You are on page 1of 2

GENERAL COMMENTARY

published: 31 January 2013


doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00013

Short-term memory as a working memory control process


Eddy J. Davelaar*
Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck College, University of London, London, UK
*Correspondence: e.davelaar@bbk.ac.uk
Edited by:
Kimron Shapiro, University of Birmingham, UK
Reviewed by:
Kimron Shapiro, University of Birmingham, UK
Ilja G. Sligte, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

A commentary on the storage causes temporary traces to forgetting is at the heart of another debate
appear which dissipate unless some action on whether there is a need to postu-
About the distinction between working is taken to maintain them. This activ- late a limited-capacity short-term buffer
memory and short-term memory ity is primary memory” (Norman, 1968, (Crowder, 1982; Greene, 1986; Howard
by Aben, B., Stapert, S., and Blokland, A. p. 525). Thus, STM is a process, not a and Kahana, 2002; Davelaar et al., 2005;
(2012). Front. Psychology 3:301. doi: structure. Brown et al., 2007). Provocatively put,
10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00301 Baddeley (1966) observed that imme- a simple span task may never measure
diate recall was particularly sensitive to STM storage capacity, as STM does not
Aben et al. (2012) take issue with the interference when maintaining acoustic exist!
unthoughtful use of the terms “work- compared to semantic information, Nevertheless, those who take the view
ing memory” (WM) and “short-term which eventually led to the assump- that there is no such thing as STM still
memory” (STM) in the cognitive and tion that verbal STM as a structural talk about WM (e.g., Brown et al., 2007),
neuroscientific literature. Whereas I agree component operates on phonological suggesting that a general consensus is that
that neuroscientists using the term WM information (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974). WM includes processes that are not related
to refer to sustained neural activation and The WM model initially separated the to short-term storage.
cognitive psychologists using the terms executive control processes from the short- Another problem with suggesting
interchangeably reflects that the field has term storage component. Thus, WM developing more tasks is that such a focus
lost control over its own dictionary, the = STM + executive control processes. may fall prey to the criticism of circularity.
recommendations to develop more tasks However, in later work, the process of That is, one creates a “simple” task that
does not seem to get to the heart of the active maintenance of information was only requires storage, a “complex” task
matter. Here, I argue in favor of a theoreti- considered one of the executive pro- that requires additional processes, and
cal approach to the constructs of WM and cesses performed by the central executive then uses these tasks to show differential
STM, as the terms have become as impure (Baddeley, 1996) and that the active part correlation patterns between the tasks and
as the tasks that purport to measure the of phonological LTM is the content of some cognitive ability. By design, the tasks
constructs. the phonological loop (Baddeley et al., will follow a WM = STM + X format.
1998). This implies that WM = execu- Showing that the STM component or the
STM vs. WM tive control processes or STM = process X component correlates with fluid intelli-
The concepts of STM and WM are theoret- within WM. gence, reasoning, or language ability, does
ical and should be treated like that. In the not say anything about the natures of and
1960s, STM was equated to primary mem- SIMPLE SPAN vs. COMPLEX SPAN distinction between STM of WM. The dis-
ory. Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) theo- Aben et al. suggest that in order to under- tinction is already built in by the choice of
rized that the cognitive system sets up stand the STM/WM distinction more tasks.
a buffer to maintain information tem- work is needed in which tasks vary in Aben et al. acknowledge that the sim-
porally. This has been interpreted as a the duration that information is kept plicity of tacking on a secondary task to
structural buffer that maintains tempo- and the cognitive load. This is a theory- a simple span task has led to a prolif-
rary information, but could also mean a laden suggestion, as it assumes that eration of WM tasks. Yet, different WM
temporary buffer that maintains durable forgetting in STM is due to time-based tasks do not load on a single WM con-
information. The theory states that “infor- decay and that WM is best assessed by struct (Miyake et al., 2000). The explana-
mation entering STS comes directly from varying cognitive load. This is a hotly tion is that WM is composed of distinct
LTS and only indirectly from the sensory debated position (see e.g., Barrouillet processes (Baddeley, 1996; Miyake et al.,
register” (Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1968, et al., 2007; Lewandowsky et al., 2009) 2000). However, this defeats the purpose
p. 115). In other words, “the activation of and it will be a theoretical nightmare of creating a complex span task to mea-
a feature in LTS is equivalent to the placing to construct a set of tasks on which all sure WM capacity, as it would require
of this item in STS” (Shiffrin, 1976, p. 194). researchers could agree. This is even more the development of several complex span
Furthermore, the “initial activation of aggravated by the fact that short-term tasks measuring capacities of “shifting,”

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 13 | 1


Davelaar STM as a WM process

“inhibition,” “updating,” “dual tasking,” REFERENCES Howard, M. W., and Kahana, M. J. (2002). A
and so on. Aben, B., Stapert, S., and Blokland, A. (2012). About distributed representation of temporal context.
the distinction between working memory and J. Math. Psychol. 46, 269–299.
A more severe problem with following
short-term memory. Front. Psychology 3:301. doi: Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W.,
labeled statistical latent variables is the dis- 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00301 Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., and Engle, R. W. (2004).
regard of procedural similarity. For exam- Atkinson, R. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). “Human The generality of working memory capacity: a
ple, Engle et al.’s (1999) WM and STM memory: a proposed system and its control latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial
latent constructs are perfectly confounded processes,” in The Psychology of Learning and memory span and reasoning. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
Motivation, Vol. 2, eds K. W. Spence and J. T. 133, 189–217.
with the dual-task and single-task require-
Spence (New York, NY: Academic Press), 89–195. Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., and Brown, G.
ment of the component tasks, respectively. Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The influence of acoustic D. A. (2009). No temporal decay in verbal
This is also the case when the modal- and semantic similarity on long-term memory for short-term memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13,
ity of presentation is taken into account word sequences. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 302–309. 120–126.
(Kane et al., 2004). In fact Oberauer et al. Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the cen- Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki,
tral executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 49, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000).
(2012), presented a computational model 5–28. The unity and diversity of executive functions
that captures the Kane et al. (2004) data Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., and Papagno, and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe”
without recourse to different processing C. (1998). The phonological loop as a lan- tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41,
capacities. In their model, the statistical guage learning device. Psychol. Rev. 105, 49–100.
latent constructs truly represent different 158–173. Norman, D. A. (1968). Toward a theory of
Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). “Working memory and attention. Psychol. Rev. 75,
behavioral patterns due to task-procedural memory,” in The Psychology of Learning and 522–536.
differences and not due to different latent Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, Oberauer, K., Lewandowsky, S., Farrell, S., Jarrold,
cognitive constructs. Hence, developing Vol. 6, ed G. H. Bower (New York, NY: Academic C., and Greaves, M. (2012). Modeling working
new complex span tasks would not pro- Press), 47–90. memory: an interference model of complex span.
Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe, E., Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 779–819.
vide evidence for separate WM and STM
and Camos, V. (2007). Time and cognitive load Shiffrin, R. M. (1976). “Capacity limitations in
constructs. in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. information in information processing, atten-
Cogn. 33, 570–585. tion and memory,” in Handbook of Learning
CONCLUSION Brown, G. D. A., Neath, I., and Chater, N. (2007). and Cognitive Processes. Vol. 4, Attention and
The review by Aben et al. highlights a A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychol. Rev. Memory, ed W. K. Estes (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum),
114, 539–576. 177–236.
rough edge in the STM/WM literature,
Crowder, R. G. (1982). Short-term memory: where do
but focuses too much on the most impure we stand? Mem. Cognit. 21, 142–145. Received: 10 October 2012; accepted: 07 January 2013;
part of the STM/WM distinction, the span Davelaar, E. J., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Askenazi, published online: 31 January 2013.
tasks. The distinction between STM and A., Haarmann, H. J., and Usher, M. (2005). Citation: Davelaar EJ (2013) Short-term memory as a
WM and, more importantly, their interre- The demise of short-term memory revisited: working memory control process. Front. Psychology 4:13.
empirical and computational investiga- doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00013
lationship becomes clear at the theoretical tions of recency effects. Psychol. Rev. 112, This article was submitted to Frontiers in Cognition, a
level, but task impurity will plague any 3–42. specialty of Frontiers in Psychology.
correlational study. Addressing the distinc- Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., and Copyright © 2013 Davelaar. This is an open-access
tion at the theoretical level allows honing Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short- article distributed under the terms of the Creative
in on the questions that speak directly to term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a Commons Attribution License, which permits use, dis-
latent-variable approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 128, tribution and reproduction in other forums, provided
the nature and mechanism of STM/WM 309–331. the original authors and source are credited and sub-
and provide directions toward more suit- Greene, R. L. (1986). Sources of recency effects in free ject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party
able approaches. recall. Psychol. Bull. 99, 221–228. graphics etc.

Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition January 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 13 | 2

You might also like