You are on page 1of 15

NeuroImage 16, 61–75 (2002)

doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1046, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

A Developmental fMRI Study of the Stroop Color-Word Task


Nancy E. Adleman,* Vinod Menon,* ,† ,‡ Christine M. Blasey,* Christopher D. White,*
Ilana S. Warsofsky,* Gary H. Glover,§ and Allan L. Reiss* ,†
*Departments of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, †Program in Neuroscience, ‡Stanford Brain Research Center,
and §Radiology, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California 94305

Received April 30, 2001

(MacLeod, 1991). In this task, the subject is instructed


We used fMRI to investigate developmental changes to name the color of the ink of an incongruent word-
in brain activation during a Stroop color-word inter- color stimulus (i.e., RED printed in green ink). Because
ference task. A positive correlation was observed be- word reading is a more automatic cognitive process
tween age and Stroop-related activation (n ⴝ 30) in the than color naming, subjects must resolve cognitive in-
left lateral prefrontal cortex, the left anterior cingu- terference and inhibit the incorrect, but more facile,
late, and the left parietal and parieto-occipital corti-
response in order to properly respond. This response
ces. No regions showed a negative correlation between
competition commonly results in an increase in re-
activation and age. We further investigated age-re-
lated differences by stratifying the sample into three
sponse time (RT) on incongruent trials as compared to
age groups: children (ages 7–11), adolescents (ages 12– congruent word-color pairs (i.e., RED printed in red
16), and young adults (ages 18 –22). Young adult sub- ink) or neutral pairs (i.e., DOG printed in red ink, or
jects (n ⴝ 11) displayed significant activation in the XXX printed in red ink).
inferior and middle frontal gyri bilaterally, the left The cognitive processes underlying the Stroop task
anterior cingulate, and bilateral inferior and superior (i.e., response inhibition, interference resolution, and
parietal lobules. Between-group comparisons re- behavioral conflict resolution) are considered executive
vealed that young adults had significantly greater ac- processes mediated by the frontal lobe. Patients with
tivation than adolescent subjects (n ⴝ 11) in the left lateral prefrontal lobe lesions commit more errors on
middle frontal gyrus and that young adults showed the Stroop task than do normal controls (Vendrell et
significantly greater activation than children (n ⴝ 8) al., 1995). Perret (1974) proposed a left hemisphere
in the anterior cingulate and left parietal and parieto- frontal location for interference resolution in the
occipital regions, as well as in the left middle frontal Stroop task after studying patients with frontal lobe
gyrus. Compared to children, both adult and adoles- lesions. Because of its critical dependence on frontal
cent subjects exhibited significantly greater activa- lobe function, the Stroop task is an ideal tool for study-
tion in the parietal cortex. Adult and adolescent
ing typical and atypical development of executive pro-
groups, however, did not differ in activation for this
cesses. Several studies have utilized the Stroop task
region. Together, these data suggest that Stroop task-
related functional development of the parietal lobe
(Dash and Dash, 1982) and modified Stroop tasks (Di-
occurs by adolescence. In contrast, prefrontal cortex amond and Taylor, 1996; Gerstadt et al., 1994) to ex-
function contributing to the Stroop interference task plore the maturation of executive processes, and have
continues to develop into adulthood. This neuromatu- found that such processes develop with age.
rational process may depend on increased ability to In conjunction with the behavioral development of
recruit focal neural resources with age. Findings from executive function, researchers have recently provided
this study, the first developmental fMRI investigation evidence for postadolescent maturation of the frontal
of the Stroop interference task, provide a template lobes (Sowell et al., 1999). Giedd et al. (1999) found that
with which normal development and neurodevelop- frontal gray matter increases during preadolescence,
mental disorders of prefrontal cortex function can be peaks during adolescence, and decreases during post-
assessed. © 2002 Elsevier Science (USA) adolescence. Although there has been evidence of
structural frontal lobe maturation as well as behav-
ioral maturation of executive processes during child-
INTRODUCTION hood, no functional imaging studies to date have used
the Stroop task to investigate the relationship between
Since its introduction in 1935, the Stroop Color-Word frontal lobe function and cognition from a developmen-
task has been a classic measure of frontal lobe function tal standpoint.

61 1053-8119/02 $35.00
© 2002 Elsevier Science (USA)
All rights reserved.
62 ADLEMAN ET AL.

Although there have been no developmental imaging MATERIALS AND METHODS


studies of the Stroop task to date, data from several
Stroop task imaging studies have been reported in Subjects
adults (Bench et al., 1993; Carter et al., 1995; Larrue et Subjects were recruited from the greater San Fran-
al., 1994; Pardo et al., 1990; Peterson et al., 1999; cisco Bay area. Two exclusion criteria were applied
Taylor et al., 1997). However, considerable variability when screening for potential participants: (1) evidence
exists in the brain regions observed to be activated by of identifiable cognitive impairment or (2) the presence
the Stroop task. For example, utilizing a similar non- of a behavioral/psychological disorder. Cognitive func-
word control task as that used in the present study, tioning was assessed using the WISC-III (ages 7
Bench et al. (1993) reported Stroop task-related acti- through 16) or WAIS-III (ages 16 through 22). Persons
vation in right orbitofrontal cortex, right frontal polar who scored lower than 80 for full scale IQ were ex-
cortex, and right anterior cingulate. Taylor et al. cluded from further participation. The presence of be-
(1997), also employing a nonlexical control task, found havioral/psychological impairments was assessed us-
activation in the right middle frontal gyrus (MFG), left ing either the Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R)
inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), left parietal region, and (ages 13 through 22) (Derogatis and Savitz, 1999) or
left insula; no activation was observed in the anterior the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL) (ages 7 through
cingulate cortex (ACC). In summary, most investiga- 18) (Achenbach, 1991). Subjects with significant eleva-
tions of the Stroop task have found activation in at tions (1 SD ⬎ population mean) on these instruments
least one of three main regions: the ACC (Carter et al., were excluded from the study.
1995; Larrue et al., 1994; Pardo et al., 1990; Peterson et Thirty healthy, right-handed, biologically indepen-
al., 1999), parietal lobe (Bench et al., 1993; Carter et dent subjects met these selection criteria and partici-
al., 1995; Peterson et al., 1999; Taylor et al., 1997), and pated in the Stroop task fMRI study after giving writ-
lateral prefrontal cortex (Bench et al., 1993; Carter et ten informed consent. The total sample included 10
al., 1995; Pardo et al., 1990; Taylor et al., 1997). The males and 20 females ranging in age from 7 to 22 years
goal of the present study was to employ the classic (mean age: 15.42, SD ⫽ 4.24). Because of behavioral
literature that suggests that by the age of seven chil-
Stroop interference task with functional magnetic res-
dren have developed their reading skills to the point
onance imaging (fMRI) to investigate neuromatura-
that they experience semantic interference (Rosinski,
tional processes underlying cognitive development in
1977; Rosinski et al., 1975) and that children under the
individuals ranging in age from early childhood to age of seven do not show interference effects (Comalli
young adulthood. In accord with past Stroop task neu- et al., 1962), we confined our subject pool to children
roimaging research, we hypothesized that develop- aged seven and older.
ment of executive processes involved in the Stroop Subjects were divided into three age groups with
interference process would be localized to these three similar male to female proportions. The group of chil-
regions. dren consisted of 3 males and 5 females (n ⫽ 8) ranging
In contrast to most functional imaging studies of in age from 7.66 to 11.96 years (mean ⫽ 10.16, SD ⫽
cognitive development, which typically employ only 1.66). The adolescent group included 4 males and 7
two groups of subjects, children and adults (e.g., females (n ⫽ 11) ranging from 12.58 to 16.85 years of
Thomas et al. (1999)), the present study utilized three age (mean ⫽ 14.68, SD ⫽ 1.27). The young adult group
developmental age groups (children, adolescents, and consisted of 3 male and 8 female subjects (n ⫽ 11),
young adults), thus permitting a more fine-grained ranging in age from 17.39 to 22.68 years (mean ⫽
investigation of the trajectory of cognitive developmen- 19.98, SD ⫽ 1.72).
tal changes. In particular, the inclusion of an adoles-
cent group is a potentially significant addition as it is Neuropsychological Stroop Test
well known that the onset of puberty affects both de- Because of head movement that would result from
velopment (Giedd et al., 1999) and behavior (Buchanan fully vocalizing responses within the scanner, we used
et al., 1992). behavioral data acquired outside the scanner to inves-
This study is a component of our laboratory’s ongoing tigate age-related changes in performance of the
efforts to provide an initial template of neurofunctional Stroop task. In the present study, a standard Stroop
maturation underlying key components of cognitive Color-Word task (Golden, 1978) was administered to
development during childhood. Broadening our knowl- every subject. The Stroop test includes 3 time-limited
edge base pertaining to the trajectory and variability of (45-s) subtests. First, subjects are asked to read a list of
typical brain development in children will eventually words printed in black ink that name colors (i.e., “red,”
permit a more precise interpretation of individual “green,” “blue”). Second, subjects are presented with a
brain development and function in children with atyp- list of “XXXXX”s that differ in ink color. Subjects name
ical cognitive development. the color of the ink for each “XXXXX.” In the third
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 63

TABLE 1
Results of Multiple Regression Analysis Examining the Relationship between FSIQ, Age, and Stroop Subscores

Zero-order Partial Part Beta


Dependent variable Predictor correlation correlation correlation R 2 delta Beta P value

Word raw score FSIQ 0.191 ⫺0.092 ⫺0.067 0.037 ⫺0.072 0.635
AGE 0.685 0.674 0.661 0.437 0.712 ⬍0.001
Color raw score FSIQ 0.340 0.059 0.032 0.116 0.035 0.761
AGE 0.837 0.814 0.765 0.585 0.824 ⬍0.001
Color–word raw score FSIQ 0.496 0.357 0.221 0.246 0.238 0.057
AGE 0.786 0.747 0.649 0.421 0.698 ⬍0.001
Interference raw score FSIQ 0.514 0.434 0.401 0.246 0.432 0.019
AGE 0.382 0.240 0.206 0.043 0.222 0.209

subtest, subjects are then presented with a list of the 30-s rest epoch, another 3 cycles of control and exper-
words “red,” “green,” and “blue;” in this presentation, imental epochs, and a final 30-s rest epoch. In each
however, the ink color of the words is discordant with epoch, there were 15 stimuli, each presented for 1350
the presented word (i.e., the word “red” printed in blue ms, with a total interstimulus interval (ISI) of 2000 ms.
ink). Again, subjects are asked to say the color of the In order to highlight the complete network of brain
ink and ignore the word’s semantic meaning (i.e., re- regions involved in Stroop task-related cognitive com-
spond “blue” instead of “red” in the previous example). ponents and their development, we employed a low-
level nonlinguistic baseline as the control condition
fMRI Stroop Task that only controlled for the process of color naming.
The experiment presented in the scanner began with The control stimuli consisted of a string of 3, 4, or 5
a 30-s rest epoch followed by 3 cycles of alternating colored “X”s, presented in red, blue, or green. The sub-
30-s epochs of experimental trials and control trials, a jects were asked to subvocalize the color of the “X”s

TABLE 2
Brain Areas That Showed Significant Activation in (1) Linear Correlation between Age and Activation; (2) Young Adult
Group Average; (3) Adult versus Child Comparison; (4) Adult versus Adolescent Comparison; (5) Adolescent versus Child
Comparison

No. of Cluster Z max Primary peak


Activated Region voxels P (cor) primary peak location

Linear correlation between age and activation


Left anterior cingulate cortex and lateral prefrontal regions 6109 ⬍0.001 4.57 ⫺4, 14, 28 L ACC (BA 24/32)
(SFG/MFG)
Left inferior parietal lobe, parieto-occipital regions, cuneus, 1700 ⬍0.001 3.67 ⫺26, ⫺62, 16 L PCC/cuneus
lingual gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex (BA31/7)
Young adult group average
Bilateral MFG, IFG, and anterior cingulate cortex 12030 ⬍0.001 4.37 ⫺34, 22, 34 L MFG (BA 8)
Right inferior parietal lobe, supramarginal gyrus, 1681 0.001 4.25 60, ⫺46, 28 R
superior/medial occipital gyrus IPL/supramarginal gyrus
(BA 40)
Left inferior and superior parietal lobe 3474 ⬍0.001 4.03 ⫺28, ⫺68, 52 L SPL (BA 7)
Adult–child
Anterior cingulate cortex 827 0.049 4.12 ⫺6, 26, 16 L ACC (BA 24)
Left MFG and SFG 2511 ⬍0.001 3.92 ⫺28, 14, 50 L MFG (BA 6)
Left parieto-occipital regions: lingual gyrus, precuneus, cuneus 937 0.025 3.58 ⫺14, ⫺66, ⫺2 L lingual gyrus
(BA 18/19)
Adult–adolescent
Left MFG, ACC, SFG 1376 0.002 3.81 ⫺28, 12, 52 L MFG (BA 6)
Adolescent–child
Left cuneus, posterior cingulate gyrus, precuneus 1858 ⬍0.001 4.11 ⫺14, ⫺74, 22 L cuneus (BA
17/18)

Note. For each cluster, the activated region(s), number of voxels activated, corrected P value, maximum Z score, and location of peak
activation are shown. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; PCC,
posterior cingulate cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus.
64 ADLEMAN ET AL.

FIG. 1. Brain regions that showed significant (P ⬍ 0.05) Stroop task-related activation in the young adult group include the bilateral
frontal regions and bilateral parietal and parieto-occipital regions. Activated regions include the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus,
inferior parietal lobe, superior parietal lobe, and the superior/medial occipital gyrus. fMRI activation was superposed on the average of the
11 young adult subjects’ individual T1-weighted images normalized to Talairach space.

quietly to themselves. The experimental stimuli were until they showed and expressed both understanding
incongruent color-word pairs, in which the print of the and compliance.
ink did not match the color specified by the word (i.e.,
the word “red” printed in green). The subjects were fMRI Acquisition
asked to identify, and say quietly to themselves, the
color of the ink in which the word was printed, and not Images were acquired on a 1.5T GE Signa scanner
to read the word itself (in our example, to say “green”). with Echospeed gradients using a custom-built whole
All subjects were trained on the task before scanning head coil that provides a 50% advantage in signal to
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 65

FIG. 2. Brain areas in which Stroop task-related activation was significantly (P ⬍ 0.05) correlated with age. (A) fMRI activation was
superposed on the average of 29 individual T1-weighted images normalized to Talairach space. (B) Surface rendering of fMRI activation on
a T1-weighted Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template image.
66 ADLEMAN ET AL.

noise ratio over that of the standard GE coil (Hayes mance on the third task (based on performance on the
and Mathias, 1996). A custom-built head holder was first two tasks) minus the actual performance. Raw
used to prevent head movement. Eighteen axial slices interference was calculated using the same equation
(6 mm thick, 1 mm skip) parallel to the anterior and the Stroop neuropsychological test provides for inter-
posterior commissure covering the whole brain were ference calculation. 1 For our experimental purposes,
imaged with a temporal resolution of 2 s using a T2*- this calculation of an interference score was derived
weighted gradient echo spiral pulse sequence (TR ⫽ from the three subtest raw scores rather than from the
2000 ms, TE ⫽ 40 ms, flip angle ⫽ 89° and 1 interleave) age-corrected scores provided in the Stroop manual
(Glover and Lai, 1998). The field of view was 240 mm (which are described as “experimental” in the manual)
and the effective inplane spatial resolution was 3.75 (Golden, 1978). The benefits of using raw scores in
mm. To aid in localization of functional data, a high cross-sectional studies of development have been dis-
resolution T1-weighted spoiled grass gradient recalled cussed (Kraemer et al., 2000) and have been used in-
(SPGR) 3-D MRI sequence with the following parame- stead of age-standardized scores in similar investiga-
ters was used: TR ⫽ 24 ms; TE ⫽ 5 ms; flip angle ⫽ 40°; tions (Bettner et al., 1971).
124 slices in coronal plane; 256 ⫻ 192 matrix; acquired Multiple regression analyses were performed to as-
resolution ⫽ 1.5 ⫻ 0.9 ⫻ 1.2 mm. The images were sess the predictive value of age and full-scale IQ
reconstructed as a 124 ⫻ 256 ⫻ 256 matrix with a 1.5 ⫻ (FSIQ), as measured by the WISC-III or WAIS-III, on
0.9 ⫻ 0.9 mm spatial resolution. Structural image av- the Stroop performance measures.
erages were made for the entire subject pool as well as fMRI data analysis. Statistical analysis was per-
for each age group. Because of excessive movement formed on individual and group data using the general
during one structural scan (an individual in the child linear model and the theory of Gaussian random fields
group), only 29 subjects were included in the average as implemented in SPM99b (Friston et al., 1995). This
structural image. method corrects for temporal and spatial autocorrela-
The task was programmed using Psyscope (Cohen et tions in the fMRI data (Worsley and Friston, 1995).
al., 1993) on a Macintosh (Sunnyvale, CA) notebook In the first step of analysis, a within-subject proce-
computer. Initiation of scan and task was synchronized dure was used to model all the effects of interest. The
using a TTL pulse delivered to the scanner timing individual subject models were identical across sub-
microprocessor board from a “CMU Button Box” micro- jects (i.e., a balanced design was used). Confounding
processor (http://poppy.psy.cmu.edu/psyscope) con- effects of fluctuations in global mean were removed by
nected to the Macintosh. Stimuli were presented visu- proportional scaling where, for each time point, each
ally at the center of a screen using a custom-built voxel was scaled by the global mean at that time point.
magnet compatible projection system (Resonance Low frequency noise was removed with a high pass
Technology, CA). filter (0.5 cycles/min) applied to the fMRI time series at
each voxel. A temporal smoothing function (Gaussian
Image Preprocessing kernel corresponding to dispersion of 8 s) was applied
Images were reconstructed, by inverse Fourier to the fMRI time series to enhance the temporal signal
transform, for each of the 225 time points into 64 ⫻ to noise ratio. The effects of interest for each subject
64 ⫻ 18 image matrices (voxel size: 3.75 ⫻ 3.75 ⫻ 7 were then defined with the relevant contrasts of the
mm). fMRI data were preprocessed using SPM99b parameter estimates. For each of these contrasts, a
(http://www.fil.ion.bpmf.ac.uk/spm). Images were cor- corresponding contrast image was made. Voxel-wise t
rected for movement using least square minimization statistics were normalized to Z scores to provide a
without higher-order corrections for spin history and statistical measure of activation independent of sam-
normalized to MNI coordinates. Images were then re- ple size. Finally, in order to determine the presence of
sampled every 2 mm using sinc interpolation and significant clusters of activation, the joint expected
smoothed with a 4-mm Gaussian kernel to decrease probability distribution of height and extent of Z scores
spatial noise. MNI coordinates were transformed to (Poline et al., 1997), with height (Z ⬎ 1.67; P ⬍ 0.05)
Talairach (Tal) coordinates using a nonlinear transfor- and extent threshold (P ⬍ 0.05), was used in order to
mation (Brett, 2000, http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/ correct for spatial correlations in the data.
Imaging/mnispace.html). Activation analysis. Group analysis was performed
using a random-effects model. The random-effects
Statistical Analysis model estimates the error variance for each condition
of interest across subjects, rather than across scans
Behavioral data. Raw scores on the Stroop task
were calculated by determining the number of items
completed within the 45-s time interval on each of the 1
The equations employed were as follows: (W ⴱ C)/(W ⫹ C) ⫽ CW⬘;
three subtests (color, word, color–word). An interfer- CW ⫺ CW⬘ ⫽ RAWINTF. W, raw word score; C, raw color score; CW,
ence score was derived from the prediction of perfor- raw color-word score.
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 67

(Holmes and Friston, 1998) and therefore provides a P ⬍ 0.001; color raw: beta ⫽ 0.824, P ⬍ 0.001; color–
stronger generalization to the population. This analy- word raw: beta ⫽ 0.698, P ⬍ 0.001). However, age was
sis proceeded in two steps. In the first step, contrast not significantly correlated with raw interference score
images for each subject and each effect of interest were (P ⫽ 0.209). In contrast, FSIQ did not account for a
generated as described above. In the second step, these significant proportion of the variance in word raw (P ⫽
contrast images were used to examine the correlation 0.635), color raw (P ⫽ 0.761), or color–word raw scores
between age and brain activation and between-group (P ⫽ 0.057), but was a significant predictor of raw
activation differences as described below. We initially interference score (P ⫽ 0.019).
analyzed data from young adult subjects to determine
the consistency of our findings with those previously Brain Activation
reported for adult samples.
Young adult group activation. Young adults
Correlation between age and activation. Linear re-
showed significant Stroop task-related activation bilat-
gression was used to determine voxels that showed
erally in the IFG, the MFG and the anterior cingulate
either positive or negative age-related changes during
(Z max ⫽ 4.37, P ⬍ 0.001), bilateral inferior parietal
the Stroop interference task (interference vs color nam-
lobe/supramarginal gyrus (R side: Z max ⫽ 4.25, P ⫽
ing condition contrast) across all subjects. Subject age
0.001; L side: Z max ⫽ 4.03, P ⬍ 0.001), and the left
was used as a covariate of interest and FSIQ was used
superior parietal lobe (Z max ⫽ 4.03, P ⬍ 0.001; for
as a nuisance covariate due to unexpected group dif-
summary of all activation see Table 2 and Fig. 1).
ferences for this measure. Voxel-wise t statistics were
Activation was also found to extend into the caudate
computed for the covariate of interest. The t statistics
(Tal x, y, z ⫽ 16, 10, 6 mm in Fig. 1).
were then normalized to Z scores. Significant clusters
of activation were determined using the joint expected Correlation between age and brain activation. After
probability distribution of height and extent of Z covarying for FSIQ, two clusters of brain activation
scores, with height (Z ⬎ 1.67; P ⬍ 0.05) and extent were significantly correlated with age (Table 2; Fig. 2).
threshold (P ⬍ 0.05). The first cluster spanned the left anterior cingulate
gyrus (BA 24/32) and prefrontal regions including the
Group differences in Stroop task-related activation.
left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) (BA 6) and bilateral
In order to further delineate the trajectory of develop-
MFG (BA 9) (Z max ⫽ 4.57, P ⬍ 0.001). Activation from
mental changes, Stroop task-related activation differ-
this cluster also was found to extend into bilateral
ences between the three groups (children, adolescents,
caudate/putamen (Tal x, y, z ⫽ 16, 10, 6 mm in Fig. 2A).
and young adults) were compared. Individual contrast
The second cluster spanned the left posterior cingulate
images reflecting task-related activation were first cre-
gyrus and cuneus (BA 31/7), the left lingual gyrus (BA
ated for each subject. Groups were compared using
18/19), and the left inferior parietal lobe (BA 40) (Z
two-sample, two-tailed, t tests after covarying out dif-
max ⫽ 3.67, P ⬍ 0.001).
ferences in full scale IQ. The following group compar-
In order to further explore the relationship between
isons were made: (1) young adult versus child; (2)
age and focal activation in these two regions, a corre-
young adult versus adolescent; and (3) adolescent ver-
lational analysis (n ⫽ 30) was run between age and
sus child.
activation level (arbitrary measure) in the foci maxima
of the two regions. The foci maxima were located in (1)
RESULTS
the left ACC (BA 24/32, Tal x, y, z ⫽ ⫺4, 14, 28 mm),
IQ and (2) the left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/cuneus
(BA 31/7, Tal x, y, z ⫽ ⫺26, ⫺62, 16 mm). The activa-
Although age was not significantly correlated with tion level in both of these foci was significantly corre-
verbal IQ (r ⫽ 0.291; P ⫽ 0.118) or performance IQ (r ⫽ lated with subject age (P ⬍ 0.0001, see Figs. 3 and 4).
0.297; P ⫽ 0.111), linear regression indicated a signif-
Young adults vs children. Young adults showed
icant correlation between age and full-scale IQ (r ⫽
greater Stroop task-related activation, as compared to
0.37; P ⫽ 0.044). This significant age-effect in IQ was
children, in the left and right anterior cingulate gyrus
related to a sampling bias in the young adult group due
(BA 24/32) (Z max ⫽ 4.12, P ⫽ 0.049), the left MFG and
to the inclusion of undergraduates at Stanford Univer-
SFG (BA 6/9/45) (Z max ⫽ 3.92, P ⬍ 0.001), and left
sity. Because of these age-related findings, we used
occipitoparietal regions, including the lingual gyrus
full-scale IQ as a nuisance covariate in all analyses of
(BA 18/19), precuneus (BA 31/7), and the cuneus (BA
group and age-related effects.
17) (Z max ⫽ 3.58, P ⫽ 0.025; for summary of all
activation see Table 2 and Fig. 5). Children did not
Stroop Task Performance show significantly greater Stroop task-related activa-
Multiple regression analyses (Table 1) showed that tion than young adults in any brain region.
age explained a significant proportion of the variance Young adults vs adolescents. Young adults had sig-
in Stroop task subtest scores (word raw: beta ⫽ 0.712, nificantly greater activation during the Stroop task, as
68 ADLEMAN ET AL.

FIG. 3. Correlation (n ⫽ 30) between age and activation level (arbitrary scale) in the peak voxel of activation in the left anterior cingulate
cortex (BA 24/32, Tal x, y, z ⫽ ⫺4, 14, 28 mm, P ⬍ 0.0001).
FIG. 4. Correlation (n ⫽ 30) between age and activation level (arbitrary scale) in the peak voxel of activation in the left posterior cingulate
cortex/cuneus (BA 31/7, Tal x, y, z ⫽ ⫺26, ⫺62, 16 mm, P ⬍ 0.0001).
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 69

FIG. 5. Brain areas showing significantly greater Stroop task-related activation in the young adults, compared to the children, included
the anterior cingulate bilaterally, the left MFG, the lingual gyrus, precuneus, and cuneus. Other details as described in the legend of Fig. 2.

compared to adolescents, in the left middle frontal gy- nificant clusters: the first included the right cerebel-
rus (BA 6/8/9) (Z max ⫽ 3.81, P ⫽ 0.002; for summary lum, right hippocampus, and midbrain; the second
of all activation see Table 2, Fig. 6). Adolescents did not cluster was localized in the right superior temporal
show significantly greater Stroop task-related activa- gyrus (STG) (BA 22/42).
tion than young adults in any brain region.
Adolescents vs children. Adolescents exhibited sig- DISCUSSION
nificantly greater activation than children in the left
cuneus (BA 17/18) and the posterior cingulate gyrus/ Improved understanding of the dynamic and vari-
precuneus (BA 31) (Z max ⫽ 4.11, P ⬍ 0.001; for able nature of brain development and function
summary of all activation see Table 2, Fig. 7). Children throughout the human lifespan is of the highest scien-
showed greater activation than adolescents in two sig- tific priority. In particular, elucidating typical develop-
70 ADLEMAN ET AL.

mental processes during childhood and adolescence the ages of 5 and 25 years (McGrew and Woodcock,
will enhance our ability to recognize and treat atypical 2001). Analyses from the behavioral Stroop task in the
development and disease processes in the future. present study, also show a significant relationship be-
The present study is the first to describe the rela- tween age and word reading skill, which develops over
tionship between Stroop task-related brain activation the course of childhood and adolescence.
and age, as well as the first to provide evidence for A positive correlation also was observed between age
ongoing developmental changes in Stroop-processing and activation in the left lateral prefrontal cortex, the
brain networks that correspond to improvements in left anterior cingulate, and the left parietal and parie-
behavioral task performance. A major strength of the to-occipital cortices. This linear correlation was most
present study was the voxel-by-voxel correlation anal- significant in the left lateral prefrontal cortex and left
ysis using a random effects model (Holmes and Friston, parieto-occipital regions. The left lateral prefrontal
1998). We examined both positive and negative age- cluster consisted of a much larger cluster of voxels (No.
related changes in brain activation during perfor- of voxels ⫽ 6109) than the left parieto-occipital cluster
mance of the Stroop task. This allowed us to examine
(No. of voxels ⫽ 1700). No regions showed decreased
whether there is an increase in activation with age
activation with age. These results suggest that the
corresponding to an ability to recruit appropriate re-
developmental trajectory of cognitive processing
sources for task performance or a decrease in activa-
needed for the Stroop task is characterized by increas-
tion corresponding to better optimization and de-
creased task difficulty with age. ing ability to recruit additional neural resources.
There are three major processes involved in perfor- Several recent developmental imaging studies of
mance of the Stroop Color–Word task: word reading, frontal lobe function have shown increased, more dif-
color naming, and interference resolution. In order to fuse, activation in children as compared to adults (Ca-
fully characterize the development of brain activity sey et al., 1997; Gaillard et al., 2000; Thomas et al.,
during Stroop task performance, we used a control task 1999). At first glance, these results may seem to con-
involving color naming. Using this nonlexical control tradict our findings of increased Stroop task-related
task (colored “XXXXX”s) allowed us to examine the activation with age. These differences may arise partly
development of interference resolution in the context of from methodological differences. Previous studies have
word reading. used broadly defined regions of interest (ROIs); for
example, in a developmental study of the gonogo, Ca-
Activation in Young Adults sey et al. (1997) examined activation in the entire ACC,
SFG, MFG, and IFG. In contrast, the present study
Young adults showed significant Stroop task-related used a voxel-by-voxel regression analysis to examine
activation bilaterally in the IFG, MFG, inferior pari- the precise voxels in which Stroop task-related activa-
etal lobe/supramarginal gyrus, superior parietal lobe, tion changes with age. Thus, it is possible that there
and the left anterior cingulate. This pattern of brain are focal increases in activation that are not captured
activation in adults is consistent with Stroop task ac- using broadly defined ROIs. It is also possible that
tivation revealed in other adult neuroimaging studies there is more diffuse activation in children, which be-
(Pardo et al., 1990; Larrue et al., 1994; Carter et al., comes more focal with age in very specific brain re-
1995; Peterson et al., 1999; Bench et al., 1993). These gions.
data, combined with those of the literature, provided a Subjects showed increases in behavioral perfor-
template against which developmental-related differ- mance in conjunction with increases in regional acti-
ences in activation could be assessed in the two
vation. The increase in activation with age may be
younger age groups.
correlated with simultaneous improvements in behav-
ioral performance. Rubia et al. (2000) studied normal
Linear Changes with Age adolescents and adults performing a motor timing task
Both behavioral performance and patterns of brain and found that better delay-task performance in adults
activation showed age-related changes. Raw word, paralleled increased activation in a brain network in-
color, and color–word scores showed linear increases cluding prefrontal and parietal cortical regions and the
with age, reflecting cognitive maturation in several putamen. Below, we further discuss age-related
major processes associated with the Stroop task (read- changes in activation during performance of the Stroop
ing, color identification, and resolution of cognitive in- task by specific brain region (lateral prefrontal cortex,
terference). The behavioral results of this study are parieto-occipital cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex).
consistent with previous behavioral studies that have We discuss these results with regard to both linear
reported improvement in word reading skill with age. age-related changes as well as differences between
Basic reading skills, as measured by the Woodcock- stratified age groups (young adult, adolescent, and
Johnson III, are shown to markedly increase between child).
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 71

Lateral Prefrontal Cortex cortex may influence both reading skills and interfer-
ence resolution skills.
Data from the present study provide evidence for
functional development in lateral prefrontal cortex re-
lated to the Stroop task. Prefrontal regions in which Parietal and Parieto-occipital Cortex
activation was significantly correlated with age in- There was a significant linear correlation between
cluded bilateral MFG (BA 9) and left SFG (BA 6). Left activation in left inferior parietal/parieto-occipital re-
MFG and SFG also showed significant differences in gions and age. In the between-group analysis, signifi-
activation in both the adult– child comparison, and the cant differences were confined to the adult– child and
adult–adolescent comparison, suggesting that the neu- adolescent– child comparisons. No differences in pari-
ral processes associated with prefrontal cortex activa- etal cortex activation, however, were evident in the
tion develop throughout childhood, adolescence, and adult–adolescent comparison. This finding suggests
into young adulthood. that Stroop task-related processes of the parietal lobe
Age-related differences in prefrontal activation were are mostly developed by adolescence.
specifically localized to the left hemisphere. Left hemi- Activation in the parietal lobe during the Stroop task
sphere differences in activation are consistent with the was observed bilaterally in the inferior parietal lobule
verbal nature of the Stroop task. Due to the low-level in agreement with previous results (Carter et al.,
baseline control task, some of the age-related changes 1995), as well as in parieto-occipital regions. Our find-
in functional activation may reflect increasing word ings also are in accord with those of Bench et al. (1993)
reading proficiency. In the present study, no age-re- and Peterson et al. (1999) who found Stroop task-re-
lated activation changes were seen in the right prefron- lated inferior parietal activation. These authors sug-
tal cortex. In particular, we did not see increased right- gest that the parietal cortex plays a role in maintaining
sided activation in young children as compared to sustained attention during the Stroop task. In the
adolescents and young adults. present study, parietal cortex activation also may re-
Previous lesion and imaging studies have implicated flect sustained attention to an important stimulus fea-
lateral prefrontal regions in both interference process- ture in the face of more salient and misleading stimu-
ing/response inhibition (Casey et al., 1997; Pujol et al., lus features. A number of studies have shown that
2000) and word reading/production (Cabeza and Ny- parietal regions are activated during sustained atten-
berg, 2000). Pujol et al. (2000), in a study of demyeli- tion, attention shifts, and when attention is reflexively
nating lesions in patients with multiple sclerosis, re- drawn to prominent features of a stimulus (see
ported that 45% of the variance in Stroop interference Chelazzi and Corbetta (2000) for a review). For exam-
time was accounted for by a combination of frontal and ple, Menon et al. (1997) found both fMRI and EEG
parietal lesions. Prefrontal cortex lesions also produce evidence that the inferior parietal cortex plays an im-
portant role in allocation of directed attention to an
deficits in response inhibition tasks (Grant and Berg,
unexpected event.
1948; Malloy et al., 1993; Perret, 1974). Casey et al.
In addition to playing a role in sustained attention,
(1997) reported that ventral and dorsal prefrontal re-
parieto-occipital activity may be associated with word
gions were activated in both children and adults during
reading (Brunswick et al., 1999; Buchel et al., 1998;
a Go/No-Go response inhibition task, although children
Shaywitz et al., 1998) and visual attention processes
activated more in these regions and performed more (Carpenter et al., 2000). In a recent review of PET
poorly than adults. Response inhibition is an impor- studies, Posner et al. (1999) reported that letter strings
tant component of the Stroop task, as subjects must activate prestriate and parietal areas in the right
inhibit an inappropriate response (i.e., reading the hemisphere, but only word-like stimuli activate these
word) in order to correctly respond (i.e., naming the regions in the left hemisphere. In the present study,
color). the presence of words in the experimental, but not the
Another important cognitive process involved in control condition could explain the left prestriate and
Stroop task performance is word reading and produc- parietal region activation evident in Stroop task per-
tion. Activation in prefrontal regions described in this formance. In addition, previous studies suggest that
study also have been observed in tasks that require extrastriate occipital regions, which were active in the
vocal word reading (Bookheimer et al., 1995; Hirano et adult– child comparison, may be involved in visual
al., 1996) as well as silent word reading (Bookheimer et word form analysis (Petersen et al., 1988; Pugh et al.,
al., 1995; Hirano et al., 1996; Petersen et al., 1990). 1996). As cognitive development occurs and subjects
This is an intriguing finding considering the positive become more familiar with words and reading, it ap-
correlation between age and word reading raw score on pears that they exhibit more activation in these pari-
the behavioral Stroop task. Word reading and produc- etal areas. Reading skill may be a factor in the activa-
tion play an important role in Stroop interference res- tion levels seen in the parietal and parieto-occipital
olution and functional development in the prefrontal regions during performance of the Stroop task.
72 ADLEMAN ET AL.

FIG. 6. Only the left middle frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex showed significantly greater Stroop task-related activation in the
young adults, compared to the adolescents. There were no differences in the parietal lobe. Other details as described in the legend of Fig. 2.

Anterior Cingulate Cortex Stroop task-related activation in the ACC is in agree-


ment with several studies of interference resolution
The present study provides evidence for Stroop task- (Bench et al., 1993; Bush et al., 1998; Carter et al.,
related functional development in the left anterior cin- 1995; Pardo et al., 1990). However, compared to Stroop
gulate cortex. There was a significant positive linear task-related activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex,
correlation between activation in the left ACC and age. activation in the anterior cingulate cortex has been
Greater activation in this region also was evident in observed less consistently. A PET study conducted by
the adult–adolescent and adult– child comparisons. Pardo et al. (1990) showed that the most robust acti-
These differences suggest that functional development vation during a Stroop task was in the ACC. Taylor et
in the left ACC continues throughout childhood, ado- al. (1997) observed that the ACC was active in one of
lescence, and into young adulthood. two different Stroop conditions: the Stroop compared to
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 73

FIG. 7. Brain areas showing significantly greater Stroop task-related activation in the adolescents, compared to the children, included
the left parietal and parieto-occipital regions such as the cuneus and posterior cingulate/precuneus. Other details as described in the legend
of Fig. 2.

neutral word contrast, but not in the Stroop compared children (Casey et al., 1997). An event-related Go/
to false font comparison. Bench et al. (1993) found No-Go task, however, did not yield ACC activation
activation in the ACC in a Stroop versus colored (Garavan et al., 1999).
crosses comparison. However, they did not find a sig- Neuroimaging studies have suggested that the ACC
nificant correlation between reaction times and re- may be involved in executive control processes such as
gional cerebral blood flow in the ACC and therefore, inhibition of inappropriate responses (Paus et al.,
concluded that activation in the ACC alone could not 1993), selective attention (Pardo et al., 1990), and mon-
explain Stroop interference. Activation in the ACC also itoring of response conflict (Barch, 1999). The ACC has
has been observed during the Go/No-Go response inhi- been shown to be active when subjects are instructed to
bition task in adults (Menon et al., 2001) as well as in produce a less practiced response instead of a more
74 ADLEMAN ET AL.

automatic, prepotent response (Barch et al., 2000). functional anatomy of attention using the Stroop test. Neuropsy-
These executive control processes are important in the chologia 31(9): 907–922.
Stroop task. In order to perform the Stroop task, sub- Bettner, L. G., Jarvik, L. F., and Blum, J. E. 1971. Stroop color-word
test, non-psychotic organic brain syndrome, and chromosome loss
jects must inhibit the inappropriate response of word in aged twins. J. Gerontol. 26(4): 458 – 469.
reading, pay selective attention to color, and monitor Bonino, S., and Ciairano, S. 1997. Effetto Stroop e capacita di inibi-
the response conflict between the possible reponses of zione: Una ricerca in campo evolutivo. Giornale Italiano di Psico-
color naming and word reading. logia 24(3): 587– 603.
Bookheimer, S. Y., Zeffiro, T. A., Blaxton, T., Gaillard, W. D., and
Conclusions Theodore, W. H. 1995. Regional cerebral blood flow during object
naming and word reading. Hum. Brain Mapp. 3: 93–106.
The results of the present study indicate that in- Brett, M. 2000. http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/Imaging/mnispace.html.
creased activation occurs with age in a number of brain Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., and Frith, U.
regions thought to be important in performance of the 1999. Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords
Stroop task, including the left lateral prefrontal cortex, by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke’s Worts-
left parietal/parieto-occipital cortex, and left anterior chatz? Brain 122(Pt. 10): 1901–1917.
cingulate cortex. Specifically, activity in the left lateral Buchanan, C. M., Eccles, J. S., and Becker, J. B. 1992. Are adoles-
prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate increases cents the victims of raging hormones: Evidence for activational
effects of hormones on moods and behavior at adolescence. Psychol.
linearly with age, suggesting an increase in focal re- Bull. 111(1): 62–107.
source recruitment during maturation. This age-re- Buchel, C., Price, C., and Friston, K. 1998. A multimodal language
lated increase in activation occurs in conjunction with region in the ventral visual pathway. Nature 394(6690): 274 –277.
better behavioral performance on some measures of Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C.,
the Stroop task. and Rauch, S. L. 1998. The counting Stroop: An interference task
The present study is the first developmental fMRI specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with
study of the color–word Stroop test. As such, the re- functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6(4): 270 –282.
sults are preliminary and warrant further investiga- Cabeza, R., and Nyberg, L. 2000. Imaging cognition II: An empirical
review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12(1):
tion. In order to minimize head movement during the 1– 47.
scan, we asked the subjects to silently respond to the Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., and Reichle, E. D. 2000. Working
Stroop task presented within the scanner. Conse- memory and executive function: Evidence from neuroimaging.
quently, a limitation of the study is that task compli- Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10(2): 195–199.
ance and behavioral performance during the scan could Carter, C. S., Mintun, M., and Cohen, J. D. 1995. Interference and
not be measured. Future studies should attempt to facilitation effects during selective attention: An H215O PET
record verbal responses during fMRI acquisition. More study of Stroop task performance. Neuroimage 2(4): 264 –272.
importantly, the differentiation between verbal and Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom,
L. E., Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Haxby, J. V., Noll, D. C.,
nonverbal components involved in the development of
Cohen, J. D., Forman, S. D., Dahl, R. E., and Rapoport, J. L. 1997.
interference processing remains to be elucidated. The A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation
present study suggests that brain regions involved in during performance of a go-no-go task. J. Cogn. Neurol. 9(6):
word reading and interference resolution processes co- 835– 847.
evolve with development. Future studies should at- Chelazzi, L., and Corbetta, M. 2000. Cortical mechanisms of visuo-
tempt to isolate interference processing from word spatial attention in the primate brain. In The New Cognitive
reading in a developmental paradigm. Neurosciences (M. S. Gazzaniga, Ed.), 2nd ed., pp. 667– 686. The
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Cohen, J. D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., and Provost, J. 1993.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS PsyScope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing
psychology experiments. Behav. Res. Methods Instr. Comp. 25(2):
This work was supported by NIH Grants MH01142, HD31715, and 257–271.
HD40761, the Packard Foundation and the Sinclair Fund. Comalli, P. E., Jr., Wapner, S., and Werner, H. 1962. Interference
effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and ag-
REFERENCES ing. J. Genet. Psychol. 100: 47–53.
Dash, J., and Dash, A. S. 1982. Cognitive developmental studies of
Achenbach, T. M. 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ the Stroop phenomena: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data. In-
4-18 and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont Department of Psy- dian Psychol. 1(1): 24 –33.
chiatry Press, Burlington, VT. Derogatis, L. R., and Savitz, K. L. 1999. The SCL-90-R, Brief Symp-
Barch, D. M. 1999. Images in neuroscience. Cognition: The anterior tom Inventory, and Matching Clinical Rating Scales, 2nd ed. Law-
cingulate and response conflict. Am. J. Psychiatry 156(12): 1849. rence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Barch, D. M., Braver, T. S., Sabb, F. W., and Noll, D. C. 2000. Diamond, A., and Taylor, C. 1996. Development of an aspect of
Anterior cingulate and the monitoring of response conflict: Evi- executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what
dence from an fMRI study of overt verb generation. J. Cogn. I said and to “do as I say, not as I do.” Dev. Psychobiol. 29(4):
Neurosci. 12(2): 298 –309. 315–334.
Bench, C. J., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M., Friston, K. J., Paulesu, E., Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, J.-P., Poline, C. D., Frith,
Frackowiak, R. S., and Dolan, R. J. 1993. Investigations of the C. D., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. 1995. Statistical parametric maps
DEVELOPMENTAL fMRI OF THE STROOP TASK 75

in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Hum. Brain Perret, E. 1974. The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of
Mapp. 2: 189 –210. habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsycholo-
Gaillard, W. D., Hertz-Pannier, L., Mott, S. H., Barnett, A. S., LeBi- gia 12: 323–330.
han, D., and Theodore, W. H. 2000. Functional anatomy of cogni- Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., and Raichle,
tive development: fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults. M. E. 1988. Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical
Neurology 54(1): 180 –185. anatomy of single-word processing. Nature 331(6157): 585–589.
Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., and Stein, E. A. 1999. Right hemispheric Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Snyder, A. Z., and Raichle, M. E. 1990.
dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual
study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96(14): 8301– 8306. words and word-like stimuli. Science 249(4972): 1041–1044.
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., and Diamond, A. 1994. The relationship Peterson, B. S., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J. C., Zhang, H., Anderson,
between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2–7 A. W., and Gore, J. C. 1999. An fMRI study of Stroop word-color
years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition 53(2): 129 – interference: Evidence for cingulate subregions subserving multi-
153. ple distributed attentional systems. Biol. Psychiatry 45(10): 1237–
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, 1258.
H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C., and Rapoport, J. L. 1999. Poline, J. B., Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., and Friston, K. J. 1997.
Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitu- Combining spatial extent and peak intensity to test for activations
dinal MRI study [letter]. Nat. Neurosci. 2(10): 861– 863. in functional imaging. Neuroimage 5(2): 83–96.
Glover, G. H., and Lai, S. 1998. Self-Navigated spiral fMRI: Inter- Posner, M. I., Abdullaev, Y. G., McCandliss, B. D., and Sereno, S. C.
leaved versus single-shot. Magn. Res. Med. 39(3): 361–368. 1999. Neuroanatomy, circuitry and plasticity of word reading [pub-
Golden, C. J. 1978. Stroop color and word test. In A Manual for lished erratum appears in Neuroreport 1999 Jun 23;10(9):inside
Clinical & Experimental Users. Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL. back cover]. Neuroreport 10(3): R12–R23.
Grant, D. A., and Berg, E. A. 1948. A behavioral analysis of degree of Pugh, K. R., Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Constable, R. T.,
reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl type Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R. K., Bronen, R. A., Shankweiler, D. P.,
card sorting problem. J. Exp. Psychol. 38: 404 – 411. Katz, L., Fletcher, J. M., and Gore, J. C. 1996. Cerebral organiza-
tion of component processes in reading. Brain 119(Pt. 4): 1221–
Hayes, C., and Mathias, C. 1996. Improved Brain Coil for fMRI and 1238.
High Resolution Imaging. Paper presented at the ISMRM 4th
Annual Meeting Proceedings, New York. Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Deus, J., Junque, C., Bello, J., Marti-Vilalta,
J. L., and Capdevila, A. 2000. The effect of medial frontal and
Hirano, S., Kojima, H., Naito, Y., and Honjo, I. 1996. Cortical speech posterior parietal demyelinating lesions on Stroop Interference.
processing mechanisms while vocalizing visually-presented lan- Neuroimage, published electronically.
guages. Neuroreport 8(1): 363–367.
Rosinski, R. R. 1977. Picture-word interference is semantically
Holmes, A. P., and Friston, K. J. 1998. Generalisability, random based. Child Dev. 48(2): 643– 647.
effects and population inference. Neuroimage 7(4): S754.
Rosinski, R. R., Golinkoff, R. M., and Kukish, K. S. 1975. Automatic
Kraemer, H. C., Yesavage, J. A., Taylor, J. L., and Kupfer, D. 2000. semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. Child
How can we learn about developmental processes from cross-sec- Dev. 46(1): 247–253.
tional studies, or can we? Am. J. Psychiatry 157(2): 163–171.
Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C.,
Larrue, V., Celsis, P., Bes, A., and Marc-Vergnes, J. P. 1994. The Simmons, A., Andrew, C., and Bullmore, E. T. 2000. Functional
functional anatomy of attention in humans: Cerebral blood flow frontalisation with age: Mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories
changes induced by reading, naming, and the Stroop effect. with fMRI. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24(1): 13–19.
J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 14(6): 958 –962.
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K.,
MacLeod, C. M. 1991. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Liberman,
An integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109(2): 163–203. A. M., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J. M., Katz, L., Marchione, K. E.,
Malloy, P., Bihrlr, A., and Duffy, J. 1993. The orbitomedial frontal Lacadie, C., Gatenby, C., and Gore, J. C. 1998. Functional disrup-
syndrome. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 8: 185–201. tion in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc.
McGrew, K. S., and Woodcock, R. W. 2001. Technical Manual, Wood- Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95(5): 2636 –2641.
cock-Johnson III. Riverside, Itasca, IL. Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L., and
Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., and Reiss, Toga, A. W. 1999. In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain mat-
A. L. 2001. Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo re- uration in frontal and striatal regions [letter]. Nat. Neurosci. 2(10):
sponse inhibition task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 12(3): 131–143. 859 – 861.
Menon, V., Ford, J. M., Lim, K. O., Glover, G. H., and Pfefferbaum, Taylor, S. F., Kornblum, S., Lauber, E. J., Minoshima, S., and Ko-
A. 1997. Combined event-related fMRI and EEG evidence for tem- eppe, R. A. 1997. Isolation of specific interference processing in the
poral-parietal cortex activation during target detection. Neurore- Stroop task: PET activation studies. Neuroimage 6(2): 81–92.
port 8(14): 3029 –3037. Thomas, K. M., King, S. W., Franzen, P. L., Welsh, T. F., Berkowitz,
Pardo, J. V., Pardo, P. J., Janer, K. W., and Raichle, M. E. 1990. The A. L., Noll, D. C., Birmaher, V., and Casey, B. J. 1999. A develop-
anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the mental functional MRI study of spatial working memory. Neuro-
Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA image 10(3 Pt. 1): 327–338.
87(1): 256 –259. Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Pujol, J., Jurado, M. A., Molet, J., and
Paus, T., Petrides, M., Evans, A. C., and Meyer, E. 1993. Role of the Grafman, J. 1995. The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task.
human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, man- Neuropsychologia 33(3): 341–352.
ual, and speech responses: A positron emission tomography study. Worsley, K. J., and Friston, K. J. 1995. Analysis of fMRI time-series
J. Neurophysiol. 70(2): 453– 469. revisited—Again. Neuroimage 2(3): 173–181.

You might also like