You are on page 1of 3

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

Science & Society


Peer Influence in alcohol, or cigarettes than when alone,
demonstrated in reports of US students
and having friends who smoke or drink is
gathering in large groups during their
Adolescence: Public- one of the biggest predictors of adolescent
spring break (https://www.theguardian.
Health Implications for engagement in these behaviours [3]. This
com/lifeandstyle/2020/mar/28/americans-
social influence is also seen online [4]. For
who-dont-take-coronavirus-seriously).
COVID-19 example, adolescents (aged 14–17 years)
1 Adolescence, the period of life between are more likely to post sexual content on-
Jack L. Andrews, ages 10 and 24 years, is often associated line if their peers have done so. The speed
Lucy Foulkes,1 and with increased risk taking, an increased and extent of peer influence is likely to be
Sarah-Jayne Blakemore1,2,* need for social connection and peer ac- amplified online due to the wide reach
ceptance, and a heightened sensitivity to and fast-acting nature of social media.
The coronavirus disease 2019 peer influence. These factors mean that
(COVID-19) pandemic has resulted adherence to social distancing rules may Adolescent social influence does not al-
in the widespread implementation be especially challenging for young peo- ways have negative consequences. Young
of social distancing measures. ple. Breaking social distancing rules is a people are also less likely to engage in a
risk-taking behaviour: it is a risk to one’s risky behaviour if a friend discourages
Adhering to social distancing may
own health and the health of others and them from doing so [5]. Adolescents are
be particularly challenging for ado-
may carry legal or financial consequences. more socially influenced than adults to en-
lescents, for whom interaction with Here, we discuss evidence demonstrating gage in (hypothetical) prosocial behaviours
peers is especially important. We the effect of peer influence on adolescent [6] and more likely to volunteer in the com-
argue that young people’s capacity risk behaviours and how this phenomenon munity if they are told that their peers volun-
to encourage each other to observe could be harnessed in a positive way to teer [7]. Young people aged 12–16 years
social distancing rules should be encourage young people to follow social give more generously in an experimental
harnessed. distancing measures. public goods game when they observe
Introduction peers being generous [8].
On 12 March 2020, the World Health Peer Influence on Adolescent
Organization announced that COVID-19 Behaviour Adolescents are particularly susceptible to
was a global pandemic. In response to The presence of peers increases the likeli-peer influence for several reasons. First,
this, governments worldwide have imple- hood that adolescents will take certain adolescents look to their peers to under-
mented a number of measures to curtail risks. For example, evidence from labora- stand social norms. They align their behav-
the spread of the disease. These include tory studies and real-world data show iour over time with the norms of their group
closing sites of public recreation and edu- that, when driving, adolescents are more or the group they want to belong to – a
cation, such as schools and universities, likely to have an accident when there is a process known as peer socialisation [9].
and limiting face-to-face interactions passenger in the car, while adults are not Second, adolescents may find it particu-
through enforced ‘social distancing’. [1,2]. Policy changes have been made to larly rewarding to gain social status, a
This will, for the vast majority of the mitigate the risk associated with young potential outcome of aligning with peers
world’s population, be an unprece- drivers carrying passengers. For example, [10]. Finally, adolescents tend to be hyper-
dented experience in which the protec- in the Canadian state of British Columbia, sensitive to the negative effects of social
tion of the most vulnerable depends on drivers from the age of 17 years are re- exclusion. They may conform to a group
strict adherence to the new measures. norm (which sometimes means taking a
stricted to driving with only one passenger,
While many people are adhering to unless they are carrying immediate family risk) to avoid this unpleasant social out-
guidelines, some are not. In particular, it members or a full licence holder over the come. The desire to avoid the social risk
is proving a challenge to convince some age of 25 years, for a minimum of 2 years. of being ostracised or left behind might
young people to refrain from physically outweigh the potential negative conse-
meeting with friends and taking part in In the presence of peers, adolescents are quences associated with health risk or
gatherings. For example, this was clearly also more likely to experiment with drugs, illegal behaviours [11].

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, August 2020, Vol. 24, No. 8 585


Trends in Cognitive Sciences

This susceptibility to peer influence – both led by the government and are likely to students led the campaign ([12];
negative and positive – has important impli- be enforced by other adults (e.g., parents, Figure 1). Similar successes have been
cations for the behaviour of adolescents in teachers, police). One possible approach observed for adolescent-led intervention
the current crisis. In the context of social to enhance their effectiveness would be programs aiming to reduce smoking,
distancing measures, if an adolescent’s to provide adolescents with the autonomy drugs, and alcohol, compared with con-
friends break these rules and meet face- to develop and deliver their own cam- trols [15].
to-face, she may feel more inclined to do paigns, with a focus on changing peer atti-
so herself. By breaking the rules, her tudes around the importance of social Given the current restrictions on face-to-
friends have established a group norm, distancing. This process was successfully face interactions, social media is likely to
whereby meeting up is seen as acceptable. demonstrated by a study that utilised a be the most effective way to promote
Fear of exclusion is also important: she peer-led approach to reduce rates of social distancing behaviours among ado-
may want to join because she misses her peer victimisation in schools. In this lescents. Young people might post con-
friends, but she may also feel a pressure study, social network analysis was used tent online about how they are following
to do so to reduce the social risk of being to find highly connected, well-liked stu- the rules; for example, by sharing a photo
rejected. [11]. By the same token, in the dents (aged 11–15 years), who were then or video of themselves at home. On plat-
current crisis, adolescents may also influ- selected to develop their own antibullying forms such as Instagram, they can add
ence each other in a positive way. As campaigns among peers. Over the ensu- social distancing tags (phrases and im-
discussed earlier, peers can influence ado- ing year there was a 25% reduction in ages) to these posts. These will then be
lescents to behave more prosocially, and victimisation rates in these schools com- seen by their peers, who may add
social norms can be changed [12]. This pared with control schools. The effect endorsements, such as comments and
can be harnessed when communicating was stronger when more well-liked likes, which increase the visibility of the
social distancing rules between young
people.

Shifting Social Norms among


Adolescents
Interventions and campaigns aimed at
influencing adolescent behaviour are
often unsuccessful [13]. Many of these
interventions are based on the theory
that increasing adolescents’ knowledge
and awareness of certain health risks
will result in positive changes to behaviour.
However, as Yaeger and colleagues argue,
these traditional interventions, which are
predominantly adult led, are often unsuc-
cessful. A metanalysis of bullying inter-
ventions found that prevention efforts
were often successful below the 7th
grade (aged under 13 years), but not
beyond 8th grade (over 13 years of age)
[14]. Interventions aimed at adolescents
are most likely to result in behaviour
change when they afford adolescents
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
respect and autonomy and account for
Figure 1. Reduced Bullying Rates Following a Peer-Led Intervention. The distribution of disciplinary events
what they value [13]. in control schools and treatment schools (who received the peer-led intervention), taken from [12]. The average
number of times each student was disciplined for peer conflict is shown from dark blue (little conflict) to dark
orange (high conflict). There is a higher concentration of dark-orange events (high conflict) in the control schools.
In the current pandemic, the campaigns Red nodes are representative of students disciplined for conflict and are scaled to the number of times they
to impose social distancing have been were disciplined across the school year.

586 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, August 2020, Vol. 24, No. 8


Trends in Cognitive Sciences

post. As more adolescents see this con- key developmental period is a considerable Science & Society
tent, social distancing can be established challenge, but can be achieved by taking ad-
as a group norm among friends. This be- vantage of adolescent social influence. Catastrophe
haviour will then be modelled by those
looking on, who may go on to post similar Acknowledgments Compassion:
content themselves. One advantage of
this approach is that it is adolescent led
S-J.B. is funded by Wellcome, the Jacobs Foundation,
Switzerland, UKRI-GCRF, and the University of
Understanding and
and autonomous: the way in which
Cambridge. J.L.A. is funded by the MRC.
Extending Prosociality
young people manage social distancing,
and their motivation for doing so, will
Under Crisis
1
UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, London, UK
stem naturally from the young people 2
Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Jamil Zaki1,*
themselves. Cambridge, UK

Public health bodies should consider *Correspondence: How do people behave when disas-
sjblakemore@psychol.cam.ac.uk (S.-J. Blakemore).
targeting, and even incentivising, influential ters strike? Popular media accounts
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.05.001
individuals online (i.e., those who have the depict panic and cruelty, but in fact
© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
capacity to diffuse information among a individuals often cooperate with
large online social network). For example, References
and care for one another during cri-
it may be particularly useful to target social 1. Chen, L.H. et al. (2000) Carrying passengers as a risk fac- ses. I summarize evidence for such
media ‘influencers’, individuals with a tor for crashes fatal to 16- and 17-year-old drivers. JAMA
283, 1578–1582 'catastrophe compassion', discuss
strong online presence and a large 2. Gardner, M. and Steinberg, L. (2005) Peer influence on its roots, and consider how it might
risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in
number of adolescent followers. If these adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev. be cultivated in more mundane
individuals model positive social distanc- Psychol. 41, 625
3. Loke, A.Y. and Mak, Y.W. (2013) Family process and peer
times.
ing behaviour and communicate the risk influences on substance use by adolescents. Int.
of COVID-19 through their platform, J. Environ. Res. Public Health 10, 3868–3885
adolescents may listen. An advantage of 4. Nesi, J. et al. (2018) Transformation of adolescent peer re- A Surprising Response to Calamity
lations in the social media context: part 2 – application to
targeting social media influencers is that peer group processes and future directions for research.
In the aftermath of Hurricane Katrina, news
they exist across a number of domains Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 21, 295–319 reports suggested that the natural disaster
5. Maxwell, K.A. (2002) Friends: the role of peer influence
of interest (e.g., different hobbies) and across adolescent risk behaviors. J. Youth Adolesc. 31,
had quickly been followed by a human
so are likely to be able to target large 267–277 one. Unchecked by law enforcement, New
6. Foulkes, L. et al. (2018) Age differences in the prosocial in-
disparate groups of young people. fluence effect. Dev. Sci. 21, e12666
Orleanians had apparently committed
7. Choukas-Bradley, S. et al. (2015) Peer influence, peer sta- countless brazen crimes [1]. The New York
tus, and prosocial behavior: an experimental investigation
of peer socialization of adolescents’ intentions to volunteer.
Times described the city as a 'snake pit of
Concluding Remarks J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 2197–2210 anarchy, death, looting, raping, marauding
8. van Hoorn, J. et al. (2016) Peer influence on prosocial be-
Although the coronavirus appears to pose a havior in adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 26, 90–100
thugs' [2].
low risk to adolescents themselves, their will- 9. Henneberger, A.K. et al. (2020) Peer influence and adolescent
substance use: a systematic review of dynamic social network
ingness to follow social distancing guidelines research. Adolesc. Res. Rev. Published online January 2, 2020.
These harrowing stories shaped the reac-
is essential to reduce the risk for other people. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-019-00130-0 tion of the authorities to the crisis – who,
10. Brechwald, W.A. and Prinstein, M.J. (2011) Beyond
Adolescent susceptibility to peer influence homophily: a decade of advances in understanding peer
for example, deployed the national guard
can be beneficial and should be harnessed influence processes. J. Res. Adolesc. 21, 166–179 to 'take control' of the city instead of focus-
11. Blakemore, S.J. (2018) Avoiding social risk in adolescence.
by public-health campaigns to increase social Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 116–122
ing on humanitarian relief. The stories were
distancing. We propose that adolescents 12. Paluck, E.L. et al. (2016) Changing climates of conflict: a also inaccurate. Although crime did occur
social network experiment in 56 schools. Proc. Natl.
themselves have a great capacity to influence Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 566–571
in New Orleans following Katrina, victims
each other to change norms and peer expec- 13. Yeager, D.S. et al. (2018) Why interventions to influence by and large remained peaceful, and
adolescent behavior often fail but could succeed.
tations towards public-health goals. Espe- Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 101–122
many helped one another [1,3].
cially important in creating change is the 14. Yeager, D.S. et al. (2015) Declines in efficacy of anti-bullying pro-
grams among older adolescents: theory and a three-level meta-
need to provide young people with the ca- analysis. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 37, 36–51
For decades, social scientists have docu-
pacity to lead and enact their own ideas 15. MacArthur, G.J. et al. (2016) Peer-led interventions to mented two narratives about human be-
within their social networks. Asking adoles- prevent tobacco, alcohol and/or drug use among young
people aged 11–21 years: a systematic review and
havior during crises. The first holds that,
cents to stay away from their friends at a meta-analysis. Addiction 111, 391–407 following disasters, individuals (i) panic,

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, August 2020, Vol. 24, No. 8 587

You might also like