a child, with fathers show- Females are better able to maximize fitness (i.e., gene
ing more intense denial (Bohannon, 1990-1991) and frequency in subsequent generations) by being more dis-
trying to control or hide their feelings (Cook, 1988). A criminating, so as to obtain a male with good genes, re-
study of children's grief(Silverman & Worden, 1993) in- sources, and parental skills; males are better able to max-
dicated that coping styles begin during childhood. Again, imize fitness by seeking to mate with many females, but
the differences can be explained both in terms of social in doing so they have to possesses features attractive to
role theory and male inexpressiveness. females and to compete with other males. Intermale
Social role theory can account for these additional aggression is viewed as being more overt and damaging
sex differences in terms of agency and communion, yet than interfemale aggression for this reason.
the alternative, more specific explanation of socialization Whenever there is internal fertilization combined
for male inexpressiveness is applicable in most cases. We with paternal care (as there is in humans), there will also
return to this issue after considering the main alternative be selection for maximizing paternity. Some aspects of
approach to social role theory--sexual selection. male aggression can be seen in terms of sexual jealousy
and possessiveness originating from the evolutionary ad-
Evolutionary Psychology and Sexual vantage of paternity certainty.
Selection
Over the last 10 years, there have been numerous other
Sexual Selection Applied to the
studies of sex differences in social behavior that have been Psychology of Men and Women
informed by a very different perspective than social role Trivers's analysis predicts that in social m a m m a l s such
theory or socialization. This is evolutionary psychology, as human beings, the sexes are likely to show different
which has formed an important theoretical development dispositions underlying their social behavior, principally
in the social sciences over this time (Archer, 1996; Buss, their sexual and courtship behavior and their aggression.
1995a; Daly & Wilson, 1988, 1994). Evolutionary psy- Because men are also limited by the evolved dispositions
chology incorporates the revolutionary change in Dar- of women, which makes them more discriminating when
winian thinking that has occurred in the last 30 years choosing a mate, there is also a conflict of interests be-
and was characterized as modern Darwinism by Cronin tween the two sexes in terms of the behavior that would
(1991). She described its two main features as, first, a maximize their fitness. The consequences of this for hu-
primary interest in the evolution of social behavior (rather man sex differences have been analyzed by Smuts ( 1992,
than structures) and, second, the realization that it is the 1995), who argued that the p a t r i a r c h y - - m e n ' s domina-
genes--as opposed to the individual or the g r o u p - - t h a t tion over w o m e n - - h a s evolved from this conflict of re-
are the units of natural selection. This eventually enabled productive interests. The reasons why men have gained
human social behavior to be viewed in an entirely different the advantage are, first, that they have the size and strength
way. Instead of being concerned with immediate causes to do so and, second, that they can gain more than women
or the individual's developmental history, a modern Dar- would lose. By overcoming female choice, a man can mate
winian explanation would ask how a particular type of with a large number of women; by resisting this, women
social behavior had originated (in terms of its contribution gain a much smaller advantage in the form of a more
to successfully perpetuating the genes of the animals dis- adaptive set of genes.
playing it). The particular emphasis in evolutionary psy- The sexual conflict of interests can be regarded as
chology has been to explain the present-day human psy- an evolutionary arms race (Dawkins & Krebs, 1979),
che (its biases and dispositions) in terms of adaptations where one side--the m a l e - - h a s an inherent advantage.
that were successful in evolutionary history. In other In addition to these general considerations, during the

912 September 1996 • American Psychologist


course of h u m a n evolution, additional factors, such as in three fourths of them women rated ambition and in-
dispersal from natal groups by women and the ability of dustriousness higher than did men,. This was again con-
some men to monopolize essential resources, have com- sistent with findings that were based on both question-
bined to make inequality both marked and institution- naires and overt behavior among U.S. samples (e.g., Buss
alized (Smuts, 1995). & Schmitt, 1993; Ellis, 1992; Feingold, 1990).
There is now a large body of research on h u m a n sex An important evolutionary pressure that has influ-
differences that has been informed by the evolutionary enced the behavior of males in species with internal fer-
perspective (see Buss, 1994, 1995b). It has involved fea- tilization and paternal care is the avoidance of cuckoldry,
tures of male behavior such as aggression and dominance, rearing another male's offspring. One widespread tactic
both to other men and to women; sexual aggression; sex for reducing this is to behave so as to discourage the fe-
differences in preferred numbers of partners; mate selec- male's interest in other males and other males' interest
tion criteria; and strategies for obtaining and keeping a in the female. Men's sexual jealousy has been viewed in
mate. I offer four illustrative examples: discrimination in this light (Daly, Wilson, & Weghorst, 1982; Symons,
mate choice, mate selection criteria, sexual jealousy, and 1979), with the prediction that it will focus on sexual
intermale aggression. access to the partner, because it is this that ensures pa-
Sexual selection theory identifies women as the dis- ternity. Women's jealousy, on the other hand, is predicted
criminating sex when it comes to mate selection. The to be more concerned with the diversion of time and re-
other side of the coin is that men will show a greater sources to a rival. Buss, Larsen, Westen, and Semmelroth
preference for sex without c o m m i t m e n t and for multiple (1992) tested this hypothesis with American undergrad-
partners. The impact of these preferences on overt be- uates, using written and auditory material depicting their
havior is likely to be partly hidden by societal constraints partner forming an emotional attachment, or having sex-
and lack of opportunity for most men. Yet it is clear from ual intercourse, with another partner. As predicted, men
several lines of ethnographic and historical evidence (e.g., rated the sexual cue and women the emotional cue as
Betzig, 1992; Symons, 1979) that when men do have the more distressing. Physiological measures were consistent
opportunity for sexual access with little or no cost, they with these ratings. Other studies have replicated these
do indeed have m a n y partners. Social psychological stud- findings (see Buss, 1995b).
ies show the same dispositions among young men in Aggression has been considered from both the evo-
North America. Symons and Ellis (1989) asked U.S. stu- lutionary and the social role perspectives, although the
dents various questions, including whether they would material used to study it has been different. Eagly and
have intercourse with an anonymous m e m b e r of the op- Steffen's (1986) meta-analytic review, which was confined
posite sex who was as attractive as their current partner, to laboratory studies, found a small overall effect size for
if there were no risks attached and no chance of forming greater male aggressiveness. Daly and Wilson (1988) car-
a relationship. Men were four times more likely than ried out an extensive analysis of sex differences in h u m a n
women to answer that they certainly would and women aggression that was based on homicide figures across cul-
two and a half times more likely to say that they certainly tures. Using this measure, which they viewed as lacking
would not. Ehrlichman and Eichenstein (1992) asked the biases inherent in self-reports, revealed enormous dif-
samples of young Americans to select preferred wishes ferences between the sexes, which were even more pro-
from a long list or to rate 20 specified wishes. Overall, nounced when only same-sex victims were concerned.
the two sexes were similar, but men showed a much When considering aggression, it is important to sep-
stronger preference for the statement "to have sex with arate same-sex exchanges, which are linked to sexual se-
anyone I choose," and for having sex without a relation- lection, from those between the sexes, which are linked
ship. In terms of an overall effect size, the meta-analysis to the sexual conflict of interests, as outlined above. Smuts
of Oliver and Hyde (1993) found the sex difference in (1992, 1995) showed that attempts by males to control
such attitudes to casual sex to be large (d = .81). females, often by force, are widespread among nonhuman
Other research has focused on the characteristics that primates. Although these are often resisted, the effective-
women and men look for in a potential partner. Sexual ness of the resistance is variable (and as indicated above,
selection theory predicts that men will value features in- selection for resistance would generally be less than that
dicative of health and reproductive potential, whereas for male coercion). The same conflict of interests has been
women will value features indicating genetic fitness and played out during human evolution and history, partic-
the ability to provide resources and protection for the ularly after the coming of agriculture, when m e n - - e s -
woman and the offspring. To test these hypotheses, Buss pecially the more powerful ones--controlled resources
( 1989b, 1992) obtained ratings of the desirability of char- necessary for women to survive and reproduce (Smuts,
acteristics for a potential mate from over 10,000 respon- 1995). Rape and violence toward women can be viewed
dents in 37 cultures from six continents. Across all cul- as originating from these selection pressures.
tures, men consistently rated youth and looks as more Overall, the pattern of sex differences found in char-
important than did women, in agreement with findings acteristics associated with sexuality, mate choice, and
from U.S. college students (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Fein- aggression fits that predicted by Darwinian theory (Buss,
gold, 1990). Buss found that in nearly all the cultures, 1995b). It is what we should expect if these differences
women preferred men with higher earning capacity, and originated from sexual selection and an evolutionary

September 1996 • American Psychologist 913


conflict of interests between the sexes, rather than from views all sex differences as having originated in the time-
another source such as the division of labor between the span of human history, cannot explain these findings.
sexes or historically produced power inequalities. Male competition not only explains the existence of sex
differences in aggression but also the pattern of cross-
Can Social Role Theory Explain These species variation.
Findings?
Although both the Darwinian and the social role per- Can Darwinian Theory Account for
spective have offered explanations for sex differences in Findings Explained by Social Role
aggression, those differences in sexuality, mate choice, Theory?
and the triggers for sexual jealousy have not generally
been addressed by social role theory, which would be I conclude that social role theory accounts very poorly,
hard-pressed to explain the particular pattern of results. if at all, for the pattern of sex differences identified by
A possible exception is one aspect of mate choice evolutionary psychologists. Does evolutionary theory fare
criteria, female preference for high earning potential in any better when applied to the sex differences most closely
a man. Caporael (1989) suggested that this represents an associated with social role theory? These differences were
attempt by women to reduce their own powerlessness (i.e., reviewed in an earlier section, where I noted that although
it was a consequence of status inequalities between men sex differences were consistent with the social role inter-
and women). However, the preference for high earning pretation, many could alternatively be viewed in terms
potential is maintained (and even strengthened) when the of either male inexpressiveness or a direct effect of status
woman herself has a high earning potential (Buss, 1989a; inequalities.
Wiederman & Allgeier, 1992). This shows that the pre- Male inexpressiveness, which underlies several im-
diction derived from sexual selection is maintained ir- portant sex differences (friendship, attachment styles, and
respective of the woman's own access to resources, thus coping with loss), can be viewed as originating from in-
supporting the Darwinian explanation over that based on termale competition, rather than characteristics asso-
structural powerlessness. ciated with the division of labor. It is necessary for men
The social role interpretation of sex differences in to avoid signs of vulnerability in a masculine competitive
aggression again views these as derived from gender roles, environment that emphasizes the importance of tough-
although with certain reservations about contextual vari- ness and reputation. To show feelings of vulnerability is
ations and the type of aggression involved (Eagly, 1987). to open oneself to exploitation. Cross-cultural surveys of
More recent findings that men's beliefs about aggression socialization patterns (e.g., Gilmore, 1990; Low, 1989)
tend to be instrumental and women's tend to be expressive reveal the generality of such social environments for boys
(Archer & Haigh, in press; Campbell & Muncer, 1994) and men. Viewed in this way, many of women's social
are consistent with the social role view (although they characteristics, such as greater disclosure in same-sex
would not contradict evolutionary predictions). friendships, more vulnerable attachment styles, and more
Nevertheless, the detailed pattern of findings for sex loss-oriented ways of coping with grief, reflect the absence
differences in aggression towards same-sex others are bet- of inexpressiveness.
ter explained by sexual selection. For example, homicide Other features of women's social behavior identified
shows a much larger sex difference than is the case for by research from the social role perspective--for example,
less damaging acts of aggression, and this is maintained nonverbal behavior associated with deference and influ-
in societies differing widely in absolute homicide rates enceability, together with aspects of female socialization
(Daly & Wilson, 1990). A Darwinian approach predicts such as obedience, responsibility, and social restraint--
that the sex difference would lie not in the overall extent can be attributed to the male domination of women. We
of aggressive feelings but in the greater risks that men have already discussed an evolutionary explanation for
would be prepared to engage in when in conflicts with why men generally dominate women rather than vice
other men (Daly & Wilson, 1988), in other words, in the versa, in terms of the conflict of interests between male
extent of escalation of dispute. The data for homicides and female reproductive strategies. Smuts (1995) argued
clearly support this. Furthermore, sexual selection theory that the patriarchy is "a human manifestation of a sexual
also predicts that it is young men, particularly those with dynamic that is played out over and over again, in many
little access to resources, who will engage in the most different ways, in other animals" (p. 22).
damaging disputes. Again, homicide data support this It is apparent that the Darwinian perspective, in the
prediction across different societies (Wilson & Daly, form of male competition and the reproductive conflict
1993), a pattern that is difficult to explain in terms of of interests between men and women, can provide alter-
social role and other standard social science explanations native explanations for the origin of the sex differences
(Archer, 1995). Social role theory would find it particu- identified by research associated with social role theory.
larly difficult to explain age-related differences in We therefore need to ask whether the division of labor
aggression. between the sexes--the main feature of social role the-
One final point is that similar sex differences in o r y - d o e s provide a more plausible alternative. In con-
aggression are found in a wide range of other species trast to the features identified by evolutionary theory, di-
(Archer, 1988; Trivers, 1972). Social role theory, which vision of labor is unique to human evolution and history.

914 September 1996 • American Psychologist


Male domination of female reproductive life occurs more concluded that the pattern of observed sex differences
widely among nonhuman primates (Smuts, 1992, 1995). better fits an analysis derived from Darwinian theory.
For this reason, it provides a more plausible explanation Various theoretical writings have highlighted the impor-
than that offered by social role theory. Whether male tance of gene-culture interactions. For example, D u r h a m
inexpressiveness or the more general features of agency ( 1991 ) concluded from his analysis of the coevolution of
and c o m m u n i o n better explain the other sex differences genetic and cultural systems that "genetic selection and
cannot be answered so readily. cultural selection are generally expected to co-operate in
the evolution of attributes that, from the point of view
G e n d e r e d Socialization as a of their selectors, are adaptively advantageous" (p. 457).
C o e v o l u t i o n a r y Process Gendered socialization provides a clear example of
Overall, the evidence indicates that the pattern of sex such coevolution. A possible scenario is that cultural tra-
differences in social behavior is better explained by a ditions encouraging aggressiveness, courage, fortitude, and
Darwinian than a social role perspective: What we observe dominance among men arose in the rearing patterns of
at the present time is more likely to have originated from our hominid and ape ancestors. These would have en-
selection pressures during h u m a n and prehuman evo- hanced such traits that would have in any case tended to
lution than as a consequence of the h u m a n division of be present to a greater degree in males than females and
labor. would have been important for fitness in males, as a result
Does this mean that the same pattern of sex differ- of sexual selection. These characteristics would have later
ences would arise independently of the cultural setting? become articulated by language into ideologies that jus-
The social role perspective is fairly clear that this would tified their perpetuation. Boys then came to learn the
not be the case. Eagly and Steffen (1986) argued that reasons why they should be tough and myths about male
women and men would show similar levels of aggression heroes.
if they were treated identically in their rearing. Daly and Smuts (1995) proposed a similar process in relation
Wilson (1994), on the other hand, argued that the two to male domination of women. She suggested that
sexes have different dispositions, which would be manifest
under a wide range of conditions irrespective of gendered Men's use of language and ideology to keep women down is not
socialization. a departure from pre-linguistic forms of male control but, rather,
a natural extension and elaboration of these forms. If male
These predictions are difficult to test empirically. In
chimpanzees could talk, they would probably develop rudi-
practice, it is almost impossible to remove the influence mentary myths and rituals that increased male political soli-
of culture or to provide an environment in which no form darity and control over females and that decreased female ten-
of gendered socialization operates. The removal of the dencies toward autonomy and rebellion. (p. 19)
influence of culture would take away so much that is
characteristic of growing up in any h u m a n society that The coevolutionary approach regards socialization not
any individual subject to such a process would inevitably as something new and separate from human nature. In-
be deprived of other important aspects of social learning. stead, it provides a distinct contribution to the develop-
It is now apparent from animal research that the nature- ment of present-day sex differences in social behavior that
nurture issue has not been well served by using the de- can at the same time be viewed as arising from evolved
privation experience to identify either what is learned or dispositions. The present-day contributions to once-
what is innate (Archer, 1992b). adaptive ends are both genes and culture, and the two
Debate over this issue may turn attention away from are closely interrelated. The biosocial model of the so-
the important connections between evolution and culture. cialization of personality by Harris (1995), which em-
Focusing on evolved dispositions or lack of them neglects phasizes major contributions from inherited dispositions
the degree to which a consistent pattern of both gender and from peer-group socialization, is consistent with this
stereotypes and gendered social learning is found in most view.
cultures. For example, toughness and a willingness to fight Cross-cultural differences in gender socialization can
are a crucial part of boys' early socialization and are also be viewed from a coevolutionary perspective as rep-
maintained as important features of masculine self-def- resenting ways in which gendered socialization is adapted
inition in most cultural contexts (Archer, 1992a, 1994; to the more specific requirements of different social en-
Gilmore, 1990), although they are learned alongside rules vironments. Low (1989) found that in societies where
regarding restraint and when it is appropriate to act out there is an opportunity for greater polygyny, male com-
aggressiveness. An analysis of 93 societies from the eth- petitive training is exaggerated. Thus, socialization reflects
nographic record by Low (1989) found that boys are gen- the future fitness requirements for men in that society.
erally taught to be more aggressive, show more fortitude, The modification of dispositions in response to the rearing
and to be more self-reliant than are girls. Girls are con- environment so as to produce adult characteristics that
sistently taught to be more industrious, responsible, obe- fit that environment is termed an alternative reproductive
dient, and sexually restrained than are boys. strategy by evolutionary biologists (e.g., Dominey, 1984).
The social role explanation for this consistency is According to this analysis, the consistency in the
that it follows from consistency in the division of labor content of gender socialization represents not the impo-
and power inequalities across cultures. We have already sition of roles that arose by historical accident from one

September 1996 • American Psychologist 915


source b u t the way that cultural l e a r n i n g interacts with Human aggression. Naturalistic approaches (pp. 25-41 ), London:
Routledge.
biological dispositions to p r o d u c e individuals who are
Archer, J. (1992a). Childhood gender roles: Social context and organi-
able to pursue the reproductive strategies appropriate for zation. In H. McGurk (Ed.), Childhood social development: Contem-
their own sex. F u r t h e r m o r e , the variation in gender so- porar)' perspectives (pp. 31-61 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
c i a l i z a t i o n represents ways in which these strategies can Archer, J. (1992b). Etholog.v and human development. Savage, MD:
be modified according to the m o r e specific r e q u i r e m e n t s Barnes & Noble.
Archer,J. (1994). Violencebetweenmen. In J. Archer(Ed.), Male violence
of different social e n v i r o n m e n t s . (pp. 121- 140). London: Routledge.
It is u n c l e a r whether this analysis can be extended Archer, J. (1995). What can ethologyofferthe study of human aggression?
to forms of behavior discouraged by m a i n s t r e a m social- Aggressive Behavior. 21. 243-255.
ization, such as the high-risk behavior of y o u n g men, Archer, J. (1996). Evolutionary social psychology. In M. Hewstone, W.
Stroebe, & G. Stephenson (Eds.), Introduction to social psychology:
which is associated with violence, a n d other d e l i n q u e n t A European perspective (pp. 25-45). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
activities. These might be a t t r i b u t a b l e to evolved dispo- Archer, J., & Haigh, A. (in press). Do beliefs about aggressive feelings
sitions that persist despite society's attempts to discourage and actions predict reported levelsof aggression?British Journal of
them, or they may be explained in coevolutionary terms Social Psj'choh)~;v
as the result of peer group influences (Archer, 1992a; Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. ( 199 I). Attachment stylesamong
young adults: A test of a four-categorymodel. Journal of Personality
Harris, 1995). and Social Pwcholog): 61. 226-244.
Betzig, L. (1992). Roman polygyny.Ethology andSociohiology 13. 309-
Conclusions 349.
The social role view and evolutionary theory both predict Bohannon, J. R. ( 1990-1991), Grief responses of spouses followingthe
death of a child: A longitudinalstudy. Omega, 22. 109-121.
that there will be widespread differences in the social be- Brennan, K. A., Shaver, R R., & Tobey,A. E. (1991). Attachment styles,
havior a n d dispositions of w o m e n a n d men. The reason gender and parental problem drinking. Journal of Social and Personal
for such wide-ranging predictions is that they are both Relationships. 8, 451-466.
theories of origins. Social role theory views sex differences Buss. D. M. (1989a). Conflict between the sexes: Strategic interference
and the evocationof angerand upset. Journal OfPersonality and Social
as arising from the division of labor between the sexes
P~')'choh~gJ: 56. 735-747.
through agentic a n d c o m m u n a l patterns of behavior. Buss, D. M. (1989b). Sex differencesin human mate preferences: Evo-
E v o l u t i o n a r y theory attributes m a n y of the same sex dif- lutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sci-
ferences to the consequences of sexual selection a n d the ences. 12, 1-49.
conflict between the different reproductive strategies of Buss, D. M. (1992). Mate preference mechanisms: Consequences for
partner choice and intrasexual competition. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cos-
m e n a n d women. Such features are widespread in other mides, & J. Tooby(Eds.), The adapted mind." Evolutionarj, ps.vcholog.v
m a m m a l s , and evolutionary theory best accounts for both and the generation tgculture (pp. 249-266). New York: Oxford Uni-
the origins of most sex differences in social behavior a n d versity Press.
their observed pattern at the present time. Buss, D. M. (1994). The evohttion o/'desire: Strategies of human mating.
At the level of the processes that currently generate New York: Basic Books.
Buss, D. M. (1995a). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for
these sex differences, the consistent pattern of gendered psychological science. Psjz'hological lnquir): 6, 1-30.
socialization can best be viewed as a coevolutionary pro- Buss, D. M. (1995b). Psychologicalsex differences:Originsthrough sexual
cess that accentuates evolved dispositions. Sexual selec- selection. American P~vchologist. 50. 164-168.
tion theory predicts a pattern that fits m e n for the social Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Weslen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex
differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology and psychology. Psy-
world of i n t e r m a l e c o m p e t i t i o n - - f o r example, assertive- chological Science, 3. 251-255.
ness, toughness, a n d not showing e m o t i o n - - a n d seeking Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. R (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evo-
to control the reproductive decisions of women; it predicts lutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Reviem 100,
a pattern for w o m e n that would foster interpersonal net- 204-232.
Campbell, A., & Muncer, S. (1994). Men and the meaning of violenee.
works a n d exercising choice over their own reproductive In J. Archer (Ed.), Mah" violence (pp. 332-351). London: Routledge.
decisions. This prediction better fits the evidence from Caporael, L. R. (1989). Mechanisms matter: The difference between
cross-cultural studies of socialization patterns t h a n one sociobiology and evolutionary psychology [Commentary on Buss,
that is simply characterized in t e r m s of agency a n d com- 1989b]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12. 17-18.
m u n i o n , as is social role theory. G e n d e r e d socialization Cook, J. (I 988). Dad's double binds: Rethinking fathers" bereavement
from a men's studies perspective. Journal ofContemporao, Ethnog-
is therefore viewed as playing an i m p o r t a n t part in m a i n - raplo: 17. 285-308.
t a i n i n g behavior consistent with the different fitness in- Cronin, H. ( 1991), The ant and thepeacock. Cambridge, England:Cam-
terests of m e n a n d women. bridge UniversityPress.
Daly, M., & Wilson,M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldinede Gruyter.
REFERENCES Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1990). Killingthe competition: Female/female
and male/male homicide.Human Nature. 1.87-107.
Anastasi, A. (1958). D~fferential psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1994). Evolutionary psychologyof male vio-
Macmillan. lence. In J. Archer (Ed.), Malt, violence (pp. 253-288). London:
Archer, J. (1976). Biologicalexplanationsof psychologicalsex differences. Routledge.
In B. B. Lloyd& J. Archer (Eds.), Exploring sex differences (pp. 241- Daly, M., Wilson, M., & Weghorst, S. J. (1982). Male sexual jealousy.
266). London: Academic Press. Ethoh~gJ' and Sociobiology, 3. I 1-27.
Archer, J. (1988). The behavioural biology of aggression. Cambridge, Darwin, C. ( 1871). The descent ~f man and selection in relation to sex.
England: Cambridge UniversityPress. London: Murray.
Archer, J. (1989). From the laboratory to the community: Studying the Davies, D. R. (1986). Children's performance as a function of sex-typed
natural historyof human aggression,in J. Archer& K. Browne(Eds.), labels. British Journal o/Social Psycholog); 25. 173-175.

916 September 1996 • A m e r i c a n Psychologist


Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1979). Arms races between and within Helgeson, V. S. (1994). Relation of agency and communion to well-
species. Proceedings of the Roj,al Society of London B, 205, 489-511. being: Evidence and potential explanations. Psychological Bulletin.
Deaux, K. (1984). From individual differences to social categories: 116. 412-428.
Analysis of a decade's research on gender. American Psychologist. 39, Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power. s~<\ and non-verbal com-
105-116. munication. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice-Hall.
Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive House, J. (1981). Social structure and personality. In M. Rosenberg &
model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369-389. R. H. Turner (Eds.), Social po'cholagy: Sociological perspectives (pp.
Dittmar, H. (1989). Gender identity-related meanings of personal pos- 525-561). New York: Basic Books.
sessions. British Journal ¢f Social Psycholog); 28, 159-171. Hutt, C. (1972). Males and li,males. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin.
Dominey, W. J. (1984). Alternative mating tactics and evolutionarily Hyde, J. S. ( 1981 ). How large are cognitive gender differences? A meta-
stable strategies. American Zoologist, 24, 385-396. analysis using w 2 and d. American Psychoh)gist. 36. 892-901.
Dosser, D. A., Balswick, J. O., & Halverson, C. F., Jr. (1986). Male Hyde, J. S., & Plant, E. A. (1995). Magnitude of psychological gender
inexpressiveness and relationships. Journal of Social and Personal differences: Another side to the story. American Ps),choh~gist, 50, 159-
Relationships, 3, 241-258. 161.
Duck, S., & Wright, P. H. (1993). Reexamining gender differences in Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1962). Birth to maturit): New York: Wiley.
same-gender friendships: A close look at two kinds of data. Se~ Roles, Low, B. S. (1989). Cross-cultural patterns in the training of children:
28, 709-727. An evolutionary perspective. Jmtrnal of Comparative Psychoh)gy 103.
Durham, W. C. ( 1991 ). Co-evolution: Genes, culture and human diversity. 311-319.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Maccoby, E. E. (1967). Sex differences in intellectual functioning. In
Eagly, A. H. (1983). Gender and social influence: A social psychological E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The deveh)pment q/se~ di[]~,rences (pp. 25-55).
analysis. American Psychologist, 38, 971-981. London: Tavistock.
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex d~fferences in social behavior A social role Maccoby, E. E.. & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). Thepsychologj, of sex d~fferences.
interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and Mischel, W. (1967). A social learning view of sex differences in behavior.
men. American Psychologist, 50. 145-158. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The deveh~pment ¢~[se~-dtffk,rences (pp. 56-
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (1981). Sex of researchers and sex-typed 81 ). London: Tavistock.
communications as determinants of sex differences in influenceability: Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality. A
A recta-analysis of social influence studies. Psychological Bulletin, 90, meta-analysis. Psychoh)gical Bulletin, 114, 29-51.
1-20. Rubin, L. (1985). Just.tkiends: The role ¢?lJriendship in our lives. New
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A York: Harper & Row.
meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 309-330. Schut, H., Stroebe, M. S., van den Bout, J., & de Keijser, J. (in press).
Ehrlichman, H., & Eichenstein, R. (1992). Private wishes: Gender sim- Intervention for the bereaved: Gender differences in the efficacy of
ilarities and differences. Sex Roles, 26, 399-422. the two counselling programs. British Journal ol(Tinical Psj,choh~gy
Ellis, B. J. (1992). The evolution of sexual attraction: Evaluative mech- Silverman, I~ R., & Worden, J. W. (1993). Children's reactions to the
anisms in women. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.). death of a parent. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson
The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation ¢~l (Eds.), Itandhook Of bereavement: Theoo', research and intervention
culture (pp. 267-288). New York: Oxford University Press. (pp. 300-316). New York: Cambridge University Press,
Feingold, A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of physical attractiveness Smuts, B. (1992). Male aggression against women: An evolutionary per-
on romantic attraction: A comparison across five research paradigms. spective. Ituman Nature, 3, 1-44.
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology, 59. 981-993. Smuts, B. (1995). The evolutionary origins of patriarchy. Human Nature,
Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. 6. 1-32.
Psychoh~gical Bulletin, 116, 429-456. Stroebe, M. S.. & Schut, H. A. W. ( 1995, June). The dualprocess model
Feingold, A. (1995). The additive effects of differences in central tendency Ofc~q~ing with h~ss. Paper presented at the International Work Group
and variability are important in comparisons between groups. Amer- on Death, Dying and Bereavement, Oxford, England.
ican Ps)z'hoh~gist, 50, 5-13. Symons, D, (1979). The evolution of human st:~ualitj'. New York: Oxford
Garai, J. E., & Scheinfeld, A. (1868). Sex differences in mental and University Press.
behavioral traits. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 77, 169-299. Symons, D., & Ellis, B. (1989) Human male-female differences in sexual
Gilmore, D. D. (1990). Manhood in the making. New Haven, CT: Yale desire. In A. S. Rasa, C. Vogel, & E. Voland (Eds.), The sociobiology
University Press. qlse\ual and repn~ductive strategies (pp. 131 - 146). London: Chapman
Gray, J. A., & Buffery, A. W. H. (1971). Sex differences in emotional & Hall.
and cognitive behaviour in mammals including man: Adaptive and Trivets, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B.
neural bases. Acta Psychologica, 35, 89-111. Campbell (Ed.), S~xual selection and the descent ~?[man (pp. 136-
Grossman, M., & Wood, W. (1993). Sex differences in intensity of emo- 179). Aldine: Chicago.
tional experience: A social role interpretation. Journal qfPersonality Wiederman, M. W., & AIIgeier, E. R. (1992). Gender differences in mate
and Social Psychology, 65, 1010-1022. selection criteria: Sociobiological or socioeconomic explanation?
Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psycho- Ethalogy and Sociobiologj: 13. 115-124.
logical Bulletin, 85. 845-857. Williams, J. E., & Best, D. k. (1982). Measuring s~v stereotypes: A
Hall, J. A. (1984). Non-verbal sex diff~,rences. Baltimore, MD: Johns thirty-nation sltttll: Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hopkins University Press. Wilson, M., & Daly. M. (1993). Lethal confrontational violence among
Hargreaves. D. J., Bates, H. M., & Foot, J. M. C. (1985). Sex-typed young men. In N. J. Bell & R. W. Bell (Eds.), Adolescent risk taking
labelling affects task performance. British Journal q/Social Psychoh~gy. (pp. 84-106). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
24. 153-155. Wright, P. H. (1988). Interpreting research on gender differences in
Harris, J. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization friendship: A case for moderation and a plea for caution. Journal q/
theory of development. Psychological Review. 102. 458-489. Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 367-373.

September 1996 • A m e r i c a n Psychologist 917

You might also like