You are on page 1of 12

c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Exploratory Report

Complexity in neuropsychological assessments of


cognitive impairment: A network analysis approach

Giorgia Tosi a,b, Carolina Borsani b, Stefania Castiglioni b, Roberta Daini a,c,
Massimo Franceschi b and Daniele Romano a,c,*
a
University of Milano-Bicocca, Psychology Department, Milano MI, Italy
b
MultiMedica Hospital, Castellanza (VA), Department of Neurology, Castellanza VA, Italy
c
NeuroMi, Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milano-Bicocca, Dept. of Neurology and Neuroscience, San
Gerardo Hospital, Monza MB, Italy

article info abstract

Article history: In a neuropsychological assessment, each test aims at measuring a single cognitive function.
Received 19 March 2019 However, test performance depends on an interconnected system of cognitive functions and
Reviewed 8 June 2019 individual characteristics. For a better understanding of cognitive deficits, it is fundamental
Revised 29 July 2019 to recognize this complexity and study the relationships between test performances.
Accepted 8 November 2019 This study aims to evaluate complexity in neuropsychological assessment through
Action editor Rob McIntosh network analysis (NA) in 165 healthy older adults, 191 patients with Alzheimer's disease
Published online 27 November 2019 (AD), and 129 patients with vascular encephalopathy (VaE).
NA is a flexible method to explore different domains where many variables are corre-
Keywords: lated with each other, and the relationships are key for understanding the domains. We
Dementia included general aspects of individual differences (i.e., age, sex, years of education) and the
Network analysis raw scores of a clinically used neuropsychological battery in the network.
Alzheimer Healthy subjects showed a segregated pattern, suggesting a good specificity of each test
Neuropsychological assessment in measuring a specific cognitive function. Moreover, the scores were related to age and
education. In the patient groups, the identified patterns changed in a consistent manner,
showing less specificity and new relationships between the various tests, thereby reducing
the impact of age and education on the performance. In particular, AD patients showed
worse performance on the tests but also a different balance between different tasks,
suggesting a reorganization of the cognitive system and not a mere decline.
These results provide a new perspective in looking at the complexity of cognitive
function assessment.
© 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

* Corresponding author. University of Milano-Bicocca, Psychology Department, Piazza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126, Milano MI, Italy.
E-mail addresses: g.tosi3@campus.unimib.it (G. Tosi), carolina.borsani@multimedica.it (C. Borsani), stefania.castiglioni@multi-
medica.it (S. Castiglioni), roberta.daini@unimib.it (R. Daini), massimo.franceschi1@gmail.com (M. Franceschi), daniele.romano@unimib.
it (D. Romano).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.004
0010-9452/© 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
86 c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

information to be investigated. A network is a model


1. Introduction composed of a set of nodes, representing entities, and a set of
edges that connect the nodes, which represent their relations
The brain is a system of interacting complex functional net-
(e.g., De Nooy, Mrvar, & Batagelj, 2011). NA provides a large
works (Buckner, Hrienen, & Yeo, 2013), and each neurological
pull of information about the variables under investigation
deficit usually affects more than a single cognitive domain.
and their relationships. For instance, the number of edges, the
In the past few years, advances in functional neuroimaging
number of negative and positive edges and their weights
have provided new tools to examine the complexity of the
(i.e., the strength of the connection). In clinical psychology, NA
interactions between brain regions, allowing the identification
has been recently proposed as a novel theoretical approach to
of connectivity patterns in the human brain (van den Heuvel &
personality and psychopathology studies (Costantini et al.,
Hulshoff Pol, 2010). Specifically, many studies have used graph
2015), and the intention of the current paper is to extend the
theory to analyse functional connectivity patterns in patients
use of this technique to the neuropsychological domain
with Alzheimer's disease (Huang, Lin, & Lin, 2016; Lehmann
(Kellermann et al., 2016).
et al., 2016) and other types of cognitive impairment
The employment of NA on the scores of various neuropsy-
(Hafkemeijer et al., 2015) to associate dementia with complex
chological tests can allow an evaluation of how they are related
patterns of dysfunction. A similar approach to complexity is
to different conditions in healthy and pathological older adults.
lacking for the neuropsychological investigation of both
NA results provide a novel perspective to approach neuropsy-
healthy and pathological ageing.
chological assessment in dementia in two ways. First, the score
In clinical neuropsychology, it is necessary to consider the
of one test (or a set of tests) can be significantly lower in those
entirety of patient performance on cognitive tests to distin-
with dementia than in healthy elderly individuals. Second, we
guish healthy elderly individuals from those with cognitive
suggest that there could be possible changes in the relations
impairment and different forms of dementia. Single test scores
between the different cognitive functions. These altered
and performances on their own are poorly informative and
relations would provide clues about the reorganization of the
need to be integrated into a wider collection of information
cognitive system, providing evidence-based support for the
(age, sex, anamnesis, other tests score, errors type and
clinical detection of different patterns of cognitive functioning.
execution modality, laboratory exams, neuroimaging) to
In this paper, NA was adopted to evaluate the balance
adequately assess the condition. Therefore, the quantitative
among the test performances in three groups of individuals:
data given by the tests must be integrated with a qualitative
healthy elderly individuals, patients affected by AD, and
analysis of the performance (Mathias & Burke, 2009;
patients with sub-cortical vascular encephalopathy (VaE). AD
Mitrushina, Boone, Razani, & D'Elia, 2005). In this framework,
and VaE patients were considered models of “cortical” and
in analogy with the interacting networks of brain regions, a
“sub-cortical” cognitive impairment, respectively.
model of the relations among neuropsychological test perfor-
mances could be useful for understanding the patient's
cognitive profile. Such an approach integrates a wider range of 2. Materials and methods
information in a manner that may not immediately evident
and more systematically considers the underlying complexity. 2.1. Participants
Van Der Maas and collaborators (2006) have shown that
performances on cognitive tasks, measured with neuropsy- A retrospective analysis of the medical records of the
chological tests, positively correlate with each other, sug- Neurology Department of the MultiMedica Hospital of
gesting that people who score well on one cognitive test are Castellanza (Varese, Italy) from 2007 to 2017 was conducted.
likely to score well on the others. In a neuropsychological We selected only patients who completed the extensive bat-
assessment, clinicians are well aware that each test requires tery of tests for the clinical examination (see below). We
the contribution of many processes and that those processes extracted two groups of patients from the database for this
have to be spared to bring out the activity of the targeted study: patients with AD and patients with VaE. The AD group
cognitive function. For example, the performances in tests included patients fulfilling diagnostic criteria for clinically
meant to assess verbal working memory are influenced by probable AD (McKhann et al., 2011). Exclusion criteria were (i)
language comprehension and vice versa (Baddeley, 2003). age below 50 years old, (ii) presence of central nervous system
For a better understanding of cognitive impairment, it is diseases (i.e., stroke, traumatic injury), and (iii) presence of
fundamental to recognize these connections and to determine another psychological disorder (i.e., depression, anxiety) or a
how they could change in different conditions. Indeed, it is history of intellectual disability.
likely that the cognitive impairment caused by Alzheimer's The VaE group included patients with clinically and
disease (AD) not only reduces the proper functioning of one or neuroimaging-documented sub-cortical cerebrovascular
more specific cognitive functions (Weintraub, Wicklund, & disease (Erkinjuntti et al., 2000a; b; Sachdev et al., 2014).
Salmon, 2012) but also might alter the balance among them, We based the selection of patients on brain imaging fea-
as reflected by the different tests. tures and included subjects with sub-cortical vascular im-
How can we quantify this balance of relations? pairments clinically documented by neurologic diagnosis and
A flexible method that is potentially useful for exploring cerebral imaging according to the gold standard of clinical
relations among different domains is network analysis (NA). neuropsychology literature (Erkinjuntti et al., 2000a, b). All
NA is especially useful to explore domains where many patients showed sub-cortical damage, including sub-cortical
variables correlate, and their relationships are the core complete infarctions, lacunar infarcts and small or micro-
c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6 87

infarcts (Kalaria & Erkinjuntti, 2006). Moreover, the presence figuredcopy and delayed recall (Caffarra et al., 2002a); digit
of a cognitive disorder and the exclusion of other factors spandforward and backward (Monaco, Costa, Caltagirone, &
accounting for the cognitive deficits (including AD pathology) Carlesimo, 2013); prose memory test (Novelli, et al., 1986);
were encompassed by the neurologists' clinical judgement. Corsi spandforward and backward (Monaco et al., 2013); Ra-
Additional exclusion criteria were the same as the AD ven's progressive matrices (Basso et al., 1987; Raven, 1947);
group. Only patients who completed the entire battery of tests Stroop test accuracy and time performance (Caffarra,
were included in the study. Vezzadini, Dieci, Zonato, & Venneri, 2002b); Trail Making
We identified two segregated groups: 191 patients with AD Testdpart A and part B, the latter was scored as over or under
(mean age: 75.76 ± 6.71; mean years of education: 7.04 ± 3.58) the cut-off (Giovagnoli et al., 1996).
and 129 patients with VaE (mean age: 75.98 ± 7.22; mean years The retrospective nature of the study prevented matching
of education: 6.53 ± 3.43). the tests included in the assessment of the healthy and the
All neurological diagnoses were double-checked (through patient groups. Although this is a limitation of the current
revision of clinical and instrumental recordings) by the clin- work, the cognitive domains investigated by the two sets of
ical neuropsychologists (the authors CB and SC) and then tests were the same, so a qualitative comparison may be
further confirmed by an experienced neurologist specialized considered informative. Future studies adopting a forward-
in dementia assessment (the author MF). looking approach will be able to directly compare the healthy
Additionally, we included 165 healthy elderly individuals in and pathological conditions.
our study (mean age: 69.33 ± 5.94; mean years of education: The following cognitive domains were investigated in both
10.88 ± 3.90). These subjects completed a neuropsychological the healthy and pathological participants: language (compre-
evaluation for data collection as part of a local interventional hension and lexical access), memory (visuospatial and verbal;
project at the University of Milano-Bicocca. short- and long-term), visuoconstructive praxis, attention and
The exclusion criteria for this cohort were the same as for executive functions.
the patient groups.
An experienced neuropsychologist (the author RD) super- 2.3. Statistical analysis
vised the selection of healthy participants.
Data and analysis code are available on the Open Science The analyses were performed in an R environment (R Core
Framework platform at the following link: https://osf.io/ Team, 2017) employing the specific package bootnet by two
8wn7e/?view_only¼2569a529e9424b0d86a31dcc05daa0fe. authors (GT and DR) who were not involved in the case
No part of the study procedures or analysis was pre- selection or evaluation.
registered prior to the research being conducted. To describe the patterns of similarity and divergence be-
tween the groups, we examined age, education, sex and test
2.2. Neuropsychological assessment scores in the AD, VaE and healthy subjects.

In the case of patients with multiple evaluations, we consid- 2.3.1. Network analysis
ered the first one that allowed the neurologist to make the A commonly used network model for estimating psychologi-
diagnosis. cal networks is a pairwise Markov random field (PMRF;
The neuropsychological battery included the following tests: Costantini et al., 2015; Van Borkulo et al., 2014). A PMRF is a
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein, & network where nodes describe variables related by edges,
McHugh, 1975); semantic fluency test (Spinnler & Tognoni, indicating conditional dependence between them. Variables
1987); phonemic fluency test (Novelli, Capitani, Vallar, & that are not connected after the weighting of the other vari-
Cappa, 1986); token test (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); drawings ables are considered to be independent (Epskamp, Borsboom,
copy (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); ReyeOsterrieth complex fig- & Fried, 2018). In our work, we used a Gaussian graphical
uredcopy and delayed recall (Caffarra, Vezzadini, Dieci, Zonato, model (GGM; Costantini et al., 2015; Lauritzen, 1996), in which
& Venneri, 2002a); digit span forward (Orsini et al., 1987); prose edges correspond to partial correlation coefficients, which
memory testdimmediate and delayed recall (Spinnler & provides the remaining association between any two variables
Tognoni, 1987); Corsi span (Orsini et al., 1987); semantically after conditioning on all other variables.
related words list learningdimmediate recall, delayed recall This method employs the “least absolute shrinkage and
and detection through distractors (Mauri et al., 1997); Raven's selection operator” (LASSO; Tibshirani, 1996), a form of regu-
progressive matrices (Basso, Capitani, & Laiacona, 1987; Raven, larising penalty that treats positive and negative edge weights
1947); attentive matrices (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); Trail Mak- equally, leading small connections to shrink to zero (Epskamp
ing Testdpart A and part B, the latter was scored as over or et al., 2018; McNeish, 2015). The rationale behind the LASSO is
under the cut-off (Giovagnoli et al., 1996). to return a conservative network model. If an edge is not set to
All the tests are included in the standard assessment used zero, it indicates that it is sufficiently robust to be included in
at the neurology department of the MultiMedica Hospital of the model. This robust method may have a low sensitivity
Castellanza (VA, Italy) and built for the assessment of cogni- (i.e., not all real edges are detected), but it has a high specificity
tive impairments. (i.e., few false positives) (Epskamp, Kruis, & Marsman, 2017),
Healthy participants were administered the following suggesting a conservative and reliable approach.
tests: MoCA (Conti, Bonazzi, Laiacona, Masina, & Vanelli, The NA, together with the partial correlation coefficients of
2015); semantic fluency test (Novelli, et al., 1986); phonemic the edges, provides other indices. A crucial factor is the
fluency test (Novelli, et al., 1986); ReyeOsterrieth complex sparsity of the network, which is defined as the proportion of
88 c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

existing relationships compared to the number of total had more years of scholastic education (10.45; 11.31). More-
possible connections between the variables (range 0e1). over, AD patients and healthy participants were characterized
Sparsity provides clues about the general connectivity of the by a higher percentage of females (AD: 68.59%; healthy:
nodes; that is, a low sparsity value suggests a network with 73.33%) compared to the VaE group (45.74%).
few specific connections, while a high sparsity value means Table 1 reports the mean and the confidence intervals for
that there are many connections in the network implying each test score in the two groups of patients. Globally, the VaE
their low specificity. To clarify the counterintuitive nature of patients seemed to perform better than the AD patients at the
the term, it can be described as an edge density index. A group level.
sparse network is a totally connected graph since it is char-
acterized by a high proportion of existing relationships rela- 3.1. Network analysis
tive to the number of total possible connections. Additionally,
local patterns could be investigated through centrality mea- NA conveys very different information, and every single node
sures. Centrality indices can be conceived as operationalisa- can be the target of specific interest and comments. Here, we
tions of node importance (Costantini et al., 2015). They are propose a possible path to read the NA results, which, in our
determined by the relative importance of the focal node in the opinion, are worthy of discussion. We estimated the best
specific network estimated. Among the numerous indices of fitting network for each group. Then, we investigated the
centrality that have been proposed, we focused on the shape of the network and its sparsity as an index of its devi-
following measures: ation from a totally connected (i.e., sparse) graph (Goswami,
Murthy, & Das, 2018). We then discussed the edges and cen-
- Betweenness is the number of shortest paths between any trality indices of a few core nodes to verify how different
two nodes that pass through the focal node; this measure cognitive conditions impact the existing relationships be-
reveals whether the node has a mediation role and facili- tween the neuropsychological performances. Namely, we
tates the connectivity among different nodes; initially deepened the study of age and years of education,
- Strength is the sum of the weights of the connections of the followed by the study of the screening tests. Finally, we
focal node in absolute value; this measures the impact of focused on unexpected or missing connections. It is important
the focal node on its neighbours. to note that, because of the exploratory nature of NA, the
examination of specific nodes and their edges can be per-
The nodes in our NA (18 in the healthy group and 23 in AD formed on any specific node and connection. In this work, the
and VaE groups) were the raw scores of the neuropsycholog- focus was on screening tests for two reasons: first because
ical tests described above, the sex, the age at the time of the they are an example of the underlying logic that can be
evaluation and the years of scholastic education. adopted to interpret the results and second because of their
importance in the daily clinical use. Any reader, interested in
other nodes, can focus his/her attention on that node and
3. Results and discussion explore its specific connections.

Both groups of patients had similar ages [AD (lower bound of 3.1.1. Healthy participants
95% CI: 74.93; upper bound of 95% CI: 76.41); VaE (75.19; 76.77)] The 18 nodes in the network created 86 edges, with a sparsity
and years of education [AD (6.64; 7.43); VaE (6.15; 6.90)]. In index of .56 and the presence of few moderate to strong re-
contrast, the healthy subjects were younger (68.68; 69.98) and lations (Fig. 1B). A proportion of .56 existing connections

Table 1 e Average scores and 95% confidence intervals for neuropsychological tests, divided by the patient groups.

Test AD score lower CI upper CI VaE score lower CI upper CI


MMSE 20.87 20.52 21.23 24.26 23.92 24.61
Phonemic Fluency 16.59 15.60 17.58 18.78 17.92 19.65
Semantic Fluency 10.91 10.53 11.29 13.17 12.69 13.65
Token test 27.58 27.22 27.94 28.50 28.15 28.85
Digit span 4.71 4.61 4.82 4.62 4.52 4.72
Memory e Immediate Recall 1.62 1.42 1.81 3.28 3.04 3.53
Memory e Delayed Recall 1.17 .97 1.37 3.41 3.14 3.69
Learning e Immediate Recall 19.60 18.78 20.43 26.55 25.41 27.70
Learning e Immediate Recall (Intrusions) 3.19 2.79 3.59 3.08 2.68 3.47
Learning e Delayed Recall 1.28 1.04 1.52 4.23 3.82 4.65
Learning e Delayed Recall (Intrusions) .66 .51 .82 1.04 .87 1.21
Learning e Detection 21.24 20.79 21.69 24.33 23.78 24.88
Corsi span 3.74 3.65 3.83 3.88 3.79 3.96
Rey-OsterriethdCopy 18.71 17.80 19.61 19.95 19.10 20.80
Rey-OsterriethdDelayed Recall 2.43 2.11 2.74 6.39 5.80 6.98
Drawings Copy 9.80 9.57 10.03 9.52 9.31 9.73
Attentional Matrices 32.54 31.41 33.68 35.39 34.20 36.57
Raven Matrices 16.79 16.24 17.34 18.28 17.60 18.96
TMT Part A 131.99 123.54 140.44 110.05 103.86 116.25
c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6 89

compared to the number of total possible connections (i.e., betweenness and strength that suggested its role in facili-
sparsity index) revealed relatively high specificity of the tating node connections, suggesting once again that it is a
network, with good independence of the nodes. On the basis valid test to integrate the information from the different
of the shape of the network, we did not identify any isolated functions tested with the more specific tasks.
cluster of scores, suggesting that the different tests were Importantly, the semantic fluency test arose as the most
segregated and integrated at the same time with specific central node, with a high number of relatively strong cor-
connections among the various tests (Fig. 1A). This finding relations and many connections passing through it. Similar
suggested that the tests are quite selective (low sparsity and to what we observed for the MMSE, semantic fluency
relatively low edge coefficients) for measuring cognitive showed high betweenness and strength. It emerged as a
functions in healthy people. focal node with relatively strong connections that mediated
Education and age were both connected with many nodes between other variables and connected most of its
of test scores. Moreover, when expressed as centrality indices neighbours. Semantic fluency appeared to be negatively
(Fig. 2A), education and age were central to the network, as correlated with age and central to the other tests in both
indicated by high strength, but they did not have a role facil- healthy and VaE groups, confirming that performance on
itating the connections (i.e., low betweenness). These results this test generally decreases during the lifespan (Crossley,
suggested that age and educational level influenced cognitive D'Arcy, & Rawson, 1997; Moreno-Martı́nez, Laws, & Schulz,
performance on different tests. On the other hand, the low 2008).
betweenness indicated that they cannot explain the majority An additional remark in the context of this network is in
of correlation among the various tests. regard to the token test that correlated with digit span and
The primary purpose of screening tests is to indicate the Raven tests. Even if the token test is considered a verbal
probability of cognitive impairment (Cullen, O'Neill, Evans, comprehension test, our network analysis suggested a close
Coen, & Lawlor, 2007). Even if they are not intended to relationship with working memory and reasoning, as typically
replace a full neuropsychological assessment, they are thought observed in clinical practice.
to explore more than one function to capture any symptoms
that may suggest cognitive deficits. Thus, we expected that 3.1.3. AD patients
screening tests would correlate with different specific tests. In this network, the 23 nodes generated 168 edges, with a
In our sample of healthy subjects, the MoCA test and the sparsity index of .66. The pattern appeared more connected
Raven Matrices were connected with almost all the other than the other groups, with a percentage of 66% edges (spar-
nodes and showed high centrality indices (betweenness and sity index .66) compared to the number of total possible con-
strength). The strength indices suggested that the MoCA test nections, and to have low specificity. From a speculative point
and the Raven Matrices were connected with the entire of view, the AD network pattern seemed to be located between
network through numerous strong connections. Moreover, the control group and the VaE group, along a continuum that
they seemed to have a faciliatory role in mediating the asso- goes from a high specific pattern, in which each function is
ciations between the other tests, as suggested by the high quite segregated and has a small number of robust relation-
betweenness value (Fig. 1B). These results indicated that the ships, to a highly connected network characterized by specific
MoCA test and the Raven Matrices are non-specific and gen- and weak connections. This network was marked by
eral assessment tools, as is required for screening purposes. numerous weak connections and few moderate-to-strong
connections that highlight a different pattern of balance
3.1.2. VaE patients among the test performances compared to both the healthy
The VaE patients included in the study showed a network of 23 and VaE groups (Fig. 1F).
nodes and 146 edges, with a sparsity value of .58. In the In the AD group, we identified two clusters: one centred on
healthy group, few robust connections showed the specificity memory functions and one incorporating the more “execu-
of the network because tests were integrated and segregated tive” function tests (Fig. 1E).
at the same time where few but solid and selective relations The memory cluster was composed of the prose memory
emerge. In the VaE sample, a few more connections appeared, test (both immediate and delayed recall) and the semantically
which were overall much weaker, suggesting the non- related word-list test (immediate and delayed recall, detection
specificity of the links and, in general, the low strength of through distractors and intrusions). These tests and their sub-
the relations (Fig. 1C, D). scores were well connected among each other and segregated
Age and years of education seemed to have a marginal role from all the other tests.
in VaE patient performances. Age and years of education On the other hand, we distinguished a frontal-executive
nodes still had many edges, but these were very weak and in a cluster that included tests assessing language, visuocon-
context where every node was connected with everything. structive praxis, abstract reasoning, attention shifting ability,
Low centrality indices further suggest their marginal role in and short-term memory. Notably, this pattern highlighted
the network (Fig. 2B). the relationship between executive functions and lexicon
The MMSE is primarily used as a screening test in cases of access tests that are typically considered to assess the
suspected cognitive impairment. This test was well connected proper functionality of the frontal lobe in clinical practice. In
with the other tests and reflected the global structure of the addition, short-term memory tests (digit and Corsi span
network since they showed numerous but weak correlations. tests) fell into the executive cluster instead of the memory
Given the scarcity of robust relationships, the MMSE appeared cluster further indicating the attentional role of working
to have a central role in the pattern. It showed high memory function.
90 c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

Fig. 1 e Networks and edge coefficient distributions. Panels A, C and E represent the estimated networks in the healthy
group, VaE patients and AD patients, respectively. Red edges indicate negative relations; green edges show positive ones.
The size and colour saturation of the edges represent the intensity of the relationships. Panels B, D and F report the edge
coefficient distribution in each group. Nodes included in the healthy group: Montreal Cognitive Assessment ¼ MoCA (Pirani,
Tulipani, & Neri, 2006); semantic fluency test ¼ SemFlu (Novelli et al., 1986); phonemic fluency test ¼ PhonFlu (Novelli et al.,
1986); ReyeOsterrieth complex figuredcopy and delayed recall ¼ ReyCopy and ReyDR (Caffarra et al., 2002a); digit
spandforward and backward ¼ DigitFw and DigitBw (Orsini et al., 1987); prose memory test ¼ Mem (Novelli et al., 1986);
Corsi spandforward and backward ¼ CorsiFw and CorsiBw (Orsini et al., 1987); Raven's progressive matrices ¼ Raven
(Basso et al., 1987; Raven, 1947); Stroop test accuracy and time performance ¼ StroopErr and StroopT (Caffarra et al., 2002b);
Trail Making Testdpart A and part B ¼ TMT_A and TMT_B (Giovagnoli, 1996); age at the assessment ¼ Age; sex of the
participant ¼ Sex; years of education ¼ Edu. Nodes included in the VaE and AD groups: Mini-Mental State
Examination ¼ MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975); semantic fluency test ¼ SemFlu (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); phonemic
fluency ¼ PhonFlu (Novelli et al., 1986); token test ¼ token (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); drawings copy ¼ DrawCopy (Spinnler
& Tognoni, 1987); ReyeOsterrieth complex figuredcopy and delayed recall ¼ ReyCopy and ReyDR (Caffarra et al., 2002a);
c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6 91

Age and years of education seemed to have a marginal role. The group comparisons between the patients revealed that
They were connected with few nodes and with weak con- VaE patients performed better than the AD patients, even
nections and reached low levels of centrality. when they were matched for age, education, and sex. Unfor-
As hypothesized for the VaE group, it is possible that once tunately, healthy participants were younger and had more
the symptoms of the cognitive impairment become years of formal schooling; given the retrospective nature of
more prominent, the influences of other variables, such as age this work, we were not able to balance the groups in all the
and education, become decreasingly relevant to the test essential characteristics. The group of healthy people was part
performances. of a different data collection project than the groups of pa-
The screening tests presented a high number of connec- tients. This difference is an explicit limitation of the present
tions, as in the VaE group. The MMSE showed high strength study, together with the various tests used for the healthy
and influenced the network connecting the memory cluster group; nonetheless, the study highlights a great potentiality in
and the frontal-executive cluster, although the low between- approaching cognitive impairments by considering global
ness index suggested a marginal role in facilitating the con- performance and not a single task score.
nections of the different nodes. In contrast, the Raven The NA results were intended to measure, in addition to a
matrices fulfilled an essential mediation role inside the quantitative evaluation of the specific performances, the
frontal-executive cluster, showing a high betweenness value balance among the different test scores, thus capturing the
as well as high strength. reorganization of the cognitive mechanisms that is thought to
Finally, the semantic fluency test arose as a focal node with happen in dementia (Chen et al., 2013; Gits, 2016; Sohn, Yoo,
high values of betweenness and strength. This variable Na, & Jeong, 2014).
contributed to linking the two clusters that we identified, had There are different types of information that can be extracted
a facilitating role and influenced the whole network with a from the NA. The aim of this paper was to show how this
high number of connections passing through it. method could be used to understand and increase the knowl-
edge about neuropsychological assessments in dementia.
We speculated that the three networks that emerged by NA
4. General discussion described a sort of a continuum from a high specific pattern, in
which each function is quite segregated and has a small
The present work aimed to analyse a standard neuropsycho- number of relatively strong relationships, to a highly con-
logical evaluation for cognitive impairment using NA in both nected and diffuse network, characterized almost only by non-
healthy and pathological conditions of cognitive ageing. specific and weak relations. On one side of this continuum, the
A network is a model composed of a set of nodes, repre- healthy group revealed high specificity and good indepen-
senting entities, and a set of edges that connect the nodes, dence of the nodes. Looking at the general shape, we did not
representing their relations (e.g., De Nooy et al., 2011). NA is a find any specific cluster, confirming that the nodes were
useful method to explore the interactions between many segregated and, at the same time, well integrated into a
variables that correlate with each other. It is a parsimonious pattern characterized by relatively few robust correlations. We
and understandable method to analyse complex systems. suggest that this feature indicated that each test was specific
This study showed the potential of NA in examining the for one cognitive function, although not uniquely related to it.
connections between cognitive tests in different mental state On the opposite side of the continuum, the VaE patients
conditions. A clear example is the condition of dementia, showed numerous weak connections, underlying a pattern
which is characterized by impairment in multiple domains, characterized by weak non-specific relations. This confirmed
and a patient typically presents with many deficiencies in a common observation in clinical practice. VaE patients may
performance at testing. have good performances on a specific test but fail to transfer
We used this novel perspective to approach neuropsy- the skill in a more integrated and composite situation. The
chological assessment in dementia and to study the reorga- disconnection of cognitive abilities might be difficult to iden-
nization of the cognitive system using the changes in the tify in single test performances. In contrast, a comprehensive
network of relations among the performances on different and qualitative observation of the complete assessment can
neuropsychological tests. highlight multiple mild impairments that affect different do-
The nodes considered in the network were age, sex, years mains. The multiple impairments are reflected by a pattern of
of education, and the raw scores obtained in the neuropsy- unspecific and fragile relations between the various tests in
chological tests. The domains assessed in the cognitive bat- the network structure.
teries were memory, language, praxis, executive functions, The network of the AD patients was characterized by
attention and abstract reasoning, together with the perfor- numerous connections with weak strength and few connec-
mance in more general screening tests. tions with moderate-to-strong relations. We identified a

digit span forward ¼ Digit (Orsini et al., 1987); prose memory testdimmediate and delayed recall ¼ MemIR and MemDR
(Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); Corsi span ¼ Corsi (Orsini et al., 1987); semantically related words list learningdimmediate
recall, delayed recall and detection ¼ ApprIR, AppDR, and AppD; detection through distractors ¼ ApprIR_I and AppDR_I
(Mauri et al., 1997); Raven's progressive matrices ¼ Raven (Basso et al., 1987; Raven, 1947); attentive matrices ¼ AttMat
(Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987); Trail Making Testdpart A and part B ¼ TMT_A and TMT_B (Giovagnoli, 1996); age at the
assessment ¼ Age; sex of the participant ¼ Sex; years of education ¼ Edu.
92 c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

Fig. 2 e Centrality indices and the correlations between them. Panels A, C and E report the centrality indices of the network
in the healthy group, VaE patients and AD patients, respectively. Strength centrality is higher if a node has a large number
of strong direct links with other nodes. Betweenness is high if the node is part of many shortest paths connecting two
different nodes. Centrality indices are standardized and expressed in z-scores. Panels B, D and F represent the correlation
plot between Strength and Betweenness in each group.

pattern composed of two clusters centred on memory and reorganization mechanisms could be related to functional
frontal-executive functions. This pattern was a unique feature compensation (Kellerman et al., 2016). Interestingly, the
among our three groups, suggesting a specific reorganization scores on the memory tests seemed to segregate from the rest
of the cognitive abilities of AD patients, which has also been of the network, somehow isolating that cognitive function
confirmed by neuroimaging studies (Pariente et al., 2005; from the others. This result is in line with behavioural ob-
Dickerson & Sperling, 2009). A similar NA study conducted in servations of patients with AD, who are strongly characterized
patients with temporal lobe epilepsy suggested that these by memory impairments. The novelty is that there are not
c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6 93

only memory performance decreases but also disconnections cognitive impairment is generally related to increasing age
of memory function from the functioning in the other cogni- (Gao, Hendrie, Hall, & Hui, 1998; Wiederholt et al., 1993), our
tive domains. Although some neuropsychological studies network analysis suggested that personal differences are
have tried to distinguish between AD and VaE patients poorly correlated with test performance.
(Mathias & Burke, 2009; Thompson, Stopford, Snowden, & Semantic fluency deserves an additional comment because
Neary, 2005), this remains a difficult differential diagnosis. it emerged as a central node in all of the groups examined,
The present network analysis can provide a different point of especially in the clinical groups. The semantic fluency node
view in this debate, offering a new method for describing the showed a high number of strong correlations and many con-
cognitive profile of these patients. Our results seemed to nections passing through it in the three networks. Moreover,
suggest that a functional cognitive system, represented by the this node contributed to linking the memory and frontal-
network in the control group, needs fewer and stronger con- executive clusters in the AD group. In the healthy group, the
nections than a damaged one. performances on this test showed a negative correlation with
The main purpose of screening tests is to obtain a reliable age, suggesting a decreasing pattern during the lifespan and
and fast index of the performances in cognitive functioning confirming previous studies (Crossley et al., 1997; Moreno-
(Cullen et al., 2007). Therefore, these tests explore multiple Martı́nez et al., 2008). The same pattern was identified in the
functions and offer a single score that accounts for the prob- VaE patients but not in the AD subjects. Semantic fluency has
ability of having cognitive impairment. been reported to be strongly impaired in Alzheimer's disease
Regarding the healthy group, centrality indices suggested (Cerhan et al., 2002; Crossley et al., 1997), and particular in-
that the MoCA test and the Raven matrices influenced the terest in the specific performances characterizing different
network with a high number of strong connections. Moreover, forms of dementia has been growing. Cerhan et al. (2002)
they seemed to have a facilitating role between other func- demonstrated that category fluency is sufficient to differen-
tions that had edges from different tests. A similar function tiate AD patients from healthy control individuals. To explain
can be described in both VaE and AD patients for the MMSE. these findings, a temporal-lobe semantic-degradation theory
Despite the controversial opinion about this test and its utility has been proposed (Butters, Granholm, Salmon, Grant, &
(Dong et al., 2010; Hoops et al., 2009), the present study indi- Wolfe, 1987; Salmon, Heindel, & Lange, 1999). The authors
cated that the MMSE is correlated with most of the tests suggested that access to temporal-lobe semantic stores is
included in a more comprehensive neuropsychological bat- necessary for category fluency, and this hypothesis is
tery. However, the low strength of these connections confirms coherent with the gradual deterioration of semantic memory
that this test could not be used as a unique assessment tool. characteristic of patients with AD. Our findings confirm the
Nevertheless, the MMSE could provide useful information crucial importance of the semantic fluency test in assessing
about the general level of the patient. In conclusion, the the organization of cognitive networks.
MMSE, MoCA and Raven matrices emerged as adequate The point of the present work is to illuminate NA potenti-
screening tests since they can explore more than one func- alities in examining a standard neuropsychological assess-
tion, and the general scores correlated with all the other tests ment. NA has the potential to either provide information about
included in the usual neuropsychological batteries. They the relation of the tests or about the underlying cognitive
appeared as non-specific and general assessment tools that functions.
should be used for this scope, without assuming or over- We showed that different information could be extracted
interpreting any specificity to the function impaired. from the NA and outlined a method for using this new po-
To better estimate the contribution of demographic infor- tential tool in the interpretation of cognitive performances
mation such as age and the level of education, we included and neuropsychological evaluations. Each test must be inter-
them as nodes of the network and considered the raw scores preted in conjunction with a wider system of information
at each test. In the control group, age and education correlated about the patient and his/her condition, and the quantitative
with the majority of the nodes. This result suggested that data obtained during the assessment must be integrated with
performance on cognitive tests is affected by these variables, a qualitative analysis of the whole performance. NA, showing
stressing the importance of normative data for standardizing the relationship between variables and their respective
the tests. The first clinical utility of normative data is to pro- importance for each other, provides a methodological foun-
vide a set of norms that show normal variation in perfor- dation for this clinical process and offers a quantitative
mance depending on age and education (Tombaugh, 2004). method to characterize the complexity of the cognitive
Thus, scores on different tests must be read while considering assessment in a parsimonious and comprehensible manner.
the normal range of variation among healthy individuals The exemplification of typical patterns of cognitive func-
(Mitrushina, 2005). tioning among patients with different types of cognitive
Nonetheless, age and education lose much of their impact impairment can inform both the interpretation of individual
on neuropsychological performance in cases of cognitive performance and the description of cohorts of patients. The
impairment. Age and years of education seemed to have a usefulness for an interpretation of patient profiles lies in the
marginal role both in the VaE group and in the AD group. We possibility to compare, at the moment from a qualitative point
hypothesized that symptoms of cognitive impairment could of view, specific patterns of performances or the temporal
mask the influences of other variables, such as age and edu- evolution of the disease with a reference network for the sus-
cation. The profile of patients with cognitive impairment pected diagnosis. Future evolution of this approach could be
scarcely depended on personal variables, but the specific imagined in the context of a machine learning algorithm to
features of the disease defined the impairments. Even if support the diagnosis of new patients. For the description of the
94 c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

statistical pattern in large cohorts, NA provides a model of the with a more compromised condition with much lower scores
relations among neuropsychological test performances, inte- on the MMSE. A possible prediction is that in the advanced
grating several sources of information that considers the un- stages of dementia, the networks emerging from different
derlying complexity. In this framework, NA can be part of dementias would be undistinguishable because of a floor ef-
modern approaches to clinical neuropsychology in an attempt fect in the performances. It could be interesting to evaluate
to overcome limitations in clinical evaluations such as the pattern in people at an early stage of AD with people at a
the “impaired/spared” dichotomy when considering cognitive late stage of AD to see if the pattern remains constant
performances. throughout the cognitive decline or if the pattern changes
with progression of the disease. This information would add
to the current study where different dementias were
5. Hypothesis generation compared, and this research would offer another potential
development in the study of the balance in cognitive function
The exploratory nature of NA suggests that every edge can be in people with cognitive impairment.
a relation that can be experimentally tested or on which a
researcher may focus his/her attention to calibrate new in-
terventions or theories. For instance, semantic fluency has CRediT authorship contribution statement
been reported to be strongly impaired in patients with AD
(Cerhan et al., 2002; Crossley et al., 1997), and particular in- Giorgia Tosi: Conceptualization, Writing - original draft,
terest in the specific performances characterizing different Writing - review & editing. Carolina Borsani: Writing - review
forms of dementia has been growing. Our data suggested that & editing. Stefania Castiglioni: Supervision, Writing - review &
semantic fluency is a central node. This result is consistent editing. Roberta Daini: Supervision, Writing - review & editing.
with the theoretical framework of AD as temporal-lobe se- Massimo Franceschi: Supervision, Writing - review & editing.
mantic-degradation dementia (Butters et al., 1987; Salmon Daniele Romano: Supervision, Conceptualization, Writing -
et al., 1999). Based on our results and the semantic degrada- original draft, Writing - review & editing, Visualization.
tion theory, we expect to find a pattern where semantic
fluency is much less central in other types of dementia where
the access to temporal-lobe semantic stores is not a main
Open practices
characteristic of the impairment (e.g., early stage of fronto-
temporal dementia).
The study in this article earned an Open Data badge for trans-
The NA approach also provides potential ideas for where to
parent practices. Data for the study are available at https://osf.
focus the cognitive stimulation of patients. Cognitive stimu-
io/8wn7e/?view_only¼2569a529e9424b0d86a31dcc05daa0fe.
lation may focus on tasks related to memory and attention to
impact two independent areas of the network. Alternatively,
cognitive stimulation may focus on semantic functions
because they seemed to be in between the memory and Acknowledgement
attentional clusters, thus potentially impacting both areas.
We hypothesize that effective cognitive stimulation could We thank Miss Gaia Salvadore and Dr. Monika Zuzanna Wajs
impact the focal node and its neighbours. For instance, it for her careful proofreading of the manuscript.
might be useful with AD patients to work on semantic fluency We thank all the members of the Neurology Department of
since it emerged as a central node with several connections. the MultiMedica Hospital of Castellanza (Varese, Italy) for
Such stimulation could influence other cognitive functions their contribution to the general assessment and the caring of
connected in the network. In contrast, stimulation focused on the patients involved in the study.
the detection of previously acquired words through a dis-
tractor (AppD in Figs. 1E and 2E) would be less impactful
because of the low influence that this particular node has on
references
the general pattern, so that any eventual effect would be un-
likely to spread to other tasks.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview.
Additionally, future directions may focus on different
Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189e208.
clinical populations to define their specific patterns of cogni-
Basso, A., Capitani, E., & Laiacona, M. (1987). Raven's Coloured
tive impairments. In addition to testing different populations, Progressive Matrices: Normative values on 305 adult normal
another future direction may test the time-dependent evolu- controls. Functional Neurology, 2(2), 189e194.
tion of the conditions. One of the anonymous reviewers sug- Buckner, R. L., Hrienen, F. M., & Yeo, T. B. T. (2013). Opportunities
gested that different performance levels on the MMSE among and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nature
our groups may have limited our results. Because NA is based Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 832e837.
Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D. P., Grant, I., & Wolfe, J.
on correlations, the small MMSE differences unlikely
(1987). Episodic and semantic memory: A comparison of
contributed to our results. Nonetheless, this observation amnesic and demented patients. Journal of Clinical and
opens an interesting question. It is possible that a different Experimental Neuropsychology, 9(5), 479e497. https://doi.org/
network characterizes the late stages of dementia than the 10.1080/01688638708410764.
early stages. AD patients with a high MMSE score may have a Caffarra, P., Vezzadini, G., Dieci, F., Zonato, F., & Venneri, A.
different balance among cognitive functions than patients (2002a). Rey-osterrieth complex figure: Normative values in
c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6 95

an Italian population sample. Neurological Sciences, 22(6), patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3),
443e447. 189e198.
Caffarra, P., Vezzadini, G., Dieci, F., Zonato, F., & Venneri, A. Gao, S., Hendrie, H. C., Hall, K. S., & Hui, S. (1998). The
(2002b). Una versione abbreviata del test di Stroop: Dati relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia
normativi nella popolazione italiana. Nuova Rivista Di and Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. Archives of General
Neurologia, 12(4), 111e115. Psychiatry, 55(9), 809e815.
Cerhan, J. H., Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. C., Giovagnoli, A. R., Del Pesce, M., Mascheroni, S., Simoncelli, M.,
Petersen, R. C., & Boeve, B. F. (2002). Diagnostic utility of letter Laiacona, M., & Capitani, E. (1996). Trail making test:
fluency, category fluency, and fluency difference scores in Normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Italian
Alzheimer's disease. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16(1), 35e42. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 17(4), 305e309. http://www.ncbi.
https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.16.1.35.8326. nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8915764.
Chen, G., Zhang, H.-Y., Xie, C., Chen, G., Zhang, Z.-J., Teng, G.-J., Gits, H. C. (2016). Relating connectivity and graph analysis to
et al. (2013). Modular reorganization of brain resting state cognitive function in Alzheimer's disease. Michigan Journal of
networks and its independent validation in Alzheimer's Medicine, 1(1), 45e65. https://doi.org/10.3998/
disease patients. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 456. mjm.13761231.0001.111.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00456. Goswami, S., Murthy, C. A., & Das, A. K. (2018). Sparsity measure
Conti, S., Bonazzi, S., Laiacona, M., Masina, M., & Vanelli, M. of a network graph: Gini index. Information Sciences, 462, 16e39.
(2015). Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA)-Italian version: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ins.2018.05.044.
Regression based norms and equivalent scores. Neurological Hafkemeijer, A., Mo € ller, C., Dopper, E. G., Jiskoot, L. C.,
Sciences, 36(2), 209e214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-014- Schouten, T. M., van Swieten, J. C., & Scheltens, P. (2015).
1921-3. Resting state functional connectivity differences between
Costantini, G., Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., Perugini, M., Mo ~ ttus, R., behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's
Waldorp, L. J., et al. (2015). State of the aRt personality disease. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 474. https://doi.org/
research: A tutorial on network analysis of personality data in 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00474.
R. Journal of Research in Personality, 54, 13e29. https://doi.org/ Hoops, S., Nazem, S., Siderowf, A. D., Duda, J. E., Xie, S. X.,
10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.003. Stern, M. B., et al. (2009). Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the
Crossley, M., D'Arcy, C., & Rawson, N. S. B. (1997). Letter and detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease.
category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: A Neurology, 73(21), 1738e1745.
comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of Huang, C. C., Lin, S. H., & Lin, C. P. (2016). The effect of network
the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and template from normal subjects in the detection of network
Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(1), 52e62. https://doi.org/ impairment. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 76, 294e303.
10.1080/01688639708403836. Kalaria, R. N., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2006). Small vessel disease and
Cullen, B., O'Neill, B., Evans, J. J., Coen, R. F., & Lawlor, B. A. (2007). subcortical vascular dementia. Journal of Clinical Neurology, 2(1),
A review of screening tests for cognitive impairment. Journal of 1e11.
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 78(8), 790e799. https:// Kellermann, T. S., Bonilha, L., Eskandari, R., Garcia-Ramos, C.,
doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2006.095414. Lin, J. J., & Hermann, B. P. (2016). Mapping the
De Nooy, W., Mrvar, A., & Batagelj, V. (2011). Exploratory social neuropsychological profile of temporal lobe epilepsy using
network analysis with Pajek (structural analysis in the social cognitive network topology and graph theory. Epilepsy &
sciences). Revised and Expanded. Behavior, 63, 9e16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.07.030.
Dickerson, B. C., & Sperling, R. A. (2009). Large-scale functional Lauritzen, S. L. (1996). Graphical models (Vol. 17). Clarendon Press.
brain network abnormalities in Alzheimer's disease: Insights Lehmann, M., Madison, C., Ghosha, P. M., Millera, Z. A.,
from functional neuroimaging. Behavioural Neurology, 21(1e2), Greiciusd, M. D., Kramera, J. H., et al. (2016). Loss of functional
63e75. connectivity is greater outside the default mode network in
Dong, Y., Sharma, V. K., Chan, B. P. L., Venketasubramanian, N., non-familial early-onset Alzheimer's disease variants.
Teoh, H. L., Seet, R. C. S., & Chen, C. (2010). The Montreal Neurobiology of Aging, 21(18), 4062e4072. https://doi.org/
Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is superior to the Mini-Mental 10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-15-0428.Bioactivity.
State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of vascular Mathias, J. L., & Burke, J. (2009). Cognitive functioning in Alzheimer's
cognitive impairment after acute stroke. Journal of the and vascular dementia: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 23(4),
Neurological Sciences, 299(1e2), 15e18. 411e423. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015384.
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating Mauri, M., Carlesino, G. A., Graceffa, A. M. S., Loasses, A.,
psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Lorusso, S., Sinforiani, E., & Caltagirone, C. (1997).
Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 195e212. https://doi.org/ Standardizzazione di due nuovi test di memoria:
10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1. Apprendimento di liste di parole correlate e non correlate
Epskamp, S., Kruis, J., & Marsman, M. (2017). Estimating semanticamente. Archivio Di Psicologia Neurologia e Psichiatria,
psychopathological networks: Be careful what you wish for. 58, 621e645.
Plos One, 12(6), 1e13. https://doi.org/10.1371/ McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T.,
journal.pone.0179891. Jack, C. R., Jr., Kawas, C. H., & Mohs, R. C. (2011). The diagnosis
Erkinjuntti, T., Inzitari, D., Pantoni, L., Wallin, A., Scheltens, P., of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations
Rockwood, K., & Desmond, D. W. (2000a). Research criteria for from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association
subcortical vascular dementia in clinical trials. In Advances in workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease.
dementia research (pp. 23e30). Vienna: Springer. Alzheimer's & dementia, 7(3), 263e269.
Erkinjuntti, T., Inzitari, D., Pantoni, L., Wallin, A., Scheltens, P., McNeish, D. M. (2015). Using lasso for predictor selection and
Rockwood, K., et al. (2000b). Limitations of clincal criteria for to assuage overfitting: A method long overlooked in
the diagnosis of vascular dementia in clinical trials: Is a focus behavioral sciences. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50(5),
on subcortical vascular dementia a solution? Annals of the New 471e484.
York Academy of Sciences, 903(1), 262e272. Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D'Elia, L. F. (2005).
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment.
state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of Oxford University Press.
96 c o r t e x 1 2 4 ( 2 0 2 0 ) 8 5 e9 6

Monaco, M., Costa, A., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2013). Sohn, W. S., Yoo, K., Na, D. L., & Jeong, Y. (2014). Progressive
Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: changes in Hippocampal resting-state connectivity across
Standardization and normative data from an Italian adult cognitive impairment. Alzheimer Disease and Associated
population. Neurological Sciences, 34(5), 749e754. https:// Disorders, 28(3), 239e246. https://doi.org/10.1097/
doi.org/10.1007/s10072-012-1130-x. WAD.0000000000000027.
Moreno-Martı́nez, F. J., Laws, K. R., & Schulz, J. (2008). The impact Spinnler, H., & Tognoni, G. (1987). Taratura e standardizzazione
of dementia, age and sex on category fluency: Greater deficits italiana di test neuropsicologici. Italian Journal of Neurological
in women with Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 44(9), 1256e1264. Sciences, 8(8), 8e120.
Novelli, P., Capitani, L., Vallar, C., & Cappa, S. (1986). Test di Thompson, J. C., Stopford, C. L., Snowden, J. S., & Neary, D. (2005).
fluenza verbale. Archivio di Psicologia. Neurologia e Psichiatria, Qualitative neuropsychological performance characteristics in
47(4). frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of
Orsini, A., Grossi, D., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Papagno, C., & Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 76(7), 920e927.
Vallar, G. (1987). Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: Tibshirani, R. (1996). Regression shrinkage and selection via the
Normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Italian lasso. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B
Journal of Neurological Sciences, 8(6), 539e548. http://www.ncbi. (Methodological), 267e288.
nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3429213. Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail making test A and B: Normative data
Pariente, J., Cole, S., Henson, R., Clare, L., Kennedy, A., Rossor, M., stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical
& Frackowiak, R. S. (2005). Alzheimer's patients engage an Neuropsychology, 19(2), 203e214.
alternative network during a memory task. Annals of Van Borkulo, C. D., Borsboom, D., Epskamp, S., Blanken, T. F.,
Neurology: Official Journal of the American Neurological Association Boschloo, L., Schoevers, R. A., et al. (2014). A new method for
and the Child Neurology Society, 58(6), 870e879. constructing networks from binary data. Scientific Reports, 4, 5918.
Pirani, A., Tulipani, C., & Neri, M. (2006). Italian translation of MoCA van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the
test and of its instructions. Retrieved from: http://www. brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional
mocatest.org. connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(8),
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical 519e534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008.
computing. Version 3.3. 3. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Van Der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P.,
Statistical Computing. Wicherts, J. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A
Raven, J. C. (1947). Progressive matrices: Sets A, AbB. Board and Book dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive
Forms. https://ci.nii.ac.jp/naid/10015457223/. manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review,
Sachdev, P., Kalaria, R., O'Brien, J., Skoog, I., Alladi, S., & 113(4), 842e861. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.842.
Ganguli, M. (2014). “Diagnostic criteria for vascular cognitive Weintraub, S., Wicklund, A. H., & Salmon, D. P. (2012). The
disorders: A VASCOG statement” Alzheimer. Drug and Alcohol neuropsychological profile of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring
Dependence, 28(3), 206e218. Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 2(4), a006171. https://doi.org/
Salmon, D. P., Heindel, W. C., & Lange, K. L. (1999). Differential 10.1101/cshperspect.a006171.
decline in word generation from phonemic and semantic Wiederholt, W. C., Cahn, D., Butters, N. M., Salmon, D. P., Kritz-
categories during the course of Alzheimer's disease: Silverstein, D., & Barrett-Connor, E. (1993). Effects of age,
Implications for the integrity of semantic memory. Journal of gender and education on selected neuropsychological tests in
the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(7), 692e703. an elderly community cohort. Journal of the American Geriatrics
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617799577126. Society, 41(6), 639e647.

You might also like