You are on page 1of 5

Perspec ve 

To Know Them is to Love Them 
Eugene Hunn 
Author address: Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Sea le, WA, U.S.A. 
Email: enhunn323@comcast.net 

Received: December 10, 2014  Volume: 5:146‐150 
Published: December 30, 2014  © 2014 Society of Ethnobiology 

Abstract: I connect the theore cal emphasis that mo vated the cogni ve ethnobiology of the 1960s and early 1970s with
the contemporary emphasis on promo ng ethnobiology as contribu ng to biodiversity conserva on. I use the words of a
popular song to highlight the necessary, if problema c, links between knowing nature – the focus of cogni ve ethnobiology,
loving nature, and ac ng to conserve nature. I argue that a highly elaborated knowledge of the living things in one’s local
environment is characteris c of Indigenous and other deeply rooted communi es, which are dependent on sustainable
harvests of local natural resources. Furthermore, this extensive knowledge goes hand in hand with a deep emo onal
engagement with those species (“love”), which is in turn powerful mo va on to treat those species with respect, absent
dominance of profit mo ves. I suggest in conclusion that ethnobiology may best contribute to biodiversity conserva on by
documen ng the detailed knowledge of and cultural apprecia on for biodiversity evident in such rooted communi es – an
effort that has defined the ethnobiological project for over the past half century. The wider community of ac vists dedicated
to biodiversity conserva on may thus be er know and thus appreciate – respect, if not “love” – those who live with and
depend for their livelihood on this biodiversity.

Keywords:  Ethnobiology, conserva on biology, classifica on and nomenclature, knowledge and emo on, applied 


ethnobiology  

“To know, know, know him is to might better communicate to a broader audience the
love, love, love him; relevance of ethnobiology to the ecological and
Just to see him smile makes my life political crises that threaten us all today. I have no
worthwhile….” easy answers but a few reflections.
‒Phil Spector Cognitive ethnobiology was defined by theoretical
issues of central concern in the 1960s, notably, how
This pop song by the Teddy Bears climbed to #1 on best to define “culture” as the proper subject of
the Billboard’s Top 100 in September 1958, the lyric anthropological understanding. We hoped to devise a
inspired by a tombstone epitaph (http:// “Theory of Culture,” “culture” understood as a
en.wikipedia.org/wiki/To_Know_Him_Is_to_Love_Him). I society’s “knowledge of the world.” Cognitive
recalled the tune as I contemplated my assigned topic ethnobiology traces an intellectual pedigree to an
for this brief perspective piece: “Cognitive Ethnobiol- emerging “Science of Mind,” which had parallel
ogy and Bio[cultural] diversity Conservation.” How contemporary elaborations in psychology and
so? And what has love got to do with it? How are we linguistics (Gardner 1986). The recent “White House
to connect cognitive ethnobiology – what I have Brain Initiative: Brain Research Through Advancing
characterized in a previous essay as “Ethnobiology Innovative Neurotechnologies,” initiated to explore
II” (Hunn 2007), noted for its sometimes obsessive the “new frontier” of the human mind, indicates that
concern with nomenclature and classification – with our interest in understanding the cognitive founda-
an emergent Ethnobiology V (Wyndham et al. 2011; tions of culture through ethnobiological classification
Wolverton 2013), which would build on what we was not misplaced. However, anthropology’s theoreti-
have learned through the previous four phases of cal efforts since have been redirected, leaving the
ethnobiology in order to promote a more loving cognitive terrain to neurotechnology.
relationship between humanity and biodiversity? This The ecological issues that have dominated
question is relevant to the larger issue of how we

Ethnobiology Le ers. 2014. 5: 146‐150. DOI: 10.14237/ebl.5.2014.297. 146


Perspec ve 

subsequent phases of ethnobiological investigation savage’s [sic.] belly and consequently to his mind is
were below the theoretical horizon during my grad very short” (1974:44). However, there is no funda-
school days. While we turned our attention inward to mental conflict between seeing human knowledge as
the “mind,” we were not unaware of the essential fact intellectually satisfying and at the same time useful. In
that knowledge of the world derives from an engage- fact, evolutionary theory requires that this intensive
ment with the world outside the mind. We reasoned that human investment in the cognitive ordering of the
words named ideas, that ideas were the grist for living world must have or have had survival value.
thought, and that thought was the foundation for Which brings us back to the topic of this paper: How
action (D’Andrade 1995). Thus, to understand how might cognitive ethnobiology inform biodiversity
people related to their natural environment it would conservation, in light of the fact that conservation
first be essential to understand how people conceptu- biologists have been slow to recognize the complex
alized that environment, to appreciate their traditional intimacy of the human relationship to nature (Rozzi
or local environmental knowledge (TEK or LEK, 1999; Saslis-Lagoudakis and Clarke 2013; Wolverton
Traditional/Local Environmental/Ecological et al. 2014)?
Knowledge), made manifest in language. In this we My title hints at an answer. Do we humans
affirmed the conclusion of eminent biologists, who treasure what we know best? Perhaps, yet it is clearly
argued likewise that the systematic naming and inadequate as an explanation of why humans some-
classification of the world’s biological species was times husband living resources and at other times
prerequisite to any proper investigation of the mercilessly exploit or destroy them. We may well
evolutionary and ecological relationships among those grant the inverse, to wit, that to be ignorant of the
species (Simpson 1961). plants, animals, and fungi in our midst is to guarantee
Knowledge, however, is no simple reflection of that we will lack the motivation to conserve them.
the surfaces of the world but rather involves an Even if our ignorance were only partial, say to the
implicit and likely innate “theory of nature.” Lan- extent that we recognized trees, but not oaks, maples,
guage, notably encapsulated in vocabulary, provides cedars, ceibas, or baobabs; birds, but not ravens,
strong evidence for the mental transformations that eagles, chickadees, or hummingbirds; and mushrooms,
give rise to the conceptual worlds we all inhabit. It is now but not morels, chanterelles, puff balls, or fly agaric,
apparent, in light of this early ethnobiological we would have next to no basis for valuing the
research, that the living world that surrounds us, the diversity of trees, birds, and fungi. The stunning
plants, animals, and fungi, is the subject of impressive ignorance of local biodiversity demonstrated by
lexical elaboration in all the world’s languages, not contemporary college students (Medin et al. 2006)
least of all, those lacking written traditions. A careful may be symptomatic of a modern malady, dubbed by
study of any such language will yield an inventory of Loev, “Nature Deficit Disorder” (2005), which in turn
at least 1000 lexemes naming “folk species” known may account for a lack of passion in defense of the
locally (Berlin 1992). These basic vocabulary entries local natural environment by the earth’s predominant-
constitute perhaps 5% of the total working vocabu- ly urban populations.
lary of a language. Such linguistic resources allow That simply knowing biodiversity – recognizing
people to describe, remember, understand, and and naming hundreds of ethnospecies – ensures that
imagine their ambient biodiversity. we therefore will love biodiversity, is far from self-
During the heyday of cognitive ethnobiology we evident. There is more to the equation of knowing
were not entirely unconcerned with how this elabo- with loving. Our hit song suggests a somewhat more
rate knowledge of ambient biodiversity might be of complex set of connections. First, “to know him is to
use in the everyday lives of the people with whom we love him” implies that knowing → loving, then “just
worked. Claude Levi-Strauss famously discounted to see him smile, makes my life worthwhile” implies
utilitarian motives for the elaboration of cultural further that loving → a life worth living. What might
knowledge, and of environmental knowledge in ethnobiology suggest with regard to these lyrical
particular (1966), attributing the primary motivation connections?
to “disinterested” curiosity, to biophilia one might say 1) Our efforts at documenting the depth and
(Wilson 1984). He rejected Malinowski’s utilitarian breadth of traditional environmental/
argument that, “The road from the wilderness to the ecological knowledge constitute, in my

Ethnobiology Le ers. 2014. 5: 146‐150. DOI: 10.14237/ebl.5.2014.297. 147


Perspec ve 

opinion, ethnobiology’s deepest and most not so much in recognition of their beauty or
lasting contribution to environmental science. positive utility but rather because of the
Our research efforts have shown that our competitive and often painful interactions
citizen colleagues, those who are Indigenous with these creatures in their daily lives.
and/or otherwise deeply engaged with local Brightman characterizes the Cree attitude
ecologies, pay close attention to the living towards animals as a mix of respect for a
world around them. They devise systematic worthy adversary and fear, rather than “love,”
inventories of local species of plants, animals, with its sentimental connotations (1973).
and fungi, as well as elaborating complex Hunters hunt their prey, kill and eat them.
ethnoanatomical, ethnomedical, and ethnoge- But, with due respect (Nelson 1983). A world
ographic vocabularies. These cultural apart from the “love” of the animal rights
inventories of biodiversity are more than activist. Yet, time and again we learn that
bland lists of names. Rather, each name Indigenous people recognize the essential
points to a web of knowledge of where, part each animal and plant must play in the
when, how, and why a plant or animal or local ecological drama. So “love” may not be
fungus exists, a “subtle ecology” (Wyndham the most appropriate term for this term of the
2009) of “ecological understanding” (Turner equation. Rather, call this an intensely
and Berkes 2006). The Zapotec children who respectful emotional engagement with nature.
taught me about the natural environment of What many urbanites have lost – insulated as
their Mexican town would readily rattle off many of us are from direct personal experi-
several hundred Zapotec plant names but also ence of nature – is this intense emotional
were eager to share many salient details about engagement, which is replaced by ignorance,
the lives of each plant and its value as food, indifference, annoyance, romantic delusion,
medicine, material, or “as ornament” (Hunn or abstract analysis.
2008). 3) Finally, can we show that this “love,” this
2) Given that humans are eminently capable of intense emotional engagement grounded in
and inclined to carefully observe ambient extensive, experiential knowledge will “make
biodiversity, developing thereby an encyclo- our lives worthwhile”? That is, will this
pedic cultural inventory of the local biota, emotion motivate action with respect to
what is the evidence that humans consequent- sustainable use and management of local
ly harbor strong emotional attachments to biodiversity? We should not expect people to
their natural worlds? Eugene Anderson’s conserve biodiversity for its own sake. Such is
theme throughout his Ecologies of the Heart far too abstract a target for “love.” Rather,
(1996) is that to conserve nature we must first our most intense emotional engagements will
love nature, that is, feel strong emotional be with particular animals or plants, places
attachments to plants, animals, even fungi. and landscapes. And such engagements are as
Kay Milton likewise argues that Loving Nature particular as the multitude of animals, plants,
is key to saving the natural world (2002). It and places for which we have names. Spotted
must be recognized that beside biophilia runs a Owls are easier to love (and to hate, if you
countercurrent of biophobia. Our contempo- were an unemployed logger) than an Old
rary urban aversion to mosquitoes, ticks, Growth forest ecosystem. Might it be the case
spiders, snakes, bats, and rats (Nolan and that our inclination to conserve biodiversity is
Robbins 2001; Nolan et al. 2006) is shared to a function of the number and intensity of our
some degree in Indigenous communities, as emotional attachments with the world around
shown by traditional classifications of “wugs” us, grounded in direct personal experience
and “noxious invertebrates,” as I found in my with the stunning diversity of natural forms?
Tzeltal Mayan ethnotaxonomies (Hunn 1977).
We have no controlled double-blind experimental
Tzeltal Maya from Tenejapa, Chiapas,
studies that might prove that for an individual to
Mexico, elaborate their classification of
know more about his or her ambient biodiversity
insects to the greatest degree in dealing with
guarantees or even encourages more careful steward-
social hymenoptera, ants, bees, and wasps,

Ethnobiology Le ers. 2014. 5: 146‐150. DOI: 10.14237/ebl.5.2014.297. 148


Perspec ve 

ship of that biodiversity (but cf. Atran et al. 2004; interlocutors and ultimately to share this with students
Dombrosky and Wolverton 2014). Yet anecdotally, in and colleagues. As ethnobiologists we return from the
our contemporary urban milieu those most supportive intense experience of participating with a local
of biodiversity conservation are those who have community, sharing in their daily encounters with
invested in learning to appreciate that biodiversity in nature, loving the people as they love the land. We
concrete detail. I include here hunters and fisher folk then do our best to communicate, by writing and
as well as birders and native plant people. We may teaching, both our knowledge and our love of “our
question this as a general rule in light of the fact that people.” I believe this has been and will be the most
commercial fishers and foresters, however knowledge- effective way for ethnobiologists to promote biocul-
able, have contributed to the depletion of global fish tural diversity, through the medium and message of
stocks and old growth forests. Two competing forces our close encounters with the citizen scientists whose
are at work here, appreciation versus accumulation. lives we briefly share, at their homes in the communi-
The fact that most Indigenous communities still ties we study.
practicing a traditional “subsistence economy” on
their ancestral lands exhibit highly elaborated TEK References Cited
may be due to the near absence of profit motives Anderson, E. N. 1996. Ecologies of the Heart: Emotion,
from their conceptual worlds, motives that drive Belief, and the Environment. Oxford University Press,
boundless accumulation. In which case, conservation New York and Oxford.
biologists should clearly recognize as their opponent Atran, S., D. Medin, and N. Ross. 2004. Evolution
not the subsistence farmer or fisher but rather the and devolution of knowledge: a tale of two biolo-
profit-making enterprises of high capitalism, a caution- gies. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 10: 395
ary fact for proponents of the “New Conserva- -420.
tion” (Wolverton et al. 2014; http://www.snap.is/
magazine/new-conservation-friend-or-foe/). This Berlin, B. 1992. Ethnobiological Classification: Principles of
highlights a critical problem: The world is rapidly Categorization of Plants and Animals in Traditional
urbanizing. Capitalist “rationality” rules politics. Yet, Societies. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New
somehow we must reclaim that essential basis for Jersey.
biodiversity conservation, the “love” of nature shared Brightman, R. A. 1973. Grateful Prey: Rock Creek
by those who live within its intimate embrace. Human-Animal Relationships. University of California
Finally, our equation here of knowledge → love Press, Berkeley and Los Angeles.
→ action may be interpreted from a different perspec- D'Andrade, R. G. 1995. The Development of Cognitive
tive. That is, it applies not only to the Indigenous and Anthropology. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
other locally rooted communities we have been bridge, UK:
inclined to study, but to ourselves (Nabhan 2013).
That is, as ethnobiologists we have come to know well Dombrosky, J., and S. Wolverton. 2014. TNR and
people who live in close proximity to and in deep conservation on a university campus: a political
dependence upon their local natural environments ecological perspective. PeerJ 2: e312 http://
(Lepofsky and Feeney 2013). Participant observation dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.312
over an extended period of collegial research in such Gardner, H. 1985. The Mind’s New Science: A History of
communities forges an intense emotional engagement the Cognitive Revolution. Basic Books, New York.
(Turner and Berkes 2006), which in turn urges our
efforts on their behalf, to deflect those social, eco- Hunn, E. S. 1977. Tzeltal Ethnozoology: The Classification
nomic, and political forces that would undermine the of Discontinuities in Nature. Academic Press, New
foundations of their lives and livelihoods. My cogni- York and London.
tive ethnobiological research masked a hidden motive. Hunn, E. S. 2007. Ethnobiology in four phases.
As an avid birder I took pleasure in sharing my Journal of Ethnobiology 27: 1-10.
enthusiasm for the fascinating diversity of birds with
Indigenous colleagues, though they often seemed Hunn, E. S. 2008. A Zapotec Natural History: Trees,
more interested in bugs and plants than birds. Thus Herbs, and Flowers; Birds, Beasts, and Bugs in the Life of
my knowledge and love of birds led me to share an San Juan Gbëë. University of Arizona Press, Tucson.
intense emotional engagement with my Indigenous

Ethnobiology Le ers. 2014. 5: 146‐150. DOI: 10.14237/ebl.5.2014.297. 149


Perspec ve 

Lepofsky, D., and K. Feeney. 2013. Ten principles of Saslis-Lagoudakis, C. H., and A. C. Clarke. 2013.
ethnobiology: an interview with Amadeo Rea. In Ethnobiology: the missing link between ecology and
Explorations in Ethnobiology: The Legacy of Amadeo Rea, evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 28: 67-68.
M. Quinlan, and D. Lepofsky, editors, pp. 34-46.
Simpson, G. G. 1961. Principles of Animal Taxonomy.
Society of Ethnobiology, Denton, TX.
Columbia University Press, New York.
Levi-Strauss, C. 1966. The Savage Mind. Weidenfeld
Turner, N. J., and F. Berkes. 2006. Coming to
and Nicholson, London.
understanding: developing conservation through
Loev, R. 2005. Last Child in the Woods. Algonquin incremental learning in the Pacific Northwest.
Books, Chapel Hill, North Carolina. Human Ecology 34: 495-513.
Malinowski, B. 1974 (1925). Magic, Science, and Religion. Wilson, E. O. 1986. Biophilia. Reprint Edition.
Souvenir Press, London. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachu-
setts.
Medin, D., N. Ross, and D. Cox. 2006. Culture and
Resource Conflict: Why Meanings Matter. Russell Sage Wolverton, S. 2013. Ethnobiology 5: interdisciplinari-
Foundation Publications, New York. ty in an era of rapid environmental change. Ethnobi-
ology Letters 4: 21-25.
Milton, K. 2002. Loving Natures: Towards an Ecology of
Emotion. Routledge, London and New York. Wolverton, S., J. M. Nolan, and W. Ahmed. 2014.
Ethnobiology, political ecology, and conservation.
Nabhan, G. P. 2013. Ethnobiology for a diverse
Journal of Ethnobiology 34: 125-152.
world: autobiology? The traditional ecological,
agricultural and culinary knowledge of us!. Journal of Wyndham, F. S. 2009. Spheres of relation, lines of
Ethnobiology 33: 2-6. interaction: subtle ecologies of the Rarámuri
landscape in northern Mexico. Journal of Ethnobiology
Nelson, R. K. 1983. Make Prayers to the Raven: A
29: 271-295.
Koyukon View of the Northern Forest. University of
Chicago Press. Wyndham, F. S., D. Lepofsky, and S. Tiffany. 2011.
Taking stock in Ethnobiology: Where do we come
Nolan, J. M., and M. Robbins. 2001. Emotional
from? What are we? Where are we going? Journal of
meaning and the cognitive organization of ethnozo-
Ethnobiology 31: 110-127.
ological domains. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 11:
240-249.
Biosketch
Nolan, J. M., K. E. Jones, K. W. McDougal, M. J. Eugene  Hunn  is  professor  emeritus  of  anthropology  at 
McFarlin, and M. K. Ward. 2006. The lovable, the the  University  of  Washington.  He  has  served  as  presi‐
loathsome, and the liminal: .emotionality in dent  of  the  Society  of  Ethnobiology  and  editor  of  the 
ethnozoological cognition. Journal of Ethnobiology 26: Journal  of  Ethnobiology.  He  was  honored  as  a  Dis n‐
126-138. guished Ethnobiologist by the Society of Ethnobiology in 
Rozzi, R. 1999. The reciprocal links between evolu- 2014. 
tionary-ecological sciences and environmental
ethics. Bioscience 49: 911-921.

Ethnobiology Le ers. 2014. 5: 146‐150. DOI: 10.14237/ebl.5.2014.297. 150

You might also like