You are on page 1of 13

Parental Reports of Spoken

Language Skills in Children


With Down Syndrome

Eva Berglund
Department of Psychology Spoken language in children with Down syndrome and in children in a normative
Stockholm University group was compared. Growth trends, individual variation, sex differences, and
Stockholm, Sweden performance on vocabulary, pragmatic, and grammar scales as well as MaxLU
and (maximum length of utterance) were explored. Subjects were 330 children with
College of Health and Down syndrome (age range: 1–5 years) and 336 children in a normative group
Caring Sciences (1;4–2;4 years;months). The Swedish Early Communicative Development
Falun, Sweden Inventory–words and sentences (SECDI–w&s) was employed. Performance of
and children with Down syndrome at ages 3;0 and 4;0 was comparable with that of
Uppsala University Hospital children in the normative group at ages 1;4 and 1;8 respectively. In comparison
Uppsala, Sweden with children in the normative group of similar vocabulary size, children with
Down syndrome lagged slightly on pragmatic and grammar scales. The early
Mårten Eriksson development proceeded in most cases with exponential or logistic growth. This
University of Gävle stresses the great potential of early intervention.
Gävle, Sweden
KEY WORDS: Down syndrome, language, parental reports, normative group,
Iréne Johansson SECDI
University of Karlstad
Karlstad, Sweden

T
he aim of the present study was to examine growth trends and
individual variation in spoken-language development in a repre-
sentative sample of Swedish children with Down syndrome.
Pueschel and Hopmann (1993) have commented on the great need to
acquire “good normative studies of the communication and language
skills associated with Down syndrome, as helpful guidelines for both
parents and professionals” (p. 354). The current study is the first large-
scale study of a national sample of children with Down syndrome in
which normative data are provided. We also studied sex-related differ-
ences in language among children with Down syndrome and children in
the normative group. The data included vocabulary size, pragmatic skills,
grammar skills, and maximum length of utterance (MaxLU) from the
Swedish Early Communicative Development Inventory–words and sen-
tences (SECDI–w&s; see Berglund & Eriksson, 2000).
Only a few studies have produced general results regarding the com-
municative development of children with Down syndrome. General find-
ings concern the developmental timetable and the existence of consider-
able variability across children. Some previous studies involved only a
few subjects and can thus be regarded as case studies in the sense that
they offer examples but present no normative data (e.g., Gillham, 1990).
Although a number of the studies encompass groups of children observed
at specific ages (e.g., Stroeminger, Winkler, & Cohen, 1984), most include
no information as to whether the children investigated were enrolled in

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001 • ©American
Berglund Speech-Language-Hearing
et al.: Language in Children With DownAssociation
Syndrome 179
1092-4388/01/4401-0179

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
specific language-stimulation programs or if they had Down syndrome, both in terms of age at onset of spoken
additional deficiencies. Children with Down syndrome language and in later performance. To our knowledge,
often suffer from eye disorders; cardiac disorders; asth- however, the magnitude of variation in relation to age
ma; allergies or immunologic diseases; impairment of has never been the primary focus of previous studies.
hearing; and various gastroenterological, neurological, Children with Down syndrome have often been
orthopaedic, and endocrinological complaints. matched with unaffected children in terms of nonverbal
Because there are very few studies describing com- mental age (see Chapman, 1995). When such matching
municative development in children with Down syn- has been used, the large variability in the groups of chil-
drome in terms of either general trends or variability dren with Down syndrome has proven to be similar to
across children, our primary aim was to provide infor- or greater than the variability among unaffected chil-
mation relevant to researchers and clinicians in this dren. Yet, there is also considerable variation in the per-
area. The age at which children with Down syndrome formance of unaffected children (Bates, Dale, & Thal,
start to talk and, in particular, when their first words 1995; Berglund & Eriksson, 2000; Eriksson & Berglund,
are uttered was one focus of the study. Several studies 1999; Fenson et al., 1994). Miller, Sedley, Miolo, Rosin,
have shown that some children with Down syndrome and Murray-Branch (1991, cited in Chapman, 1995)
utter their first words when they are 12 to 24 months found that when spoken and signed words were added
old, though substantial individual variation exists. Other children with Down syndrome who used manual signs
studies have noted, however, that it is not until age 3;9 obtained vocabulary scores similar to those of their un-
that some children with Down syndrome uttered their affected peers matched on mental age. When vocabu-
first words (Gillham, 1990; Stroeminger et al., 1984). In laries increased in magnitude, the child mostly added
general, the development of a first vocabulary is ex- spoken words.
tremely slow in children with Down syndrome (Gillham, To identify the ages at which the performance of
1990). children with Down syndrome parallels the performance
We also wished to describe individual variation in of unaffected children was a further aim of this study.
spoken-language development. Inasmuch as there is Mervis (1990), for example, compared the performance
extensive variation in the age at which first spoken of children with Down syndrome at age 3;0 to that of
words appear, there is also considerable variability in unaffected children at age 1;6, suggesting similar quali-
later communicative development. Gillham’s (1990) sub- tative aspects of conceptual development but slower
jects reached the 50-word level between the ages of 3;6 vocabulary development in children with Down syn-
and 6;0. In their age 3;0 subjects, Stroeminger et al. drome. In a study by Mundy, Sigman, Kasari, and
(1984) found that vocabulary size ranged from 0 to 85 Yirmiya (1988) children with Down syndrome with a
words. There is also sizeable variation in the develop- mean age of 3;8 obtained expressive language scores
ment of mean length of utterance (MLU; Brown, 1973). comparable to those of unaffected children at age 1;6.
Caselli et al. (1998) found that children with Down syn- Bates et al. (1995) found that infants with Down syn-
drome were severely delayed in reaching developmen- drome lagged several months or years behind unaffected
tal stages and that the children exhibited individual infants in communicative abilities, measured by the
differences. Caselli et al. also found that children with American Communicative Development Inventories:
Down syndrome were more advanced in lexical compre- Infant version (CDI: Infants; see Fenson et al., 1993).
hension and gestures than in lexical production. Also In the period covered by CDI: Toddlers (Fenson et al.,
Chapman, Seung, Schwartz, and Kay-Raining Bird 1993), the delay was found to be approximately 2 years
(1998) found a specific expressive language impairment (Bates et al., 1995). Smith, von Tetzchner, and Michalsen
in their subjects. However, they found no evidence of a (1988) found that about half of their subjects with Down
critical period for language development nor of a “syn- syndrome at age 5;0 had scores comparable to those of
tactic ceiling” of MLU. In a longitudinal study, Fowler unaffected children at age 2;6 on the McCarthy scales
(1988) found that at age 4:0, the highest MLU scores of children’s abilities, whereas the remaining children
obtained by three children were 1.5, 2.2, and 2.2 (pp. with Down syndrome had lower scores. Delay was more
236–238); at age 5;0 the highest scores of five children pronounced with regard to language skills as opposed
were 1.9, 2.2, 2.3, 3.7, and 3.8. One criterion for inclu- to their general cognitive functioning among children
sion in Fowler’s sample was that the child should pro- with Down syndrome. Interestingly, the delay was
duce two-word utterances. Accordingly, performance in present in nonverbal communication as well as in speech,
the Fowler study is expected to be higher than the aver- suggesting that the language problems among children
age performance of children with Down syndrome. with Down syndrome are not merely articulatory in
In summary, the studies reviewed here indicate that nature. Miller (1995) found a language-specific delay in
there is extensive individual variation in children with a comparison of children with Down syndrome and un-
affected children matched on mental age. He reported

180 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
that 65% of children with Down syndrome performed at Few studies have documented sex-related language
vocabulary levels below the 10th percentile of unaffected differences in children with Down syndrome. Nonethe-
children (i.e., the performance of the lowest scoring 10% less, evidence of sex differences in language abilities in
of the unaffected children); the rate of vocabulary learn- children with Down syndrome was noted in the results
ing was slower than general cognitive development from a parental questionnaire study (Buckley & Sacks,
among children with Down syndrome. Harris (1983) 1987). In children under the age of 14, 70% of the girls
found that children with Down syndrome between the and 46% of the boys used sentences of five or more words,
ages of 2;6 and 6;9 performed comparably to unaffected whereas in children over the age of 14, 77% of the girls
children between the ages of 1;5 and 2;5 on MLU. Fi- and 70% of the boys used such sentences. To our knowl-
nally, Fowler’s (1988) subject, Rebecca, reached the age edge, the only study directly addressing the issue of sex
2;4 MLU level of Brown’s (1973) subjects (Adam and differences in language skills is that of Beeghly et al.
Sarah) at about age 5;0. (1990). Those authors reported that girls tended to
The evidence strongly suggests developmental de- progress more rapidly than boys did.
lay among children with Down syndrome. Whereas the
first spoken words may appear at the expected time, Aims
spoken language development proceeds at a slower pace.
The age 1;6 level is reached at about ages 3;0 to 3;8 by The primary aim of this study was to describe the
children with Down syndrome. Further, there is large spoken-language development of children in a nation-
individual variation with respect to onset of language wide sample of Swedish children with Down syndrome
and later performance. and then to compare their development with that of
In the studies thus far reviewed a variety of lan- unaffected children in a normative group. The study
guage measures, such as size of vocabulary, MLU, the addressed three target problems: (1) growth trends and
McCarthy Scale, and expressive language scores, were individual variation in language performance, (2) sex
employed. Some findings suggest that there are irregu- differences in language performance, and (3) differences
larities in the development of language skills. From sev- between children in the normative group and children
eral studies, there are reports of specific limitations in with Down syndrome.
the development of syntax (both production and com-
prehension) but no such deficiencies in vocabulary
(Beeghly, Weiss-Perry, & Chapman, 1995; Chapman, Method
Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 1991; Cicchetti, 1990; Participants
Miller, 1995). Bates et al. (1995), for instance, reported
Group With Down Syndrome
dissociation between grammar scores and vocabulary
in children with Down syndrome (i.e., children with The group with Down syndrome comprised 330 chil-
Down syndrome had lower grammar scores than their dren (124 girls and 206 boys) between the ages of 1 and
unaffected peers despite matching vocabulary). However, 5;6 (see Table 1). The subjects constituted approximately
it was not clear whether the difference in rate of devel- 45% of all children with Down syndrome born in Swe-
opment between vocabulary and syntax (grammar) was den during 1991–1995. (The percentage is based on data
already present at the first stages of development. In from Sweden’s Medical Birth Register.) Our subjects
our study, therefore, we regarded it as crucial to exam- constitute an even larger proportion of the actual popu-
ine differences between children with Down syndrome lation because perinatal mortality among children with
and unaffected children on pragmatic skills, grammar Down syndrome is very high (as high as 10% to15%
skills, and maximum length of utterance (MaxLU) when over the first 5 years of life). For 102 of the children
vocabulary was treated as a covariate. (31%), parents indicated that their child had additional

Table 1. Number of children with Down syndrome as a function of age, gender, additional deficiencies, and use of manual signs.

Age in years;months

1 1;6 2 2;6 3 3;6 4 4;6 5 5;6 Total

Girls 12 14 15 8 11 19 11 15 11 8 124
Boys 29 17 15 15 20 17 37 21 19 16 206
Additional deficiencies 9 11 5 6 6 12 15 17 11 10 102
Manual signs 30 26 27 20 28 30 45 34 26 22 288
Total number of children 41 31 30 23 31 36 48 36 30 24 330

Berglund et al.: Language in Children With Down Syndrome 181

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
deficiencies: 14% had cardiac disorders, 7% had ear com- (2000), comprised 900 reports on 336 children from two
plaints or frequent otitis media, 15% had eye disorders, cohorts. Each child was measured on one to three occa-
and 5% had neurological or orthopedic problems. sions. The children were followed over 6 months. Paren-
Most of the children were enrolled in language- tal reports were requested three times during this period.
stimulation programs where speech and manual signs The chief inclusion criterion was that the child should be
were used simultaneously (n = 288, 87%). Among one- able to speak Swedish. A few children who lived abroad
year-olds, 73% used manual signs, whereas in the older and were recently internationally adopted or who were
age groups the proportions varied between 83% and 94%. diagnosed as developmentally delayed were excluded. A
Parents did not indicate which words their child signed. response rate of 88% was obtained. In total, 476 reports
Other communication aids were letters, used by 32 of on girls and 424 reports on boys were collected.
the children (10%), and computers, used by 28 (8%); both
methods were used more commonly by older children. Instrument and Procedure
Parents did not give more detailed descriptions of how The SECDI–w&s, as described in Berglund and
their children used letters or computers. Seven chil- Eriksson (2000), was used to obtain scores for vocabu-
dren used pictograms (2%), and one child used BLISS lary, pragmatic skills, and grammar skills. The vocabu-
symbols. lary score was computed from all words the parents had
Offices from all regions of Sweden responsible for indicated their child produced from a 710-item check-
services for children who are mentally delayed recruited list containing words from 21 semantic categories. De-
the subjects. Criteria for inclusion were specified age viation from standard pronunciation was accepted.
range (preschool age) and diagnosis of Down syndrome. Pragmatic skills is here used in a restricted sense to
The local offices distributed the SECDI to parents, who denote the ability to communicate about future or past
then returned the completed inventory to the research events, about absent objects or persons, and to express
group. If necessary, reminders were sent to parents by ownership, disregarding linguistic form. These achieve-
the local offices. Because we did not make a special re- ments increase the child’s competence “to do things with
quest for medical records, the data of children with such words” but do not themselves reflect different commu-
conditions as translocation or mosaicism are not reported nicative functions. The parents answered five questions
separately. about their child’s pragmatic skills. Two measures of
Before data collection, staff members at the local grammar were employed. The grammar score was cal-
offices were contacted by telephone in order to obtain culated from questions about whether the child com-
information on the number of potential subjects in the bined words and whether he or she used morphological
relevant ages. During data collection, approximate re- markers for the possessive form, definite singular, defi-
gional response rates were recorded. To examine whether nite plural, plural marking of nouns, and past tense of
there was any general effect of differential response rates verbs. The child received a score of 1 on items in the
on language and communicative skills, we compared pragmatic and grammar scales if the parent reported
children from regions with a response rate over 60% with that the child occasionally performed the target behav-
those from regions with a lower response rate. For this ior and a score of 2 if this occurred often. The MaxLU
comparison, we employed data on vocabulary perfor- score is a MLU count (in morphemes) of examples of
mance in that this is a measure of the earliest develop- three long utterances the parents recently had heard
ing language skill of the measures used in the present their child produce. Reliability scores for vocabulary,
study. A two-way ANOVA (age by response group) was pragmatic, and grammar were examined previously in
conducted, where children were divided into five groups the normative group. Intra-individual test-retest scores
according to age. Whereas the age effect was significant (i.e., percentage of items the child performed with a
(p < 0.01), indicating that older children obtained higher similar or larger score 3 months later) were calculated
scores, the response rate by region was not (p = 0.91). for the normative group (Berglund & Eriksson, 2000).
Thus, we aggregated the scores of children from regions The test-retest reliability scores for the 19–22 month
with different response rates in subsequent analyses. interval in children in the normative group are given
in Table 2 for each language skill measure. Also, the
Cronbach alpha scores for 22-month-old children (n =
The Normative Group
311) are specified.
The children with Down syndrome were compared
Additional information on the validity and reliabil-
with a representative sample of unaffected children be-
ity measures in relation to the SECDI–w&s was reported
tween the ages of 1;4 and 2;4. The sample was drawn
in Berglund and Eriksson (in press). The use of paren-
from the Swedish Birth Register and included two thirds
tal reports on language of mentally delayed children was
of all Swedish children born on two specific days. The
discussed in Berglund (1992) and in Miller, Sedley, and
data, which were derived from Berglund and Eriksson

182 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 2. Description of scales and measures. Reliability scores were the normative group between the ages of 16 and 25
intra-individual test-retest scores in the 19–22 month interval in months were used, as well as all data of children with
children in the normative group. Cronbach alpha was calculated in Down syndrome. To make the analysis feasible, scores
22-month-old children. of zero in vocabulary, pragmatic skills, and grammar
skills were changed to 0.01. Language development, in
No. Max Cronbach
measures yielding significant sex effects, were modelled
items score Reliability alpha
by using the best-fitting equations for each sex sepa-
Vocabulary 710 710 92% 0.99 rately. To describe development in measures without
Pragmatic skills 5 10 88% 0.81 significant sex effects, the total scores were used.
Grammar skills 6 12 93% 0.83
MaxLU 3

Note. MaxLU was MLU calculated over 1–3 long utterances written
Results
down by parents. A score of 0 was given if children did not use any The results of the growth trend and individual varia-
words. A score of 1 was given if the parent had not given any tion in language performance are reported separately
examples of long sentences and the child had a vocabulary of 1–100
for each of the four language measures. Next, we ex-
words (indicated in the vocabulary section). The MaxLU score was
plore age and sex differences in language skills. An
coded as missing if the parent had not given any examples of long
sentences and had indicated that the child combined words (item 6 -
evaluation of which regression functions best repre-
grammar skills) or if the parent had not given any examples of long sented the language skills of children was also under-
sentences and the child had a vocabulary of more than 100 words taken. Finally, we present the development of the prag-
(indicated in the vocabulary section). matic and grammar scales in children with Down
syndrome as they compare with those of children in the
normative group matched for vocabulary size or MaxLU.
Miolo (1995). Parents of children with Down syndrome
are able to describe the language skills of their children
and are as competent as parents of unaffected children. Developmental Trends
Vocabulary Scores
Statistical Procedures Some of the children with Down syndrome uttered
The children with Down syndrome were divided into their first spoken words by their first birthday. Propor-
10 age groups. These half-year groups were used for tions of children passing the milestones of 1, 2, 10, and
descriptions of developmental trends and individual 50 spoken words at various ages are shown in Table 3,
variations on the four language measures. Number and together with maximum language scores for each age
sex of children in each age group are given in Table 1. group. A majority of the children used more than 10
Developmental trends and individual variation were words at age 2. The 50-word milestone was passed by
described by median values and first and third quartile more than half of the children at about age 4.
for all language measures (vocabulary size, pragmatic Median vocabulary scores (50th percentile) increased
skills, grammar score, and MaxLU). In addition, the 10th steadily with age: 0 words in 1-year-olds, 10.5 words in
and 90th percentiles are also given for vocabulary size, 2-year-olds, 16 words in 3-year-olds, 53 words in 4-year-
the scale with the greatest variation. For the five age olds, and 198 words in 5-year-olds (see Figure 1). To il-
groups representing whole years, proportions of children lustrate the large individual differences, in each age
using more than 1, 2, 10, and 50 words were reported.
The same age groups were used to test age and sex ef-
fects in the ANOVAs for each of the language measures. Table 3. Percentage of children passing the 1-word, 2-word, 10-
word, and 50-word milestones and the maximum number of words
We also sought to determine which equation—lin- reported for the best-performing child in each age group.
ear, exponential, or logistic—best fit the data. Of course,
linguistic skills cannot proceed exponentially in the long Age in years
run. Proficient users acquire by definition few new fea-
tures. Hence, logistic equations have greater face valid- 1 2 3 4 5
ity than exponential functions in early language devel- 1 word 12 80 90 98 94
opment. However, logistic growth curves in the present 2 words 7 77 90 98 92
stages of language development might reflect ceiling 10 words 3 53 81 88 91
effects of the employed scales rather than a genuine 50 words 0 3 23 54 73
pattern of language acquisition. Therefore, we model Maximum number 11 71 165 668 655
both logistic and exponential equations as well as lin- of words
ear equations. In this analysis all data of children in

Berglund et al.: Language in Children With Down Syndrome 183

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 1. Developmental trends in vocabulary among children with group there were children who did not use any words at
Down syndrome: 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles. all, and there were also a number of older children who
had a vocabulary of more than 600 words.

Pragmatic Skills
Pragmatic skills were measured using five items
from the SECDI (maximum score 10 points). There
seems to be a ceiling effect for pragmatic scores, whereby
the 75th percentile lies close to the maximum score be-
yond age 4;6 (see Figure 2). Yet, 19% of the parents of
the 5-year-olds did not indicate that their children per-
formed any of the pragmatic activities. The pragmatic
skills section of the SECDI contains one item indicating
whether a child understands when the parent talks
about absent persons or objects. This was the first prag-
matic skill developed in most of the children and was
mastered by 19% of the children at age 1;6, by 40% at
age 2, 62% at age 3, 81% at age 4, and 79% at age 5.
Figure 2. Developmental trends in pragmatic skills among children
with Down syndrome 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles.
Grammar Score
The grammar score section of the SECDI comprises
six items, yielding a maximum score of 12 points. There
were examples of children who combined words (13%)
or used morphological markers (3%) at age 2, though
the overall median score for grammar skills did not rise
above 0 until after age 3;6 (see Figure 3).

MaxLU
A score of 1 on the MaxLU indicates that a child
uses single words only, and at least 25% of the children
in all age groups had a score of 1 (or did not use any
words). As a consequence the 25th percentile is not dis-
played in Figure 4. A higher score indicates that a child
uses at least two-word expressions (or two-morpheme
utterances). Most of the children age 4 and over com-
bined words; nonetheless, there were over 25% who did
Figure 3. Developmental trends in grammar skills among children
with Down syndrome: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles.
Figure 4. Developmental trends in Maximum Length of Utterance
among children with Down syndrome: 50th and 75th percentiles.

184 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 4. Results of two-way ANOVAs (age by sex) performed on Developmental Comparisons for the
the four communicative and language measures.
Different Language Measures
Age groups F Df p We examined the R2 values to evaluate which func-
tions were most efficient in representing the different
Vocabulary score 1, 2, 3, 4, 5
distributions. Three kinds of function (linear, exponen-
Age 23.79 4 <.01
Sex 4.49 1 <.05
tial, and logistic) were examined for each language mea-
Age × Sex 1.15 4 ns sure in both groups of children (see Table 5). The expo-
nential and logistic growth curves yielded identical
Pragmatic score 2, 3, 4, 5
values and had the highest R2 values for vocabulary size,
Age 15.54 3 <.01
Sex 7.48 1 <.01
grammar skills, and MaxLU, regardless of group. Fur-
Age × Sex .46 3 ns ther, linear functions yielded the best fit for pragmatic
skills, regardless of group. The ages when children with
Grammar score 3, 4, 5
Down syndrome and children in the normative group
Age 14.52 2 <.01
Sex 2.17 1 ns
performed similarly are illustrated in Figure 5, each
Age × Sex 1.55 2 ns panel of which illustrates either a linear or exponential
regression plot depending on which type yielded the
MaxLU score 3, 4, 5
highest explained variance.
Age 14.02 2 <.01
Sex 1.57 1 ns
Age × Sex .49 2 ns Table 5. The statistic characteristics for three kinds of regression
equation representing the growth (i.e., by age) factor for the four
communicative and language measures.
not combine words, even among those age 5;6. Median
group scores for maximum length of utterances suggest Group –
that there are wide plateaus in the syntax development Measure Equation R2 B constant
of children with Down syndrome. It should be noted,
however, that a few children performed at quite ad- DS – Vocabulary Linear .315 7.490 –194.05
Exponential .414 .0825 1.186
vanced levels.
Logistic .414 .921 .843
Pragmatic skills Linear .379 .146 –2.105
Effects of Age and Sex Exponential .307 .094 .023
To examine whether there were any effects of age Logistic .307 .910 43.463
and sex on the various language measures, two-way Grammar skills Linear .251 .105 –2.819
ANOVAs (age by sex) were performed. For vocabulary Exponential .382 .127 .0005
(the earliest developing skill), performance at ages 1, 2, Logistic .382 .880 1837.40
3, 4, and 5 was examined. Pragmatic skills (developing MaxLU Linear .314 .075 –.878
slightly after the first utterances) were analyzed for the Exponential .373 .030 .503
age 2, 3, 4, and 5 groups. For analyses of grammar and Logistic .373 .970 1.987
MaxLU scores (late-developing skills), the age 3, 4, and NG – Vocabulary Linear .347 30.806 –485.11
5 groups were considered. All analyses revealed signifi- Exponential .414 .298 .150
cant effects of age. The age effects were further explored Logistic .414 .742 6.666
by one-way ANOVAs and Tukey post hoc tests (p < .05). Pragmatic skills Linear .375 .589 –6.942
In the case of vocabulary, 1-, 2-, and 3-year-olds did not Exponential .159 .230 .025
differ significantly, yet 4-year-olds produced significantly Logistic .159 .794 39.858
higher scores than the younger children, and 5-year-olds Grammar skills Linear .315 .538 –8.714
produced significantly higher scores than 4-year-olds. Exponential .366 .556 3.4 × 10–6
In pragmatic skills, the scores of 2-, 3-, and 4-year olds Logistic .366 .574 296057
differed significantly, yet 4- and 5-year-olds did not dif- MaxLU Linear .347 .344 –4.427
fer significantly (close to ceiling). The grammar scores Exponential .393 .135 .132
of 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds differed significantly. Finally, Logistic .393 .874 7.584
in MaxLU, 3-year-olds produced significantly shorter
Note. For Vocabulary, Pragmatic, and Grammar skills a score of 0 was
utterances than 4- and 5-year-olds, who did not differ altered to a score of 0.01 to make possible the examination of logistic
significantly. and exponential functions. Children with Down syndrome (DS) age 1–5
The effects of sex for vocabulary and pragmatic skills years and children in the normative group (NG) age 16, 19, 22, and
were significant. Girls scored higher than boys did. No 25 months were included in the analysis.
age-by-sex interaction was revealed (see Table 4).

Berglund et al.: Language in Children With Down Syndrome 185

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 5. A. Estimated vocabulary development in girls and boys from the normative group (NG) and in children with Down syndrome (DS):
exponential growth. B. Estimated development of pragmatic skills in girls and boys from the NG group and in children with DS: linear
growth. C. Estimated development of grammar for children in the NG group and in children with DS: exponential growth. D. Estimated
development of MaxLU in children from the NG group and in children with DS: exponential growth.

A B

C D

Furthermore, we wanted to explore whether the these kinds of functions yielded highest explained vari-
early word growth could be described by a growth func- ance. The girls scored higher than the boys in both
tion other than an exponential one. To test this possibil- groups of children, and the differences between girls with
ity we performed an analysis in which only children with Down syndrome and boys with Down syndrome were
vocabularies of 50 or fewer words were included. (This larger than the differences between girls and boys in
is before the vocabulary spurt is expected to occur.) The the normative group for both vocabulary size and prag-
linear function (R2 = 0.181) and exponential and logistic matic skills.
functions yielded close results (R2 = 0.174). Hence, it
seems that linear, exponential, and logistic functions il- Developmental Differences: Pragmatic
lustrate the growth of the small vocabularies (<50 words) Scale, Grammar Scale, and MaxLU
about equally well.
Three stepwise regression analyses (with entering
Data for boys and girls are plotted separately in Fig- criterion of F < .05 and removal criterion of F > .10)
ure 5 (A and B), illustrating the developmental trends, were computed to explore differences between children
matched for age, for the skills with significant sex differ- with Down syndrome and children in the normative
ences (vocabulary size and pragmatic skills). In the vo- group on the pragmatic scale, on the grammar scale,
cabulary plot (A) we used exponential regression, and in and on MaxLU. Vocabulary size and sex were added to
the pragmatic plot (B) we used linear regression because the models in addition to group (children with Down

186 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
syndrome and children in the normative group) to con- Table 6. Stepwise regression analyses (vocabulary, group, and
trol the possible influences from these variables. Vocabu- sex) on MaxLU, the Grammar scale, and the Pragmatic scale
lary size entered first in all three equations, explaining significant variables.
most of the variance. Group entered all equations in the
second step. Although always significant, the increase Variable Df SE B b R2
in explained variance caused by group was small: .036 Pragmatic scale (model) 1221 .58
for the pragmatic scale, .004 for MaxLU, and .002 for Vocabulary .00 .69
the grammar scale. Children in the normative group Group .12 .20
with vocabulary equal to that of children with Down Grammar scale (model) 1102 .88
syndrome generally scored slightly higher on all three Vocabulary .00 .87
scales. Sex never entered the models (see Table 6). Group .12 .04
MaxLU (model) 1108 .59
Developmental Differences on Single Vocabulary .00 .75
Skills for Subjects Matched for Group .10 .07

Vocabulary
The order of development of single pragmatic skills
To further explore the differences between children
was strikingly similar between children, regardless of
with Down syndrome and children in the normative group,
group, when matched on vocabulary size. The main or-
logistic regressions were computed for the individual items
der was the following: “understood when parent talked
of the pragmatic scale and the grammar scale. Vocabu-
about absent objects,” “talked about absent owner,” “talked
lary size, MaxLU, and sex were included in the models
about absent object,” “talked about future events,” and
as covariates. These analyses revealed that children in
“talked about past events.” Also the order in develop-
the normative group more often than children with Down
ment of grammatical markers was strikingly similar
syndrome with a similar vocabulary size talked about
between children with Down syndrome and children in
ownership, talked about missing things, and understood
the normative group. Children first started to combine
talk about missing things. Further, children in the nor-
words; thereafter they used genitive markers; and use
mative group more often than children with Down syn-
of definite singulars, definite plurals, and indefinite plu-
drome marked genitives and the definite and indefinite
rals appeared before the use of past tense. Children also
forms in singular. Boys spoke slightly more about past
started to combine words at similar percentages over
times than girls did (see Table 7).
the different vocabulary size.

Table 7. Results of logistic regressions, with different kinds of single skills as dependent variables, and size
of vocabulary, MaxLU, sex, and group (Down syndrome vs. normative group) as covariates. Method:
forward entering–Wald.

Vocabulary MaxLU Sex Group

Wald R Wald R Wald R Wald R

Pragmatic skills
Speaks about …
passed time 138.42 .30 26.42 .13 4.75 .04 B NE
future 103.92 .26 39.18 .16 NE NE
missing things 78.54 .23 17.56 .10 NE 11.55 .08 NG
Understands talk about
missing things 61.18 .23 NE NE 74.10 .26 NG
Speaks about owners 59.64 .20 16.90 .10 NE 22.69 .12 NG

Grammar skills
Genitive markers 147.24 .33 17.82 .11 NE 5.33 .05 NG
Definite singular 268.33 .47 NE NE 9.05 .08 NG
Definite plural 130.39 .37 4.26 .05 NE NE
Indefinite singular 131.70 .36 15.46 .11 NE 7.62 .07 NG
Past tense 117.80 .36 12.24 .11 NE NE
Combines words 69.99 .23 89.80 .26 NE NE

Note. NE = not in equation. B = Boys highest. NG = Children from the normative group highest.

Berglund et al.: Language in Children With Down Syndrome 187

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
20-month-old unaffected children. Mundy et al. (1988)
Discussion compared children with Down syndrome at age 3;8 with
Growth Trends and Individual Variations unaffected children at age 1;6, thus making a compari-
in Language Performance son between groups that we found had matching results
in this study. Bates et al. (1995) reported that children
We found that the first word appears approximately with Down syndrome lagged approximately 2 years be-
one year after birth in about 10% of the children with hind unaffected subjects over the period covered by CDI:
Down syndrome and that after 2 years 80% had begun Toddlers, and our results are also in line with this find-
to talk. Furthermore, some children with Down syn- ing. In our study, 3-year-olds lagged by about 20 months
drome start to talk at about the same age as children in and 4-year-olds by about 28 months.
the normative group (cf. Eriksson & Berglund, 1999).
Smith et al. (1988) obtained similar results for 5-
In this respect, we agree with Chapman (1995) that the
year-old children with Down syndrome and unaffected
rate of acquisition is slower in most children but not
children at age 2;6 in terms of performance on the
necessarily the onset of lexical acquisition. However,
McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. However, they
between 10% and 20% of the children with Down syn-
found that delay in language skills of children with Down
drome had less than 10 words in their spoken vocabu-
syndrome was even more prolonged. This finding seems
laries when they were 3 to 5 years old, and there were
comparable to our result regarding magnitude of delay.
some children who had not started to talk at all, regard-
Harris (1983) found the MLU of children with Down
less of age.
syndrome between ages 2;6 and 6;9 years was compa-
The 50-word milestone was achieved by about 25% rable to that of unaffected children between ages 1;5
of the children with Down syndrome at age 3, by about and 2;5. Our results indicate that about 5% to 10% of
50% at age 4, and about 75% at age 5. Thus, a substan- children with Down syndrome combine words in the
tial proportion of the children in our study attained this period from age 2 to 2;6 and that about 70% do so at age
level at an earlier age than Gillham’s (1990) subjects. 5;6. In this respect, Harris’s findings are in accord with
At age 3;0 our subjects had an average vocabulary of ours.
36.0 words (range: 0–165 words), which can be compared
In the comparison of different growth functions, we
with the Stroeminger et al. (1984) finding of a mean of
observed almost identical patterns in the children with
18.5 words (range: 0–85 words). The discrepancy partly
Down syndrome and the children in the normative
might be explained by the use of different language
group, suggesting common growth patterns across chil-
measures in the two studies.
dren independent of the presence of dysfunction. An ex-
The majority (87%) of children in this study used ponential or a logistic curve yielded the highest ex-
speech and manual signs simultaneously. Therefore, the plained variance (R2) in three of the measures, whereas
children with Down syndrome with the smallest spo- the development of pragmatic skills was better described
ken vocabularies are slightly underestimated in terms by a linear equation. That the pragmatic scale was dif-
of the size of their “total” vocabularies in relation to chil- ferent from the other scales suggests that the develop-
dren in the normative group. However, when the vocabu- ment measured by the pragmatic scale may be governed
laries increase in size, children with Down syndrome by other cognitive or linguistic principles. However, more
mostly add spoken words to their repertoire (Miller et data on the development of pragmatic skills and their
al., 1991, cited in Chapman, 1995). Hence, the higher relations to other measures are needed before this issue
vocabulary scores in the present study should be close can be settled conclusively.
to the total vocabularies of children with Down syn-
Robinson and Mervis (1998) have explored indi-
drome. However, it would be expedient of future studies
vidual growth curves of vocabularies in children with
in this area to assess the children’s manual vocabular-
Williams or Down syndrome. They found that a logistic
ies as well.
growth curve was most typical and that growth rate was
Our language predictions from linear regression and negatively correlated with a linear fit. However, in 3 of
exponential equations indicate that the performance of the 5 children with Down syndrome in the Robinson and
36-month-old children with Down syndrome parallels Mervis study, language development was best charac-
the performance of unaffected children at about 16 terized by linear growth. In the present study, we found
months old (see Figure 5[A–D]). This finding confirms that several of the measures are best described by expo-
the result of Mervis (1990) that, on average, 3-year-old nential or logistic curves. As we have shown here, lin-
children with Down syndrome perform similarly to un- ear, exponential, and logistic equations yield about the
affected children at age 1;6. same explained variance before the 50-word milestone
We noted that 48-month-old children with Down is reached. Thus, it seems that the Robinson and Mervis
syndrome performed at a level approaching that of findings are compatible with our group data.

188 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
The Significance of Sex Differences of children with Down syndrome to talk about absent
in Language Skill objects, owners, and past and future situations. They
just did it a little later than the children in the norma-
Our study confirms earlier findings of sex differences
tive group.
in language skills of children with Down syndrome
(Beeghly et al., 1990; Buckley & Sacks, 1987). Signifi- Limitations of the Study
cant sex differences in our study were found for both
vocabulary and pragmatic skills, on which girls on av- Because the study used a parental questionnaire, in-
erage perform better than boys do. However, in contrast formation dealing with nonverbal mental age could not
to Fenson et al. (1994) among others who reported small be obtained. For children who have small spoken vocabu-
but consistent differences between unaffected boys and laries and who use signs to facilitate communication, their
girls, Berglund and Eriksson (2000) and Eriksson and true vocabularies might have been underestimated.
Berglund (1999) found no differences between boys and Another limitation is that because we used paren-
girls among unaffected Swedish-speaking children. It tal questionnaires for the whole population of preschool
therefore becomes important to ask if there are factors children with Down syndrome in Sweden, it was not fea-
associated with Down syndrome that are responsible for sible to obtain medical records. Thus, such important
the superior language performance of girls as compared information as hearing status was not available. None-
to boys among children with Down syndrome. One such theless, we did ask parents a global question about the
set of factors would be if boys with Down syndrome had health conditions of their child. Berglund (1998) ana-
more additional deficiencies than girls with Down syn- lyzed the potential detrimental effects that additional
drome. Yet, in analyzing additional deficiencies in the deficiencies have on vocabulary scores. If a child had
present study group, Berglund (1998) found no differ- ear disorders or a hearing impairment, this was signifi-
ence in the distribution between boys and girls. We there- cantly associated with increased risk for low vocabulary
fore suggest that more research should be devoted to scores, but only when the child had other deficiencies
explaining the sex differences in language skills among (e.g., eye disorders, cardiac disorders, or neurological or
children with Down syndrome. orthopaedic problems). Having an ear or hearing disor-
ders alone did not yield a significantly increased rela-
Language Skills Among Children tive risk for low vocabulary scores. Obviously, these find-
With Down Syndrome and Children ings suggest that medical records on hearing or screening
of hearing status (and the presence of other deficien-
in the Normative Group
cies) should be included in language studies of children
The order of development of single items on the with Down syndrome.
grammar and the pragmatic scales was strikingly simi-
lar on the part of children with Down syndrome and Implications of Findings on Language
children in the normative group. However, when matched Therapy in Children With Down
on vocabulary size, children with Down syndrome lagged
Syndrome
behind children in the normative group on the pragmatic
scale, on the grammar scale, and on MaxLU. This effect The timetables from this study on the development
was small but was probably somewhat underestimated of language skills in children with Down syndrome are
because only the spoken vocabularies of children with clinically useful. Using timetables makes it possible to
Down syndrome were counted. Their total vocabular- give parents a reasonable idea about the language de-
ies with manual signs included would have been larger, lay in their child and what to expect in the development
thus emphasizing the lag (cf. the results on lexical com- of spoken vocabulary. Because of significant sex differ-
prehension and gestures by Caselli et al., 1998). A lag ences in language skills, parents need to be informed
in grammar skills is in accord with the findings of Miller, about how this affects their child. According to the mea-
Miolo, Sedey, and Murray-Branch (1993, cited in sures used here, children with Down syndrome are mainly
Chapman, 1995) and Wiegel-Crump (1981). The lag in delayed and only marginally deviant in their language
pragmatic skills among children with Down syndrome skills. However, the vocabulary develops slightly ahead
as compared with children in the normative group of pragmatic and grammar skills. Therefore, it seems
matched on vocabulary size was actually greater than important that language stimulation programs pay spe-
the lag in grammar skills. A lag in pragmatic skills is cial attention to pragmatic and grammar skills.
related to the often-cited restriction among children The form of the developmental function for various
with Down syndrome to “talk about here and now” (cf. linguistic skills also has possible implications for lan-
Layton & Sharifi, 1978; Leudar, Fraser, & Jeeves, 1981; guage stimulation programs. Other things being equal,
Rondal, 1978, all cited in Chapman, 1995). However, the a small improvement in a skill with an exponential or
present findings do not suggest an inability on the part logistic function yields a higher later payoff than an

Berglund et al.: Language in Children With Down Syndrome 189

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
improvement of equivalent size in a linearly developing Berglund, E. (1998). The relative risk of low vocabulary
skill. Taken together with the argument above and the scores among children with Down syndrome and addi-
tional deficiencies (No. 847). Reports from the Department
present finding of a linear development of pragmatic of Psychology, Stockholm University. Stockholm, Sweden.
skills a good case can be made for developing programs
Berglund, E., & Eriksson M. (2000). Communicative
directed at stimulating grammar skills in children with development in Swedish children 16–28 months old: The
Down syndrome. However, other things are rarely equal, Swedish Early Communicative Development Inventory–
and there are probably causal links between different words and sentences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
linguistic skills. For example, it seems reasonable that 41, 133–144.
a basic categorization of the world in the past, in the Berglund, E., & Eriksson, M. (in press). Reliability and
present, and in the future, as assessed by the pragmatic content validity of a new instrument for assessment of
scale, is necessary before the particular grammatical communicative skills and language abilities in young
Swedish children. Logopedics, Phoniatrics, Vocology.
form that marks these categories in language can be
acquired. If this is true, training programs should focus Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
on the pragmatic skills before tackling grammar skills.
A lexicon-driven model for the acquisition of grammar Buckley, S., & Sacks, B. (1987). The adolescent with
Down’s syndrome: Life for the teenager and for the family.
is also consistent with the present findings. However, Portsmouth, U.K.: Portsmouth Down’s Syndrome Trust.
such a model must explain why children with Down syn-
Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, L.,
drome in general need a somewhat larger vocabulary Pizzoli, C., & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in
than children in the normal group to reach a correspond- early development of children with Down syndrome.
ing level of grammar. A likely explanation would be in Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41,
terms of weaker representational or computational re- 1125–1135.
sources in children with Down syndrome. Chapman, R. S. (1995). Language development in children
and adolescents with Down syndrome. In P. Fletcher & B.
MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp.
Acknowledgments 641–663). Oxford, U.K.: Basil Blackwell.
Chapman, R. S., Schwartz, S. E., & Kay-Raining Bird,
The research in this paper was supported by grants
E. (1991). Language skills of children and adolescents
from the Centre for Caring Sciences, Uppsala University; with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal of
the Dalarna Research Institute; the Mayflower Foundation; Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1106–1120.
the Research and Development Department of Gävleborg
County Council; Falun College of Health and Caring Chapman, R. S., Seung, H. K., Schwartz, S. E., & Kay-
Sciences; and the department of Caring Sciences at Gävle Raining Bird, E. (1998). Language skills of children and
adolescents with Down syndrome: II. Production deficits.
University. The authors are indebted to Professor Håkan
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41,
Stattin at the Department of Psychology, Örebro University,
861–873.
for valuable methodological advice and comments on the
manuscript. Professor Gunilla Preisler at the Department of Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early
Psychology, Stockholm University, also provided useful communicative development. First Language, 19, 55–90.
comments on the manuscript; and Professor Lennart Bodin, Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D. J., Bates,
Örebro University, helped with the statistics. Tomas E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1993).
Berglund, Gothenburg University, helped with coding of Communicative development inventories: User’s guide and
the data. technical manual. San Diego: Singular.
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal,
D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early
References communicative development. Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development, 59.
Bates, E., Dale, P. S., & Thal, D. (1995). Individual
differences and their implications for theories of language Fowler, A. E. (1988). Determinants of rate of language
development. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The growth in children with DS. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The
handbook of child language (pp. 96–151). Oxford, U.K.: psychobiology of Down syndrome (pp. 217–245). Cam-
Basil Blackwell. bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Beeghly, M., Weiss-Perry, B., & Cicchetti, D. (1990). Gillham, B. (1990). First words in normal and Down
Beyond sensorimotor functioning: Early communicative syndrome children: A comparison of content and word-
and play development of children with Down syndrome. In form categories. Child Language Teaching and Therapy,
D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children with Down 6, 25–32.
syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 329–368). Harris, J. (1983). What does mean length of utterance
Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. mean? Evidence from a comparative study of normal and
Berglund, E. (1992). The validity of parental and teacher Down’s syndrome children. British Journal of Disorders of
predictions of the word knowledge of retarded and non- Communication, 18, 153–169.
retarded children (No. 2). Falun, Sweden: Falun Univer- Layton, T., & Sharifi, H. (1978). Meaning and structure of
sity College of Health Sciences. Down’s syndrome and nonretarded children’s spontaneous

190 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 44 • 179–191 • February 2001

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
speech. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 83, communication (pp. 335–362). Baltimore: Brookes
439–445. Publishing.
Leudar, I., Fraser, W. I., & Jeeves, M. A. (1981). Social Robinson, B. F., & Mervis, C. B. (1998). Vocabulary growth
familiarity and communication in Down syndrome. curves of children with Williams or Down syndrome. Paper
Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 25, 133–142. presented at the 19th Symposium on Research in Child
Mervis, C. B. (1990). Early conceptual development of Language Disorders. University of Wisconsin–Madison,
children with Down syndrome. In D. Cicchetti & M. June 4–6.
Beeghly (Eds.), Children with Down syndrome: A develop- Rondal, J. (1978). Maternal speech to normal and Down’s
mental perspective (pp. 252–301). Cambridge, U.K.: syndrome children matched for mean length of utterance.
Cambridge University Press. In C. E. Meyers (Ed.), Quality of life in severely and
Miller, J. (1995). Individual differences in vocabulary profoundly mentally retarded people: Research foundations
acquisition in children with Down syndrome. In Etiology for improvement (pp. 6–7). Washington, DC: American
and pathogenesis of Down syndrome (pp. 93–103). New Association on Mental Deficiency.
York: Wiley-Liss. Smith, L., von Tetzchner, S., & Michalsen, B. (1988). The
Miller, J. F., Miolo, G., Sedey, A., & Murray-Branch, J. emergence of language skills in young children with Down
(1993). The emergence of multiword combinations in syndrome. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The psychobiology of Down
children with Down syndrome. Poster presented at the syndrome (pp. 145–165). New York: Bradford.
Symposium for Research in Child Language Disorders, Stroeminger, A. Z., Winkler, M. R., & Cohen, L. T. (1984).
Madison, WI. Speech and language evaluation. In S. M. Pueschel (Ed.),
Miller, J. F., Sedley, A. L., & Miolo, G. (1995). Validity of The young child with Down syndrome (pp. 253–261). New
parent report measures of vocabulary development for York: Human Sciences Press.
children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech and Wiegel-Crump, C. A. (1981). The development of grammar
Hearing Research, 38, 1037–1044. in Down’s syndrome children between the mental ages of
Miller, J. F., Sedey, A., Miolo, G., Rosin, M., & Murray- 2–0 and 6–11 years. Education and Training of the
Branch, J. (1991). Spoken and sign vocabulary acquisi- Mentally Retarded, February, 24–30.
tion in children with Down syndrome. Poster presented at
the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Received March 17, 1999
convention, Atlanta.
Accepted October 20, 2000
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Yirmiya, N. (1988).
Nonverbal communication skills in Down syndrome DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/016)
children. Child Development, 59, 235–249. Contact author: Eva Berglund, PhD, Uppsala University
Pueschel, S. M., & Hopmann, M. R. (1993). Speech Hospital, Department of Nursing Research and Develop-
and language abilities of children with Down syndrome. ment, S–751 85, Uppsala, Sweden.
In A. Kaiser & D. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing children’s Email: eva.berglund@adm.uas.lul.se

Berglund et al.: Language in Children With Down Syndrome 191

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 02/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like