You are on page 1of 7

Psychological Medicine, 1971, I, 292 29S

Developmental dyscalculia: a brief


report on four cases
P. D. SLADE AND G. F. M. RUSSELL 1
From the Institute of Psychiatry, University of London

SYNOPSIS Four patients with 'developmental dyscalculia' are described who were initially identified
on the basis of clinical tests, reports of longstanding and severe difficulties in arithmetic, and psycho-
logical test results. Descriptive psychological testing indicated (1) that the arithmetical deficits could
not be accounted for on the basis of a generally low level of intellectual functioning; (2) that of the
four basic arithmetical processes that of multiplication was relatively more deficient; (3) that this
relative deficiency in multiplication stemmed from a faulty grasp of basic multiplication tables; that
this was a real difficulty, experienced subjectively by the patient, and one that could not be accounted
for simply by carelessness; and finally, that the degree of difficulty experienced varied with the multi-
plication table involved. Two kinds of attempt were made to improve arithmetical functioning.
The first involved brief remedial coaching programmes directed at the basic multiplication tables:
relatively little success was obtained with this method. The second kind of attempt involved methods
of bypassing the necessity for learning multiplication tables: some success was obtained with these
methods. The discussion concentrates on some of the features which may provide possibly useful cues
for identifying 'developmental dyscalculia': these are classified in terms of: (1) pattern of deficit dis-
played; (2) qualitative features of arithmetical performance; and (3) response to remedial teaching.
Some suggestions are made concerning further lines of investigation.

The term 'acalculia' was first proposed by been an unfortunate tendency in the literature to
Henschen (1919) to describe a condition in apply the term 'dyscalculia' to any observed
which there was 'a disturbance in calculating, disorder of arithmetical functioning, whether or
produced by a focal lesion of the brain'. Since not the disorder itself can be understood as
that time the term has come to be used more secondary to other cognitive defects. It is clear
loosely, as a simple descriptive label, and in that a wide range of cognitive impairments—for
many cases without implication that the con- example, dementia, memory and retention dis-
dition is necessarily acquired or that the dis- orders, aphasic disorders, agnosic and apraxic
turbance is due to a particular type of cerebral disorders, etc.—may affect the ability of a
pathology. In addition, the term itself has been patient to solve arithmetical problems, while the
superseded by that of 'dyscalculia', with the patient's ability per se may not necessarily be
implication of a disorder rather than of a affected. Grewel (1952) proposed that this type
complete absence of calculating ability. of condition be termed 'secondary' dyscalculia
Following Henschen, two major distinctions as opposed to 'primary' cases in which 'cal-
have been recognized in the use of the term culation itself is affected'. However, the dis-
'dyscalculia'—namely, (1) as a primary or a tinction is a difficult one to draw, especially in
secondary disorder, and (2) as a developmental view of the controversy concerning the associa-
or an acquired disorder. ted correlates of the disorder. Henschen (1919),
With respect to the first distinction, there has in his analyses, concluded that disturbances
of calculation produced by focal lesions were
Present address: Academic Department of Psychiatry, i ar(T pi v ; n H P npnHpnt nf nnhacin noranhia
at Royal Free Hospital School of Medicine, Friern Hospital, ^rg?1V ^dependent Ot aphasia agraphia,
NewSouthgate.LondonN.il. alexia, and other kindred disorders; while
Developmental dyscalculia: a brief report on four cases 293

Gcrstmunn (1924) described a new syndrome would account for the disability. The aim of this
consisting of linger agnosia, agraphia, right-left paper is to describe some preliminary observa-
confusion, and acalculia, demonstrating that tions made on four adolescents with severe and
these four disorders were associated with one longstanding difficulties in arithmetical calcula-
another with a significantly greater frequency tion, who may be considered examples of
than expected by chance. 'developmental dyscalculia'.
Although Henschen and Gerstmann differed
in terms of their views on the independence of
the disorder, both were concerned with dis- THE CASES
turbances of calculation which had been acquired Since there is as yet no generally acceptable,
as the result of a cerebral lesion. Many years operational definition of dyscalculia, patients
passed before Guttmann (1937) drew attention were selected for investigation if they fulfilled
to dyscalculia as a developmental disorder in three criteria—namely:
which the ability of the child to acquire calcula- 1. If they performed badly on a series of clinical
tion is itself disturbed. Since then a few papers tests involving simple calculating and money
have appeared in the literature which have been problems;
concerned with developmental cases of dys- 2. If they complained of longstanding difficul-
calculia and its relationship to other types of ties in doing arithmetic or their school reports
disorders ( Strauss and Lehtinen, 1947; Kephart, indicated this to be the case;
1960; Kinsbourne and Warrington, 1963b; 3. If they obtained scores on various psycho-
Johnson and Myklebust, 1967). So far, the logical tests which indicated severe backwardness
evidence is insufficient to arrive at any firm in arithmetical calculation, which was specific
conclusions concerning the correlates of develop- and could not be accounted for by a generally
mental dyscalculia or of what areas of the cortex low level of intellectual functioning.
may be involved, if any. In addition, there seems So far, four patients meeting these criteria
to be a relative dearth of information concerning have been studied. The age, sex, psychiatric and
the phenomenology of the condition. 'neurological' status of these patients is shown in
The term 'developmental dyscalculia' is Table 1. (The term 'neurological' is used for the
specifically reserved for cases in which dis- sake of brevity to describe disorders of higher
abilities in arithmetic are known to have been cerebral function such as impaired left-right
present from an early age; moreover there must discrimination, finger agnosia, and dyslexia.)
be no suggestion that the subject's arithmetical It can be seen from Table I that the psychiatric
abilities were formerly at a higher level and had status of each of the cases is different, while
shown deterioration; and, finally, there must be neurological correlates are present to some extent
no evidence of gross cerebral disorder such as in three cases and absent in the remaining one.

TABLE 1
AGE, SEX, PSYCHIATRIC AND NEUROLOGICAL STATUS

C.S. S.B. D.L. N.R.


Age 16 yr. 6 mth. 15 yr. 16 yr. 6 mth. 14 yr.
Sex Male Female Female Male
Psychiatric status Episodic, acute Anorexia nervosa Behaviour disorder Normal
schizophrenic reaction

Neurological status
1. Left-right discrimination Slight difficulties Slight difficulties Correct Slight difficulties
2. Finger agnosia Slight Slight Absent Slight
3. Dyslexia Absent Absent Absent Present
4. Others Myoclonic tics Nil Nil Nil
294 P. D. Slatle ami G. F. M. Russell

TABLK 2
WKCHSLKR INTKLLKiKNC'l-: SCAI.1-: IOR CIIII.DKI-.N (WISC) RI.SUI.TS

C.S. S.B. D.L. N.R. A vcraK?

Full-scale 1Q 106 96 88 117 101 -75


Verbal IQ 113 90 90 109 1005
Performance 10 97 103 89 124 103-25
Comprehension 16 10 II 12 12-25
Arithmetic 5 4 5 12 6-5
Similarities 17 II II 9 12
Vocabulary 14 8 6 16 II
Digit span 8 9 9 8 8-5
Picture completion 13 II 8 9 10-25
Picture arrangement 7 13 4 18 10 5
Block design 13 10 10 12 11-25
Object assembly 10 7 10 13 10
Coding 5 11 10 15 10-25

Results of intellectual testing were available for hypothesis, the more difficult tests of the Schonell
all four cases in the form of the WISC: these are Diagnostic Arithmetic Tests were administered,
shown in Table 2. the results of which are presented in Table 3.
Table 2 shows that two of the cases were These tests were used, despite the fact that they
slightly below average generally, while two were were standardized more than 20 years ago on
somewhat above average. Their subtest scores a possibly non-representative sample of Bir-
were generally characterized by inter-individual mingham school-children, as the primary focus
variability, although the patients did tend to of interest was their relative abilities in the
obtain lower scores on the arithmetic, and, to four basic processes, rather than their absolute
some extent, the digit span subtests. levels.
It can be seen from Table 3 that all four
patients obtained lower arithmetic ages on the
PSYCHOLOGICAL FINDINGS
multiplication test compared with the tests of
1. ATTAINMENT IN THE BASIC ARITHMETICAL addition and subtraction. Surprisingly, how-
PROCESSES ever, only one of the four patients obtained an
As a result of clinical observations it was hypo- arithmetic age as low or lower on the test of
thesized that the area of maximum difficulty for division. On the basis of these results it was
these patients is in the processes of multiplica- tentatively concluded that the area of maximal
tion and division, in contrast with the processes defect in these four cases is in the basic process
of addition and subtraction. In order to test this of multiplication.

TABLE 3
RESULTS ON THE SCHONELL DIAGNOSTIC ARITHMETIC TESTS! (ARITHMETIC AGES)

C.S S.B. D.L. N.R. A verage


Arithmetic process (yr.) (mth.) (yr.) (mlh.) (yr.) (mth.) (yr.) (mth.) (yr.) (mth.)

Graded
Addition 10 5 10 3 10 6 11 6 10 8
Subtraction 9 11 9 7 8 9 10 9 9 9
Multiplication 8 11 8 8 7 II 9 8 8 9-5
Division 10 1 9 8 8 8 9 0 9 4-25
Average 9 10 9 6-5 8 11-5 103
Developmental dyscalciilia: a brief report on jour cases 295
2. KNOWI.I'IKii; AND GRASI' <)!•' HASIC MUI.TI- compared with problems to which they obtained
1'I.ICAIION TARI.I-S correct answers and they all showed a strong
On the basis of further clinical observation it was relationship between their objective performance
hypothesized that the defect in the process of and their subjective confidence ratings. If their
multiplication may be due to a faulty or non- errors were simply due to carelessness, one
existent grasp of the basic multiplication tables. would not expect to find any difference in terms
Two subsidiary hypotheses were derived— of time for the two types of problems nor any
namely: systematic relationship with subjective confidence
1. That errors on the multiplication tables are level.
due to a real, experienced difficulty on the part 3. With respect to the second subsidiary
of the patient and not simply the result of care- hypothesis, all three patients obtained their
lessness, and highest score on the 5 x table and their lowest
2. That the degree of difficulty experienced will scores on the 8 x and 9 x tables.
vary with the particular multiplication table On the basis of these results, it was therefore
involved. tentatively concluded that the basic difficulty in
In order to test these three hypotheses a the process of multiplication stems from a faulty
Multiplication Tables Test was developed. This grasp of basic multiplication tables, that this is a
consisted of a set of 36 multiplication problems real difficulty, experienced subjectively by the
involving every possible combination of the patient, and one that cannot be accounted for
numbers 4 to 9—that is, 4 x 4 , 4 x 5 - > - 4 x 9 - » simply by carelessness, and that the degree of
9 x 4 , 9 x 5 -> 9 x 9 . Each problem was typed difficulty experienced varies with the multiplica-
out on a separate index card. Problems were tion table involved.
presented one at a time in random order. In
addition, each problem was timed individually
and the patient asked to rate his/her subjective
3 . ATTEMPTS AT IMPROVING ARITHMETICAL
confidence in the solution, after each problem,
FUNCTIONING
on a simple five-point scale—that is, very
confident, fairly confident, doubtful, fairly a. COACHING IN THE BASIC MULTIPLICATION
unconfident, very unconfident. Four measures TABLES
were derived from this test: Two methods of achieving this have so far been
i. Number of problems correct and number of attempted with individual patients. The first
problems incorrect. method involved a relatively uncontrolled
ii. Total mean time, mean time for problems learning situation with feedback, while the
correct, and mean time for problems incorrect. second was based on controlled learning trials.
iii. The relationship between objective score
and subjective confidence.
iv. Number correct on each of the six mul- 1. UNCONTROLLED LEARNING SITUATION WITH
tiplication tables—that is, 4 x , 5 x , 6 x , 7 x , FEEDBACK D.L., whose main difficulties lay
8 x and 9 x. with the 6 x, 7 x, 8 x, and 9 x tables, was
This test was administered to three of the four presented with a set of these tables and encour-
patients (S.B., D.L., and N.R.); the results aged to practise them whenever she had the time
showed that: (plenty of time was available to her to do this).
1. All three patients produced incorrect She was then retested on the Multiplication
answers to approximately one third to one half Tables Test four days later, and her results dis-
of the simple multiplication problems, thereby cussed with her. She was then encouraged to
providing some confirmation for the major practise them for another two days and then
hypothesis of a faulty grasp of basic multiplica- retested again. Her results showed very little
tion tables. improvement in terms of number of problems
2. With respect to the first subsidiary hypo- correct, a large improvement in terms of time
thesis all three patients spent approximately taken and no change in terms of subjective
twice as long on problems which they failed confidence.
2% P. D. Slade and G. F. M. Russell

2 . CONTROIXHI) I.HARNING TRIALS S . B . , w h o WilS TABLE4


found to have difficulties with the 4 x, 6 x, 7 x, KliSUl.TS ON THIv SCTIONKLI. MULTIPLICATION AND DIVISION
8 x , and 9 x tables, was given five learning TESTS, WITH AND WITHOUT MULTIPLICATION TABLES
sessions, on five consecutive days, each session
being devoted to one particular table. The D.L. N.R.
procedure employed was based on the normal (yr.) (mill.) (yr.) (mlh.)
paired-associate learning technique. S.B. was
Multiplication
first shown a set of 12 cards on which both Without 1 1! 9 8
problems (stimuli) and answers (responses) were With 9 2 9 II
presented—for example, 4 x 8 =32. She was then Division
shown 12 cards on which the problems (stimuli) Without 8 8 9 0
alone were presented and was asked to supply With 9 8 II 0
the answer. Learning and test trials were repeated
alternately in this manner until S.B. had pro-
duced five consecutive correct answers to each tion tables which they could use in answering
problem. the problems. The results of this procedure,
It was decided to standardize the format, which are presented in Table 4, indicate that
procedure and criteria in this way as, with the both patients improved their performance
usual form of remedial teaching, it is often when they were allowed the use of a set of
difficult to know afterwards what and how much multiplication tables.
has been learned and to what degree. With the
technique adopted here it is possible to state 2. COACHING IN TRACHTENBERG SYSTEM C.S.
that each problem has been learnt to at least a was given weekly coaching in the Trachtenberg
criterion of five consecutive correct responses. System (Cutler and McShane, 1962) by a
Four days, and again one month, after this qualified teacher accustomed to the coaching of
brief remedial coaching programme had been psychiatric patients. In this system, multiplica-
completed, the Multiplication Tables Test was tion is carried out by means of a complex
repeated. The results indicated very little sequence of additions, while division is similarly
improvement in terms of 'no correct' score, mediated by a complex sequence of subtractions.
although, again, there was a large improvement During the first 11 months of coaching most of
in terms of 'time taken'. the time has been spent on mastering the pro-
cedure involved in multiplication. After four
b. ATTEMPTS AT AVOIDING NECESSITY OF LEARN- months coaching C.S. exhibited an increase of 12
ING BASIC MULTIPLICATION TABLES months in arithmetic age on the Schonell graded
Two methods of achieving this have so far been multiplication test over the previous baseline,
attempted with individual patients. The first while after 11 months the increase was 15
method was simply to provide patients with a set months. Thus, there was some evidence that
of multiplication tables and see whether their coaching in the Trachtenberg System led to an
ability at multiplication and division problems improvement in multiplication ability in the
was consequently improved. The second method case of C.S., although the increases observed
involved an attempt to teach a patient the were not as large as had been hoped for.
Trachtenberg System of calculating, in which all
the basic arithmetical processes are mediated by
the processes of addition and subtraction. CLINICAL OBSERVATIONS
Clinically, a number of typical features in the
1. USE OF MULTIPLICATION TABLES IN MULTIPLICA- procedures adopted by these patients for tackling
TION AND DIVISION The graded multiplication arithmetical problems were identified and these
and graded division tests of the Schonell Diag- will now be outlined briefly. Firstly, the use of a
nostic Arithmetic Tests were readministered to 'dotting' system was noted quite commonly. This
D.L. and N.R. On this occasion, however, they system was used by patients for all four basic
were provided with a complete set of multiplica- processes but most particularly for multiplica-
Developmental dyscalcnlia: a brief report on four cases 297

tion. An extreme example of this was shown by limited scope of inquiry and the small number of
one patient, D.L., who solved the problem 9 x 7 cases involved, do not permit this gap to be
by making nine groups of seven dots and then filled. However, they do suggest a tentative set of
proceeding to count up all the dots. criteria for identifying cases of developmental
A second feature which was noted typically dyscalculia. Three kinds of criteria seem relevant
was the tendency to break down multiplication in this respect—namely, (1) pattern of deficit
problems into a series of apparently simpler displayed; (2) qualitative features of arithmetical
stages. For example, one patient tackled the performance; and (3) response to remedial
problem 8 x 7 by first solving 2 x 7 = 14 and teaching.
then adding 14 four times to obtain 56. When a The pattern of deficit displayed by the four
patient adopts this particular strategy it was patients studied has been elaborated at some
noted that he often becomes muddled and has length. The psychological findings suggest that
difficulty in aligning the figures appropriately in one of the areas of maximal deficit may reside
a column so that failure is a common result. in the process of multiplication and that this may
A third feature which was observed was that stem from a faulty grasp of the basic multiplica-
of gradual approximation. An example of this tion tables, especially the 8 x and 9 x tables.
was provided by one patient who attempted to However, it is clear that the deficit in multiplica-
solve the problem 102-^-6 by first multiplying tion is only relatively more severe, and that the
8 x 6 (breaking this down into stages as des- patients exhibit varying degrees of difficulty with
cribed above) to obtain 48 and then adding 48 all four basic processes.
to itself to arrive at an approximation to 102. The qualitative features of performance were
The three features described above can all be described under the heading of clinical observa-
seen as adaptive manoeuvres on the part of the tions. The major features noted were the use of
patients to deal with problems, which, for them the dotting system, the tendency to break down
are too difficult. However, because they generally multiplication problems into simpler stages, the
involve an increase in the number of stages use of a procedure of gradual approximation,
required they often lead to more incorrect than the tendency to reverse two digit numbers when
correct solutions. Certain other features were a carrying operation is required, and, finally, the
noted which reflect a more basic difficulty on the tendency to exhibit performance variability. All
part of the patient. One such feature is the these features were displayed by the four
tendency to reverse two-digit numbers when a patients to some extent and would seem to pro-
carrying operation is required. For example, one vide useful indices for purposes of identification.
patient, faced with the problem 17x5, multiplied The third criterion, and perhaps the most
5 x 7 correctly to obtain 35, but then proceeded important, is the apparent intractability of the
to fill in 3 in the digits column and carry forward deficits and the poor response to various attempts
5 to the 10s column. Another common feature at treatment. In addition to the minor attempts
was the variability in performance demonstrated
at remedial coaching described here, three of
by all the patients. This affected all four basic
the patients (C.S., S.B., and N.R.) had under-
processes, particularly multiplication, and both
gone prolonged and intensive remedial pro-
easy and difficult problems. For example, even
the very simple problem, 8 + 1, was on occasions grammes in arithmetic with relatively limited
solved incorrectly by the patients who normally benefit.
had no difficulty with this. In addition, it has been emphasized by earlier
writers that dyscalculia may be associated with
defects of higher cortical functioning, such as
failure of left-right discrimination, finger ag-
DISCUSSION
nosia, and dyslexia. Kinsbourne and Warring-
The point which was made at the beginning of ton (1963b) reported a series of cases of'develop-
this paper was that, as yet, there is no readily mental Gerstmann's syndrome' in children, in
available, adequate operational definition of whom this was so. But, their cases cannot be
developmental dyscalculia. The findings des- considered 'developmental' in the sense of the
cribed in this report, because of their somewhat definition used here, as they were all found to
298 /'. D. Slaclc ami G. /•. M. Russell

have detectable cerebral pathology of one kind or would seem to merit further attention. The first
another. In their papers (1963 a, b) Kinsbourne one can be expressed in terms of an hypothesis of
and Warrington hypothesize that the basic deficit a simple deficit in learning the association bet-
underlying the Gerstmann syndrome may be ween numbers or number combinations, while
one of a failure in sequential ordering: consistent the second involves the hypothesis that the
with such an explanation was the tendency present deficit is due to faulty past learning
observed in at least one of our patients to reverse which competes with and interferes with any
two-digit numbers and to have difficulty aligning attempt to establish correct learning. From the
figures correctly in columns. However, a point of view of trying to develop an effective
'sequential ordering' deficit cannot seemingly programme of remedial education, it would be
account for all the difficulties we observed. important to distinguish between these two
Hecaen, Angelergues, and Houiller (1961) alternatives.
studied a series of 183 patients with acquired
arithmetical disorders. On the basis of their
clinical and statistical findings they divided their REFERENCES
cases into three separate categories—namely, Cutler, A., and McShane, R. (1962). The Traclitenberg Speed
(1) dyscalculia of spatial-deficit origin; (2) System of Basic Mathematics. Pan Books: London.
Gerstmann, J. (1924). Fingeragnosie: einc umschriebenc
acalculia related to specific disabilities in reading Stoning der Orienterung am eigener Kb'rper. Wiener
and writing numbers, and (3) pure dyscalculia klinische Wochenschrift,31, 1010-1012.
Grewel, F. (1952). Acalculia. Brain, 75, 397-407.
(anarithmetie) in which the basic arithmetical Guttmann, E. (1937). Congenital arithmetic disability and
operations are themselves impaired. None of our acalculia (Henschen). British Journal of Medical Psychology,
patients showed the type of disabilities indicated 16, 16-35.
Hecaen, H., Angelergues, R., and Houiller, S. (1961). Les
by the second category of Hecaen et al.; they were varietes cliniques des acalculies au cours des lesions
all able to read and write numbers with ease. If retrorolandiques: approche statistique du probleme.
anything, they would seem to represent a mixture Revue Neurologique, 105, 85-103.
Henschen, S. E. (1919). Uber Sprach- Musik- und Rechen-
of the disabilities described by their first and mechanismen und ihre Legalisation im Gehirn. Zeitschrifi
third categories. But it is not clear as yet to what fiir die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 52, 273-298.
extent cases of acquired and cases of develop- Johnson, D. J., and Myklebust, H. R. (1967). Learning
Disabilities. Grune and Stratton : New York.
mental dyscalculia can be compared in terms of Kephart, N. (I960). The Slow Learner in the Classroom.
their phenomenological aspects. Merill: Columbus.
Kinsbourne, M.,and Warrington, E. K. (1963a). The develop-
Rutter, Yule, Tizard, and Graham (1966) have ment of finger differentiation. Quarterly Journal of Experi-
also reported a statistical association between mental Psychology, 15, 132-137.
severe reading and arithmetical retardation in Kinsbourne, M., and Warrington, E. K. (1963b). The
developmental Gerstmann's syndrome. Archives of
normal children. Further cues for the identifica- Neurology, 8,490-501.
tion of developmental dyscalculia and its under- Rutter, M., Yule, W., Tizard, J., and Graham, P. (1966).
lying mechanisms may therefore be forthcoming Severe reading retardation: its relationship to maladjust-
ment, epilepsy and neurological disorders. In What is
from a more intensive investigation- of its Special Education? Proc. First International Conference,
correlates. Association for Special Education, London, 1966.
Strauss, A. A., and Lehtinen, L. E. (1947). Psychopathology
To return to the findings of a faulty grasp of and Education of the Brain-Injured Child. Grune and
multiplication tables, two possible explanations Stratton: New York.

You might also like