You are on page 1of 14

Attachment-Based Interventions:

Heading for Evidence-Based Ways to Support Families

FEMMIE JUFFER, MARIAN J. BAKERMANS-KRANENBURG


and MARINUS H. VAN IJZENDOORN
Leiden University, The Netherlands

Introduction discovered that children vary in their quality of


A quick glance at the countless internet sites attachment security. In her study, and replicated by
providing information about ‘attachment therapy’ numerous empirical studies and a meta-analysis (De
and similar approaches such as ‘holding’ or ‘trauma Wolff & Van IJzendoorn, 1997), children who had
therapy’ might raise high expectations about the experienced insensitive parenting developed
sound theoretical foundations of such interventions insecure (avoidant or ambivalent) attachment
and the proven effectiveness of the treatment relationships with their parent whereas children who
efforts. Unfortunately, many so-called attachment had received sensitive care were securely attached.
therapies not only miss a solid theoretical Ainsworth described parental sensitivity as the
foundation and empirical evidence-base, but they ability to accurately perceive the child’s signals and
may even be harmful and dangerous for children to respond to these signals in a prompt and adequate
(Chaffin et al., 2006; Pignotti & Mercer, 2007). way (Ainsworth et al., 1978). Later, a fourth
Here we present our work on attachment-based category was added to the categories of secure,
interventions from the perspectives of attachment avoidant, and ambivalent attachment: insecure
theory and empirical research in this area. Based on disorganised attachment (Main & Solomon, 1990).
extensive experience and meta-analyses of the Anomalous parental behaviour was found to be
pertinent attachment-based intervention studies, we related to disorganised attachment (Hesse & Main,
developed an intervention model that was tested in 2006; Madigan et al., 2006), while in its turn
various samples of parents and children at risk. We disorganised attachment was associated with
elaborate on how to use this video-feedback elevated risk for later child psychopathology (Van
intervention in practice, illustrated with a case IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg,
example. 1999).
Compared to insecure attachments, early
Attachment-based interventions: theory and secure attachment relationships have been
research associated with fewer externalising problems and
Departing from an evolutionary perspective and better social competence in later childhood and
drawing on research that examined the effects of adolescence (e.g., Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg,
separations of (primate) infants from their parents Van IJzendoorn, Lapsley, & Roisman, in press;
(e.g., Harlow, 1958), John Bowlby (1982) Jaffari-Bimmel, Juffer, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-
developed attachment theory. According to that Kranenburg, & Mooijaart, 2006; Sroufe, Egeland,
theory, infants are biologically predisposed to use Carlson, & Collins, 2005). Although insecure and
their parents as a haven of safety to provide comfort disorganised attachment do not constitute clinical
and protection when they are distressed, and as a concepts per se but are considered risk factors in the
secure base from which to explore the world. development of psychopathology, both attachment
Securely attached children feel free to play and to and the related concept of parental sensitivity
express their (negative) feelings because they trust appear to be relevant for the clinical field, and for
their parents to support them whenever they need the development and evaluation of attachment-
help. However, not all children experience their based interventions in at-risk and clinical families.
parents as a haven of safety and a secure base In particular, parental sensitivity as the best
(Waters & Cummings, 2000). Mary Ainsworth documented determinant of children’s attachment
(Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) security has been the focus of numerous
ACAMH Occasional Papers No. 29 (2009), Attachment: Current Focus and Future Directions
47
intervention efforts tested in multiple studies. But Surprisingly, however, interventions with more than
how do we know which intervention methods and 16 sessions were less effective than interventions
models work best? To find answers to this with a smaller number of sessions (Less is more; see
theoretically and clinically significant question, we Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2003).
adopted a meta-analytic approach. Twenty-nine intervention studies (involving
1,503 families) aimed at promoting attachment
Meta-analyses of attachment-based interventions security. The combined effect size for attachment
Meta-analysis may be one of the most obvious tools security was small but significant (d = 0.19).
to come to evidence-based conclusions about best Twenty-three studies (including 1,255 families)
intervention practices. With meta-analysis, data presented randomised controlled trials resulting in a
from multiple studies can be synthesised, and similar effect size (d = 0.20). Interventions focusing
conclusions can be drawn about essential features of on parental sensitivity were significantly more
effective attachment-based interventions (such as effective in fostering secure attachment than
the duration or the focus of the intervention). interventions targeting other aspects. In fact, only
We examined 70 published intervention studies sensitivity-focused interventions showed a
with 88 interventions directed at either sensitivity or significant combined effect size on attachment (d =
attachment or both (see Bakermans-Kranenburg, 0.39). Most importantly, those interventions that
Van IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003; for an overview of were most effective in enhancing parental
the studies see Juffer, Van IJzendoorn, & sensitivity (d > 0.40) were also most effective in
Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2008). All intervention enhancing children’s attachment security (d = 0.45;
studies reported observed sensitivity and/or Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2003).
attachment as an outcome. The intervention studies In 15 intervention studies (including 842
were not restricted to a specific population. Some families) insecure disorganised attachment was
samples were middle-class families with typically reported as an outcome measure. The combined
developing infants, but studies with clinical and at- effect size of these studies was not significant, d =
risk populations were included as well. 0.05 (see Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn,
Eighty-one studies (including 7,636 families) & Juffer, 2005). However, the five interventions
presented intervention effects on parental focusing on sensitivity only were again most
sensitivity; and the combined effect size (d, the effective; in this case they were significantly more
standardised difference between the means of the effective in reducing attachment disorganisation (d
intervention and control groups) was a moderately = 0.26) than other interventions (d = -0.08).
strong effect of 0.44 (p < .001). To estimate the Summarising, our meta-analyses showed that
combined effect size in the set of studies with the interventions can enhance parental sensitivity and
most adequate designs, randomised controlled trials children’s attachment security, but attachment
were selected. In this set of 51 studies (including security to a lesser extent than sensitivity (the same
6,282 participants), interventions were also conclusion was reached in a previous meta-analyses
significantly effective in enhancing parental including a much smaller number of studies; see
sensitivity (d = 0.33), but the combined effect size Van IJzendoorn, Juffer, & Duyvesteyn, 1995). In
was significantly lower than the combined effect particular, interventions with a focus on sensitive
size of studies that did not use a randomised design behaviour were successful in decreasing insensitive
(d = 0.61). Nonrandomised studies may run the risk parenting as well as children’s attachment insecurity
of inflated effects. Interventions with a relatively and disorganisation.
narrow focus, aiming at enhancing parental
sensitivity through a behavioural approach, were Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive
found to be significantly more effective (d = 0.45) Parenting (VIPP)
than broader interventions (such as representational Based on attachment theory and our meta-analytic
interventions or social support) (d = 0.27). studies, we developed an intervention to promote
Interventions with video feedback were more sensitive parenting and positive parent-child
effective than interventions without this method. interactions. Essential outcomes of the meta-
Interventions with fewer than 5 sessions were as analyses of attachment-based interventions (Baker-
effective as interventions with 5 to 16 sessions. mans-Kranenburg et al., 2003) were integrated in

48
our model: as a result VIPP is a short-term or adaptations of the program, proved to be effective
intervention directed at parental sensitivity by in enhancing sensitive parenting (see Figure 1). In a
utilising video feedback. sample of Dutch low-SES insecure mothers (see
below for a case study), VIPP resulted in a
Development of VIPP significant increase in sensitivity of the intervention
A first attempt at enhancing parental sensitivity group compared to the control group (Klein
through an attachment-based videotaped model of Velderman, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, & Van
sensitive parenting appeared to be ineffective IJzendoorn, 2006). Stein and his colleagues (2006)
(Lambermon & Van IJzendoorn, 1989). A problem implemented an adapted version of the VIPP in a
with videotaped model behaviour is that parents sample of mothers with eating disorders and their
may not identify with the specific model of a parent- babies in the United Kingdom and found a
child dyad on the videotape. Parents apparently significant improvement in maternal mealtime
need a mirror of their own daily interactions with interaction with the infant (including appropriate
their child to change their behaviour. Based on (non-)verbal responses to infant cues). In Lithuania,
attachment theory (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, insensitive mothers significantly gained in
1982), a first version of our video-feedback sensitivity after receiving VIPP compared to a
intervention program was developed in a study of control group of insensitive mothers (Kalinauskiene
families with adopted children (Juffer, 1993). The et al., 2009). Mothers of adopted children who
intervention consisted of three sessions and received a first version of the VIPP (see before) also
appeared to be successful in promoting maternal outperformed their control counterparts in
sensitivity, secure infant-mother attachment, and the sensitivity. A VIPP program was implemented in a
prevention of disorganised attachment (Juffer, sample of mothers with preterm children and
Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2005). children suffering from dermatitis but outcomes
On the basis of these results a new, nationwide and have not yet been reported (Cassiba et al., 2008).
state subsidised adoption aftercare service was Finally, mothers of toddlers screened for high levels
started and since the year 2000 Dutch parents can of externalising problem behaviour showed
ask for this service for each newly adopted child, significantly more sensitive discipline after the
including special-needs or older-placed children and VIPP-SD intervention (Van Zeijl et al., 2006).
sibling placements (Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Concluding, VIPP appeared to be effective with
& Van IJzendoorn, 2008a). respect to sensitive parenting (see Figure 1) and the
The first version of the video-feedback effect sizes were in the same range or higher
intervention was elaborated for other types of compared to the combined effect size for parental
families and extended from infants to toddlers and sensitivity found in the meta-analysis of
preschoolers at the Centre for Child and Family attachment-based intervention studies (Bakermans-
Studies (Leiden University, the Netherlands) into the Kranenburg et al., 2003).
current Video-feedback Intervention to promote A definite conclusion about the effectiveness of
Positive Parenting (VIPP; Juffer, Bakermans- VIPP for enhancing attachment security can not be
Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2008b) consisting of drawn yet. Children’s attachment security was not
four to six sessions, organised into structured stages reported in the study on mothers with eating
according to a detailed protocol. An extension of the disorders nor in the study on children with
VIPP program was developed and tested a few years externalising problems. Mixed outcomes were
later, adding a component aimed at enhancing found in the other studies, with significant, positive
adequate discipline (VIPP-SD: VIPP with an outcomes in one adoption sample, and no
additional focus on Sensitive Discipline; Juffer et al., significant effect in the study on insensitive
2008b; Van Zeijl et al., 2006). The Centre for Child mothers. Interestingly, however, although in the
and Family Studies offers training courses on how to Leiden intervention study of insecure mothers no
use VIPP and VIPP-SD in practice. overall intervention effect on attachment security
could be traced, we found that the outcomes varied
Effectiveness of VIPP for children differing in temperamental reactivity:
VIPP has been used in various countries in several some children gained more from the intervention
studies involving at risk parents or children. VIPP, than others.

49
d d
Cohenʼs
0.9
Cohen's
0.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0

Externalizing
Insensitive

Mothers of
Disordered
Insecure

Analysis
Mothers

Mothers of
Adopted
Children
Mothers

Meta-
Mothers

2003

Children
Eating

Figure 1. Effectiveness of VIPP on sensitive parenting in various samples of at risk parents and parents
of at risk children (total N = 579), compared to the effectiveness on sensitivity in randomised control
trials in the 2003 meta-analysis of attachment-based interventions (N = 6,282; Bakermans-
Kranenburg et al., 2003)

Differential susceptibility highly reactive intervention infants, attachment


Our intervention may not be equally effective for all security and change in pre- to posttest maternal
children involved. Children may be differential sensitivity were significantly correlated, r = .64, p <
susceptible to environmental changes (Belsky, .05. In the less reactive intervention group the
1997; see also Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van correlation was r = .11, ns. The difference in
IJzendoorn, 2007; Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, correlations was significant (p = .03; Klein
& Van IJzendoorn, 2007). According to the Velderman et al., 2006). The experimentally
evolutionary-based differential susceptibility induced change in maternal sensitivity appeared to
hypothesis children vary in their susceptibility to impact more strongly on attachment security in the
parental rearing, for better (when receiving sensitive highly reactive infant group. Thus, the outcomes
care, or improved sensitive care after a successful confirmed the prediction that highly reactive
intervention) and for worse (when receiving less children are more susceptible to experimentally
optimal care). Previous studies indicated that highly induced environmental change than less reactive
reactive children may be the more susceptible infants. Parents of highly reactive infants may
children (Belsky, Hsieh, & Crnic, 1998). Against therefore be the most rewarding targets of
this background, the differential effectiveness of our intervention efforts.
intervention was tested in children with high versus Recent outcomes also document intervention
average to low negative reactivity. effects at a neurobiological level: In our sample
We found that the mothers of highly reactive with 1- to 3-year-old children screened for relatively
infants were more susceptible to the influence of the high levels of externalising behaviour the VIPP-SD
intervention and gained more in sensitivity, and that program proved to be effective in decreasing daily
highly reactive infants in their turn were more cortisol production in children with the DRD4 7-
susceptible to (changes in) their mothers’ sensitivity repeat allele (a polymorphism that is associated with
(see below for a case example). In the group of motivational and reward mechanisms and ADHD in

50
children), but not in children without the DRD4 7- modest number of sessions (usually four to six
repeat allele. These findings indicate that children sessions). Building a supporting relationship
are differentially susceptible to intervention effects between the intervener and the parent (Bowlby,
dependent on the presence of the 7-repeat DRD4 1988) is a crucial element of the intervention.
allele (Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, In the VIPP programs, parent and child are
Mesman, Alink, & Juffer, 2008). Moreover, VIPP- videotaped during daily situations at their home (for
SD proved to be effective in decreasing example, playing together, bathing, mealtime)
externalising behaviour in the children with the during brief episodes of 10 to 30 minutes. Parents
DRD4 7-repeat allele. These effects were largest in are encouraged to react to their children the way
children with the DRD4 7-repeat allele whose they normally do. In the period between the home
parents showed the largest increase in the use of visit and the intervention session, the intervener
positive discipline as a result of the intervention reviews the videotape and prepares her comments
(Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, on the parent-child interaction as shown on the
Mesman, & Juffer, 2008). This first experimental videotape. The intervener writes down her
test of (measured) gene by (observed) environment comments, directed by the guidelines of the
interaction in human development indicates that protocol and at the same time screens the videotape
children may be differentially susceptible to for suitable fragments to review with the parent
intervention efforts depending on genetic during the intervention session. For example, when
differences. the theme of exploration versus attachment
In sum, we documented the evidence-base of behaviour (see Table 1) will be discussed in the next
the VIPP program as an effective intervention for visit, the intervener searches for relevant fragments.
enhancing sensitive parenting and revealed Thus fragments of the child making eye contact or
promising outcomes regarding children’s differ- seeking proximity are used to illustrate the child’s
ential susceptibility to intervention effects based on attachment behaviour, whereas fragments of the
their temperamental or genetic characteristics. But child’s play behaviour are used to illustrate
how can VIPP be used in practice? exploration. In the intervention the intervener
connects the fragments to general messages
How to use VIPP? described in the protocol. For example, while
In the VIPP programs parents are offered short-term showing attachment and exploration fragments, the
behaviourally focused interventions aimed at intervener may explain that these behaviours ask for
enhancing sensitive parenting (VIPP) and adequate differential parental reactions: Children’s
discipline (VIPP-SD). The programs are attachment signals should be met with prompt,
standardised and individualised, meaning that the adequate reactions, whereas parents should not
interveners work from a standard protocol (Juffer, interfere in children’s play activities. The intervener
Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2008c) may also comment that play is important for
but attune the guidelines to the individual parent- children because they learn a lot from manipulating
child dyad. The VIPP programs use videotaped toys. At the same time, playing together provides
interactions of the parent and child involved and children with an extra dimension compared to
video feedback: interveners watch and discuss the playing alone: their overtures are responded to,
videotape together with the parent. The VIPP making them feel understood, and moments of joy
programs are home-based and short-term: the can be shared (the intervener may say: ‘A toy does
interventions are implemented in the home in a not smile back, you do!’).

Table 1. Sensitivity themes used in VIPP and sensitivity and discipline themes used in VIPP-SD

Session Sensitivity Discipline

1. Exploration versus attachment behaviour Inductive discipline and distraction


2. ‘Speaking for the child’ Positive reinforcement
3. ‘Sensitivity chain’ Sensitive time-out
4. Sharing emotions Empathy for the child

51
During the next visit the videotape is shown to two intervention sessions focus on child behaviour
the parent, and the intervener reviews the videotape (for example by actively ‘speaking for the child’).
with her, discussing the selected fragments on the The following two intervention sessions are (also)
basis of the comments prepared before the session. directed at parental behaviour, for example by
Positive interaction moments shown on the discussing parental behaviour in a ‘sensitivity
videotape are always emphasised. Focusing on chain’. This specific order is part of the VIPP
positive interactions serves the goal of showing the protocol: addressing parental behaviour is
mother that she is able to act as a sensitive, postponed until the parent and the intervener have
competent parent: she should feel empowered by had more time to build on a working relationship.
positive feedback instead of incompetent by Another advantage is that a first and primary focus
negative feedback. To focus the parent’s attention on the child’s perspective is guaranteed in the first
on positive moments, the videotape is stilled and sessions.
the parent is shown a picture of a successful
interaction or a happy child. By repeating positive VIPP-SD
fragments, reinforcing messages are emphasised VIPP-SD is based on an integration of attachment
and negative moments are counterbalanced. In case theory (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1982) and
of insensitive parental behaviour, the parent is coercion theory (Patterson, 1982). Coercion theory
encouraged to use more sensitive behaviours, describes how ineffective parental discipline results
preferably behaviours she displayed at other in increasingly difficult and challenging child
moments on the videotape, so that she is her own behaviour. VIPP-SD can be characterised as a
model of competent parenting. behaviourally oriented intervention using video
Video feedback provides the opportunity to feedback to promote parental sensitivity as well as
focus on the child’s videotaped behaviour, thereby adequate discipline strategies (see Mesman et al.,
stimulating the parent’s observational skills and 2008).
empathy for her child. It also enables positive The VIPP-SD program consists of four specific
reinforcement of the parent’s moments of sensitive themes (see Table 1) that are elaborated during the
behaviour shown on the videotape. Video feedback intervention sessions in addition to the sensitivity
thus enables the intervener to focus on both parts of themes: (1) Inductive discipline and distraction:
Ainsworth’s definition of sensitivity: accurately recommending induction and distraction as non-
perceiving child signals and adequately responding coercive responses to difficult child behaviour or
to them (Ainsworth et al., 1978). potentially conflict evoking situations; (2) Positive
reinforcement: praising the child for positive
VIPP behaviour and ignoring negative attention-seeking;
VIPP consists of four themes (Table 1) that are (3) the use of a ‘Sensitive time-out’ to sensitively
elaborated successively during four home visits: (1) de-escalate temper tantrums; and (4) Empathy for
Exploration versus attachment behaviour: showing the child, in particular in consistent discipline and
the difference between the child’s contact-seeking clear limit setting. For example, in the first session
behaviour and play, and explaining the differential the parent is encouraged to distract her child in case
responses needed from the parent; (2) ‘Speaking for of challenging behaviour and direct the child’s
the child’; promoting the accurate perception of attention to objects or situations that are allowed,
children’s (subtle) signals by verbalising their facial thus creating the opportunity for positive
expressions and non-verbal cues shown on the behaviours. At the same time, the parent is
videotape; (3) ‘Sensitivity chain’: explaining the encouraged to use induction, that is providing the
relevance of prompt and adequate responding to the reasons for a prohibition or parental intervention
child’s signals (‘chain’: child signal — parental (Hoffman, 2000), thus helping the child to
response — reaction of the child); and (4) Sharing (gradually) understand the background of parental
emotions: showing and encouraging parents’ rules and empathise with other people’s interests.
affective attunement to the positive and negative Extra ‘booster’ visits (intervention sessions 5 and
emotions of their child (see below for a case 6) may be used to review all feedback and
example illustrating these themes). information from the first four intervention
The themes are ordered in a way that the first sessions.

52
Case example of VIPP: Sarah and Linda Intervention
Sarah and her daughter Linda participated in the In the next home visit the first session of video
Leiden intervention study with low-SES parents. In feedback was implemented and the intervener
this study we selected mothers on the basis of their emphasised the balance between children’s
insecure mental representation of attachment, attachment behaviour (e.g., contact seeking) and
measured with the Adult Attachment Interview exploration (e.g., playing). Sarah said that she found
(George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985). Compared to it important for Linda to get a lot of space to play by
secure parents, insecure parents show more herself. The intervener agreed but also stressed the
insensitive parenting behaviour and their children importance of contact and playing together. She
are at elevated risk of insecure attachment (Van explained to Sarah that for Linda, as for all young,
IJzendoorn, 1995). The VIPP program aimed at dependent children, the mother figure is extremely
enhancing Sarah’s sensitivity and Linda’s important, and Sarah seemed flattered.
attachment security by providing Sarah with four In the second intervention visit the intervener
sessions of video-feedback (see also Klein noticed that the parent-child interaction had
Velderman, Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van changed since the previous visit: Linda was
IJzendoorn, 2008). communicating more and Sarah responded to her
In her childhood, Sarah’s parents divorced and daughter’s positive cues (smiles, vocalisations) and
from the age of 7 years Sarah was reared by her seemed to enjoy it. During this session the
mother, living on welfare. Because Sarah’s mother intervener used the method of ‘speaking for the
had been hospitalised several times, she had lived child’ (Carter, Osofsky, & Han, 1991) to draw
with her grandmother as well. Sarah had a 6-month- Sarah’s attention to Linda’s signals and expressions,
old daughter, Linda, who belonged to the 20% most and she identified when Linda showed pleasure or
reactive infants in our study (assessed with a distress. After the visit, the intervener noted in her
temperament questionnaire). Highly reactive infants logbook ‘There is more eye contact between Sarah
can be characterised as the more negative emotional and Linda. Sarah is more aware of the moments that
infants who become easily distressed. In our study Linda is looking for contact and reacts with
we hypothesized that highly reactive children would watching, smiling, or talking.’
be more susceptible to (changes in) their mother’s During the third intervention session, Sarah
behaviour (see before). reacted positively to the video fragments. She
spontaneously noticed that the interaction looked
Pretest different this time, and attributed it to the fact that
At the pretest home visit, Sarah was friendly but Linda was older now (it seemed not to occur to her
superficial in her contact with the intervener. Sarah that her own behaviour also had changed). Adequate
acknowledged that she was not used to playing with and prompt reactions to children’s signals, the crux
Linda and explained: ‘Babies don’t play at this age, of sensitivity, were addressed in this session. The
they only put things in their mouths.’ The intervener intervener emphasised that Linda’s behaviour was
noted that there was little parent-child interaction, related to Sarah’s behaviour, and used interaction
while close physical contact and eye contact were chains – child signal, the parent’s response, and the
markedly lacking. Sarah said she did not like Linda child’s positive reaction to this response – to
to cry or fuss. In a free-play episode, Sarah’s highlight sensitive parenting. For example, a video
behaviour was rated as insensitive (rating of 3.5) on fragment revealed that during playing together
Ainsworth’s 9-point Sensitivity scale (Ainsworth, Linda showed that she enjoyed a particular game,
Bell, & Stayton, 1974), thereby scoring below the Sarah thereupon repeated it, and Linda then
mean (5) of the scale. The mean rating of the 81 vocalised in a cheerful manner. The intervener
participants in our study was 4.23 (SD = 1.27). After showed that Sarah gave Linda enough time and
the pretest, the intervener rated the possibilities to space to explore, while eye-contact and a happy
successfully change the mother’s behaviour as atmosphere were also highlighted in the video
reasonable. There were some openings: Sarah was feedback. Sarah said that she did not want a child
curious about how other mothers handled things who always needed her. The video made her see that
(e.g., playing together) and she showed interest in this was not the case. Linda could do a lot by herself
learning about child development. and every now and then looked for her.

53
The central theme of the fourth intervention An innovative avenue of intervention research
session was sharing emotions. When children meet addresses the neurobiological effects of
understanding when they express their feelings, interventions, testing whether attachment-based
they will feel free to share their positive and interventions are not only effective at observed
negative emotions with their parent. One of the behavioural levels but also at neurobiological levels
video fragments was very instructive: When Linda and, for example, result in altered daily cortisol
was crying, Sarah reacted sensitively and comforted production (e.g., Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van
her daughter. The video then showed a peaceful IJzendoorn, Mesman et al., 2008; Dozier, Peloso,
‘together’ atmosphere. After the visit, the intervener Lewis, Laurenceau, & Levine, 2008; Fisher, Gunnar,
noted that Sarah had learned a lot from the video Dozier, Bruce, & Pears, 2006). Another issue that
feedback. Although she still had impatient remains to be resolved is whether centre-based,
moments, Sarah was more capable of seeing Linda’s group interventions (e.g., Hoffman, Marvin, Cooper,
perspective and more often interacted in a sensitive & Powell, 2006; Niccols, 2008) are as effective as
way. The intervener hoped that she had made home-based, individualised programs such as VIPP.
Linda’s needs sufficiently clear to Sarah. In future studies we hope to test the VIPP
programs in new settings and clinical samples, for
Posttest example in parents with children with autism
In the posttest, Sarah was assigned a rating of 5.25 spectrum disorder, in ethnic minority families, and
on the Sensitivity scale, thus showing a large in childcare as well as in institutional settings. Also,
increase from pre- to posttest of almost two scale the role of fathers in enhancing attachment security
points. Comparable to the other intervention of the children will be examined. Hopefully, short-
mothers in our study (Klein Velderman et al., 2006), term, attachment-based interventions with a focus
Sarah outperformed the control mothers who did not on parental sensitivity may offer parents the support
receive parenting support. In the Strange Situation they need, and promote sensitive parenting and
Procedure (Ainsworth et al., 1978) Linda was positive family interactions.
classified as a secure infant (B2). Sarah’s response
to a questionnaire completed after the intervention Acknowledgements
sessions showed that she evaluated the video Support from the Netherlands Organisation for
feedback as very positive and instructive. Scientific Research to the second author (NWO
This case example also illustrates our findings VIDI grant) and to the third author (NWO
that parents of highly reactive children were more SPINOZA Prize) is gratefully acknowledged.
susceptible to the influence of the intervention Femmie Juffer is supported by Wereldkinderen.
sessions (Sarah showed a large increase in
sensitivity from pre- to posttest), and that their References
highly reactive infants were susceptible to their Ainsworth, M.D.S., Bell, S.M., & Stayton, D.J.
parents’ improved sensitivity so that their (1974). Infant-mother attachment and social
attachment security was enhanced: Linda appeared development: Socialization as a product of
to be securely attached after the intervention. reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In M.P.M.
Richards (Eds.), The integration of a child into
Conclusion a social world (pp. 99-135). London:
Our series of meta-analyses of attachment-based Cambridge University Press.
intervention studies showed that short-term, Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., &
behaviourally oriented interventions that focus on Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. A
parental sensitivity and make use of video feedback psychological study of the Strange Situation.
are most effective. Based on these insights, we Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
developed Video-feedback Intervention to promote Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn,
Positive Parenting (VIPP and VIPP-SD: VIPP with M. H. (2007). Research Review: Genetic
an additional focus on Sensitive Discipline). In vulnerability or differential susceptibility in
several studies VIPP appeared to be an efficient and child development: The case of attachment.
effective intervention for enhancing parental Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
sensitivity. 48, 1160-1173.

54
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzendoorn, Cassiba, R., Van IJzendoorn, M.H., Coppola, G.,
M.H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta- Bruno, S., Constantini, A., Gatto, S., Elia, L., &
analysis of sensitivity and attachment Tota, A. (2008). Supporting families with
interventions in early childhood. Psychological preterm children and children suffering from
Bulletin, 129, 195-215. dermatitis. In F. Juffer, M.J. Bakermans-
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzendoorn, Kranenburg & M.H. van IJzendoorn (Eds.),
M.H., & Juffer, F. (2005). Disorganized infant Promoting positive parenting: An attachment-
attachment and preventive interventions: A based intervention (pp. 91-110). New York:
review and meta-analysis. Infant Mental Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis.
Health Journal, 26, 191-216. Chaffin, M., Hanson, R., Saunders, B.E., Nichols,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzendoorn, T., Barnett, D., Zeanah, C.,
M.H., Mesman, J., Alink, L.R.A., & Juffer, F. Berliner, L., Egeland, B., Newman, E., Lyon, T.,
(2008). Effects of an attachment-based LeTourneau, E., & Miller-Perrin, C. (2006).
intervention on daily cortisol moderated by Report of the APSAC Task Force on
dopamine receptor D4: A randomized control attachment therapy, reactive attachment
trial on 1- to 3-year-olds screened for disorder, and attachment problems. Child
externalizing behavior. Development and Maltreatment, 11, 76-89.
Psychopathology, 20, 805-820. De Wolff, M. S., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997).
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzendoorn, Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on
M.H., Pijlman, F.T.A., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. parental antecedents of infant attachment.
(2008). Differential susceptibility to Child Development, 68, 571-591.
intervention: Dopamine D4 Receptor Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lewis, E., Laurenceau, J. P.,
Polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates & Levine, S. (2008). Effects of an attachment-
effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a based intervention on the cortisol production of
randomized control trial. Developmental infants and toddlers in foster care.
Psychology, 44, 293-300. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 845-
Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to 859.
environmental influence: An evolutionary Fearon, R.P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.,
argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182-186. Van IJzendoorn, M.H., Lapsley, A., &
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Roisman, G.I. (in press). The significance of
IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for insecure attachment and disorganization in the
worse: Differential susceptibility to development of children’s externalizing
environmental influences. Current Directions behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child
in Psychological Science, 16, 300-304. Development.
Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Fisher, P., Gunnar, M., Dozier, M., Bruce, J., &
Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as Pears, K. (2006). Effects of therapeutic
antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems interventions for foster children on behavioral
and inhibition at age 3: Differential suscept- problems, caregiver attachment, and stress
ibility to rearing influence? Development and regulatory neural systems. Annuals of the New
Psychopathology, 10, 301-319. York Academy of Sciences, 40, 1-11.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Adult
Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Attachment Interview. Unpublished
Books. manuscript.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical Harlow, H.F. (1958). The nature of love. American
applications of attachment theory. London: Pychologist, 13, 673-685.
Routledge. Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2006). Frightened,
Carter, S.L., Osofsky, J.D., & Hann, D.M. (1991). threatening, and dissociative (FR) parental
Speaking for the baby: A therapeutic behavior as related to infant D attachment in
intervention with adolescent mothers and their low-risk samples: Description, discussion, and
infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12, 291- interpretations. Development and
301. Psychopathology, 18, 309-343.

55
Hoffman, K. T., Marvin, R. S., Cooper, G., & Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Bakermans-
Powell, B. (2006). Changing toddlers’ and Kranenburg, M.J. (2008). Attachment-based
preschoolers’ attachment classifications: The interventions in early childhood: An overview.
circle of security intervention. Journal of In F. Juffer, M.J. Bakermans-Kranenburg &
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1017- M.H. van IJzendoorn (Eds.), Promoting
1026. positive parenting: An attachment-based
Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and moral intervention (pp. 37-57). New York: Lawrence
development: Implications for caring and Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis.
justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Kalinauskiene, L., Cekuoliene, D., Van IJzendoorn,
Press. M.H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Juffer, F.,
Jaffari-Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, & Kusakovskaja, I. (2009). Supporting
M.H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & insensitive mothers: The Vilnius randomized
Mooijaart, A. (2006). Social development control trial of video feedback intervention to
from infancy to adolescence: Longitudinal promote maternal sensitivity and infant
and concurrent factors in an adoption attachment. Child: Care, Health &
sample. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1143- Development, 35, 613-623.
1153. Klein Velderman, M. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.,
Juffer, F. (1993). Verbonden door adoptie. Een Juffer, F., & Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2006).
experimenteel onderzoek naar hechting en Effects of attachment-based interventions on
competentie in gezinnen met een maternal sensitivity and infant attachment:
adoptiebaby. [Attached through adoption. An Differential susceptibility of highly reactive
experimental study of attachment and infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 266-
competence in families with adopted babies.] 274.
Amersfoort, the Netherlands: Academische Klein Velderman, M., Juffer, F., Bakermans-
Uitgeverij. Kranenburg, M.J., & Van IJzendoorn, M.H.
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van (2008). A case study and process evaluation of
IJzendoorn, M.H. (2005). The importance of video feedback to promote positive parenting
parenting in the development of disorganized alone and with representational attachment
attachment: Evidence from a preventive discussions. In F. Juffer, M.J. Bakermans-
intervention study in adoptive families. Journal Kranenburg & M.H. van IJzendoorn (Eds.),
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 263- Promoting positive parenting: An attachment-
274. based intervention (pp. 23-36). New York:
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis.
IJzendoorn, M.H. (2008a). Supporting adoptive Lambermon, M.W.E., & Van IJzendoorn, M.H.
families with video-feedback intervention. In F. (1989). Influencing mother-infant interaction
Juffer, M.J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, & M.H. through videotaped or written instruction:
van IJzendoorn (Eds.), Promoting positive Evaluation of a parent education program.
parenting: An attachment-based intervention Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 4, 449-
(pp. 139-153). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum / 458.
Taylor & Francis. Madigan, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Moran, G., Pederson, D. R.,
IJzendoorn, M.H. (Eds.) (2008b). Promoting & Benoit, D. (2006). Unresolved states of
positive parenting: An attachment-based mind, anomalous parental behavior, and
intervention. New York: Lawrence disorganized attachment: A review and meta-
Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis. analysis of a transmission gap. Attachment &
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van Human Development, 8, 89-111.
IJzendoorn, M.H. (2008c). Manual Video- Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for
feedback Intervention to promote Positive identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented
Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP-SD) during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In
(version 2.0). Leiden University: Centre for M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & E.M.
Child and Family Studies. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool

56
years. Theory, research, and intervention (pp. relationship between mothers with bulimic
121-182). Chicago: The University of Chicago eating disorders and their infants: A
Press. randomized, controlled trial of video feedback.
Mesman, J., Stolk, M.N., Van Zeijl, J., Alink, The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 899-
L.R.A., Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, 906.
M.J., Van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Koot, H.M. Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (1995). Adult attachment
(2008). Extending the video-feedback representations, parental responsiveness, and
intervention to sensitive discipline: The early infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the
prevention of antisocial behavior. In F. Juffer, predictive validity of the Adult Attachment
M.J. Bakermans-Kranenburg & M.H. van Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387-
IJzendoorn (Eds.), Promoting positive 403.
parenting: An attachment-based intervention Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., & Duyvesteyn, M.
(pp. 171-191). New York: Lawrence G. C. (1995). Breaking the intergenerational
Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis. cycle of insecure attachment: A review of the
Niccols, A. (2008). ‘Right from the Start’: effects of attachment-based interventions on
Randomized trial comparing an attachment maternal sensitivity and infant security.
group intervention to supportive home visiting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 225-248.
49, 754-764. Van IJzendoorn, M.H., Schuengel, C., &
Patterson, G.R. (1982). Coercive family process. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (1999).
Eugene, OR: Castilia. Disorganized attachment in early childhood:
Pignotti, M., & Mercer, J. (2007). Holding therapy Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and
and dyadic developmental psychotherapy are sequelae. Development and Psychopathology,
not supported and acceptable social work 11, 225-249.
interventions: A systematic research synthesis Van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Van IJzendoorn, M.H.,
revisited. Research on Social Work Practice, Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Juffer, F., Stolk,
17, 513-519. M.N., Koot, H.M., & Alink, L.R.A. (2006).
Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E.A., & Collins, Attachment-based intervention for enhancing
W.A. (2005). The development of the person. sensitive discipline in mothers of one – to
The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation three-year-old children at risk for externalizing
from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford behavior problems. A Randomized controlled
Press. trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Stein, A., Woolley, H., Senior, R., Hertzmann, L., Psychology, 74, 994-1005.
Lovel, M., Lee, J., Cooper, S., Wheatcroft, R., Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure
Challacombe, F., Patel, P., & Nicol-Harper, R. base from which to explore close relationships.
(2006). Treating disturbances in the Child Development, 71, 164-172.

57
58
59
60

You might also like