You are on page 1of 31

姝 Academy of Management Review

2007, Vol. 32, No. 4, 1199–1228.

WHAT’S THE DIFFERENCE? DIVERSITY


CONSTRUCTS AS SEPARATION, VARIETY,
OR DISPARITY IN ORGANIZATIONS
DAVID A. HARRISON
The Pennsylvania State University

KATHERINE J. KLEIN
University of Pennsylvania

Research on organizational diversity, heterogeneity, and related concepts has prolif-


erated in the past decade, but few consistent findings have emerged. We argue that
the construct of diversity requires closer examination. We describe three distinctive
types of diversity: separation, variety, and disparity. Failure to recognize the meaning,
maximum shape, and assumptions underlying each type has held back theory devel-
opment and yielded ambiguous research conclusions. We present guidelines for
conceptualization, measurement, and theory testing, highlighting the special case of
demographic diversity

Differences are a challenge. Organizations Smith, 2001). Cumulative findings about the con-
have struggled to embrace and manage them sequences of within-unit differences have been
successfully. Researchers have struggled to con- weak, inconsistent, or both. A recent meta-
ceptualize and study them effectively. Theories analysis of the effects of task-related (e.g. ten-
predict differing effects of such differences— ure) and non-task-related (e.g., ethnic and gen-
that they will spark integrative insights, creativ- der) diversity revealed no dependable effects on
ity, and innovation (e.g., Finkelstein & Ham- performance or cohesiveness (Webber & Don-
brick, 1996; Hoffman & Maier, 1961), or that they ahue, 2001). Summarizing a narrative review of
will provoke conflict, division, and dissolution the same domain, Jackson, Joshi, and Erhardt
(e.g., Chatman, 1991; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). concluded, “Our examination of these studies
Guided by these theories, research on differ- yielded few discernible patterns in the results.
ences of many kinds, including demographic For most diversity dimensions, the findings
variables, values, skills, personality, and pay, across studies were mixed” (2003: 810). Beyond
has burgeoned. The volume of such research demographic diversity, Bloom and Michel
has nearly doubled every five years, with a joint reached a similar conclusion: “Research has
ABI-Inform–PsycInfo search yielding 19 “diver- yielded mixed results about what amount of pay
sity” investigations in 1988, 45 in 1993, 66 in 1998, dispersion is optimal. In some cases, more dis-
and 134 in 2003. persed pay distributions have been positively
Yet, as noted by many authors, the payoff from related to performance outcomes. . . . In other
this profusion of research has been disappoint- cases, greater dispersion has been negatively
ing (e.g., Harrison, Price, & Bell, 1998; Pitcher & related to performance outcomes” (2002: 33).
In response to these disappointing cumulative
results, organizational scholars have refined
Author order was determined by the flip of a coin. We are their theories and analyses, typically by elabo-
grateful to guest editor Terry Mitchell and two anonymous
reviewers for supportive and constructive suggestions. Paul
rating possible mediators and moderators of
Goodman, Don Hambrick, David Hofmann, Susan Jackson, connections between within-unit diversity and
Charles O’Reilly, Ben Schneider, and Jonathan Ziegert also unit outcomes (e.g., Bloom & Michel, 2002; Car-
provided helpful feedback and recommendations on earlier penter, 2002; Chatman & Flynn, 2001; Simons,
drafts of this manuscript. We further appreciate the assis- Pelled, & Smith, 1999; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998).
tance of several current and former doctoral students in
background research for this manuscript: Ellen Godfrey, An-
We applaud these efforts. But we suggest that
drew Knight, Jessica Saltz, Amy Nicole Salvaggio, and Hock- the very construct of diversity requires closer
Peng Sin. examination and refinement. Authors frequently
1199
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright
holder’s express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.
1200 Academy of Management Review October

and casually use the term diversity and such ities in the literature. We then offer a detailed
synonyms as heterogeneity, dissimilarity, and presentation of our typology, highlighting differ-
dispersion. Yet the precise meaning of diversity ences in the meanings, maxima, and major the-
is not clear. Casting a deliberately broad net, oretical streams associated with the separation,
we seek to clarify the meaning of differences variety, and disparity types. We explore the ty-
within organizational units. We use “diversity” pology’s implications for the special case of de-
to describe the distribution of differences among mographic diversity, showing that the same de-
the members of a unit with respect to a common mographic differences within units may be
attribute, X, such as tenure, ethnicity, conscien- conceptualized as separation, variety, or dispar-
tiousness, task attitude, or pay. Diversity is a ity. Following this, we describe methodological
unit-level, compositional construct. Thus, in de- consequences of our diversity typology. We em-
scribing the diversity of a given attribute within phasize the importance of matching a specific
a unit (e.g., a group or organization), one de- operationalization of diversity to a specific con-
scribes the unit as a whole, not a focal member’s ceptualization of diversity. In the final section of
differences from other members—which is the the paper, we describe new directions for diver-
subject of most relational demography research sity theory and empirical investigation, enabled
(e.g., Tsui, Egan, & O’Reilly, 1992). Diversity, as and motivated by the diversity typology we pro-
we use the term, is also attribute specific. A unit pose.
is not diverse per se. Rather, it is diverse with
respect to one or more specific features of its
WHAT DO WE TALK ABOUT WHEN WE TALK
members.
ABOUT DIVERSITY?
We propose that diversity is not one thing but
three things. The substance, pattern, operation- For many reasons, the organizational litera-
alization, and likely consequences of those three ture on diversity is confusing— difficult to un-
things differ markedly. Diversity within an or- derstand and difficult to synthesize. It is difficult
ganizational unit may be indicative of separa- to synthesize in part because consistent find-
tion: differences in position or opinion among ings and cumulative insights have not emerged,
unit members. Such differences reflect disagree- but also simply because the diversity literature
ment or opposition— horizontal distance along a itself is so diverse. Much of it addresses within-
single continuum representing dissimilarity in a unit differences in demographic variables: gen-
particular attitude or value, for example. Alter- der (e.g., O’Reilly, Williams, & Barsade, 1997),
natively, within-unit diversity may be indicative race and ethnicity (e.g., Riordan & Shore, 1997),
of variety: differences in kind or category, pri- age (Pelled, 1996), tenure (e.g., Hambrick, Cho, &
marily of information, knowledge, or experience Chen, 1996), education (e.g., Jackson et al., 1991),
among unit members. And, finally, within-unit functional background (e.g., Jehn, Northcraft, &
diversity may indicate disparity: differences in Neale, 1999), and marital status (e.g., Harrison,
concentration of valued social assets or re- Price, Gavin, & Florey, 2002). Further, research-
sources such as pay and status among unit ers have investigated differences in many non-
members—vertical differences that, at their ex- demographic variables, including values (e.g.,
treme, privilege a few over many. We use this Jehn et al., 1999), attitudes (e.g., Harrison et al.,
insight—implicit in some of the diversity litera- 1998), conscientiousness (e.g., Barrick, Stewart,
ture, but neither widely recognized across do- Neubert, & Mount, 1998), affect (e.g., Barsade,
mains nor fully understood—as the starting Ward, Turner, & Sonnenfeld, 2000), dress (e.g.,
point for an analysis of the construct definition, Rafaeli & Pratt, 1993), network ties (e.g., Beck-
theoretical substance, and methodological im- man & Haunschild, 2002), individual perfor-
plications of each diversity type. We present this mance (e.g., Doerr, Mitchell, Schriesheim, Freed,
typology in the hope of bringing greater theoret- & Zhou, 2002), and pay (e.g., Pfeffer & Langton,
ical insight, as well as empirical rigor, to the 1988).
study of organizational diversity of all forms. Synthesis is also made difficult by the varied
Below, we first provide a brief overview of theoretical perspectives used to guide diversity
several examples of diversity theory and re- research. Often, these perspectives suggest con-
search, noting conceptual confusion in defini- tradictory effects. And each perspective has re-
tions of diversity and some resulting ambigu- ceived some (albeit mixed) support in empirical
2007 Harrison and Klein 1201

studies. One such perspective draws on similar- fined value diversity by noting that it “occurs
ity attraction (Byrne, 1971) and social categoriza- when members of a workgroup differ in terms of
tion (Tajfel, 1981; Turner, 1985) theories to sug- what they think the group’s real task, goal, tar-
gest that diversity of attributes such as age, get, or mission should be” (1999: 745).
values, and personality limits within-unit be- None of these definitions is inappropriate. In-
havioral and social integration, fosters conflict deed, our definition of diversity above is quite
and turnover, and diminishes morale, cohesion, similar. However, even the most exacting of
and performance (Williams & O’Reilly, 1998). A these definitions ties diversity to differences
second theoretical perspective draws from eco- and goes no further. The definitions do not
logical and cognitive models of variation, selec- pinpoint and substantiate the nature of those
tion, and retention (e.g., Campbell, 1960) and the differences, nor do they specify the collective
cybernetic principle of requisite variety (Ashby, distribution—the compositional pattern of dif-
1956) to highlight the benefits of heterogeneity ferences—within a unit. On the one hand, read-
in information resources. This perspective sug- ers may discern from such definitions the mean-
gests that diversity of attributes such as func- ing of minimal diversity: it occurs when there
tional background, tenure, and range of network are no differences on attribute X among the
ties may enrich the supply of ideas, unique ap- members of a unit. On the other hand, the dis-
proaches, and knowledge available to a unit, tribution, shape, and meaning of maximal diver-
enhancing unit creativity, quality of decision sity are less clear. We know what is least di-
making, and complex performance (Williams & verse but not what is most diverse, and,
O’Reilly, 1998). A third perspective builds on dis- therefore, the direction of “more” (or less) diver-
tributive justice theory (e.g., Adams, 1963; sity can be ambiguous. For example, when a
Deutsch, 1985), tournament theory (e.g., Lazear, researcher studies diversity in tenure, personal-
1995; Lazear & Rosen, 1981), and stratification, ity, or pay, is diversity maximized when the at-
status hierarchy, or status characteristics theo- tribute in question is evenly distributed among
ries (e.g., Berger, Fisek, Norman, & Zelditch, 1977; unit members such that each point along the
Blau, 1960). This perspective highlights relative continuum or each category is represented? Or
comparisons among members to suggest that is diversity maximized when unit members are
within-unit diversity, typically of pay but also of polarized, half very high and half very low, on
other valued assets or resources that connote the attribute in question? Could diversity be
prestige or power, leads to internal competition, maximized when one unit member clearly sur-
suppression of voice, reduced (quality of) com- passes all the other members? Last, is the shape
munication, and interpersonal undermining. of maximum diversity different depending on
Finally, synthesis is made difficult because, the X attribute a researcher is investigating, his
as a term, diversity has a taken-for-granted or her theoretical perspective, or the predicted
quality in the organization literature; it is sel- effects of increasing diversity? One can confi-
dom explicitly defined. Researchers use a vari- dently order organizational units in terms of in-
ety of labels, often interchangeably, to refer to creasing amounts of cohesion, morale, size, or
diversity, including dispersion, heterogeneity, performance. But units that are ordered in terms
dissimilarity, disagreement, divergence, varia- of increasing diversity are difficult to visualize.
tion, and inequality, or their opposites, includ- Because the diversity literature has devoted
ing homogeneity, similarity, agreement, consen- little attention to these kinds of elemental ques-
sus, convergence, and equality. But the more tions, the construct of diversity is murky. The
serious problem is that diversity investigators field needs a deeper understanding of the
have offered only spare or generic definitions of meanings(s) of within-unit differences (Law-
the principal construct. For example, Hambrick rence, 1997). Scholars lack a shared terminology
and his colleagues defined top management or typology to compare their conceptualizations
team heterogeneity as “variation in team mem- of within-unit differences. We present such a
bers’ characteristics” (1996: 662). Pelled, Eisen- typology of diversity below. We believe it has
hardt, and Xin defined demographic diversity as the potential to aid scholars in refining their
“the extent to which a unit (e.g., a work group or theoretical propositions, in matching their opera-
organization) is heterogeneous with respect to tionalizations to their constructs, and in inte-
demographic attributes” (1999: 1). Jehn et al. de- grating their research findings.
1202 Academy of Management Review October

A DIVERSITY TYPOLOGY: SEPARATION, reflects separation: team members hold oppos-


VARIETY, AND DISPARITY ing positions on a task- or team-relevant issue.
In Team V, team member diversity in disciplin-
We start with a brief example to juxtapose
ary background reflects variety: together, team
and preview the distinctive nature of each di-
members bring a multiplicity of information
versity type. Consider three research teams.
Each team is studying how patients experience sources to bear on the research question. In
medical treatment in hospitals. Each team con- Team D, diversity is associated with disparity:
sists of eight members. Members of Team S dif- one member of the team is superior to the other
fer in their attitude toward a particular research team members in research expertise, and pre-
paradigm. Half of the team’s members revere sumably in status as well. The three teams not
richly descriptive, interpretive inquiry; the other only differ in the type of diversity they represent
half disparage it. The members of Team V differ but also in the attribute of diversity present in
in their disciplinary backgrounds. One is a psy- each team (attitude toward qualitative research,
chologist, another is a human factors engineer, disciplinary background, and member prestige).
and the others include a microeconomist, soci- Figure 1 provides a graphic illustration of
ologist, anthropologist, linguist, hospital admin- these three types of diversity, and Table 1 sum-
istrator, and practicing physician. Finally, the marizes key differences among the types. In de-
members of Team D vary in their research emi- scribing each type below, we highlight the key
nence or rank. One member of the team is a assumptions that investigators implicitly or ex-
highly accomplished professor who is renowned plicitly make when studying each one. They
for having formulated seminal theories of pa- deal with (1) the meaning and extent of diversity
tient interactions with health care professionals; within units, (2) between-unit variance in the
the other members of the team are getting their amounts of within-unit diversity, and (3) the cor-
first behavioral science research experience. relates and consequences of between-unit vari-
Diversity is obvious within each team. Yet the ance in within-unit diversity. We clarify these
content and likely outcomes of diversity differ fairly complex assumptions for each diver-
across the teams. In Team S, diversity in team sity type. Further, we describe the nature of
members’ endorsement of qualitative research minimum, maximum, and moderate diversity

FIGURE 1
Pictorial Representation of Types and Amounts of Three Meanings of Within-Unit Diversity
2007 Harrison and Klein 1203

TABLE 1
Meanings and Properties of Within-Unit Diversity Types

Diversity Meaning and Attribute Shape at Attribute Predicted Foundational


Type Synonyms Maximum Diversity Examples Outcomesa Theories

Separation (on Composition of differences Bimodal distribution, Opinions, beliefs, Reduced cohesiveness, Similarity attraction;
attribute S) in (lateral) position or with half of unit values, and more interpersonal social categor-
opinion among unit members at attitudes, conflict, distrust, ization; attraction,
members, primarily of highest and especially decreased task selection, and
value, belief, or attitude; lowest endpoints regarding team performance attrition (ASA)
disagreement or of S continuum goals and
opposition processes
Variety (on Composition of differences Uniform distribution, Content expertise, Greater creativity, Information
attribute V) in kind, source, or with even spread functional innovation, higher processing; law of
category of relevant of members across background, decision quality, requisite variety;
knowledge or experience all possible nonredundant more task conflict, variation,
among unit members; categories of V (no network ties, increased unit selection, and
unique or distinctive continuum) industry flexibility retention (VSR)
information experience
Disparity (on Composition of (vertical) Positively skewed Pay, income, More within-unit Distributive
attribute D) differences in proportion distribution, with prestige, status, competition, (in)justice and
of socially valued assets one member at decision- resentful deviance, (in)equity; status
or resources held among highest endpoint making reduced member hierarchy;
unit members; inequality of D continuum authority, social input, withdrawal tournament; social
or relative concentration and others at power stratification
lowest

a
Generally, but not in all diversity conceptualizations or studies.

amounts for each type. Finally, we note exam- one another, but in other units, members are
ples from the literature of theoretical and empir- more widely spread; and (3) differences among
ical analyses consistent with each diversity units in the extent to which their members are
type. In choosing examples, we have tempo- dispersed along S lead to a set of systematic
rarily excluded those involving organizational consequences (e.g., higher or lower cohesion). In
demography, precisely because demographic our lexicon, theories and studies of this type rest
diversity within a unit may be conceptualized as on an idea of diversity as separation. McGrath,
separation or variety or inequality. We argue Berdahl, and Arrow’s (1995) description of VBA
that this uncertainty contributes to the difficulty (value, belief, attitude) diversity is generally
in conceptualizing and testing diversity’s ef- consistent with the idea of diversity as separa-
fects. Accordingly, a discussion of alternative tion, as is the notion of value diversity (Williams
conceptualizations of demographic differences & O’Reilly, 1998). Typically, theorists and re-
as separation, variety, or disparity follows our searchers who examine separation also assume
initial presentation of each diversity type. that this diversity type has symmetric effects.
That is, homogeneity on S is often predicted to
be beneficial whether all members are high on S
Separation
or all members are low on S.
Key assumptions. Many hypotheses or theo- Minimum, maximum, and moderate separa-
ries about diversity propose that (1) within units, tion. Row 1 of Figure 1 depicts minimum, mod-
members differ from one another in their posi- erate, and maximum separation within a unit.
tion along a single continuous attribute—a lat- Minimum separation occurs when all of the
eral continuum S (e.g., positive affect, percep- members of a unit occupy the same position at
tions of leader charisma, organizational any location along the S continuum. In some
commitment); (2) units differ in the extent to research areas, minimum separation might be
which their individual members are colocated referred to as perfect agreement within the unit
along S—in some units, members are close to (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1993). Minimum sepa-
1204 Academy of Management Review October

ration can happen anywhere on the continuum similar in other ways as well (Janis & Mann,
of interest. Number theory, however, shows that 1977; Nemetz & Christensen, 1996).
maximum separation occurs only when unit Few diversity theorists have addressed the
members are equally split and at opposing end- shape and likely consequences of maximum
points of the S continuum (Harrison & Sin, 2006). separation. When separation is at its maximum,
That is, the utmost separation occurs when there unit members are, by definition, polarized on S.
are two (and only two) staunchly divided but The unit comprises two extreme and opposing
balanced blocs within a team, each holding a factions. If the diversity attribute is of central
position on S as far from the other as possible. importance to team identity and task comple-
Team S, above, shows maximum separation tion, then a unit’s social network is also likely to
along the continuum of “attitude toward inter- bifurcate into two dense clusters or cliques, with
pretive research methods.” Moderate or limited few or no team members bridging the structural
separation occurs when unit members show hole between them. Gibson and Vermeulen’s
some, but only some, spread or disagreement discussion of subgroups within teams is consis-
along the continuum. Perhaps most unit mem- tent with this notion: “Members of a subgroup
bers occupy the same position, and only a few may . . . cohere and share opinions more often
hold differing positions. Or perhaps unit mem- within the subgroup than with others, which
bers are uniformly spread across the continuum, may lead to irritation in the team and disputes
as Figure 1 shows (middle cell of first row). between the different factions” (2003: 203).
Diversity as separation in theory and re-
search. Theories of similarity attraction (Byrne, Variety
1971; Clore & Byrne, 1974; Newcomb, 1961), social
Key assumptions. In a second approach to
identity and self-categorization (Hogg & Terry,
within-unit diversity, theorists and researchers
2000; Tajfel & Turner, 1979), and attraction-
assume that (1) within units, members differ
selection-attrition (Schneider, 1987; Schneider &
from one another qualitatively—that is, on a
Goldstein, 1995) are invoked most often for di-
categorical attribute V (e.g., functional back-
versity as separation. Investigators adopting
ground, source of external information); (2) units
these arguments posit that greater similarity—
differ in the extent to which their members are
reduced separation—yields higher levels of co-
evenly spread across all the categories of V; and
operation, trust, and social integration (e.g.,
(3) differences between units in their relative
Locke & Horowitz, 1990). Conversely, diversity spread or diversity on V will be associated, usu-
scholars often draw on these theories to posit ally positively, with vital unit consequences
that units whose members differ markedly on a (e.g., problem-solving or group decision quality,
continuum will experience low cohesion, high firm performance). Symmetry or asymmetry with
conflict, high rates of withdrawal, and poor per- respect to attribute V is a nonissue; V has no
formance (e.g., Tsui, Ashford, St. Clair, & Xin, high or low. Instead, the distribution of V is
1995). For example, Harrison et al. examined the assumed to indicate variety: the number and
consequences of team members’ diversity in spread of “batches” of information content, ex-
task meaningfulness and outcome importance, perience, or unique network ties available
positing that “people find it more pleasurable to across unit members. Variety broadens the cog-
interact with others who have similar psycho- nitive and behavioral repertoire of the unit.
logical characteristics, because that interaction McGrath et al. (1995) borrow from human re-
verifies and reinforces their own beliefs, affect, sources management in referring to one version
and expressed behaviors” (2002: 1031). of variety as KSA (knowledge, skill, and ability)
Minimum separation is thus likely to be psy- diversity. Blau (1977) originally termed a form of
chologically comforting to unit members with this diversity type heterogeneity, but that term
regard to S. No one challenges anyone else’s itself has gained a variety of meanings in the
position on the S continuum, because each of literature (Lau & Murnighan, 1998). Others (e.g.,
their positions is equivalent. As many have Jehn et al., 1999; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998) refer
noted, this kind of attitudinal or psychological to a similar concept as information diversity.
conformity on S may cause individuals to as- Miner, Haunschild, and Schwab call this type
sume that they are not only similar on S but “categorical variability” (2003: 790).
2007 Harrison and Klein 1205

Minimum, maximum, and moderate variety. tions can translate greater information richness
As with the other types of within-unit diversity, within a unit into better choices, plans, or prod-
minimum variety is straightforward. It occurs ucts. Units whose members draw from different
when all members belong to the same category pools of informational resources—their knowl-
of attribute V (see row 2 of Figure 1). Using com- edge, functional background, experience, or
munication theory terms, such members are re- range of external social ties (e.g., Argote & In-
dundant in that there is no information gained gram, 2000; Austin, 2003; Finkelstein & Ham-
by adding more persons to the unit who occupy brick, 1996)—will make more effective decisions
the same V category (Shannon, 1948). Maximum and deliver more creative products than units
variety is the more interesting case. It is the whose members draw from the same pool of
richest possible distribution of information, resources (Jackson, May, & Whitney, 1995). In
when each member within a unit comes from a
fact, Carpenter describes this type of diversity
unique category of V. These lower and upper
as “sociocognitive horsepower” (2002: 280). Mem-
bounds of variety connect to how a number of
bers with diverse kinds of human capital or ac-
researchers refer to “perfect homogeneity” or
cess to different sources of data (Hambrick &
maximum heterogeneity within a unit (e.g.,
Priem, 1990). The hospital researchers in Team V Mason, 1984; Marsch, 2002; Wiersema & Bantel,
have maximum heterogeneity or variety for 1992) collectively serve as a team’s lens, each
scholarly background; each member represents filtering unique environmental cues and inter-
a distinct scientific field. preting them for the rest of the unit’s members.
The distinction between minimum and moder- Similarly, units whose members have nonredun-
ate variety is fairly stark. The latter requires at dant (i.e., nonoverlapping) external network ties
least one member to come from a different cat- have access to information that other units, lack-
egory on attribute V than all the other members. ing in such variety, cannot easily obtain (Austin,
That first, unique individual with respect to V 2003; Beckman & Haunschild, 2002). Units whose
brings the biggest step up or the largest mar- members bridge structural holes in an interunit
ginal increase in information for the unit (Shan- network are thus likely to be more creative and
non, 1948). From a variety viewpoint, the “odd productive (Burt, 2002; Hansen, 1999; Reagans &
person out” with respect to V is the most crucial. Zuckerman, 2001). As Ferrier describes it: “By
Note that because there are only two categories, way of greater awareness in sensing strategic
a dyad can have only minimum or maximum problems, heterogeneous teams can match com-
variety, but not moderate variety (a dyad can plex competitive challenges and uncertain con-
have moderate separation and moderate dispar- texts with a requisite level of cognitive and ex-
ity). Hence, the study of diversity as variety is periential variety” (2001: 858).
fundamentally altered for units with two versus When the within-unit distribution of V is at its
more than two members. A more extreme view maximum, “everybody is different, and every-
(Harrison & Sin, 2006) would be that any type of body has a unique viewpoint to offer” (Gibson &
diversity is undefined for dyads because there is
Vermeulen, 2003: 208). When maximum variety is
only a single difference—no distribution of dif-
present, members may be quite open and recep-
ferences— between members.
tive to one another’s views. Because each unit
Diversity as variety in theory and research.
member is different from everyone else, unit
Most formulations of diversity as variety are
consistent with the idea that an organizational members cannot form coalitions with like oth-
unit is an information processing instrument for ers; they have to reach across category bound-
the organization (Hinsz, Tindale, & Vollrath, aries to coordinate and connect. In contrast,
1997). The unit senses, evaluates, and responds moderate variety, evidenced by the presence of
in ways that are designed to be adaptive to the substantial overlap in some but not all mem-
environment. Based on well-known axioms in bers’ knowledge, may lead to problems of “un-
information processing or cybernetic theory (law shared information” (Gruenfeld, Mannix, Wil-
of requisite variety; Ashby, 1956), population liams, & Neale, 1996; Stasser, Vaughan, &
ecology, and even human cognition theory (vari- Stewart, 2000). Members may fail to discuss in-
ation and selective retention; Campbell, 1960), formation not shared by all or the majority of the
the fundamental idea is that teams in organiza- group.
1206 Academy of Management Review October

Disparity ity is member parity with respect to D. Maximum


disparity, however, occurs when only one unit
Key assumptions. In the organizational litera-
member outranks all others—that is, when one
ture, conceptual and empirical treatments of di-
unit member’s level of D is at the top of the
versity as separation or as variety are relatively
continuum and all other members’ levels are at
common. Treatments of diversity as disparity
the bottom. Research Team D, studying patients’
are not. They are more common and long-
treatment experiences in hospitals, exhibits
standing in sociology, where the term inequality
maximum diversity. There is one veteran re-
reigns (Blau, 1977). We forward disparity as a
searcher accompanied by seven rookies. Allison
different term to clearly distinguish our within-
aptly described maximum disparity as “one in-
unit discussion from the dominant focus of so- dividual has everything and everyone else has
ciological studies on distributions of income nothing” (1978: 869). Moderate or limited dispar-
across entire populations. In developing and ity occurs when unit members show some, but
testing hypotheses regarding the effects of with- only some, differences along the continuum de-
in-unit diversity as disparity, theorists and re- fined by the valued resource. Some members
searchers focus on a socially valued or desired outrank and outclass the others, but differences
resource D (e.g., pay, power, prestige, status); among unit members are compressed. Moderate
more of D is always better. Disparity-based re- disparity is of focal interest in some areas of
search assumes that (1) within units, members sociology (Homans, 1961; Phillips & Zuckerman,
can differ in the extent to which they hold or 2001) and social psychology (e.g., Berkowitz &
receive a share, amount, or proportion of D; (2) McCaulay, 1961). Those in the middle of D con-
units differ in the extent to which D is distrib- form more readily to unit norms than those at
uted among or possessed by their members—in high and low ends (although the latter segments
some units members have equal shares of D, but have different reasons for nonconformity).
in other units one or a few members hold a Diversity as disparity in theory and research.
disproportionate share of D relative to other unit Disparity (inequality) in asset, resource, or val-
members; and (3) differences among units in the ued good distribution is a central pattern in so-
extent to which their D is distributed equally ciological theories of stratification (Grusky,
among unit members lead to predictable and 1994). Although inequality of valued resources is
important consequences (e.g., fewer member ex- also typical in organizational units, theories and
pressions of voice). investigations addressing diversity as disparity
A disparity conceptualization also involves in organizations are surprisingly rare. The clear-
one more assumption that the direction of differ- est examples emerge from the literature on pay
ence along the D continuum matters. Disparity dispersion (Bloom, 1999). As Bloom and Michel
is asymmetric. This asymmetry assumption is explain, “More dispersed pay structures concen-
nonintuitive. Because disparity describes the trate pay among fewer employees at the top . . .
relative distribution of a valued asset or re- thereby providing relatively rich rewards for
source, disparity is high if 10 percent of a unit’s these employees and relatively poor rewards for
members “own” a great deal of D (e.g., prestige the rest” (2002: 33). Building on theories of rela-
or pay) while 90 percent of unit members have tive deprivation (e.g., Deutsch, 1985) and tourna-
very little. Disparity is low, however, if 90 per- ment compensation (e.g., Lazear, 1995; Lazear &
cent of a unit’s members own a great deal of D Rosen, 1981), researchers commonly predict that
while only 10 percent have very little. In the first status, power, or pay disparity incites competi-
case, 90 percent of the unit’s members are dis- tion, differentiation, and (resentful) deviance
advantaged relative to the privileged few. In the among some unit members (e.g., Bloom, 1999;
second case, only 10 percent of the unit’s mem- Homans, 1961; Pfeffer & Langton, 1993; Siegel &
bers are disadvantaged relative to the over- Hambrick, 2005). Disparity might also foster con-
whelming majority. formity, silence, suppression of creativity, and
Minimum, maximum, and moderate disparity. withdrawal (Hollander, 1958; Pfeffer, 1998; Pfef-
Row 3 of Figure 1 depicts minimum, moderate, fer & Davis-Blake, 1992).
and maximum disparity within a unit. Minimum A conceptualization of diversity as disparity is
disparity occurs when all of the members of a also apparent in theory and research address-
unit occupy the same position. Minimum dispar- ing status and power hierarchies in work units
2007 Harrison and Klein 1207

(for some of the foundational theories, see Phil- Implications of the Diversity Types for Theory
lips & Zuckerman, 2001). When disparity in a Building
group is at its maximum, one member of the unit
The three diversity types are fundamentally
outranks all others. He or she holds the lion’s
distinct. Separation describes differences
share if not all of a valued unit resource. Eisen-
among unit members in their position on a hor-
hardt and Bourgeois’ (1988) study of top manage- izontal continuum. Variety describes differences
ment team (TMT) strategic decision-making pro- among unit members from different categories,
cesses provides an example. When a CEO’s reflecting access to unique sources of knowl-
power far exceeded that of other TMT members, edge. Disparity describes differences among
creating high power disparity in the team, the unit members in their portion of a valued re-
CEO was likely to engage in “tactics for control- source. Separation thus reflects stand point or
ling and withholding information,” and the position: the distribution of where members
other executives in the team were likely to en- stand on a value, belief, attitude, or orientation.
gage in “alliance and insurgency behaviors” Variety reflects information: the distribution of
(1988: 743). Ultimately, marked disparities in what each unit member knows that is unique
team member power diminished team perfor- from other members, as a function of the distinct
mance by distracting team members from key content of his or her education, training, or ex-
tasks and interrupting the flow of information. perience. Disparity reflects possession: the dis-
These team-level observations are corroborated tribution of how much of a socially valued com-
by research documenting behaviors when indi- modity each unit member has.
viduals are located at (dis)advantaged or dis- The three diversity types also differ in their
parate status positions in teams (e.g., Berger, relevance to key theoretical perspectives and
Rosenholtz, & Zelditch, 1980; Cohen & Zhou, 1991; are commonly (but not necessarily) associated
Lefler, Gillespie, & Conaty, 1982). As Keltner, with different outcomes. These distinctions are
Gruenfeld, and Anderson summarize, “High- obscured when researchers and theorists define
power individuals talk more, interrupt more, are diversity loosely or not at all. Conversely, ex-
more likely to speak out of turn, and are more plicit reference to diversity types should
directive of others’ verbal contributions than are sharpen researchers’ predictions, requiring the
lower-power individuals” (2003: 277). adoption, explanation, and justification of a spe-
Finally, social network analysis provides an- cific conceptualization. Further, explicit refer-
other intriguing link to this diversity type. If the ence to diversity types allows scholars to con-
valued resource, D, is social capital, the struc- trast and debate their views; they will have a
ture of a network might illustrate disparity. So- common language to capture their convergent
cial capital is accessed and conveyed through or divergent ideas. Finally, explicit reference to
interpersonal ties (Adler & Kwon, 2002). When a diversity types paves the way for researchers to
unit’s network structure is highly centralized, address the potentially interactive effects of
network ties are unevenly distributed. Only one separation, variety, and disparity, a point we
or a few members are highly central and, thus, return to in the final section of the paper. Ac-
highly influential. The other unit members are cordingly, we propose the following.
on the periphery of the network, with few or even
Guideline 1: Theory building regard-
no links to other unit members. Centralization
ing diversity is enhanced by authors’
may thus “be viewed as a measure of how un-
explicit specification and justification
equal the individual actor values are” (Wasser-
of the diversity type of interest: sepa-
man & Faust, 1994: 176). Suppose knowledge of ration (S), variety (V), or disparity (D).
task process is conceptualized as a valued re-
source, D (a form of expert power; French & Despite distinctions among their meanings,
Raven, 1959), and it is held by a single person. there is little to distinguish the three types of
The unit therefore has maximum disparity. Such diversity when each is minimized (see Figure 1).
a condition might translate into a social network All members within a unit hold the same value
with maximum centralization, in which unit of a variable, regardless of what the variable
members build ties or turn to only that person represents. The three diversity types are
for advice. strongly differentiated, however, by their dis-
1208 Academy of Management Review October

tinctive shape and meaning under maximum to have tight interpersonal bonds within their
diversity. subunit and antipathy toward those in the op-
As shown in row 1 of Figure 1, maximum sep- posing subunit. Perhaps maximum variety does
aration occurs when a unit is divided into two not lead to conflict, as is sometimes predicted
divergent camps. As separation increases from (e.g., Jehn et al., 1999; Pelled et al., 1999), but to
minimum to maximum, unit members grow in- openness toward other ideas, since no one
creasingly polarized in their placement along a shares the same idea. These conjectures lead us
continuum. Were a researcher studying other to suggest the following.
types of diversity, however, the same distribu-
tion would be indicative of modest variety (only Guideline 2: Theory building about di-
two of a possible eight categories are present) or versity is enhanced by a careful visu-
of medium disparity (50 percent of the unit are alization of the shape and conse-
“haves” and 50 percent are “have nots”). Maxi- quences of maximum separation,
mum variety, shown in row 2 of Figure 1, occurs maximum variety, or maximum dis-
when a unit is composed of members who each parity. In contemplating these max-
represent a different category. As variety in- ima, scholars shift their focus from dif-
creases from minimum to maximum, unit mem- ferences within dyads to the pattern of
bers grow increasingly differentiated in their differences within the unit as a whole.
knowledge, experience, or perspectives; they oc-
cupy distinct information categories within the
Implications for Theories and Evidence About
unit. Were those eight categories reoriented as
Demographic Diversity
points along a continuum, however, the same
distribution would indicate only modest separa- In some areas of research, the conceptualiza-
tion or modest disparity. Distance among the tion of diversity as separation, variety, or dis-
unit members, which is part of the evidence for parity is already quite clear (cf. Bloom, 1999). In
either separation or disparity, is limited. Finally, others areas it is much less so. An important—
maximum disparity is evident when a single even dominant— case under the latter condition
(upper echelon) member towers over all other is investigation of demographic diversity. The
(lower echelon) members in a unit, as shown in demographic variables most frequently in-
row 3 of Figure 1. As disparity increases from cluded in diversity studies are age, sex, race/
minimum to maximum, the concentration of a ethnicity, organization and team tenure, educa-
unit’s resource in a single member grows dis- tion level, educational content, and functional
proportionately. background (Jackson et al., 2003; Ragins &
In contemplating these maxima, scholars may Gonzalez, 2003; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998). The
gain a new and deeper appreciation of the last two on this list— educational content (major
meaning of unit diversity. Attention shifts from a or specialty) and functional background—seem
consideration of isolated differences among the to be obvious forms of diversity as variety. These
members of dyads within a unit to a consider- two attributes capture qualitative differences in
ation of the pattern of differences within the unit the kinds of information held by unit members.
as a whole. Maximum separation does not sim- But within-unit diversity with respect to each of
ply mean that unit members are quite different the other demographic attributes may be mean-
from one another; it means that they are polar- ingfully conceptualized as separation or as va-
ized into opposing subunits. Maximum variety riety or as disparity. Uncertainty regarding the
means that each member is one of a kind. Max- precise meaning of tenure, age, gender, and
imum disparity means that power, status, or race/ethnicity differences within a unit has, we
other valued resources are grossly unbalanced. believe, compounded the challenge of conceptu-
We see the possibility of new theoretical prop- alizing and testing diversity effects (Priem,
ositions and hypotheses as scholars build on Lyon, & Dess, 1999).
these insights. Perhaps maximum separation Tenure is a noteworthy and often studied ex-
leads to strong subunit identification and weak ample (Pfeffer, 1983). Researchers might argue
unit identification (Gibson & Vermeulen, 2003). that tenure diversity within a unit is best con-
In units with maximal separation on an impor- ceptualized as separation; the less separated
tant team-relevant attribute, members are likely team members are along the tenure continuum,
2007 Harrison and Klein 1209

the more likely they are to have similar atti- ing arguments that relationship difficulties
tudes, beliefs, and values, to be drawn to one between identity groups are caused by misun-
another, and to identify with one another. derstandings due to cultural differences while
O’Reilly, Caldwell, and Barnett have posited downplaying the effects of prejudice and dis-
that “similarity in time of entrance into the crimination denies the everyday inequalities
group may facilitate both attraction and interac- experienced by disadvantaged identity groups”
tion” (1989: 33). Alternatively, tenure diversity (2003: 11).
might be conceptualized as variety. In this view, Each of these examples still begs a general
teams that vary in tenure include members of question: Absent a deductive theory, if a demo-
differing cohorts who thus differ in their experi- graphic variable distributed within a unit can
ences, their information bases, and their inter- indicate any diversity type, what meaning
nal and external network ties (e.g., Ancona & should scholars impute for it? In short, which
Caldwell, 1992; Hambrick et al., 1996). Finally, type is right? We believe that this depends, in
tenure diversity might be conceptualized as dis- part, on unit members’ context-dependent in-
parity (e.g., Tsui, Xin, & Egan, 1995). Because terpretations of the variable in question. Thus,
individual tenure may be positively associated the meaning of within-unit diversity in demo-
with status or authority within a team (e.g., Dra- graphic attribute X is likely to be be shaped
zin & Rao, 1999), tenure diversity within a team by the unit’s primary purpose or superordinate
may result in empowerment (access to valued goal and unit members’ beliefs regarding the
resources) of the team’s long-entrenched elites association between demographic attribute X
and disempowerment of the team’s initiates. and deeper-level attributes (see Harrison et al.,
Gender diversity might also be conceptual- 1998).
ized as separation, as variety, or as disparity. It Consider the following contexts for age diver-
does not fall along a continuum. But if gender is sity. In employee advisory committees for firm
instead treated as a salient marker or a symbol retirement benefits, member age might covary
for a status continuum or for task preferences with self-interest in particular policies. Further,
within a unit (Konrad & Gutek, 1987; Ragins & members’ implicit theories of age may include
Sundstrom, 1989; Ridgeway, Johnson, & this link, leading age diversity to operate as
Diekema, 1994), each of the other conceptualiza- separation. In teams of software engineers as-
tions of diversity appears plausible. Scholars sembled to solve problems in a firm’s informa-
implicitly conceptualize gender diversity as tion systems architecture (e.g., Year 2000 prob-
separation when they suggest that (1) it reflects lems), age might covary with a particular
a distribution of opposing beliefs about the ap- “legacy” versus “object-oriented” programming
propriateness of critical team processes or out- orientation, and team members should recog-
comes, (2) it is negatively related to cohesion nize it. Here, age diversity might be taken to
and identification within a unit, and (3) these mark variety. In units such as orchestras, law
effects are symmetric such that gender diversity enforcement squads, or student project teams,
has comparable effects when a unit is numeri- older members (not just longer-tenured ones)
cally dominated by men or by women. In sug- might be seen as possessing higher levels of
gesting that men and women have qualitatively task-relevant experience, tacit knowledge, or
different caches of knowledge such that gender street smarts. Age would thus be associated
diversity within a unit may spark creativity and with status, and diversity in age could be
innovation, scholars instead invoke gender di- treated as disparity.
versity as variety (e.g., Wood, 1987). Finally, in Parallel arguments can be made for within-
highlighting power differences between men unit diversity in education level, race, or eth-
and women and the asymmetric consequences nicity as reflecting each of the three diversity
of within-unit gender diversity, scholars de- types (e.g., Watson, Kumar, & Michaelson, 1995;
scribe gender diversity as disparity (e.g., Cohen see Ragins & Gonzalez, 2003, for a review). Be-
& Zhou, 1991; Linnehan & Konrad, 1999; Ragins & cause demographic variables are the most com-
Sundstrom, 1989; Tsui, Egan, & O’Reilly, 1992). monly studied vehicles of diversity, but perhaps
Indeed, Konrad has proposed this conceptual- the least well connected to any of the three di-
ization over a conceptualization of gender diver- versity types, we offer the following recommen-
sity as separation or variety: “Scholarship mak- dation.
1210 Academy of Management Review October

Guideline 3: In cases for which diver- indexed at the unit level by cumulating abso-
sity of unit members on the same char- lute or squared distances between pairs of indi-
acteristic (e.g., tenure) may be mean- viduals. Within-unit SD reflects such cumulative
ingfully conceptualized in different distances, and hence requires interval or better
ways—that is, as separation, as vari- scales, for the S continuum. If an organizational
ety, or as disparity—precise specifica- scholar were interested in separation of mem-
tion of diversity type is essential. It bers’ attitudes about qualitative research in the
allows theorists to differentiate and medical research team described above, SD
compare conceptual models, facilitat- would be a viable choice. Note that in this con-
ing understanding and cross-fertiliza- text, SD describes within-unit diversity as a
tion and paving the way for empirical sample value; it does not estimate a population
tests of contrasting conceptions. parameter. Hence, its denominator contains n
and not n ⫺ 1.
METHODOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF THE SD is maximized under the extreme bipolar
DIVERSITY TYPOLOGY: LINKING THEORY distribution shown in the first row of Figure 1,
ABOUT DIFFERENCES TO METHOD where half of the members within a unit are
“stacked” at the lower bound of S (point l) and
The diversity typology also has implications
half are stacked at the upper bound (point u).
for research design. Overlooking or blurring the
The SD equals (u ⫺ l)/2 under such conditions of
distinctions among the diversity types can lead
maximum separation. If, for example, variable S
to theoretical confusion. It can also lead to meth-
odological errors and mistaken research conclu- consisted of a seven-point continuum that
sions. Below we explain the appropriate opera- ranged from l ⫽ 1 to u ⫽ 7, then the maximum
tionalization for each type of diversity (see SD would be 3. It would occur when half the
Table 2). We note the consequences of concep- unit’s members were located at 1 and half at 7
tualizing one diversity type while operation- (the same maximum would be obtained if l were
alizing another, discuss whether and how to use ⫺3 and u were ⫹3; changing the scale by adding
measures of “overall” diversity and “perceived or subtracting a constant would not change the
diversity,” and describe the sampling schemes SD).
that are needed to fully test theories of every Unlike some other diversity measures, maxi-
diversity type. mum SD does not increase with the size of a unit
or team; larger n’s do not create larger estimates
of within-unit diversity. However, SD has the
Operationalizing Separation
same (interval-level) metric as the original at-
Standard deviation (SD). Because of its sym- tribute S. One of its disadvantages, therefore, is
metric nature, separation on variable S is best that it cannot be readily compared across differ-

TABLE 2
Operationalizations of Within-Unit Diversity Types

Minimum to Assumed Scale of


Diversity Type Index Formula Maximum Measurement

Separation (on Standard deviation √[⌺(Si ⫺ Smean)2/n] 0 to [(u ⫺ l)/2] Interval


attribute S) Mean Euclidean distance ⌺√[⌺(Si ⫺ Sj)2/n]/n 0 to [(u ⫺ l)/√(2)] Interval

Variety (on Blau 1⫺⌺pk 2


0 to (K ⫺ 1)/K Categorical
attribute V) Teachman (entropy) ⫺⌺[pk 䡠 ln(pk)] 0 to ⫺1 䡠 ln(1/K) Categorical

Disparity (on Coefficient of variation √[⌺(Di ⫺ Dmean) /n]/Dmean


2
0 to √(n ⫺ 1) Ratio
attribute D) Gini coefficient (⌺兩Di ⫺ Dj兩)/(2 䡠 N 䡠 Dmean)
2
0 to 1 ⫺ (1/n) Ratio

Note: For separation (S) and disparity (D), the diversity attribute is continuous and can range from a lower bound of l to an
upper bound of u. Theoretically, l can be ⫺⬁ for separation attributes and (commonly) 0 for disparity attributes, while u can
be ⫹⬁ in either case. Operationally, l and u are limited by the instrument used to measure the attribute in question. For variety
(V), the attribute is nominal or discrete. It can take k ⫽ 1, . . . K possible categories.
2007 Harrison and Klein 1211

ent separation variables. Relative amounts of individuals from the same or different catego-
diversity on, say, team members’ protestant ries, respectively (see Figure 1; Harrison & Sin,
work ethic cannot be put side by side with their 2006). Blau’s index (1977) builds on this qualita-
attitudes toward their supervisor, if the protes- tive difference foundation, and it is the most
tant work ethic measure ranges from 1 to 7 and commonly employed measure for diversity as
the attitude toward supervisor measure ranges variety (e.g., Bunderson & Sutcliffe, 2002).
from 1 to 5. Blau’s index is also known as Herfindahl’s
Mean Euclidean distance. Within a unit, the (1950) index and Hirschman’s (1964) index, but it
Euclidean distance of one member, i, from all was originally proposed by Simpson (1949) as a
the other members, j, is the root mean squared measure of species diversity in an ecosystem.
distance between each of those i,j pairs on at- Language from the latter domain can be helpful
tribute S: √[⌺(Si ⫺ Sj)2/(n)] (Tsui et al., 1992). Total in understanding the meaning of Blau’s index
Euclidean distance is the sum of Euclidean dis- scores. Its computational formula is 1 ⫺ ⌺pk2,
tances for all n unit members. The maximum where p is the proportion of unit members in kth
(team-level) Euclidean distance is √[(n ⫺ 1)(u ⫺ category. Values of Blau’s index can range from
l)2/n]. For our polarized team S, the maximum is zero to (K ⫺ 1)/K. Its maximum occurs when
5.61. members of a team, Vk, are spread equally—
Because this— or any other diversity measure called “evenness” or relative abundance of spe-
summed over all possible dyads—increases cies in the ecological literature (Hill, 1973)— over
with team size, we recommend use of an aver- all possible K categories— called “richness” of
age Euclidean distance, which has a maximum species. Having an even spread of members
value of (u ⫺ l)/ [√(2)] ⫽ 4.24. That formula has a over the richest number of information sources
very similar structure to the maximum SD for- corresponds well to our definition of maximum
mula: [(u ⫺ l)/2]. Befitting proper indexes of sep- variety; evenness and richness contribute to a
aration, both average Euclidean distance and higher Blau’s index. A more statistical interpre-
SD reach their peak under the same conceptual tation is that Blau’s index reflects the chance
conditions of maximum separation—a bimodal that two randomly selected group members be-
distribution with the modes occurring on both long to different categories (tap into different
endpoints of the S continuum. The Euclidean information sources).1
distance index shares a weakness with SD in Because its maximum is limited by K (but less
that it also cannot be compared across variables so as K gets larger; if K ⫽ ⫹⬁, the limit would be
with different metrics. Given their conceptual 1.0), Blau’s indexes are also not directly compa-
and mathematical similarities, there is no par- rable when the number of categories is not the
ticular advantage of either operationalization same across diversity variables. For instance,
over the other, save for researcher familiarity one cannot equate Blau’s index for gender
with SD (see Bedeian & Mossholder, 2000). (maximum Blau value is .5) with Blau’s index for
the disciplinary backgrounds in team V (with
eight persons spread evenly across K ⫽ 8 cate-
Operationalizing Variety
gories, the maximum Blau value is .875). Some
Blau’s index. Continuous distances are not might regard this difference in maxima as rea-
meaningful under a conceptualization of diver- sonable; if there are K ⫽ 2 versus K ⫽ 8 informa-
sity as variety; qualitative distinctions are. It tion categories, there is less potential variety
is through the spread of members across quali- from gender than from disciplinary background.
tatively different or novel categories that the Further, if two groups differ in size but each
cognitive and behavioral repertoire of a unit is group shows maximum variety (i.e., in each
presumed to increase. When variable V is dis-
persed across members who might be in one of
k ⫽ 1, . . . K possible categories, such as the 1
Blau’s index was originally developed by Simpson (1949)
different scientific backgrounds represented by for sampling with replacement from an infinite population.
When used to measure diversity within a group, sampling is
the research team (V) studying hospital patient
done with replacement from a finite population, and 1 ⫺
experiences described above, SD is inappropri- ⌺pk2 might be substituted by 1 ⫺ ⌺[nk(nk ⫺ 1)/(n(n ⫺ 1))]. In
ate. Euclidean distances are sensible only to the the latter formula, nk is the frequency of unit members in the
extent that a 0 or 1 “distance” refers to pairs of kth category, and n is unit size.
1212 Academy of Management Review October

group no two members have the same func- attribute D. When a privileged minority holds
tional background), there is a potential for more most of resource D relative to an underprivi-
distinct categories to be present in the larger leged majority, there is greater disparity than
group. Therefore, if one takes Blau’s index (or when the situation is reversed. Similarly, in the
Teachman’s, see below) as a literal or “true” CV, division of the SD by the mean expresses the
value for variety, maximum possible variety in- idea that diversity on a given attribute matters
creases with unit size; there is a “richer” set of less when resource D is typically high versus
possible information categories to draw from. If typically low (Sorenson, 2002). In the sociology
this is an unpalatable assumption, a way to literature, CV is often used as a measure of
standardize Blau’s index is to divide by its the- income inequality (Allison, 1978). There, the
oretical maximum, yielding the Index of Quality logic of dividing the SD by the mean to capture
Variation (IQV: Agresti & Agresti, 1978; Mueller, disparity is more obvious. The same amount of
Schuessler, & Costner, 1970). Blau’s index and variability (say, SD[D] ⫽ $20K) in unit members’
IQV are used in social network research to opera- income is of less importance when unit mem-
tionalize availability of unique sources of infor- bers’ average income is higher (Dmean ⫽ $1M)
mation or social capital (Marsden, 1990). than when it is lower (Dmean ⫽ $40K; Allison,
Teachman’s (entropy) index. The computa- 1978; Sorenson, 2002).
tional formula for Teachman’s index (1980; orig- The CV reaches its maximum when n ⫺ 1
inally developed by Shannon, 1948, in the com- individuals are at the lower bound, l, of a ratio-
munications literature) is ⫺⌺[pk 䡠 ln(pk)], where p level continuum (Allison, 1978; Bedeian & Moss-
is again the proportion of unit members in the holder, 2000; Kimura, 1994). If l is zero, then all
kth category. As unit members are spread more members of the unit but one hold none of D, and
evenly and across a richer number of V catego- the absolute value of the something held by the
ries, Teachman’s index rises. Its maximum oc- nth person does not matter. That sole person has
curs under the same conditions as Blau’s index, all of D in the unit. For example, if “number of
but it takes a slightly different value: ⫺1 䡠 ln(1/K). refereed journal articles” were the status contin-
Therefore, Teachman’s index is likewise not di- uum being studied in Team D, the maximum CV
rectly comparable across different V-type vari- would not change if the single highly regarded
ables, as long as each variable has a different and veteran researcher had 1 or 10 or 100 pub-
total number of categories. It is further limited lished papers—as long as the lower-ranking
by the situation described above for Blau’s in- novice members all had 0. Under these condi-
dex, when the number of unit members n is less tions, the CV is maximized in a way that reflects
than the possible total number of information only within-unit size: √(n ⫺ 1) or, in this case, √(8
categories K. Both indexes have been shown to ⫺ 1) ⫽ 2.65 (Champernowne, 1974).
be part of a general class of diversity or infor- Because the maximum CV is sensitive to sam-
mation richness measures that have desirable ple size, highly dispersed units with fewer mem-
mathematical properties (Hill, 1973), although bers likely would be regarded as having less
Teachman’s index always yields a larger num- disparity than those with more members. One
ber than Blau’s. The only advantage one opera- person who has great power (D) over dozens of
tionalization of variety would have over the others is assumed to create more disparity than
other is that Blau’s index occupies a somewhat the individual who has great power over just a
tidier range of 0 to (a value close to) 1. handful of others. Martin and Gray (1971) offer a
standardizing adjustment for unit size when this
assumption is untenable (see Beckman & Hauns-
Operationalizing Disparity
child, 2002, for an application to external net-
Coefficient of variation. The formula for the work ties).
coefficient of variation (CV), √[⌺(Di ⫺ Dmean)2/n]/ If the lower bound, l, is some number higher
Dmean or, more simply, SD(D)/Dmean, captures the than zero, the upper bound, u, does matter. The
asymmetry that is fundamental to the conceptu- farther the distance on D that one or a few elite
alization of diversity as disparity. Disparity re- persons are above the rest of the persons within
flects both the distances between unit members a unit, the higher the CV. The more lopsided the
and the dominance of (concentration of the re- within-unit distribution of D, with more persons
sources in) those who have higher amounts of at the bottom and a valued resource concen-
2007 Harrison and Klein 1213

trated in a few or one person at the top (see row Klein, Conn, Smith, & Sorra, 2001; Simons,
3 of Figure 1), the larger the resulting CV value. Pelled, & Smith, 1999; Wiersema & Bird, 1993).
These properties of CV (and of the Gini coeffi- In using CV to operationalize diversity, re-
cient, below) make disparity measures perhaps searchers frequently cite Allison (1978), noting
the most volatile or sensitive of the three types his arguments that the use of CV is metricless
of diversity to the status of a single member. If a and that CV therefore provides a comparable
situation in which all unit members have none measure of diversity in different attributes (un-
(or equally small portions) of the resource in like SD or Teachman’s index; Sorenson, 2002).
question changes such that one person now However, Allison recommended CV for opera-
gains a large share, the unit quickly jumps from tionalizing our disparity type of diversity. For
minimum to maximum disparity. example, he noted that CV provides a compara-
Gini coefficient. The Gini coefficient or index ble measure of income inequality whether the
is used less often in organizational behavior or attribute in question is dollars or yen. In the
human resources research than in strategy and studies cited above, and in many others, re-
organization theory, where it has been borrowed searchers sought to assess either separation
from its widespread application as a concentra- (testing ideas based on similarity attraction) or
tion ratio in finance and economics. Its compu- variety (testing ideas based on the leveraging of
tational formula is the sum of all pairwise ab- unique information sources within a unit), but
solute differences between unit members on instead assessed disparity. Hence, the conclu-
variable D, divided by [2 䡠 Dmean 䡠 n2]; see Table sions of such research might be incorrect; the
1. Its maximum value is 1 ⫺ (1/n). Similar to CV, index used was asymmetric but the conceptual-
this maximum occurs when all but one unit
ization of diversity was symmetric.
member have the lowest possible standing on D.
Two examples may be instructive. Suppose
Also as with CV, the Gini index is only appro-
that diversity in the variable “commitment to
priate for D attributes that have ratio-level prop-
meeting project deadlines” was conceptualized
erties (Allison, 1978; Kimura, 1994). Researchers
in terms of separation. Also suppose the teams
might choose the Gini index over CV if they
had equal separation; SDs of commitment were
prefer their diversity measures to follow a com-
the same (SD ⫽ 10) in Team S1 and Team S2. But
mon metric, in this case ranging from 0 to
in Team S1 the mean level of commitment was
(nearly) 1. The maximum value of the Gini index
40 and in Team S2 it was 20. If an investigator
depends on n as well, but it should be less of a
limiting factor than for CV when it is used in used CV as an operationalization of within-unit
larger versus smaller groups (see Martin & separation, he or she would mistakenly con-
Gray, 1971, for a possible standardizing correc- clude that Team S2 (CV ⫽ .5) had twice the
tion for n). separation as Team S1 (CV ⫽ .25).
Suppose that diversity in age was conceptu-
alized in terms of variety: different age cohorts
On the Folly of Conceptualizing S or V While learn and know qualitatively different things
Operationalizing D that might contribute to a unit. If one member of
When researchers specify one diversity type an eight-person team were relatively old and all
but operationalize another, they can draw erro- the rest young (resulting in a low mean for age),
neous conclusions from their analyses. Figure 1 a misapplied CV to member age would yield the
helps to illustrate the point. As noted above, the conclusion of maximum team variety, but only
distribution of member scores or values that in- two “batches” of knowledge would exist. The CV
dicates maximum disparity is not indicative of would be nearly minimized if there were one
maximum separation or maximum variety but, young member and the rest were relatively old
rather, of very limited separation or variety. Wil- (resulting in a high mean for age), but this team
liams and O’Reilly (1998) report CV to be the would have, as above, just two “batches” of
most widely applied diversity index in the liter- knowledge.
ature. Yet using CV (an appropriate operation- These examples lead us to the guideline be-
alization of disparity) to assess separation or low. The basic principle is familiar: conceptual-
variety is misleading. Unfortunately, research- ization must be aligned with operationalization.
ers often do just this (e.g., Harrison et al., 1998; Theory must guide measurement, which is nu-
1214 Academy of Management Review October

anced for diversity constructs. CV is not a uni- variability) tenure cohorts tend to be more so-
versal diversity index. cially cohesive and committed to their unit, and
therefore will be less likely to quit. The theory
Guideline 4: The choice of a diversity predicts a positive relationship at the unit level
index should be driven by the theoret- between tenure diversity and turnover rate. Yet
ical specification of diversity type; re- if lower variability in tenure was confounded
searchers must answer what the vital with higher mean tenure across units, observing
difference is before they choose or de- the predicted positive relationship might be at-
sign indexes to measure it. If the spec- tributed to the empirical fact that those with
ified conceptualization and index of longer tenure (and greater investment with a
diversity are mismatched, research firm) are simply less likely to leave. Thus, we
conclusions may be misleading. Re- join others (e.g., Jackson et al., 2003: 808) in urg-
searchers specifying diversity as sep- ing investigators to include the mean in tests of
aration should use versions of SD or diversity as separation and disparity.
average Euclidean distance. Re-
searchers specifying diversity as vari- Guideline 5: In testing the relation-
ety should use versions of Blau’s or ship between separation or disparity
Teachman’s (entropy) index. Re- of an attribute and other variables,
searchers specifying diversity as dis- researchers should first statistically
parity should use versions of CV or the control for the within-group mean of
Gini coefficient. the attribute.

It might be asserted that because the mean is


Accounting for the Mean When Testing for already in the denominator of the CV formula, if
Diversity Effects researchers use CV as a disparity index, they
will already be controlling for the mean effects
The only difference between the formulas
mentioned above. However, as Sorenson (2002)
given above for CV and SD is that CV is a ratio
shows, the CV ratio does not statistically control
of SD to the within-unit mean. This is more than
for the mean; it forces a particular moderating
a statistical quirk; it has significant substantive
structure on within-unit data. Such a ratio forces
implications. Thus, it invites a consideration of
the effect of SD(D) to be tempered or weakened
the role of the within-unit mean in analyses of
at a rate of 1/Dmean, mandating a SD(D) ⫻
diversity effects.
1/Dmean interaction. The mean value of D has an
The mean specifies the typical location of unit
effect on the criterion of interest that opposes
members on an S or D attribute. When separa-
the effect of variability at a very specific, 1/Dmean
tion and disparity attributes have no upper or
rate. This mandated form of interaction in dis-
lower bounds (they can range from ⫺⬁ to ⫹⬁),
parity measures is also a testable assumption.
the within-unit mean tends to be uncorrelated
Researchers could enter both the SD(D) and
with within-unit variability (Harrison & Sin,
1/Dmean in hypothesis testing, similar to the sug-
2006). However, most attributes do have upper
gestion in Guideline 5. That is, in predicting unit
and lower bounds in real data. Because the SD
turnover rate, Y, from disparity in tenure, the
of a within-unit distribution is often lower when
first regression model would be
the mean is near the lower or upper bound
(members are clustered near the “floor” or “ceil-
Y ⫽ ␤ 0 ⫹ ␤ 1 关 SD(Tenure)兴 ⫹
ing”), there may be an artifactual overlap of
means and SDs across units. In short, mean and ␤2关1/mean(Tenure)兴 ⫹ ␧.
SD can be confounded.
Because of that potential confound, what ap- The second regression model that checks the
pears to be the effect of within-unit diversity contribution of the specific functional form of
may be the (inverse) effect of the mean instead; disparity in the CV formula would be
the substantive interpretation of the latter effect
is entirely different. Suppose, for example, a di- Y ⫽ ␤ 0 ⫹ ␤ 1 关 SD(Tenure)兴 ⫹ ␤2关1/mean(Tenure)兴
versity theory predicts that units with members
who are part of more homogeneous (low- ⫹ ␤3关SD(Tenure) ⫻ 1/mean(Tenure)兴 ⫹ ␧.
2007 Harrison and Klein 1215

The impact of greatest interest to disparity re- value that characterizes each unit in the sample
searchers is reflected in the strength of ␤3. (e.g., Chatman & Flynn, 2001; Randel & Jaussi,
2003).
Guideline 6: The use of common dis- For several reasons, we believe that this fus-
parity measures carries an implicit as- ing of diversity indexes for several variables is
sumption about the shape of a nega- unwise. First, as we have noted, a unit or team
tive interactive effect between the itself does not have diversity (Jackson et al.,
variability and the mean of a diversity 2003); an attribute of individuals within units
attribute. That assumption should be has diversity, whether the within-unit distribu-
tested by entering variability, mean, tion of that attribute is thought of as separation,
and the variability-by-mean interac- variety, or disparity. Second, it is clear that more
tion to see if the former is indeed mod- diversity on such attributes has a unique mean-
erated by the latter. ing for each of the three diversity types. This
prohibits summing, say, SD for separation in
Operationalizing Demographic Diversity: age with Blau’s index for variety in functional
Separation, Variety, and Disparity background and CV for disparity in tenure. The
resulting amalgam has an indefinite interpreta-
The two guidelines above argue for a second tion at best. Pitcher and Smith have noted this,
look at previous studies of demographic diver- stating, “Multiple proxies seem to cancel one
sity. As we have shown, diversity of age, sex, another out and almost guarantee nonfindings”
race, tenure, and education can all be, and have (2001: 14). Lau and Murnighan also have warned
been, conceptualized as separation, variety, or that such a combination would be “like cross-
disparity. Researchers have often conceptual- fertilizing apples and oranges” (1998: 327).
ized one form of diversity (most commonly, sep- That leaves the possibility of adding several
aration or variety) but then operationalized an- variables that, perhaps together, reflect a single
other (disparity). There is no post hoc statistical diversity type. For example, if diversity in gen-
adjustment that can be applied to published der and diversity in ethnic background were
findings from such research. Instead, authors both conceptualized as variety, would it be rea-
would need to revisit their previous work and sonable to sum their diversity indexes and cre-
reanalyze their primary data to match indexes ate an overall variety measure? To use the logic
to diversity types. In the absence of such reanal- of conventional (reflective) measurement, one
yses, the validity of the conclusions of many assumes that different operationalizations are
specific studies, as well as of the narrative and positively correlated, manifesting the same un-
quantitative reviews of the demographic diver- derlying (variety) construct. Still, there is little
sity literature, may be called into question. reason to expect measures of the variety (or sep-
Given the importance of the topic of demo- aration or disparity) of two independent at-
graphic diversity within organizations, we hope tributes—such as gender and ethnicity—to be
our observations and guidelines will spur new consistently and positively correlated within
research, as well as reexaminations of prior re- units. Most diversity theories do not predict such
search. But the implications of our framework go relationships, nor do published correlations
beyond the sphere of single demographic differ- among diversity variables reveal convergent
ences, and we broaden our discussion below. validity (e.g., Klein et al., 2001). Hence, we cau-
tion against this approach.
It might be counterargued that measures of
“Overall” Diversity
demographic diversity, such as gender variety
Diversity indexes are sometimes cumulated to and ethnic background variety, serve not as re-
indicate the “total” amount of dissimilarity, het- flective indicators but as “formative” indicators
erogeneity, or difference within a unit—summed (Bollen & Lennox, 1991; MacCallum & Browne,
over several attributes. That is, researchers 1993). As with the variables in a principle com-
might seek to assess overall within-unit diver- ponent, each formative indicator contributes to a
sity by adding or averaging diversity indexes linear composite that is simply the sum of its
for, say, gender, education, race, and tenure. parts. Diversity variables serving as formative
After doing so, they arrive at a single, global indicators would not need to share an underly-
1216 Academy of Management Review October

ing construct that positively binds them together Guideline 7: Simple (average or total)
(although they would then have a different em- operationalizations of overall diver-
pirical meaning in every study), nor would they sity should be avoided unless (a) the-
have to be the same diversity type. oretical motivations for their aggre-
However, there are two critical methodologi- gate (formative) effects are clear, or (b)
cal drawbacks to this approach. First, a measure evidence of their convergent validity
of overall diversity treats the causal force of can be shown.
each component variable as equal (Edwards,
2001), masking effects that might be due mainly
Perceived Diversity
to one variable (e.g., gender variety) rather than
another (e.g., ethnic background variety). Sec- Another approach sometimes used to opera-
ond, an overall diversity measure masks sub- tionalize diversity is to ask individuals how they
stantive differences among units that have the perceive the diversity variable to be arrayed
same overall composite score. Suppose that one within their unit (Dooley, Fryxell, & Judge, 2000;
of the medical research teams described above Harrison et al., 1998; Harrison et al., 2002; Jehn et
is composed of six whites and two African- al., 1999; Miller, Burke, & Glick, 1998; Turban &
Americans, or six African-Americans and two Jones, 1988). Such measures are reasonable—
whites. Its Blau index for race is the same in even required—when authors seek to test theo-
either case. Suppose, further, that the team is ries specifically addressing perceptions of dif-
composed of six women and two men, or two ferences (see Riordan, 2000, for a review).
women and six men. Again, its Blau index for Indeed, perceived diversity within a unit may
gender is the same in either case. As a result, have unique and more proximal explanatory
the team’s overall diversity would be the same if power than actual diversity, as a stream of or-
there were (1) six white men and two African- ganizational research suggests individuals’ per-
American women; (2) six African-American men ceptions of their social environment have stron-
and two white women; (3) six African-American ger, more direct influences on behavior than
women and two white men; (4) six white women does the social environment itself (Eisenberger,
and two African-American men; (5) four white Huntington, Hutchison, & Sowa, 1986; Krack-
men, two white women, and two African- hardt, 1990).
American men; and so on.2 Theory and evidence For several reasons, however, measures of
suggest, however, that these groups would dif- perceived diversity are not likely to be con-
fer markedly in their networks, coalitional dy- struct-valid measures of “actual” diversity
namics, and information sources (e.g., Ibarra, (McGrath, 1984). First, individuals may lack the
1993; Mollica, Gray, & Treviño, 2003; Tsui et al., necessary information to accurately assess the
1992). To treat all of these groups as the same— diversity of the rest of their unit members, espe-
and we have not listed all possible combina- cially on the thornier and perhaps more sensi-
tions that would yield the same overall tive psychological characteristics that might
scores—is to introduce a great deal of theoreti- have sparked the researcher’s move toward us-
cal ambiguity into one’s operationalization of ing perceptions in the first place. See the low
diversity. Thus, we suggest the following. correlations in Harrison et al. (2002) for per-
ceived and actual “deep” diversity, as well as
Miller et al.’s (1998) correlations between per-
2
This example recalls Lau and Murnighan’s (1998) in- ceived and actual cognitive diversity. Second,
sightful analysis of group faultlines. The gender and race perceived diversity ratings are likely to be bi-
faultline is stronger in the first four groups specified than in
ased, relative to measures of actual diversity.
the fifth. The construct of faultline strength, to which we
return later in this article, bears some similarity to the notion On the one hand, attraction and social desirabil-
of overall diversity, although the construct of faultline ity biases can promote perceptions of greater
strength is far more precise and focused. Even so, it has similarity (less diversity) than actually exists
proven challenging to operationalize faultline strength out- (Clore & Byrne, 1974). On the other hand, theory
side of environments such as the laboratory, where individ-
and research regarding the outgroup homoge-
uals can be assigned to groups to create weak versus strong
coalignments of differences. Recent developments in the neity effect suggest that, under some conditions,
measurement of faultline strength in naturally occurring ingroup members may overestimate their own
groups are encouraging (Shaw, 2004) but remain little tested. unit’s diversity. This is particularly likely when
2007 Harrison and Klein 1217

judgments are made relative to other units (Lee Finally, a researcher interested in perceived
& Ottati, 1993, 1995; Park & Judd, 1990; Park, disparity of team member influence in team de-
Ryan, & Judd, 1992; Quattrone & Jones, 1980). cision making might ask, “To what extent do
Third, under either form of perceptual error, team members differ in their influence over im-
common method bias might inflate the observed portant team decisions?” Here, an appropriate
correlation of perceived diversity measures and response scale might be “1 ⫽ Every member has
member reports of group outcomes (e.g., group the same amount of influence”; “3 ⫽ Some group
cognition, conflict, performance, etc.; Harrison, members have more influence than others”; and
McLaughlin, & Coalter, 1996). This renders sus- “5 ⫽ One team member has substantially more
pect any comparison of the strength of relation- influence than all other members.“
ships of perceived diversity relative to actual The examples above are only suggestions.
diversity with such outcomes. Accordingly, we However, their verbal descriptors map directly
make the following recommendation. onto our diversity typology, as well as onto the
minimum, maximum, and moderate levels of
Guideline 8: Measures of perceived di- those types. Regardless of the specific words
versity should not substitute for mea- used in such perceptual scales, we propose the
sures of actual diversity. Measures of following.
perceived diversity can, however, pro-
vide an operationalization of a useful, Guideline 9: If researchers use mea-
substantive construct—members’ per- sures of perceived diversity, their
ceptions of unit diversity—that may questions, response formats, and an-
be related to but is different from ac- chors should reflect the diversity type
tual unit diversity. under consideration, following the
distributions shown in Figure 1.
Our efforts to distinguish diversity as separa-
tion, as variety, and as disparity suggest that if
Sampling the Full Range: Between-Unit
member perceptions of within-unit differences
Variance of Within-Unit Diversity
serve a theoretical purpose, researchers may
need to develop distinct instruments for per- The recommendations above are designed to
ceived separation, perceived variety, and per- ensure that a researcher’s diversity indexes are
ceived disparity. Perhaps a researcher inter- construct valid, capturing the specific diversity
ested in perceived separation in satisfaction type specified in the theory the investigator is
with a supervisor might ask, “How much do testing. But researchers may still draw incorrect
team members differ in their satisfaction with conclusions from their research if their sample
the team supervisor?” An appropriate response of units does not show the full breadth of diver-
scale might thus be “1 ⫽ Not at all; all team sity. To avoid range restriction, researchers
members feel the same way about the supervi- seeking to test diversity theories must gather
sor”; “3 ⫽ Somewhat; some team members are samples in which there is sufficient between-
more satisfied with the supervisor than others”; unit variability in diversity to allow effects to be
and “5 ⫽ A great deal; half of the team’s mem- revealed.
bers are very satisfied with the supervisor and The implications of this admonition differ for
the other half of the team’s members are very researchers studying separation, variety, and
dissatisfied with the supervisor.” disparity. Investigators testing the unit-level ef-
A researcher interested in perceived variety of fects of separation must ensure not only that
functional backgrounds might ask respondents, their sampled units vary in amount of (maxi-
“To what extent do group members differ in their mum to minimum) separation but also that sam-
functional backgrounds?” An appropriate re- pled units include different combinations that
sponse scale might be “1 ⫽ All group members lead to the same amount of separation on S.
have the same functional background”; “3 ⫽ That is, for strong tests of the assumed symmet-
Some group members’ functional backgrounds ric effects of attitude separation on S, a re-
differ from others’ “; and “5 ⫽ Each group mem- searcher’s sample must include some units in
ber has his/her own unique functional back- which all members have negative attitudes,
ground.” other units in which all members have positive
1218 Academy of Management Review October

attitudes, still other units in which all members sumption fundamental to that diver-
have neutral attitudes, and, of course, some sity type.
units that are widely split on the focal attitude,
representing maximum separation.3 This allows
the researcher to assess whether the effects of FUTURE DIRECTIONS FOR THEORY AND
minimal separation are, in fact, symmetric. Such RESEARCH
a test would involve entering main effects of the
within-unit SD and mean in an analysis, fol- By justifying their choice of separation, va-
lowed by the SD ⫻ mean interaction, reiterating riety, or disparity, theorists may deepen their
our Guideline 6 above. A significant interaction understanding of the likely consequences of
indicates asymmetric separation effects (i.e., the attribute-specific diversity within units and
impact of separation depends on the level of S). may refine their predictions. Further, precise
See Barsade et al. (2000) for an example. specification of diversity types facilitates con-
Researchers testing the unit-level effects of struct-valid operationalizations and appropri-
variety require samples that have different ate tests of the effects of each diversity type.
amounts of category richness or unevenness The diversity typology we have presented also
across units. Samples must include units in suggests new avenues of diversity research. In
which variety is very low and units is which explicating the meaning of disparity, we noted
variety is very high. Without this full sampling, how little research addresses the unit-level
researchers will underestimate relationships consequences of within-unit inequality in
between variety and its antecedents or out- power, status, and other valued resources.
comes because of restricted range. Testing the This is an important and understudied aspect
unit-level effects of disparity requires, of course, of diversity (Konrad, 2003). We also see real
samples in which units vary from a single indi- promise in research integrating the study of
vidual lording resources over all the others diversity and social networks (e.g., Reagans &
(high disparity) to an even distribution of the Zuckerman, 2001). Social network analysis
resource across members (low disparity). We may allow researchers to enrich their under-
thus make the following suggestion. standing of the relational processes associ-
ated with each diversity type, even allowing a
Guideline 10: To allow an adequate single demographic variable to be associated
test of hypotheses regarding separa- with more than one diversity type and to have
tion, variety, and disparity, respec- simultaneous, opposing effects (see Reagans,
tively, a researcher’s sample must ev- Zuckerman, & McEvily, 2004). Further, we en-
idence substantial between-unit courage more research on the antecedents of
variability in within-unit separation, separation, variety, and disparity in organiza-
variety, or disparity. Units should rep- tions. With some important exceptions (e.g.,
resent the full range of minimum to Boone, van Olffen, van Witteloostuijn, & De
maximum separation, variety, or dis- Brabander, 2004; Klein et al., 2001; Miner et al.,
parity, respectively. In the specific 2003; Pfeffer & Davis-Blake, 1990), this territory
case of separation, the sample must of research remains largely uncharted.
allow verification of the symmetry as- Finally, as we noted in previous sections, we
see both a real need and a real opportunity for
research and theory building regarding the
3
relationships among the three diversity types.
Racial diversity—conceptualized as separation—may Given the complexity of this broad topic, we
provide a more intuitively obvious example. If a researcher
wants to argue that the effects of racial separation are sym- focus on it below, addressing three issues that
metric, he or she needs to show that the effects of homoge- each touch on and may help to clarify the
neity (minimal separation) are the same regardless of independent and interdependent nature of
whether the group is all African-American or all white. Fur- separation, variety, and disparity in organiza-
ther, he or she needs to show that the effects of separation
tions. More specifically, we consider (1) strat-
are the same regardless of whether the group is numerically
dominated by whites or by African-Americans (by definition, egies for disentangling assumptions of demo-
at the point of maximum separation, a team has an equal graphic separation, variety, and disparity; (2)
number of whites and African-Americans). relationships among and interactive impacts
2007 Harrison and Klein 1219

of diversity types; and (3) multilevel diversity Kilduff, Angelmar, & Mehra, 2000), they have re-
effects. ceived so little attention.
To answer these questions, a researcher must
examine the individual-level relationship of the
Disentangling Separation, Variety, and
variables within units. Suppose a researcher
Disparity: One Step Backward and Several
conceptualizes tenure diversity as separation,
Steps Forward for Demographic Diversity
assuming that tenure is associated with orga-
Research?
nizational commitment. A unit-level test of that
We have urged theorists and researchers to assumption would cloud interpretation. A signif-
specify their chosen diversity types. But we have icant unit-level relationship between, say, ten-
also shown that within-unit diversity of several ure SD and commitment SD may occur even in
demographic variables, including tenure, age, the absence of any connection between tenure
gender, race or ethnicity, and education, can be and commitment within units (Ostroff & Harri-
meaningfully conceptualized as separation or son, 1999). Random coefficient modeling (also
variety or disparity. How, then, should scholars referred to as hierarchical linear modeling; Hof-
interested in demographic diversity proceed? mann, Griffin, & Gavin, 2000) allows a re-
Our answer is to suggest research designed to searcher to simultaneously test within-unit rela-
test the theoretical assumptions specific to each tionships for each unit of his or her sample. Of
diversity type. Suppose the demographic at- course, such a test requires careful measure-
tribute in question is tenure. Within units, is ment of demographic attributes and their theo-
tenure significantly correlated with key percep- retical sources of diversity (e.g., separation in
tions, beliefs, attitudes, values, or other vehicles member attitudes, variety of knowledge, or in-
of separation? Or do tenure cohorts vary in their equality in prestige).
knowledge, experience, or perspective, as sug- If this fine-grained, within-unit test reveals
gested by a conceptualization of tenure diver- that the data for most or all teams in the sample
sity as variety?4 Alternatively, is tenure associ- conform to the predictions associated with one
ated with the unequal distribution of power and diversity type, the researcher has a powerful
status, consistent with a conceptualization of and empirically grounded foundation for his or
tenure diversity as disparity? These are basic her subsequent investigation of the unit-level
questions of construct validity—so basic it is correlates of attribute diversity of that type. But
remarkable that, with few exceptions (e.g., the results of such a test might be more complex,
lending support to more than one diversity type,
or revealing predicted relationships of the de-
4
mographic attribute within some units but not
When conceptualizing tenure as variety, researchers others. The test might even provide no support
may be tempted to simply divide tenure into convenient
subgroupings: zero to five years, six to ten years, etc. But, in for any diversity type. The latter result would
fact, this choice warrants careful consideration, since it is suggest that diversity of the attribute in ques-
not always clear what the cohorts should be that represent tion is highly unlikely to have predicted unit-
different “batches” of knowledge or experience. Cohort def- level consequences.
initions should ideally be supported by a conceptualization
Is such extensive testing really called for,
that specifies why, for example, one to four years of tenure is
markedly different from five to eight, or nine to fifteen (or prior to the investigator’s examination of his or
whatever cohort boundaries are chosen). Clear events in a her real interest—the effects of tenure diversi-
firm or unit, such as before and after a reorganization or ty—just because there is uncertainty in the pre-
acquisition, might support demarcations of tenure cohorts as cise conceptualization of it as one of the three
sources of variety. These arguments are relevant beyond the
archetypes? We think so. The “no support” result
bounds of demographic research. For example, if political
ideology is conceptualized not as a continuous variable from is within the realm of possibility given the in-
conservative to liberal (engendering potential separation consistent nature of past diversity findings. In
within a unit) but as a source of variety of perspectives the past two decades a wealth of theory and
within a unit, a researcher faces a comparable puzzle: research on demographic diversity has yielded
Where should he or she draw the lines distinguishing those
few cumulative or conclusive effects. Research-
who are very conservative from those who are conservative,
those who are moderate from those who are liberal, and so ers can, we believe, resolve some of this uncer-
on? Self-identification in one category or another is a rea- tainty by stepping backwards, and down a level
sonable starting point. of analysis, to test the fundamental and distinc-
1220 Academy of Management Review October

tive assumptions underlying diversity as sepa- about interpretive methods arise. Polarization
ration, as variety, and as disparity. The payoff (sharp separation) of attitudes toward interpre-
from such research will come as researchers tive research may remain, but the variety of
then move several steps forward in deciphering research expertise may increase within the unit.
the nature and effects of demographic diversity. As separation can engender variety, so vari-
ety can engender separation. Variety in disci-
plinary or scientific training might lead to sep-
Relationships Among and Interdependent
aration in support for qualitative research in
Effects of Diversity Types
Team V for example; some functional back-
In describing the meaning, shape, mecha- grounds may be more accepting and supportive
nism, and ideal operationalization of each di- of qualitative research than others. This as-
versity type, we have described them in turn. Yet sumption can be seen in research treating vari-
separation, variety, and disparity are likely to ety in functional background as a source of be-
cooccur within units. Indeed, separation, vari- lief and attitude differences in top management
ety, and disparity may, in some cases, be caus- teams (e.g., Beyer et al., 1997; Chattopadhyay,
ally related. Further, they may well have joint Glick, Miller, & Huber, 1999). As separation leads
consequences for unit outcomes. New theory de- to conflict, separation might then mediate the
velopment and research are needed to examine relationship between variety and conflict. But it
these possibilities. Does within-unit diversity of is also possible to imagine cases in which vari-
one type lead to within-unit diversity of other ety leads to neither separation nor conflict.
types? Does diversity of one type moderate the Rather, unit members who differ in their respec-
effects of diversity of other types? We offer some tive areas of expertise may each fulfill a distinct
speculative answers to these questions below, role in the team, coordinating efficiently and
with the hope of sparking researchers’ interest respectfully with one another and paying due
in these topics. attention to one another’s inputs (Stewart &
Diversity begetting diversity. Although the Stasser, 1995). Further, when variety within a
three diversity types are qualitatively and dis- unit grows toward its maximum, the polariza-
tinctly different, they may nevertheless be tion indicative of maximum separation seems
linked to one another over time. Diversity of one increasingly unlikely; there are simply too many
type might engender diversity of a second type. V types represented for unit members to fall
We briefly explore a few possibilities. neatly into two clearly opposing factions. In
Separation can engender variety, particularly sum, we suspect that variety may sometimes
increases from minimum to moderate variety. If lead to separation and then conflict, but not
unit members are sharply separated—for exam- inevitably or even generally.
ple, in their endorsement of a particular unit Finally, we consider disparity. Disparity may
goal or strategy—they might be motivated to get lead to both an increase in separation and an
new information to support their position, in- increase in variety. When the members of a unit
cluding seeking out (external) others who sup- differ sharply in their relative power, prestige,
port their own perspective. In this way the mem- or pay, they perceive the unit’s policies and ac-
bers of a subunit may develop network contacts tivities from different vantage points. Their in-
and information not shared by the members of terests differ as well. Those at the top of the
the opposing subunit (Peterson & Thompson, hierarchy are likely to support the status quo.
1997). For example, the members of the subunit Those at the bottom may seek change. As a
of Team S who favor interpretive research may result, disparity may cause separation in be-
seek out experts in qualitative research in an liefs, attitudes, or values related to unit pro-
effort to bolster their own position. Conversely, cesses and outcomes that might dislodge the
the members of the subunit of Team S who op- current status hierarchy (Phillips & Zuckerman,
pose that research paradigm may seek out ex- 2001). Further, individuals who have less status,
perts who are aligned with their own prefer- power, and influence than an “upper” minority
ences. In both cases the knowledge and of their teammates are likely to respond by form-
argument base associated with each of the fac- ing coalitions with others who are similarly less
tions is likely to increase and to be brought powerful, acquiring valued resources through
forward to the rest of the unit when discussions illicit means or through contacts outside the unit
2007 Harrison and Klein 1221

(e.g., Murnighan, 1978). In addition, those having ices) were the same team? Our speculative an-
different levels of such resources are likely to swer is that higher levels of disparity would
socialize with different individuals and, thus, to tend to diminish or even neutralize the benefits
develop distinct sets of network ties (e.g., Ma- expected from the presence of differing sources
man, 2000; McGuire, 2000). In this way disparity of task-relevant knowledge within the team.
may engender variety in experiences, knowl- Unit members, such as the renowned expert in
edge, and social contacts. Team D, who hold the highest concentration of
Diversity moderating diversity. The three di- the team’s valued resources and have the high-
versity types may also interact to influence unit est status are likely to dictate the team’s prac-
outcomes. Lau and Murnighan (1998) have al- tices and performance. Team members with less
ready forwarded a rich set of ideas about joint power or status are likely to stay mum, deferring
effects of within-unit demographic differences. to the top members’ wishes and failing to share
In their theory, for joint occurrences of demo- critical information (Cruz, Henningsen, & Smith,
graphic attributes to become increasingly po- 1999; Janis & Mann, 1977). Pitcher and Smith offer
tent faultlines within a unit, the diversity at- a similar interpretation: “A significant power
tributes in question must (1) be apparent to unit shift renders the heterogeneity and the diverse
members, (2) covary or coincide strongly within cognitive perspectives that it represents less
the unit, and (3) create a small number of homo- and less influential in the strategic decision-
geneous subgroups or factions. Note that the making process” (2001: 9). Accordingly, we sus-
mere coincidence or correlation of two attributes pect that disparity within a team is likely to
within a unit is insufficient to form a faultline. If, moderate the expected positive relationship be-
for example, task satisfaction and organization- tween variety and positive team outcomes.5
al commitment are positively correlated within The interdependent effects of within-unit sep-
a team, their joint occurrence will not neces- aration and variety may prove even more com-
sarily divide the unit into two or more clearly plex. What if Team V (maximum variety) and
differentiated factions. Moreover, as the number Team S (maximum separation in attitude toward
of factions grows, faultlines will weaken (there qualitative research) were the same team?
is no single, large “chasm” within the team but, Members would find it difficult to develop and
rather, several small “cracks”). This suggests implement an integrative, creative plan of ac-
that strong faultlines are most likely to occur tion. Conflict and division seem more likely than
within a unit—translating Lau and Murnighan’s creativity and performance. If sharing unique
propositions into an important case in our information requires motivated behavior from
framework—when two or more variables have unit members, even in the face of risk (others in
coincidentally maximum separation or dispar- the unit cannot validate it), it is likely that mo-
ity, or both, but also when they are coupled with tivation would fall and the risk would rise as
only modest variety. Strong or near maximum members saw themselves separated from one
separation within a unit creates opposing fac- another on fundamental beliefs (Wittenbaum,
tions, which is a building block of strong fault- Hollingshead, & Botero, 2004). Thus, low separa-
lines. High disparity within a unit can do so as tion, especially on team goals and task-related
well. In contrast, when variety is high, faultlines values, may allow a team to realize the benefits
will weaken (a clear distinction from our frame- of team members’ variety of expertise and expe-
work), since unit members cannot be divided rience (cf. Edmondson, 1999; Gruenfeld et al.,
into two or just a few sharply divided subsets. 1996; Harrison, Mohammed, McGrath, Florey, &
Our differentiation of separation, variety, and Vanderstoep, 2003).
disparity suggests other intriguing interdepen-
dent effects, complementing Lau and Mur-
nighan’s (1998) insights. Consider our recurring 5
Disparity does not always trump variety. If the source of
example of the research Teams S, V, and D, who disparity is status based on task experience or expertise, the
are studying patient experiences with medical unit’s “ear” is likely to be tuned toward those who have the
best and most relevant information to share (Wittenbaum,
treatment. What if Team V (maximum variety,
1998). If, however, the source of disparity is based on another
with each member from one of eight different attribute unrelated to the unit’s task (physical attractiveness,
disciplines) and Team D (maximum disparity, wealth), disparity may mitigate the advantages of variety
with one prestigious researcher and seven nov- (Hollingshead, 1996).
1222 Academy of Management Review October

And yet a team that has no separation in composition of its members. Still, most studies
member goals, attitudes, or beliefs but is high in of diversity are single-level studies, focusing on
variety may fail to recognize and make use of unit-level outcomes of within-unit diversity. We
members’ diverse areas of knowledge and ex- join others (Jackson et al., 2003; Tsui & Gutek,
pertise (Larson, Christensen, Abbott, & Franz, 1999) in urging a more in-depth and comprehen-
1998; Stasser & Titus, 1985, 1987). Attention- sive multilevel analysis of diversity. One ap-
capturing differences—if they are not too proach would be to move up a level of analysis
large— could motivate members to “dig deeper and consider diversity across units within orga-
and to learn from contrasting examples” (Beck- nizations along the S, V, or D attribute in ques-
man & Haunschild, 2002: 98). Members who see tion. The effects of within-unit diversity may de-
themselves as identical on those underlying pend, in part, on the composition—specifically,
features might also assume that they share sim- the diversity— of the organizational context
ilar experiences and stores of knowledge and, (e.g., Alderfer & Smith, 1982). The effects of min-
thus, at least initially, might discuss shared ex- imum separation (perfect agreement) within a
periences and information (Wittenbaum et al., focal unit on S may depend on the extent to
2004). Some minimal separation of attitudes which the unit’s perspective is itself close to or
within the unit— especially early, and about separated from predominant views for S within
preferences for task processes (Jehn & Bender- other units of the organization.
sky, 2003)—may trigger constructive debate and For example, minimum separation within a
discussion, stimulating members to reveal to management department regarding the value of
one another their distinctive knowledge and ex- publishing in top-tier journals is likely to be a
pertise (Simons et al., 1999). much greater source of departmental cohesion if
The above ideas deal with potential bivariate other departments in the business school do not
effects of D, S, and V. What about the joint im- share the management department’s perspec-
pact of all three forms of diversity? We consider tive. If other units do share the focal unit’s per-
two possibilities. Suppose the hospital research spective, constituting minimum separation in
team is indeed deeply divided on attitudes to- the organization as a whole, then minimum sep-
ward research paradigms and, hence, is maxi- aration within the unit is likely to be less salient
mized on S. Suppose further that the two (inter- to unit members; it doesn’t make the unit dis-
pretive, quantitative) factions are perfectly tinctive within the organization. Turning back to
aligned with D and V: one faction holds fre- Team S, members’ maximal separation on atti-
quently published and renowned members with tude toward interpretive research might be
medical backgrounds, and the other faction taken as a healthy state of task conflict if wide,
holds behavioral scientists with no established within-unit splits in such attitudes (maximal
research records. Although the team is only separation) are also characteristic of most other
moderate on V and D, it exhibits a strong fault- research teams within their sponsoring organi-
line (Lau & Murnighan, 1998; Li & Hambrick, zation. If, however, the researchers who make up
2005). The team would be divided and dysfunc- the more positivistic faction of Team S know that
tional, but it would not stalemate because of the virtually all of the other units in their sponsoring
power of the first faction. In contrast, if D and V organization are strongly positivistic (and not
crosscut S (a weak faultline; Brewer & Miller, split), the presumption of normative correctness
1984), with prestige and disciplinary back- of their own position might lead to deeper, more
ground equally vested in opposing S factions, severe, and more interpersonally charged bat-
the team would have conflict but would be able tles within the team.
to reach across factional boundaries to leverage A complementary multilevel approach would
its within-unit variety. Other trivariate configura- be to consider the implications of diversity types
tions are viable and interesting and could readily for the experiences and reactions of individuals
be studied in both field and laboratory designs. within units. This is the focus of most studies of
relational demography (e.g., Jackson et al., 1991)
and other forms of dyadic similarity (e.g.,
Multilevel Influences
Turban & Jones, 1988). For example, Dose (1999)
Diversity is inherently a multilevel construct. examined the correlates of an individual unit
It describes a unit in terms of the collective member’s distance from the other members of
2007 Harrison and Klein 1223

the unit, testing whether individuals whose val- Adler, P. S., & Kwon, S. W. 2002. Social capital: Prospects for
ues were similar to their teammates’ were more a new concept. Academy of Management Review, 27:
17– 40.
attached to the team.
The extent of separation, variety, or disparity Agresti, A., & Agresti, B. F. 1978. Statistical analysis of qual-
itative variation. In K. F. Schuessler (Ed.), Sociological
within a unit may color and shape an individu-
methodology: 204 –237. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
al’s experience of difference from other unit
members. For instance, if a research team is low Alderfer, C. P., & Smith, K. K. 1982. Studying intergroup rela-
tions embedded in organizations. Administrative Sci-
in variety of disciplinary backgrounds, say, with
ence Quarterly, 27: 35– 65.
a single psychologist amidst an array of seven
Allison, P. D. 1978. Measures of inequality. American Socio-
physicians, the lone psychologist is likely to be
logical Review, 43: 865– 880.
seen as an outlier on the team. This is docu-
mented in theory and research on minorities Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. 1992. Demography and
design: Predictors of new product team performance.
and tokenism (distinctiveness theory—McGuire,
Organization Science, 3: 321–341.
1999; minority-majority influence—Nemeth,
Argote, L., & Ingram, P. 2000. Knowledge transfer: A basis for
1986). From a multilevel perspective, however,
competitive advantage in firms. Organizational Behav-
the experiences of someone who is different ior and Human Decision Processes, 82: 150 –169.
from all the others in the team will be radically
Ashby, W. R. 1956. An introduction to cybernetics. New York:
changed depending on the unit context: Do all
Wiley.
the others on the team also differ from one an-
Austin, J. R. 2003. Transactive memory in organizational
other, or are they all the same? If the research
groups: The effects of content, consensus, specialization,
team has maximum variety, like our Team V, the and accuracy on group performance. Journal of Applied
lone psychologist (whose average difference Psychology, 88: 866 – 878.
from everyone else is still “1”) is not an interper-
Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., Neubert, M. J., & Mount, M. K.
sonal outlier on the team but, in effect, just like 1998. Relating member ability and personality to work-
everyone else. As with every other team mem- team processes and team effectiveness. Journal of
ber, he or she is the only representative of a Applied Psychology, 83: 377–391.
particular background. Barsade, S. G., Ward, A. J., Turner, J. D. F., & Sonnenfeld, J.
2000. To your heart’s content: A model of affective diver-
CONCLUSION sity in top management teams. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 45: 802– 836.
The challenge of differences— of diversity—is Beckman, C. M., & Haunschild, P. R. 2002. Network learning:
a crucial one for managers and scholars. How- The effects of partners’ heterogeneity of experience on
ever, even as research has accumulated, the corporate acquisitions. Administrative Science Quar-
nature and effects of diversity have remained terly, 47: 92–124.
uncertain. We have tried to address that chal- Bedeian, A. G., & Mossholder, K. W. 2000. On the use of the
lenge by positing that diversity constructs coefficient of variation as a measure of diversity. Orga-
have three fundamental types: separation, vari- nizational Research Methods, 3: 285–297.
ety, and disparity. The three types differ in their Berger, J., Fisek, M. H., Norman, R. Z., & Zelditch, M., Jr. 1977.
substance, shape, maxima, and implications. Status characteristics and expectation states: A graph-
Crucial attributes and relevant theoretical per- theoretical formulation. In J. Berger (Ed.), Status charac-
teristics and social interaction: An expectations states
spectives differ for each type, as do the within-
approach: 91–134. New York: Elsevier.
unit processes and outcomes likely to be asso-
ciated with them. We urge investigators to Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., & Zelditch, M. 1980. Status orga-
nizing processes. Annual Review of Sociology, 6: 479 –
specify the diversity types they are studying and
508.
to align them with specific, appropriate opera-
Berkowitz, L., & Macaulay, J. R. 1961. Some effects of differ-
tionalizations. By systematically asking and an-
ences in status level and status stability. Human Rela-
swering, “What’s the difference?” management tions, 14: 135–147.
scholars may reveal a clearer, more cumulative
Beyer, J. M., Chattopadhyay, P., George, E., Glick, W. H.,
understanding of diversity in organizations.
Ogilvie, dt, & Pugliese, D. 1997. The selective perception
of managers revisited. Academy of Management Jour-
REFERENCES nal, 40: 716 –737.
Adams, J. S. 1963. Toward an understanding of inequity. Blau, P. M. 1960. Patterns of deviation in work groups. So-
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67: 422– 436. ciometry, 23: 245–261.
1224 Academy of Management Review October

Blau, P. M. 1977. Inequality and heterogeneity. New York: Cruz, M. G., Henningsen, D. D., & Smith, B. A. 1999. The
Free Press. impact of directive leadership on group’s information
Bloom, M. 1999. The performance effects of pay dispersion on sampling, decisions, and perceptions of the leaders.
individuals and organizations. Academy of Manage- Communication Research, 26: 349 –369.
ment Journal, 42: 25– 40. Deutsch, M. 1985. Distributive justice: A social psychological
Bloom, M., & Michel, J. G. 2002. The relationships among perspective. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
organizational context, pay dispersion, and managerial Doerr, K. H., Mitchell, T. R., Schriesheim, C. A., Freed, T., &
turnover. Academy of Management Journal, 45: 33– 42. Zhou, X. 2002. Heterogeneity and variability in the con-
Bollen, K., & Lennox, R. 1991. Conventional wisdom on mea- text of flow lines. Academy of Management Review, 27:
surement: A structural equation perspective. Psycholog- 594 – 607.
ical Bulletin, 110: 305–314. Dooley, R. S., Fryxell, G. E., & Judge, W. Q. 2000. Belaboring
Boone, C., van Olffen, W., van Witteloostuijn, B., & De Bra- the not-so-obvious: Consensus, commitment, and strat-
bander, B. 2004. The genesis of top management team egy implementation speed and success. Journal of Man-
diversity: Selective turnover among top management agement, 26: 1237–1257.
teams in Dutch newspaper publishing, 1970 –94. Acad- Dose, J. J. 1999. The relationship between work values simi-
emy of Management Journal, 47: 633– 656. larity and team-member and leader-member exchange
Brewer, M. B., & Miller, N. 1984. Beyond the contact hypoth- relationships. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
esis: Theoretical perspectives on desegregation. In Practice, 3: 20 –32.
N. Miller & M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Groups in contact: The Drazin, R., & Rao, H. 1999. Managerial power and succession:
psychology of desegregation: 281–302. Orlando, FL: Aca- SBU managers of mutual funds. Organization Studies,
demic Press. 20: 167–196.
Bunderson, J. S., & Sutcliffe, K. M. 2002. Comparing alterna- Edmondson, A. 1999. Psychological safety and learning be-
tive conceptualizations of functional diversity in man- havior in work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly,
agement teams: Process and performance effects. Acad- 44: 350 –383.
emy of Management Journal, 45: 875– 893.
Edwards, J. R. 2001. Multidimensional constructs in organi-
Burt, R. S. 2002. The social capital of structural holes. In M. F. zational behavior research: An integrative analytical
Guillen, R. Collins, P. England, & M. Meyer (Eds.), The framework. Organizational Research Methods, 4: 144 –
new economic sociology: 148 –189. New York: Russell 192.
Sage Foundation.
Eisenberger, R., Huntington, R., Hutchison, S., & Sowa, D.
Byrne, D. 1971. The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic 1986. Perceived organizational support. Journal of Ap-
Press. plied Psychology, 71: 500 –507.
Campbell, D. T. 1960. Blind variation and selective retention Eisenhardt, K. M., & Bourgeois, L. J. 1988. Politics of strategic
in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. decision making in high-velocity environments: Toward
Psychological Review, 67: 380 – 400. a midrange theory. Academy of Management Journal,
Carpenter, M. A. 2002. The implications of strategy and so- 31: 737–770.
cial context for the relationship between top manage- Ferrier, W. J. 2001. Navigating the competitive landscape:
ment team heterogeneity and firm performance. Strate- The drivers and consequences of competitive aggres-
gic Management Journal, 23: 275–284. siveness. Academy of Management Journal, 44: 858 – 878.
Champernowne, D. 1974. A comparison of measures of in- Finkelstein, S., & Hambrick, D. C. 1996. Strategic leadership:
equality of income distribution. Economic Journal, 84: Top executives and their effects on organizations. Min-
26 –54. neapolis: West Educational Publishing.
Chatman, J. A.. 1991. Matching people and organizations: French, J. R. P., Jr., & Raven, B. 1959. The bases of social
Selection and socialization in public accounting firms. power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power:
Administrative Science Quarterly, 36: 459 – 484. 150 –165. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Chatman, J. A., & Flynn, F. J. 2001. The influence of demo- Gibson, C., & Vermeulen, F. 2003. A healthy divide: Sub-
graphic heterogeneity on the emergence and conse- groups as a stimulus for team learning behavior. Ad-
quences of cooperative norms in work teams. Academy ministrative Science Quarterly, 48: 202–239.
of Management Journal, 44: 956 –974.
Gruenfeld, D. H., Mannix, E. A., Williams, K. Y., & Neale, M. A.
Chattopadhyay, P., Glick, W. H., Miller, C. C., & Huber, G. P. 1996. Group composition and decision making: How
1999. Determinants of executive beliefs: Comparing member familiarity and information distribution affect
functional conditioning and social influence. Strategic process and performance. Organizational Behavior and
Management Journal, 20: 763–789. Human Decision Processes, 67: 1–15.
Clore, G. L., & Byrne, D. A. 1974. A reinforcement-affect model Grusky, D. B. 1994. The contours of social stratification. In
of attraction. In T. L. Huston (Ed.), Foundations of inter- D. B. Grusky (Ed.), Social stratification: Class, race, and
personal attraction: 143–170. New York: Academic Press. gender in sociological perspective: 3–35. Boulder, CO:
Cohen, B. P., & Zhou, X. 1991. Status processes in enduring Westview Press.
work groups. American Sociological Review, 56: 179 –188. Hambrick, D. C., Cho, T. S., & Chen, M. 1996. The influence of
2007 Harrison and Klein 1225

top management team heterogeneity on firms’ compet- Homans, G. C. 1961. Social behavior: Its elementary forms.
itive moves. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41: 659 – New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World.
684.
Ibarra, H. 1993. Personal networks of women and minorities
Hambrick, D. C., & Mason, P. A. 1984. Upper echelons: The in management: A conceptual framework. Academy of
organization as a reflection of its top managers. Acad- Management Review, 18: 56 – 87.
emy of Management Review, 9: 193–206.
Jackson, S. E., Brett, J. F., Sessa, V. I., Cooper, D. M., Julin, J. A.,
Hansen, M. T. 1999. The search-transfer problem: The role of & Peyronnin, K. 1991. Some differences make a differ-
weak ties in sharing knowledge across organization ence: Individual dissimilarity and group heterogeneity
subunits. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44: 82–111. as correlates of recruitment, promotions, and turnover.
Harrison, D. A., McLaughlin, M. E., & Coalter, T. M. 1996. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76: 675– 689.
Context, cognition, and common method variance: Psy- Jackson, S. E., Joshi, A., & Erhardt, N. L. 2003. Recent research
chometric and verbal protocol evidence. Organizational on team and organizational diversity: SWOT analysis
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68: 246 –261. and implications. Journal of Management, 29: 801– 830.
Harrison, D. A., Mohammed, S., McGrath, J. E., Florey, A. T., & James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. 1993. rwg: An assess-
Vanderstoep, S. 2003. Time matters in team task perfor- ment of within-group interrater agreement. Journal of
mance: Effects of member familiarity, entrainment, and Applied Psychology, 78: 306 –309.
task discontinuity on speed and quality. Personnel Psy- Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. 1977. Decision making: A psychological
chology, 56: 633– 669. analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York:
Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., & Bell, M. P. 1998. Beyond rela- Free Press.
tional demography: Time and the effects of surface- and Jehn, K., & Bendersky, C. 2003. Intragroup conflict in organi-
deep-level diversity on work group cohesion. Academy zations: A contingency perspective on the conflict-
of Management Journal, 41: 96 –107. outcome relationship. Research in Organizational Be-
Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., Gavin, J. H., & Florey, A. T. 2002. havior, 25: 187–242.
Time, teams, and task performance: Changing effects of Jehn, K. A., Northcraft, G. B., & Neale, M. A. 1999. Why differ-
surface- and deep-level diversity on group functioning. ences make a difference: A field study of diversity, con-
Academy of Management Journal, 45: 1029 –1045. flict, and performance in workgroups. Administrative
Harrison, D. A., & Sin, H.-S. 2006. What is diversity and how Science Quarterly, 44: 741–763.
should it be measured? In A. M. Konrad, P. Prasad, & J. K. Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. 2003. Power,
Pringle (Eds.), Handbook of workplace diversity: 191–216. approach, and inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 110:
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 265–284.
Herfindahl, O. C. 1950. Concentration in the U.S. steel indus- Kilduff, M., Angelmar, R., & Mehra, A. 2000. Top manage-
try. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia Uni- ment–team diversity and firm performance: Examining
versity, New York. the role of cognitions. Organization Science, 11: 21–34.
Hill, M. O. 1973. Diversity and evenness: A unifying notation Kimura, K. 1994. A micro-macro linkage in the measurement
and its consequences. Ecology, 54: 427– 431. of inequality: Another look at the GINI coefficient.
Hinsz, V. B., Tindale, R. S., & Vollrath, D. A. 1997. The emerg- Quality and Quantity, 28: 83–97.
ing conceptualization of groups as information proces- Klein, K. J., Conn, A. B., Smith, D. B., & Sorra, J. S. 2001. Is
sors. Psychological Bulletin, 121: 43– 64. everyone in agreement? An exploration of within-group
Hirschman, A. O. 1964. The paternity of an index. American agreement in employee perceptions of the work environ-
Economic Review, 54: 761–762. ment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 3–16.
Hoffman, L. R., & Maier, N. 1961. Quality and acceptance of Konrad, A. M. 2003. Defining the domain of workplace diver-
problem solutions by members of homogeneous and sity scholarship. Group and Organization Management,
heterogeneous groups. Journal of Abnormal and Social 28: 4 –17.
Psychology, 62: 401– 407. Konrad, A. M., & Gutek, B. A. 1987. Theory and research on
Hofmann, D. A., Griffin, M., & Gavin, M. 2000. The application group composition: Applications to the status of women
of hierarchical linear modeling to organizational re- and ethnic minorities. In S. Oskamp & S. Spacapan
search. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multi- (Eds.), Interpersonal processes: 85–121. Thousand Oaks,
level theory, research, and methods in organizations: CA: Sage.
467–511. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Krackhardt, D. 1990. Assessing the political landscape:
Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. 2000. Social identity and self- Structure, cognition, and power in organizations. Ad-
categorization processes in organizational contexts. ministrative Science Quarterly, 35: 342–369.
Academy of Management Review, 25: 121–140. Larson, J. R., Jr., Christensen, C., Abbott, A. S., & Franz, T. M.
Hollander, E. P. 1958. Conformity, status, and idiosyncrasy 1998. Diagnosing groups: The pooling, management,
credit. Psychological Review, 65: 117–277. and impact of shared and unshared case information in
team-based medical decision making. Journal of Person-
Hollingshead, A. B. 1996. Information suppression and status
ality and Social Psychology, 75: 93–108.
persistence in group decision making. Human Commu-
nication Research, 23: 193–219. Lau, D. C., & Murnighan, J. K. 1998. Demographic diversity
1226 Academy of Management Review October

and faultlines: The compositional dynamics of organi- McGuire, G. M. 2000. Gender, race, ethnicity, and networks:
zational groups. Academy of Management Review, 23: The factors affecting the status of employees’ network
325–340. members. Work and Occupations, 27: 501–523.
Lawrence, B. S. 1997. The black box of organizational demog- McGuire, W. J. 1999. Constructing social psychology: Creative
raphy. Organization Science, 8: 1–22. and critical processes. New York: Cambridge University
Lazear, E., & Rosen, S. 1981. Rank-order tournaments as op- Press.
timum labor contracts. Journal of Political Economy, 89: Miller, C. C., Burke, L. M., & Glick, W. H. 1998. Cognitive
841– 864. diversity among upper-echelon executives: Implications
Lazear, E. P. 1995. Personnel economics. Cambridge, MA: MIT for strategic decision processes. Strategic Management
Press. Journal, 19: 39 –58.

Lee, Y., & Ottati, V. 1993. Determinants of in-group and out- Miner, A. S., Haunschild, P. R., & Schwab, A. 2003. Experience
group perceptions of heterogeneity: An investigation of and convergence: Curiosities and speculation. Indus-
Sino-American stereotypes. Journal of Cross-Cultural trial and Corporate Change, 12: 789 – 813.
Psychology, 24: 298 –318. Mollica, K. A., Gray, B., & Treviño, L. 2003. Racial homophily
Lee, Y., & Ottati, V. 1995. Perceived in-group homogeneity as and its persistence in newcomers’ social networks. Or-
a function of group membership salience and stereotype ganization Science, 14: 123–137.
threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21: Mueller, J. H., Schuessler, H. L., & Costner, H. L. 1970. Statis-
610 – 619. tical reasoning in sociology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Lefler, A., Gillespie, D. L., & Conaty, J. C. 1982. The effects of Murnighan, J. K. 1978. Models of coalition behavior: Game
status differentiation on nonverbal behavior. Social Psy- theoretic, social psychological and political perspec-
chology Quarterly, 45: 153–161. tives. Psychological Bulletin, 85: 1130 –1153.
Li, J. T., & Hambrick, D. C. 2005. Factional groups: A new Nemeth, C. J. 1986. Differential contributions of majority and
vantage on demographic faultlines, conflict, and disin- minority influence. Psychological Review, 93: 23–32.
tegration in work teams. Academy of Management Jour-
nal, 48: 794 – 813. Nemetz, P. L., & Christensen, S. L. 1996. The challenge of
cultural diversity: Harnessing a diversity of views to
Linnehan, F., & Konrad, A. M. 1999. Diluting diversity: Impli- understand multiculturalism. Academy of Management
cations for intergroup inequality in organizations. Jour- Review, 21: 434 – 462.
nal of Management Inquiry, 8: 399 – 414.
Newcomb, T. M. 1961. The acquaintance process. New York:
Locke, K. D., & Horowitz, L. M. 1990. Satisfaction in interper- Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
sonal interactions as a function of similarity in level of
dysphoria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, O’Reilly, C. A., III, Caldwell, D. F., & Barnett, W. P. 1989. Work
58: 823– 831. group demography, social integration, and turnover.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 34: 21–37.
MacCallum, R. C., & Browne, M. W. 1993. The use of causal
indicators in covariance structure models: Some practi- O’Reilly, C. A., III, Williams, K., & Barsade, S. 1997. Group
cal issues. Psychological Bulletin, 114: 533–541. demography and innovation: Does diversity help? Re-
search in the Management of Groups and Teams, 1:
Maman, D. 2000. Who accumulates directorships of big busi- 183–207.
ness firms in Israel? Organizational structure, social
capital and human capital. Human Relations, 53: 603– Ostroff, C., & Harrison, D. A. 1999. Meta-analysis, levels of
629. analysis, and best estimates of population correlations:
Cautions for interpreting meta-analytic results in orga-
Marsch, J. B. T. 2002. Cultural diversity as human capital.
nizational behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84:
Communication and Cognition, 35: 37– 49.
260 –270.
Marsden, P. V. 1990. Network diversity, substructures and
Park, B., & Judd, C. M. 1990. Measures and models of per-
opportunities for contact. In C. Calhoun, M. Meyer, &
ceived group variability. Journal of Personality and So-
R. S. Schott (Eds.), Structures of power and constraint:
cial Psychology, 42: 1051–1068.
Papers in honor of Peter Blau: 397– 410. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press. Park, B., Ryan, C. S., & Judd, C. M. 1992. Role of meaningful
sub-groups in explaining differences in perceived vari-
Martin, J. D., & Gray, L. N. 1971. Measurement of relative
ability for in-groups and out-groups. Journal of Person-
variation: Sociological examples. American Sociologi-
ality and Social Psychology, 63: 553–567.
cal Review, 36: 496 –502.
Pelled, L. H. 1996. Demographic diversity, conflict, and work
McGrath, J. E. 1984. Groups: Interaction and performance.
group outcomes: An intervening process theory. Organi-
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
zation Science, 6: 207–229.
McGrath, J. E., Berdahl, J. L., & Arrow, H. 1995. Traits, expec-
Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. M., & Xin, K. R. 1999. Exploring the
tations, culture, and clout: The dynamics of diversity in
black box: An analysis of work group diversity, conflict,
work groups. In S. E. Jackson & M. N. Ruderman (Eds),
and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44:
Diversity in work teams: Research paradigms for a
1–28.
changing workplace: 17– 45. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association. Peterson, E., & Thompson, L. 1997. Negotiation teamwork: The
2007 Harrison and Klein 1227

impact of information distribution and accountability and productivity: The social capital of corporate R&D
for performance depends on the relationship among teams. Organization Science, 12: 502–517.
team members. Organization Behavior and Human De- Reagans, R., Zuckerman, E. W., & McEvily, B. 2004. How to
cision Processes, 72: 364 –383. make the team: Social networks vs. demography as cri-
Pfeffer, J. 1983. Organizational demography. Research in Or- teria for designing effective teams. Administrative Sci-
ganizational Behavior, 5: 299 –357. ence Quarterly, 49: 101–133.
Pfeffer, J. 1998. The human equation: Building profits by put- Ridgeway, C. L., Johnson, C., & Diekema, D. 1994. External
ting people first. Boston: Harvard Business School Press. status, legitimacy, and compliance in male and female
groups. Social Forces, 72: 1051–1077.
Pfeffer, J., & Davis-Blake, A. 1990. Determinants of salary
dispersion in organizations. Industrial Relations, 29: 38 – Riordan, C. M. 2000. Relational demography within groups:
57. Past developments, contradictions, and new directions.
Research in Personnel and Human Resource Manage-
Pfeffer, J., & Davis-Blake, A. 1992. Salary dispersion: Location
ment, 19: 131–173.
in the salary distribution, and turn-over among college
administrators. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, Riordan, C. M., & Shore, L. 1997. Demographic diversity and
45: 753–763. employee attitudes: Examination of relational demogra-
phy within work units. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Pfeffer, J., & Langton, N. 1988. Wage inequality and the or-
82: 342–358.
ganization of work: The case of academic departments.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 33: 588 – 606. Schneider, B. 1987. The people make the place. Personnel
Psychology, 40: 437– 453.
Pfeffer, J., & Langton, N. 1993. The effect of wage dispersion
on satisfaction, productivity, and working collabora- Schneider, B., & Goldstein, H. W. 1995. The ASA framework:
tively: Evidence from college and university faculty. An update. Personnel Psychology, 48: 747–773.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 38: 382– 407. Shannon, C. E. 1948. A mathematical theory of communica-
Phillips, D. J., & Zuckerman, E. W. 2001. Middle-status con- tion. Bell System Technical Journal, 27: 379 – 423, 623– 656.
formity: Theoretical restatement and empirical demon- Shaw, B. J. 2004. The development and analysis of a measure
stration in two markets. American Journal of Sociology, of group faultlines. Organizational Research Methods,
107: 379 – 429. 7: 66 –100.
Pitcher, P., & Smith, A. D. 2001. Top management team het- Siegel, P. A., & Hambrick, D. C. 2005. Pay disparities within
erogeneity: Personality, power, and proxies. Organiza- top management groups: Evidence of harmful effects
tion Science, 12: 1–18. on performance of high-technology firms. Organization
Priem, R. L. 1990. Top management team group factors, con- Science, 16: 259 –274.
sensus, and firm performance. Strategic Management Simons, T., Pelled, L. H., & Smith, K. A. 1999. Making use of
Journal, 11: 469 – 478. difference: Diversity, debate, and decision comprehen-
Priem, R. L., Lyon, D. W., & Dess, G. G. 1999. Inherent limita- siveness in top management teams. Academy of Man-
tions of demographic proxies in top management team agement Journal, 42: 662– 673.
heterogeneity research. Journal of Management, 25: 935– Simpson, E. H. 1949. Measurement of diversity. Nature, 163:
953. 688.
Quattrone, G. A., & Jones, E. E. 1980. The perception of vari- Sorenson, J. 2002. The use and misuse of the coefficient of
ability within ingroups and outgroups: Implications for variation in organizational demography research. So-
the law of small numbers. Journal of Personality and ciological Methods and Research, 30: 475– 491.
Social Psychology, 13: 269 –277.
Stasser, G., & Titus, W. 1985. Pooling of unshared information
Rafaeli, A., & Pratt, M. 1993. Tailored meanings: On the in group decision making. Journal of Personality and
meaning and impact of organizational dress. Academy Social Psychology, 48: 1467–1478.
of Management Review, 18: 32–55.
Stasser, G., & Titus, W. 1987. Effects of information load and
Ragins, B. R., & Gonzalez, J. A. 2003. Understanding diversity percentage of shared information on the dissemination
in organizations: Getting a grip on a slippery construct. of unshared information during group discussion. Jour-
In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Organizational behavior: The state nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53: 81–93.
of the science: 125–163. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Stasser, G., Vaughan, S. I., & Stewart, D. D. 2000. Pooling
Associates.
unshared information: The benefits of knowing how ac-
Ragins, B. R., & Sundstrom, E. 1989. Gender and power in cess to information is distributed among group mem-
organizations: A longitudinal perspective. Psychologi- bers. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
cal Bulletin, 105: 51– 88. cesses, 82: 102–116.
Randel, A. E., & Jaussi, K. S. 2003. Functional background Stewart, D. D., & Stasser G. 1995. Expert role assignment and
identity, diversity, and individual performance in cross- information sampling during collective recall and deci-
functional teams. Academy of Management Journal, 46: sion-making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
763–774. ogy, 69: 619 – 628.
Reagans, R., & Zuckerman, E. W. 2001. Network, diversity, Tajfel, H. 1981. Human groups and social categories: Studies
1228 Academy of Management Review October

in social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Methods and applications. New York: Cambridge Uni-
Press. versity Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1979. An integrative theory of inter- Watson, W. E., Kumar, K., & Michaelson, L. K. 1993. Cultural
group conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The diversity’s impact on interaction process and perfor-
social psychology of intergroup relations: 33– 47. mance: Comparing homogenous and diverse task
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. groups. Academy of Management Journal, 36: 590 – 602.
Teachman, J. D. 1980. Analysis of population diversity. So- Webber, S. S., & Donahue, L. 2001. Impact of highly and less
ciological Methods and Research, 8: 341–362. job related diversity on work group cohesion and per-
Tsui, A. S., Ashford, S. J., St. Clair, L., & Xin, K. R. 1995. formance: A meta analysis. Journal of Management, 27:
Dealing with discrepant expectations: Response strate- 141–162.
gies and managerial effectiveness. Academy of Man- Wiersema, M. F., & Bantel, K. A. 1992. Top management team
agement Journal, 38: 1515–1543. turnover as an adaptation mechanism: The role of the
Tsui, A. S., Egan, T. D., & O’Reilly, C. A., III. 1992. Being environment. Strategic Management Journal, 14: 485–
different: Relational demography and organizational at- 504.
tachment. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37: 549 –579. Wiersema, M. F., & Bird, A. 1993. Organizational demography
Tsui, A. S., & Gutek, B. A. 1999. Demographic differences in in Japanese firms: Group heterogeneity, individual dis-
organizations: Current research and future directions. similarity, and top management team turnover. Acad-
Lanham, MD: Lexington Books. emy of Management Journal, 36: 996 –1025.
Tsui, A. S., Xin, K. R., & Egan, T. D. 1995. Relational demog- Williams, K. Y., & O’Reilly, C. A., III. 1998. Demography and
raphy: The missing link in vertical dyad linkage. In S. E. diversity in organizations: A review of 40 years of re-
Jackson & M. N. Ruderman (Eds.), Diversity in work search. Research in Organizational Behavior, 20: 77–140.
teams: 97–129. Washington, DC: American Psychological Wittenbaum, G. M. 1998. Information sampling in decision-
Association. making groups: The impact of members’ task relevant
Turban, D. B., & Jones, A. P. 1988. Supervisor-subordinate status. Small Group Research, 29: 29 –57.
similarity: Types, effects, and mechanisms. Journal of Wittenbaum, G. M., Hollingshead, A. B., & Botero, I. C. 2004.
Applied Psychology, 73: 228 –234. From cooperative to motivated information sharing in
Turner, J. C. 1985. Social categorization and the self-concept: groups: Moving beyond the hidden profile paradigm.
A social cognitive theory of group behavior. Advances in Communication Monographs, 7: 59 – 83.
Group Processes: Theory and Research, 2: 77–122. Wood, W. L. 1987. Meta-analytic review of sex differences in
Wasserman, S., & Faust, K. 1994. Social network analysis: group performance. Psychological Bulletin, 102: 53–71.

David A. Harrison (dharrison@psu.edu) is Distinguished Professor of Management in


the Smeal College of Business, The Pennsylvania State University. He received his
Ph.D. in social/organizational/individual differences psychology from the University
of Illinois. His current research deals with time in organizations, team diversity, work
role adjustment, and executive decision making.

Katherine J. Klein (kleink@wharton.upenn.edu) is the Edward H. Bowman Professor of


Management at the University of Pennsylvania’s Wharton School. She received her
Ph.D. in community psychology from the University of Texas. Her current research
interests include multilevel theory and processes, diversity, social networks, team
leadership and effectiveness, and employee ownership.

You might also like