You are on page 1of 31

Psychological Bulletin

1977, Vol. 84, No. 5, 8-918

Attitude-Behavior Relations: A Theoretical Analysis


and Review of Empirical Research
Icek Ajzen Martin Fishbein
University of Massachusetts—Amherst University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

Research on the relation between attitude and behavior is examined in light of


the correspondence between attitudinal and behavioral entities. Such entities are
defined by their target, action, context, and time elements. A review of available
empirical research supports the contention that strong attitude-behavior relations
dre obtained only under high correspondence between at least the target and
action elements of the attitudinal and behavioral entities. This conclusion is
compared with the rather pessimistic assessment of the utility of the attitude
concept found in much contemporary social psychological literature.

Reports of rather low or nonsignificant concept and its utility (Ajzen & Fishbein,
relations between attitudinal predictors and 1973; Fishbein, 1967, 1973; Fishbein &
behavioral criteria have been accumulating Ajzen, 1972, 1974, 1975). We have argued
for more than 40 years (cf. Wicker, 1969). that a person's attitude toward an object in-
These negative findings led many investiga- fluences the overall pattern of his responses
tors to reconsider the nature of the attitude- to the object, but that it need not predict any
behavior relation (e.g., Calder & Ross, 1973; given action. According to this analysis, a
D. T. Campbell, 1963; DeFleur & Westie, single behavior is determined by the inten-
1958, 1963; Deutscher, 1966, 1969, 1973; tion to perform the behavior in question. A
Ehrlich, 1969; Kelman, 1974; Rokeach, person's intention is in turn a function of his
1967; Tittle & Hill, 1967). In a parallel de- attitude toward performing the behavior and
velopment, it was possible to discern a grow- of his subjective norm. It follows that a single
ing disenchantment with the attitude concept, act is predictable from the attitude toward
and the general consensus was that measures that act, provided that there is a high corre-
of attitude have little value for the predic- lation between intention and behavior.1
tion of overt behavior. These arguments can be incorporated with-
Recently, however, social psychology has in a more general framework that focuses on
been witnessing a revival of interest in the the question of correspondence between mea-
relationship between attitude and action sures of attitude and behavior. The purpose
(e.g., Brannon, 1976; Liska, 1975; D. J. of the present article is to reexamine the
Schneider, 1976; Schuman & Johnson, 1976). attitude-behavior relationship. A theoretical
The emerging position seems to be that atti- analysis of the correspondence between atti-
tude is only one of many factors determining tudinal predictors and behavioral criteria is
behavior. Although this position reaffirms the followed by a review of pertinent empirical
importance of attitudes, it leads to the expec- research. It is shown that people's actions are
tation that attitudes will often be unrelated found to be systematically related to their
to behavior. attitudes when the nature of the attitudinal
In a number of publications we have pre- predictors and behavioral criteria are taken
sented ideas and data that are clearly at into consideration.
variance with this assessment of the attitude
1
Even when the intention is primarily under the
Requests for reprints should be sent to Icek Ajzen, control of normative considerations, its correlation
Department of Psychology, University of Massa- with attitude toward the action is usually found to
chusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts 01003. be quite high.
888
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 889

Attitude-Behavior Correspondence a relationship between attitude toward Rus-


sia and a measure of this behavior.
Attitudes are held with respect to some It can be seen that a given behavior is
aspect of the individual's world, such as an- assumed to be consistent or inconsistent with
other person, a physical object, a behavior, or a person's attitude on the basis of largely
a policy. Although many definitions of atti- intuitive considerations. In the absence of an
tude have been proposed, most investigators explicit and unambiguous definition of atti-
would agree that a person's attitude repre- tude-behavior consistency, therefore, many
sents his evaluation of the entity in question. tests of the attitude-behavior relation reduce
For purposes of the present review, only mea- to little more than tests of the investigator's
sures that place the individual on a bipolar intuition. From a theoretical point of view,
evaluative or affective dimension are consid- such tests of the relation between arbitrarily
ered to be measures of attitude. selected measures of attitude or behavior are
Behavioral criteria consist of one or more of rather limited value.
observable actions performed by the indivi- The following analysis attempts to specify
dual and recorded in some way by the in- the conditions under which attitudes can or
vestigator. Behavioral acts include attending cannot be expected to predict overt behavior.
a meeting, using birth control pills, buying a Our point of departure is the notion that
product, donating blood, and so forth. Some- attitudes are held and behaviors are performed
times investigators have relied on "behavior- with respect to certain entities. Two impor-
oid" measures (Aronson & Carlsmith, 1968), tant questions in research on the attitude-
that is, on the individual's commitment to behavior relation can then be identified: (a)
perform the behavior under consideration or What are the entities of the attitudinal pre-
on self-reports of behavior. Such measures are dictors and of the behavioral criteria? (b)
treated below as acceptable behavioral cri- What is the degree of correspondence between
teria only when it would have been difficult the attitudinal and the behavioral entities?
or impossible to obtain a direct measure of
the behavior in question. Attitudinal and Behavioral Entities
Attempts to predict behavior from attitudes
are largely based on a general notion of con- Attitudinal and behavioral entities may be
sistency. It is usually considered to be logical viewed as consisting of four different ele-
or consistent for a person who holds a favor- ments: the action, the target at which the
able attitude toward some object to perform action is directed, the context in which the
favorable behaviors, and not to perform un- action is performed, and the time at which it
favorable behaviors, with respect to the ob- is performed. The generality or specificity of
ject.2 Similarly, a person with an unfavorable each element depends on the measurement
attitude is expected to perform unfavorable procedure employed.
behaviors, but not to perform favorable be- Behavioral criteria based on single observa-
haviors. The apparent simplicity of this no- tions always involve four specific elements.
tion is deceptive, since there is usually no That is, a given action is always performed
theoretical basis for the assumption that a with respect to a given target, in a given con-
behavior has favorable or unfavorable im- text, and at a given point in time. Criteria
plications for the object under consideration. based on multiple observations of behavior
Obviously, many behaviors have no evalua- generalize across one or more of the four
tive implications for a given object. Eating elements. For example, when the behavioral
hamburgers, for example, implies neither a
favorable nor an unfavorable evaluation of "Peabody's (1967) work suggests that the basis
Russia. However, an investigator might as- for consistency may often be logical or denotative
sume that an unfavorable evaluation of Rus- rather than evaluative. However, logical and evalua-
tive consistency are usually confounded and the
sia is implied by the act of volunteering for distinction appears to be of greater theoretical than
military service and might therefore predict practical significance.
890 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

observations constituting the criterion measure actions (cooking dinner, cooperating), toward
involve a very heterogeneous sample of tar- contexts (in St. Mary's Cathedral, at home),
gets, the target element is essentially left toward times (3:00 p.m. tomorrow, August),
unspecified. However, when the different tar- or toward any combination of elements (co-
gets constitute a more homogeneous set, their operating with Jews, cooking dinner at home
common attributes determine the target ele- at 3:00 p.m. tomorrow).
ment. When all targets are other human be-
ings, for example, the target element is people Correspondence Between Attitudinal and
in general; when the individuals serving as
Behavioral Entities
targets are of the same sex, religion, or race,
then males, Jews, or Orientals might consti- After defining entities in terms of their
tute the target element. elements, we can approach the question of
Similar considerations apply to the defini- attitude-lpehavior correspondence. An atti-
tion of the action, context, and time elements. tudinal predictor is said to correspond to the
To illustrate, when a very heterogeneous sam- behavioral criterion to the extent that the
ple of behaviors is observed, the action ele- attitudinal entity is identical in all four
ment is left unspecified. Sometimes, however, elements with the behavioral entity. For ex-
the specific acts may represent a more gen- ample, a measure of attitude toward a target
eral class of behaviors, such as cooperation, such as "my church" (without specification
aggression, or altruism. Here, the action ele- of action, context, or time) corresponds di-
ment is defined by the class of behaviors.3 rectly only to a behavioral criterion based on
In conclusion, the measurement procedure the observation of different behaviors with
determines the behavioral entity. When the respect to the person's church (e.g., donating
same action is observed with respect to money, attending Sunday worship services,
heterogeneous targets, in different contexts, participating in church-sponsored activities,
and at different points in time, we obtain a etc.), in different contexts, and at different
behavioral index whose entity is defined only points in time. Similarly, when the attitude
by the action element. The target serves as measure is an evaluation of a specific action
the entity when heterogeneous behaviors toward a given target, such as the attitude
toward the same target are observed in dif- toward "donating money to my church," the
ferent situations and at different points in corresponding behavioral criterion is an index
time. In a similar manner, indices can be of monetary donations to the person's church
obtained such that the contextual element, based on multiple behavioral observations in
the time element, or any combination of ele- different contexts (e.g., at home, in the church,
ments defines the entity for the behavioral etc.) and on different occasions. Alternatively,
criterion. when the behavioral criterion is a single act,
As in the case of behavioral criteria, atti- such as the person's attendance or nonattend-
tudes are also directed at entities that may be ance of next Sunday's worship service in his
defined by a single element or by combina-
tions of two or more elements. Attitudinal
3
predictors frequently specify only the target. It is interesting to note that, as in the case of
Attitudes have been measured toward the attitudes, personality traits have been found to have
church, various ethnic groups, specific per- little validity for the prediction of specific behaviors
(Mischel, 1968; Wiggins, 1973). Personality traits
sons, and so on without reference to any par- such as dominance or authoritarianism represent
ticular action, context, or time. However, an general behavioral tendencies without reference to a
investigator can specify an entity in terms of specific target, context, or time. Given the nature of
personality measures, it seems reasonable to suggest
any combination of elements and can obtain that an appropriate behavioral criterion is not a
a measure of attitude toward that entity. For single action but rather an index based on a set of
example, an evaluative semantic differential behaviors reflecting the trait in question; target,
context, and time should be left unspecified. Data
could be used to measure attitudes toward supporting this notion have been reported by Jac-
targets (Martin Luther King, Jews), toward card (1974).
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 891

church at 10:00 a.m., the corresponding atti- Whenever the single-act criterion involves an
tudinal predictor would be a measure of the action that is little more than an evaluation
person's evaluation of "attending my church's of the target, it should be predictable from a
worship service next Sunday at 10:00 a.m." measure of attitude toward the target in
Correspondence and the attitude-behavior question.
relation. The central thesis of this article is The relatively frequent use of petition
that the strength of an attitude-behavior rela- signing or voting as measures of behavior
tionship depends in large part on the degree deserves attention in this context. Both of
of correspondence between attitudinal and these behaviors constitute single-act criteria
behavioral entities. Although, in theory, cor- that specify the target element as well as
respondence is denned in terms of all four the action element. Under most circum-
elements of the entities involved, for purposes stances, however, the act of signing a peti-
of the present article, examination of the tar- tion or voting for a given candidate involves
get and action elements is sufficient. little more than expressing an evaluation of
Considering target and action elements the target in question. For example, when a
alone, two attitudinal predictors can be iden- person signs or refuses to sign a petition sup-
tified that deserve special attention. The most porting the legalization of marijuana, the act
common measure specifies a given target (be itself involves little more than the expression
it an object, a person, or an institution) with- of a favorable or unfavorable attitude with
out reference to a particular action. This respect to the issue. Viewed in this light, a
predictor may be termed attitude toward a measure of attitude toward the target (legali-
target. Of less frequent use is attitude toward zation of marijuana) should permit satis-
an action, a predictor that specifies both factory prediction of the petition-signing be-
action and target elements (e.g., attitude havior.
toward smoking marijuana). Similarly, in the United States, the act of
A similar distinction can be made with voting for a given candidate or issue reflects
reference to behavioral criteria. When the in large part the voter's evaluation of the
criterion is an index based on observations of candidate or issue under consideration. A
heterogeneous behaviors with respect to a measure of attitude toward the candidate or
given target, only the target element is speci- issue would therefore be expected to correlate
fied and the resulting measure may be called highly with voting behavior.
a multiple-act criterion. When only one be- Considerations of this kind may also apply
havior toward a given target is observed, both to other single-act criteria. That is, a specific
target and action elements are specified and act may sometimes have relatively direct
we obtain a single-act criterion. evaluative implications for a given target. A
The above discussion suggests that atti- procedure for determining such evaluative
tudes toward targets will predict multiple-act implications of single-act criteria was dis-
criteria, provided that the attitudinal and
cussed by Fishbein and Ajzen (1974). How-
behavioral entities involve the same target
elements. Similarly, attitudes toward actions ever, even when it can be shown that an
are expected to predict single-act criteria if action has evaluative implications for the
the target and action elements of the atti- target, the most appropriate predictor of the
tudinal entity are identical with those of the single-act criterion is the attitude toward the
behavioral entity (cf. Fishbein & Ajzen, action rather than the attitude toward the
1974, 197S). target.
Generally speaking, high attitude-behavior The review of empirical research below
correlations cannot be expected in the ab- deals with studies that provide evidence con-
sence of correspondence between attitudinal cerning the relation between an evaluative
and behavioral entities. Lack of correspon- measure of attitude and some behavioral cri-
dence, however, does not necessarily preclude terion—whether or not these studies were
a relationship between attitude and behavior. designed to examine the attitude-behavior
892 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

relation.4 Studies with inappropriate mea- Lack of Correspondence


sures of either the attitudinal predictor or the
behavioral criterion are included if they were Many studies concerning the attitude-be-
designed to be explicit tests of the relation havior issue have obtained measures with lit-
between attitude and behavior. tle or no correspondence between the ele-
Our review is structured in terms of cor- ments of the attitudinal and behavioral enti-
respondence with respect to target and action ties. Usually, attitudes are measured toward a
elements. Although it is possible to consider class of people in general without reference to
degrees of correspondence in each element, for any particular action. The behavioral cri-
the sake of simplicity we have chosen to teria, however, consist of specific acts with
classify attitudes and behaviors as either cor- respect to, or in the presence of, one or more
responding or not corresponding in their tar- particular members of the class of people
gets and actions. For each study reviewed, we that serves as the target of the attitude.
identify the target and action elements of the A good example is the study by Himelstein
attitudinal and behavioral entities. When the and Moore (1963). Subjects in this study
two targets are identical, the attitudinal pre- were 100 white male undergraduates who had
dictor and behavioral criterion are classified volunteered for a psychological experiment.
as corresponding in their target elements. A Prior to the experiment, the subjects com-
similar judgment is made with respect to the pleted a 9-item scale measuring attitudes
action elements. In accordance with our pre- toward blacks, which had been adapted from
vious discussion, if a single-act criterion in- the F scale. The sample of subjects was
volves signing a petition or voting, the action dichotomized at the median attitude score.
element of the behavioral entity is viewed as Upon reporting for the experiment, the sub-
unspecified. ject found another student, either black or
white, already seated in the room. Actually,
this student was an experimental assistant.
Research on Attitude-Behavior Relations While they were waiting for the experiment to
begin, a (white) confederate entered the room
Studies that provide data on the relation with a petition in his hand. The petition con-
between attitude and behavior fall into sev- tained a proposal to extend the library hours
eral categories. One category contains studies on Saturday until 8:00 p.m. The black or
in which neither the target nor the action white assistant either signed or refused to sign
element of the attitudinal entity corresponds the petition, and following this manipulation,
to the target or action element of the behav- the subject was requested to sign.0 The sub-
ioral entity. Our analysis suggests that stud- ject's compliance or lack of compliance with
ies of this kind should obtain very low atti- the assistant's response served as the behav-
tude-behavior relations. Other studies cor- ioral criterion. A secondary analysis of the
respond in one of the two elements but not in data revealed a nonsignificant phi coefficient
the other. Our analysis indicates that the of .06 between attitudes toward blacks and
results of such studies are likely to be in- compliance with the black confederate.
consistent and that the obtained relations Using the Asch (1951) and M. Sherif
between attitude and behavior will generally (1935) procedures, several other studies have
be quite low. It appears that investigators
have questioned the predictive utility of atti- *We tried to identify all relevant studies, but
tude measures primarily on the basis of stud- there are obviously some that we have overlooked.
ies in these two categories. However, we shall Furthermore, because of the increased interest in
the attitude-behavior relation, several new studies
see that results are quite consistent, and sig- will have appeared by the time this review is pub-
nificant relations between attitudes and be- lished.
6
havior are usually obtained when there is Note that this behavior might serve as an indica-
tion of the subject's attitude toward "extending
correspondence with respect to both the tar- library hours on Saturday until 8:00 p.m.," but not
get and the action elements. as an indication of attitude toward blacks.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 893

also attempted to predict conformity with element (children in general) clearly did not
specific members of racial or ethnic groups on correspond to the behavioral target element
the basis of attitudes toward those groups in (the mother's own child). Schwartz and Tes-
general (Berg, 1966; Boyanowski & Allen, sler (1972) assessed women's attitudes
1973; Bray, 19SO; Malof & Lott, 1962; F. toward "reducing transplant activities" and
W. Schneider, 1970). In addition, attitudes recorded their willingness to "join a pool of
toward blacks have been used to predict potential bone marrow donors." Dean (1958)
conditioning in the Taffel (1955) procedure attempted to predict union members' attend-
by means of a black or white experimenter ance at regular local union meetings on the
(Smith & Dixon, 1968); inviting black or basis of the members' attitudes toward labor
white confederates for coffee (Rokeach & unions in general and their liking for their
Mezei, 1966); signing releases for photo- jobs. C. W. Sherif, Kelly, Rodgers, Sarup,
graphs to be taken with specific black indi- and Tittler (1973, Study 5) assessed atti-
viduals (Tarter, 1969); sending messages to tudes toward personal adjustment, black
a black confederate and rejecting that confed- rights, and achievement in education and
erate from the group (Burnstein & McRae, attempted to predict attendance at a group
1962); and performing each of 21 different discussion dealing with one of these issues.
behaviors toward black group members while Finally, Genthner and Taylor (1973) mea-
working on a variety of tasks in racially mixed sured attitudes toward desegregation and used
groups (Katz & Benjamin, I960)." Like Him- the average intensity of electric shocks ad-
elstein and Moore (1963), virtually all of ministered to a black confederate as their
these studies have reported nonsignificant behavioral criterion. Once again, in none of
relations between attitude and behavior. One these studies were attitudes found to make
exception was the investigation by Burnstein significant contributions to the prediction of
and McRae (1962), who found that attitudes behavior.
toward blacks in general had a significant Although the study by Kutner, Wilkins,
effect on the proportion of messages sent to a and Yarrow (1952) has usually been con-
specific black confederate in a communica- sidered as a test of the relation between atti-
tion network. Using Friedman's (1968) table tude and behavior, from our point of view it
for the rapid estimation of the magnitude of failed to obtain an adequate measure of atti-
an experimental effect (f,H), the correlation tude. These investigators recorded whether
between attitude and behavior in this study reservations for a racially mixed group were
is found to be relatively low (r^—.30). accepted over the telephone by 11 different
Furthermore, the attitude toward blacks had restaurants. Prior to the requests for reserva-
no significant effect on rejection of the black tions, all 11 restaurants had allowed a black
confederate from the group. woman to join two white women already
Lack of correspondence between attitudi- seated at a table. It can be seen that no mea-
nal and behavioral entities can also be found sure of attitude was obtained. On the two
in several studies that measured attitudes occasions the investigators assessed two dif-
toward targets other than racial or ethnic ferent behaviors: accepting or refusing to
groups. Zunich (1961), like Katz and Benja- accept a reservation for a racially mixed
min, obtained a number of behavioral cri- group and admitting or refusing to admit a
teria. Specifically, he observed the extent to black woman. No relation between these two
which mothers engaged in each of 17 different behaviors was found. Although all of the
behaviors with respect to their children. In
addition, attitudes toward 16 different child- a
Had Katz and Benjamin constructed a multiple-
rearing practices (e.g., breaking a child's will, act criterion on the basis of their 21 behaviors, the
egalitarianism) were measured. Although behavioral action element would have been unspeci-
there may have been limited correspondence fied and would thus have corresponded to the atti-
tudinal action element. Even so, their measures of
in the action elements of some attitudinal attitude and behavior would still have lacked cor-
and behavioral entities, the attitudinal target respondence in terms of the target elements.
894 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

restaurants admitted the black woman, none to fall into this category. Seven of these
of the restaurants accepted the reservation. studies lacked correspondence in the target
An analysis of the entities of the two be- elements because the attitudinal predictors
haviors revealed that there was no corre- involved unspecified or general targets,
spondence between their target elements or whereas the behaviors were observed with
between their action elements. In one case respect to very specific targets.
the target was a racially mixed group, and For example, Corey (1937) measured stu-
the action element consisted of accepting a dents' attitudes toward cheating and at-
reservation. The second behavior involved a tempted to predict actual cheating on a given
specific target person (the black woman) and set of tests. Over a period of 5 weeks, Corey's
a different action element (admittance to a students took five true-false examinations.
restaurant). Although this study had little Each week's test papers were returned un-
to do with the attitude-behavior relation, its marked after the students' scores had been
negative results are hardly surprising. recorded. The students then graded their own
papers during the following class session.
Conclusion The difference between the true score and the
This section reviewed 17 studies in which score each student reported for himself, sum-
the target and action elements of the atti- med over the five tests, constituted the pri-
tudinal entity failed to correspond to the mary behavioral criterion. A second index
target and action elements of the behavioral was derived by computing the ratio of this
entity. Only one conclusion can be reached actual cheating score over the maximal cheat-
in light of this research: Attitude-behavior ing score (the difference between the true
relations under lack of correspondence are score and the best possible score on each test).
low and not significant. It can be seen that the attitudinal and
behavioral entities in this study corresponded
Partial Correspondence in terms of their action elements, in that both
dealt with cheating. However, the attitudinal
Whereas the previous section dealt with target element did not correspond closely to
the effects of complete lack of correspondence, the behavioral target element. The former
the studies reviewed below employed mea- refers to cheating in general, whereas the
sures of attitude and behavior that corre- latter concerns cheating on a given set of
sponded in one of the two major elements of tests. That this distinction is important can
the attitudinal and behavioral entities. Our be seen by examining the results. The atti-
analysis of correspondence between entities tude score did not correlate significantly
suggests that under conditions of partial
with either of the two behavioral criteria
correspondence, relations between attitude (rs — .024 and .13, respectively). By way of
and behavior will tend to be inconsistent, and comparison, the author reported that the
even where they are significant, they should maximal cheating score (representing an
normally be quite low. In most investigations index of "temptation") correlated signifi-
involving partial correspondence, we find cantly (r — .46) with actual cheating. That
correspondence between target elements but is, students were likely to cheat on a test if
little correspondence between action elements. cheating on that test was potentially useful
First, however, we examine several studies
or desirable, irrespective of their attitudes
in which the opposite is true, that is, in toward cheating in general.
which the action elements corresponded but Nonsignificant correlations between atti-
in which there was little correspondence tudes toward cheating in college and actual
between the target elements. cheating on specific examinations were also
reported by Freeman and Ataov (1960).
Lack of Correspondence between Target
Sample and Warland (1973) and Warland
Elements
and Sample (1973) measured attitudes to-
Ten investigations, dealing with a variety ward participating in student government
of different targets and actions, were found among undergraduates enrolled in social sci-
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 895

ence classes. A IS-item Likert scale developed havioral intention scale. The behavioral cri-
by Tittle and Hill (1967) was used. The be- terion was a 4-item Guttman scale of activ-
havioral criterion, based on voting record, was ities with respect to members of the National
whether or not a subject had voted (i.e., par- Association for the Advancement of Colored
ticipated) in a given student election (a much People (NAACP), ranging from refusing to
more specific target than student govern- participate in a group discussion sponsored
ment). Although the attitude-behavior corre- by the NAACP to signing up for various
lations in the two studies were significant, civil rights activities with members of the
they were of relatively low magnitude (rs = NAACP. In a second study, Fendrich
.29 and .26). (1967a) used only the Likert scale to mea-
The other studies in this group assessed sure attitudes toward black students and
beliefs (Bickman, 1972) or intentions (La- added one item to the behavioral scale. Since
Piere, 1934; McGrew, 1967) instead of ob- the behavioral criterion in both studies
taining measures of attitude, as defined in generalized across a variety of specific actions,
this article. All three reported nonsignificant it may be viewed as having an unspecified
relations with behavior. action element that corresponded to that of
Undoubtedly the best known of these the attitudinal predictor. However, the be-
studies is LaPiere's investigation of racial havioral target element involved members of
prejudice. In the early 1930s, LaPiere ac- the NAACP, whereas attitudes were mea-
companied a young Chinese couple in their sured toward black students in general.
travels through the United States. Calling Secondary analyses of the data resulted
upon 251 restaurants, hotels, and other in significant correlations between attitudes
establishments, they were refused service only and behavior. In the first study, the two
once. About 6 months later, LaPiere sent a scales correlated .49 and .38 with the criter-
letter to each establishment visited, asking ion, and in the second study the correlation
the same question: "Will you accept members was .30."
of the Chinese race as guests in your estab-
lishment?" Of the 128 establishments that 7
For a discussion of other problems in the La-
replied, over 90% answered, "No." Piere study, see Ajzen, Darroch, Fishbein, and Hor-
Clearly, the letter-questionnaire in this nik (1970) and Dillehay (1973).
8
The secondary analyses were performed in order
study was not a measure of evaluation. It can to enable comparisons of the results in the two
best be described as a measure of behavioral studies. The data from the two conditions in the
intention, or perhaps of behavioral commit- first study were combined for these analyses. That is,
ment. Disregarding this problem, we can see Fendrich (1967b) administered the two attitude
that the entities of the two measures corre- scales in counterbalanced order, and order of ad-
ministration was found to have a strong effect on
sponded only in part. Whereas the action the attitude-behavior relation. When the Likert
element in both measures involved accepting scale was administered before the intention scale,
someone in an establishment, the target ele- neither measure predicted the behavior significantly
ments differed. The overt behavior was (7s = .12 and .18, respectively). However, when
directed at a specific Chinese couple (usually the scales were administered in the reverse order,
the gamma coefficients were .69 for the Likert scale
accompanied by LaPiere). The letter, how- and .72 for the intention scale, both significant.
ever, referred more generally to "members of Fendrich argued that a typical attitude measure,
the Chinese race." 7 such as his Likert scale, elicits role playing, whereas
The final three studies used measures of the intention scale involves commitment to actual
behavior. However, the intention scale also failed
attitudes toward targets to predict criteria to predict behavior when it was preceded by the
with unspecified action elements. Attitudinal Likert scale. Furthermore, after dichotomizing the
and behavioral entities thus corresponded variables, a secondary analysis revealed an overall
in their action elements, but unfortunately, gamma of 1.0 for the Likert scale, which compares
they involved different target elements. with a gamma of .71 in the second study (Fend-
rich, 1967a). Finally, we shall see below that ap-
Fendrich (1967b) measured attitudes to- propriate measures of attitude can predict behavior,
ward blacks by means of two instruments: even when they involve no explicit behavioral com-
a 32-item Likert scale and a 10-item be- mitment.
896 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

Finally, DeFriese and Ford (1968, 1969) 1970; Regan, 1971; Schopler & Thompson,
also measured attitudes toward "Negroes in 1968); and verbal and nonverbal communica-
general." Their behavioral criterion was the tion (Byrne, Baskett, & Hodges, 1971;
act of signing a petition for or against inte- Byrne, Ervin, & Lamberth, 1970; Ehrlich &
grated housing. As noted in the introduction, Graeven, 1971; Goldberg, Kiesler, & Collins,
this behavior may be viewed as an evaluation 1969; Goldberg & Mettee, 1969; S. Rosen,
of the issue in question, and it may thus be Johnson, Johnson, & Tesser, 1973; Sapolsky,
argued that the behavioral entity, like the 1960; Worthy, Gary, & Kahn, 1969).
attitudinal entity, had no specific action Since in all of these studies the measures
element. However, the target elements (blacks of attitude made no reference to any partic-
versus integrated housing) were quite dif- ular behavior, the attitudinal and behavioral
ferent. A secondary analysis of the data was entities corresponded only in terms of their
performed by assigning a score of 1, 2, or 3 target elements but not in terms of their
to subjects who signed the petition against action elements. These studies, perhaps more
integrated housing, neither petition, or the than any other category of research, illustrate
petition in favor of integrated housing, re- the inconsistent and disappointingly low rela-
spectively. These scores were correlated with tions between attitude and behavior.
the attitude scores, yielding a correlation Consider, for example, three studies on
coefficient of .39 (p < .01). cooperation and competition in the Prisoner's
To summarize briefly, the studies reviewed Dilemma. Gardin, Kaplan, Firestone, and
above obtained measures that corresponded Cowan (1973) asked male undergraduates
in their action elements but not in their to play SO trials of a Prisoner's Dilemma
target elements. Five of these studies reported game. The number of cooperative moves by
nonsignificant relations and five studies each subject served as one behavioral criter-
reported relatively low but significant ion. After the game, the subjects were asked
attitude-behavior relations. to be seated around a table with eight un-
occupied chairs. Each subject's physical dis-
Lack of Correspondence between Action tance from the other participant was recorded;
Elements this measure may be viewed as a second be-
havioral criterion. While seated at the table,
Individuals as targets. Most studies exhibit- the subjects completed a questionnaire that
ing lack of correspondence between action included two measures of attitude toward the
elements have measured attitudes toward a other person. Correlations were computed be-
person and have attempted to predict specific tween the two attitude measures and the two
behaviors with respect to, or in the presence behavioral criteria. All four attitude-behavior
of, that person. Various types of persons have correlations were below .20 and nonsignificant.
served as the targets of the attitudinal and Tornatzky and Geiwitz (1968) were some-
behavioral entities, including opponents in what more successful in their attempt to pre-
experimental games, interviewers, the experi- dict the behavior of male college students in
menter, confederates of the experimenter, the Prisoner's Dilemma game. These investi-
and simulated or fictitious persons. The gators used Byrne's (1961) similarity manip-
actions directed at these target persons have ulation to create favorable or unfavorable
included cooperation and competition (Gar- attitudes toward the partner in the game.
din, Kaplan, Firestone, & Cowan, 1973; A manipulation check indicated that the
Krauss, 1966; Oskamp & Perlman, 1966; desired difference in attitudes had been estab-
Tornatzky & Geiwitz, 1968); imitation, lished. The subjects then played the game for
modeling, and conformity (Baron, 1970; 18 trials. Tornatzky and Geiwitz found that
Greenberg & Frisch, 1972; Hendrick & Tay- players in the high-similarity condition made
lor, 1971; Sampson & Insko, 1964; Staple- significantly more cooperative choices than
ton, Nacci, & Tedeschi, 1973); prosocial or did subjects in the low-similarity condition.
helping behavior (Baron, 1971; Epstein & Note that the significant relation reported
Hornstein, 1969; Goodstadt, 1971; Nemeth, was between similarity and cooperation, not
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 897

between attitude and cooperation.9 Using due to either the kind of individual serving
Friedman's (1968) table, the magnitude of as the target, the type of action considered,
this effect is estimated to be .25. That is, even or the specific measurement procedures
assuming that similarity can be equated with employed.
attitude, the correlation between attitude and Organizations and institutions as targets.
behavior is relatively low. Lack of correspondence between action ele-
To further confound the relation between ments can also be found in studies that have
attitudes toward a target person and coop- attempted to predict specific behaviors to-
eration with that person in Prisoner's ward such targets as the church, public hous-
Dilemma games, Oskamp and Perlman ing, and the person's job or company, from
(1966) reported a significant linear relation attitudes toward these targets.
for one sample of subjects and a significant For example, numerous studies have been
curvilinear relation for another. The male conducted in attempts to predict absenteeism,
college students in this study played 30 trials turnover, tardiness, productivity, and other
of a Prisoner's Dilemma game against either work-related behaviors from various measures
their best friends, acquaintances, strangers, of job satisfaction or morale (e.g., Bernberg,
or disliked persons. A 7-point posttest mea- 19S2; Cherrington, Reitz, & Scott, 1971;
sure of liking for the other player was tri- Waters & Roach, 1971; Weitz & Nuckols,
chotomized. One group of subjects was 1953). Since several reviews of this literature
sampled at Pomona College, the other at are available (e.g., Brayfield & Crockett,
Claremont Men's College. Cooperation in- 1955; Porter & Steers, 1973; Schwab &
creased with liking only at Pomona College. Cummings, 1970; Vroom, 1964), no attempt
At Claremont, the most cooperation was is made to discuss studies of this kind in
exhibited by players with neutral attitudes detail, here or in subsequent sections. It is
toward their partners, and the least coopera- sufficient to note that the studies cited above,
tion by players with the most favorable as well as most other attempts to predict
attitudes. specific work-related actions from general
The three studies reviewed illustrate the measures of job satisfaction and morale, have
inconclusive findings obtained when measures met with little success.
of attitude toward a specific target individual Other studies concerned with work pro-
(without reference to any particular action) ductivity have measured attitudes toward
are used to predict a specific behavior with work in general (Friedlander & Greenberg,
respect to the target person. In fact, of the 1971), toward groups or training programs
23 studies cited above, 8 reported nonsig- (Sagi, Olmsted, & Atelsek, 1955; Webb &
nificant attitude-behavior relations (Ehrlich Hollander, 1956), and toward effort on the
& Graeven, 1971; Gardin et al., 1973; Gold- job (Graen, 1969), in attempts to predict
berg et al., 1969; Goldberg & Mettee, 1969; specific work-related behaviors. Studies of
Goodstadt, 1971; Hendrick & Taylor, 1971; this kind have also provided little evidence
Nemeth, 1970; Stapleton et al., 1973), 5 for a systematic relation between attitude and
reported significant attitude-behavior rela- behavior.
tions of low to moderate magnitude (Byrne Lack of correspondence in action elements
et al., 1970; Greenberg & Frisch, 1972; led to inconsistent findings in a study by
Krauss, 1966; Tornatzky & Geiwitz, 1968; Ostrom (1969). Students' attitudes toward
Worthy et al., 1969), and 10 studies obtained
inconsistent findings across different condi- 9
Although similarity was shown to have a signifi-
tions of a given experiment (Baron, 1970, cant effect on attitudes, the study does not provide
1971; Byrne et al., 1971; Epstein & Horn- direct evidence for a link between attitudes and
stein, 1969; Oskamp & Perlman, 1966; behavior. Studies of this kind are reviewed here
Regan, 1971; S. Rosen et al., 1973; Sampson only because the sole purpose of the similarity
manipulation was to create different degrees of
& Insko, 1964; Sapolsky, 1960; Schopler & attraction. Nevertheless, as far as the attitude-be-
Thompson, 1968). Examination of the pat- havior relation is concerned, results of such studies
tern of results reveals no systematic effects must be treated with caution.
898 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

the church were measured by means of 12 be used for seven different purposes, ranging
attitude scales (3 Thurstone scales, 3 Gutt- from use in "laboratory experiments where
man scales, 3 Likert scales, and 3 self-rating it will be seen only by professional sociol-
scales).10 Seven self-reports of behavior were ogists" to use in a "nation-wide publicity
elicited, including attendance of church serv- campaign advocating racial integration." By
ices, time spent in church-related activities, permitting the subject to sign any, all, or
and meditation outside formal church services. none of the release agreements, this measure
In addition, subjects could leave their names resulted in an 8-point behavioral index. Al-
and addresses if they were interested in at- though DeFleur and Westie concluded that
tending a discussion of the results of this their data revealed a great deal of inconsist-
research project and its contribution to the ency between attitudes and behavior, their
church in modem society. Thus, the criteria analysis showed a statistically significant
in this study were seven self-reported be- association (x~ = 7.264, p < .01). A second-
haviors and one behavioral commitment. A ary analysis of the data was performed to
total of 96 attitude-behavior correlations (12 obtain an estimate of the magnitude of this
attitude scales X 8 behavioral criteria) were relation. The phi coefficient was found to be
computed. Of these correlations, 37 (39%) .40, suggesting a correlation of moderate size.
were below .15 and not significant. The re- In a similar study, Green (1972) mea-
maining correlations, although significant, sured subjects' willingness to be photographed
were also very low. Only for self-reports of with blacks in a variety of situations and
church attendance did the attitude-behavior found a significant relation of comparable
correlations exceed .40. magnitude (rm = .40) between this criterion
Finally, rather than measuring attitudes, and attitudes toward blacks.
Bellin and Kriesberg (1967) assessed several Linn (1965) used a measure of attitude
specific beliefs about public housing and toward blacks as well as a measure of willing-
found them to be unrelated to whether or ness to be photographed with blacks to pre-
not the respondents actually applied for dict signing releases for interracial photo-
public housing in the course of a 3-year graphs. Although Linn reported no significant
period. relations, a secondary analysis showed that
Blacks as targets. In contrast with some of the attitude measure had a correlation of .29
the research reviewed under Lack oj Corre- (ns), whereas the commitment scale corre-
spondence, a few studies have measured lated .39 (p < .05) with the criterion.
attitudes toward blacks in general and have Significant but relatively low attitude-
used criteria that involved specific actions behavior relations were also reported by
with respect to hypothetical representatives Warner and DeFleur (1969) and Warner and
of this racial group (DeFleur & Westie, 1958; Dennis (1970). Warner and DeFleur mea-
Green, 1972; Linn, 1965; Warner & DeFleur, sured attitudes toward blacks by means of a
1969; Warner & Dennis, 1970). Attitudinal 16-item Likert scale. The sample of 537
and behavioral entities thus corresponded in college students was divided at the median
their target elements (blacks) but not in their attitude score. The behavioroid criterion was
action elements. Although the criteria in these each subject's signed indication of willingness
studies were intentions instead of behaviors, or refusal to perform a given behavior with
they are reviewed here because they were respect to blacks. These commitments were
explicitly designed to test the attitude- elicited by means of a letter sent to each
behavior relation. subject. Although a given subject received
The best known of these investigations was
the study by DeFleur and Westie (1958),
10
which measured attitudes toward blacks and One of the self-rating scales could be viewed as
used as its criterion a behavioroid measure a self-report of behavior, rather than as a measure
of attitude.
involving signed agreements to be photo- 11
Strictly speaking, the target of this behavior was
graphed with a black person of the opposite blacks of the opposite sex, whereas the attitudinal
sex.11 Subjects could allow the photograph to target was blacks in general.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 899

only one letter dealing with a single behavior, possible that actual behavior (e.g., actually
a total of eight behaviors were used for dif- posing for a photograph and releasing it)
ferent subjects. These behaviors ranged from would show a lower or nonsignificant relation
"making an anonymous contribution to a to attitude.
Negro educational charity" to "dating a
Negro." Conclusion
Although data for single behaviors were
not made available, the authors reported the The studies reviewed above reported rela-
number of students who failed to return the tions between attitudes and behavioral cri-
letter, as well as the number of students who teria whose entities corresponded only in one
returned signed agreements or signed refusals of their two major elements. Ten investiga-
to perform one of the eight behaviors. A sec- tions used measures that corresponded only
ondary analysis of these data resulted in a in terms of their action elements, but the
significant index of association (x2 = 8.68, majority of studies (37) corresponded in
p < .05). A phi coefficient of .13 indicates their target elements and failed to provide
that the relation between attitude and be- for correspondence in their action elements.
havior was of very low magnitude. Note, Our analysis of the nature of attitude-
however, that most subjects did not reply behavior relations suggests that partial corre-
to the letter. When only the subjects who spondence between attitudinal and behavioral
replied are considered, the phi coefficient is entities cannot be expected to yield consis-
found to be .26—somewhat higher but still tently high relationships. The review of
not very impressive. empirical research above supports this con-
As part of the same study, attitudes toward tention. Forty-seven studies conducted under
blacks were also measured by means of an partial correspondence were reviewed. Of
8-item Guttman scale (see Warner & Dennis, these studies, 17 reported nonsignificant rela-
1970). These attitude scores, dichotomized tions, IS reported significant relations of low
at the median, correlated .27 with the above to moderate magnitude, and the remaining IS
criterion when only subjects who replied studies obtained inconsistent results.
were considered.
The five studies reviewed in this section High Correspondence
all found relatively low but significant rela-
tions between attitude and behavior. These The research reviewed in this section per-
findings contrast with the generally nonsig- mits us to compare the low and inconclusive
nificant relations reported by other studies attitude-behavior relations found under lack
whose attitudinal and behavioral entities of correspondence and partial correspond-
failed to correspond in their action elements. ence with attitude-behavior relations under
One possible explanation is that the studies high correspondence. Some studies have ex-
reviewed above all used more than a single amined relations between attitudes toward
response to construct their criteria. In the behaviors and single-act criteria. Others have
studies by DeFleur and Westie, Green, and measured attitudes toward targets in an
Linn, subjects could sign a number of differ- attempt to predict multiple-act criteria. The
ent release agreements. Subjects in the remaining studies have investigated the rela-
Warner and DeFleur and Warner and Dennis tion between attitudes toward candidates or
studies could volunteer for only one activity, issues and voting or signing a petition for or
but the activities varied for different subjects. against the candidate or issue. As noted in
Behavioral criteria of this kind tend to pro- the introduction, the acts of voting or signing
vide some degree of generality with respect to a petition are viewed as behavioral criteria
the action element, thus leading to somewhat with unspecified action elements. They should
greater correspondence with the action-free therefore be related to attitudes toward the
attitudinal predictor. In addition, it must targets in question.
be recalled that all five studies used be- Our analysis suggests that high corre-
havioral commitments as their criteria. It is spondence between attitudinal and behavioral
900 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

entities will result in strong relations, pro- .36, and the remaining correlations ranged
vided that the investigator has in fact ob- from .54 to .82. The average correlation, after
tained measures of attitudes and behaviors, transformation to Fisher's Z, was .69.
as denned in this article. The first group of Although Janis and Hoffman (1970) and
studies reviewed below not only demonstrate Nisbett (1968) failed to report coefficients
high correspondence between attitudinal and of association, examination of their data
behavioral entities but also appear to have again reveals strong attitude-behavior rela-
employed appropriate measures of attitudes tions. Six months after participation in a
and behaviors. These studies should reveal program to reduce smoking, Janis and Hoff-
high attitude-behavior relations. The second man's subjects whose attitudes toward cig-
group of studies reviewed in this section used arette smoking were above the median, re-
predictor or criterion measures that, from our ported smoking 29 cigarettes per day, whereas
point of view, may be problematic. Inappro- subjects with attitudes below the median
priate measures of this kind tend to attenuate smoked only 9.4 cigarettes per day. Nisbett
the attitude-behavior relations. provided data concerning the relation between
attitudes toward the ice cream that subjects
Appropriate Measures had just eaten and the amount that they had
eaten. A secondary analysis of the plotted
Attitudes toward behaviors and single-act data revealed .a correlation coefficient of
criteria. An example of a study conforming about .80 for the total sample of 168 college
to our requirements is Veevers' (1971) in- students.
vestigation of drinking behavior, in which Attitudes toward targets and multiple-act
attitudes toward a behavior were used to criteria. Studies measuring attitudes toward
predict a single-act criterion. Residents of targets and multiple-act criteria have also
two Alberta communities reported the fre- obtained strong attitude-behavior relations.
quency of their drinking, and the amount For example, Bandura, Blanchard, and Rit-
and kind of alcoholic beverages consumed. ter (1969) obtained two measures of attitude
On the basis of these self-reports, 25 heavy toward snakes. The avoidance behavior of 48
drinkers, 25 light drinkers, and 25 abstainers male and female subjects was then recorded
were identified. Each respondent also com- with respect to a 29-item graded Guttman-
pleted five instruments measuring attitudes type scale involving various interactions with
toward drinking alcoholic beverages. Gamma a snake. These interactions ranged from "ap-
coefficients were computed to assess the proaching the snake in an enclosed glass cage"
degree of relationship between each attitude to "passively permitting the snake to crawl
measure and the self-reported behavior. The in one's lap." Following the behavior, atti-
five attitude measures were all significantly tudes were reassessed. Both attitude mea-
related to the behavioral criterion, the co- sures were found to predict the criterion with
efficients ranging from .46 to .72. a high degree of accuracy. When administered
Strong relations between attitudes toward prior to the behavior, the two attitude scales
actions and single-act criteria were also re- correlated .73 and .56 with the criterion, re-
ported by Janis and Hoffman (1970), Koth- spectively. Measured after the behavior, the
andapani (1971), and Nisbett (1968). corresponding correlations were .87 and .70.
Kothandapani investigated birth control prac- Similar results were found by Potter and
tices among 452 married black women. Atti- Klein (1957), with respect to the relation
tudes toward personal use of birth control
between maternal attitudes and behavior, and
methods were measured by means of 12 atti-
by Goodmonson and Glaudin (1971), in a
tude scales: 3 Thurstone scales, 3 Guttman
scales, 3 Likert scales, and 3 self-rating scales. study dealing with posthumous organ trans-
The criterion was the self-reported use or plants.
nonuse of birth control methods. All 12 atti- Voting behavior. Political scientists study-
tude-behavior correlations were significant ing the electoral process have collected ex-
and generally quite high. One correlation was tensive data on the determinants of voting
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 901

behavior. Although much of this work has associates attempted to predict voting for or
dealt with determinants other than attitude, against legalization of marijuana. As in the
evidence concerning the relationship between case of voting for a candidate, the action ele-
attitude and voting behavior has also been ment of this behavioroid criterion may be
reported. A complete review of this litera- viewed as unspecified. Albrecht, DeFleur, and
ture is beyond the scope of the present paper. Warner (1972) measured students' attitudes
The following discussion examines several toward legalization of marijuana by means
studies that have made an explicit attempt to of a standard Likert scale. In one condition
investigate the relationship between attitudes of the experiment, subjects could vote for or
and voting behavior. Since, as noted in the against legalization of marijuana by means of
introduction, attitude toward a candidate and a secret ballot. In a second condition, they
voting for or against that candidate may be could sign a petition supporting or opposing
viewed as corresponding in both target and legalization of marijuana. The two conditions
action elements, we would expect high atti- were combined, since they yielded comparable
tude-behavior correlations. results. A gamma coefficient of .71 attests to
In a widely cited series of surveys, A. the relatively high attitude-behavior relation.
Campbell, Converse, Miller, and Stokes Similar results were reported by Frideres,
(1960) obtained sizable correlations between Warner, and Albrecht (1971) and by Acock
attitudes toward candidates and voting be- and DeFleur (1972).
havior in the 1952 and 19S6 presidential Finally, using a somewhat different behav-
elections. Using a free-response format, re- ioral criterion, Kamenetsky, Burgess, and
spondents of voting age indicated what they Rowan (1956) also found a relatively strong
liked and what they disliked about each party attitude-behavior relation. Several measures
and about each candidate (Eisenhower & of attitude toward "the desirability of legis-
Stevenson). References to the candidates were lative measures to abolish discrimination
scored as favorable or unfavorable, and against Negroes in employment matters" were
summed. This measure of attitude toward obtained, and student subjects at the Univer-
each candidate correlated significantly with sity of Illinois could sign or refuse to sign a
self-reported voting. For example, in the 1956 petition "requesting Illinois congressmen to
election, attitude toward Eisenhower had a give their support to the passage of a Fair
correlation of .52 with voting choice. A de- Employment Practices Act during the next
tailed analysis of the attitude-behavior rela- session of congress." The attitude-behavior
tions can be found in A. Campbell and Stokes correlations were quite high and significant,
(1959). Using a similar measure of attitude, ranging from .54 to .61.
Repass (1971) reported a correlation of .60 To summarize briefly, the studies reviewed
with voting for Johnson or Goldwater in the above clearly indicate that significant atti-
1964 campaign. tude^behavior relations of considerable mag-
Strong attitude-behavior correlations were nitude can be obtained consistently by estab-
also reported by Fishbein and Coombs lishing high correspondence between attitudi-
(1974), who used attitudes toward Johnson nal and behavioral entities. A total of 14
and Goldwater to predict voting in the 1964 studies, judged to have used appropriate mea-
presidential election. Two measures of atti- sures of attitude and behavior, were reviewed.
tude were obtained, a 5-item evaluative se- Without exception these studies found strong
mantic differential and a more indirect 24- and significant attitude-behavior relations.
item scale based on Fishbein's (1963) sum-
mation theory of attitude. The two measures Questionable Measures
of attitude toward Goldwater correlated .70 The studies reviewed above were selected
and .73 with voting, respectively. Attitudes on the basis of considerations concerning the
toward Johnson provided correlations of .51 validity of their attitudinal and behavioral
and .72. measures. Six other studies with apparent
In a series of studies, DeFleur and his high attitude-behavior correspondence em-
902 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

ployed more questionable measures, especially approximately 80% of the respondents be-
with respect to their attitudinal predictors. haved in accordance with their intentions.
They are reviewed here because they were The study by Newton and Newton (1950),
designed to test the attitude-behavior relation however, had an additional problem. Women's
and because they illustrate some of the diffi- intentions to breast-feed their babies were
culties involved in establishing high corre- used to predict the degree to which they were
spondence between attitudinal and behavioral successful in doing so. Unfortunately, all
entities. mothers were instructed to breast-feed their
In one of these studies, Mitchell and babies, whether they intended to or not, and
Nebeker (1973) obtained two measures of success was defined in terms of the mother's
students' attitudes toward "time spent on ability to provide a sufficient quantity of
academic activities."12 The behavioral cri- milk. Clearly, this criterion was not com-
terion was a self-report of the average number pletely under the mothers' control. Despite
of hours per week that each subject had spent this problem, a secondary analysis revealed a
on academic activities during the preceding significant correlation of .48 between the
quarter. Superficially, it may appear that this mothers' intentions and their breast-feeding
criterion corresponded both in its target ele- behaviors.
ment and in its action element to the atti- Bowers (1968) used self-reports rather than
tudinal predictors. Although it is true that actual observations of behavior. Although we
the target (academic activities) was the have argued that self-reports can often sub-
same, there was an important difference be- stitute for actual behavior, their use in this
tween the attitudinal and behavioral action study is problematic, since the 10 behaviors
elements. The attitude measures asked sub- in question (destroying school property, tak-
jects to evaluate "spending time [on academic ing books from the library without properly
activities]," without specifying how much checking them out, etc.) were socially unde-
time. Subjects could obtain high attitude sirable and the validity of the self-reports is
scores if they had favorable evaluations of thus open to question. Nevertheless, a sec-
spending very little time, a moderate amount ondary analysis of the data revealed signifi-
of time, or a great deal of time on academic cant attitude-behavior correlations ranging
activities. The behavioral criterion, however, from .36 to .61.
was scored in terms of amount of time ac- The final two studies in this section (Ma-
tually spent; the only way to obtain a high zen & Leventhal, 1972; Winters, 1971) en-
behavioral score was to report spending many tailed methodological problems that make it
hours per week on academic activities. As impossible to reach any conclusions concern-
might be expected, the attitude-behavior cor- ing the attitude-behavior relation. For ex-
relations, although significant, were very low. ample, Mazen and Leventhal changed atti-
One attitude measure had a correlation of .27
with the criterion, the other measure a corre- 12
lation of .23. One of these measures ostensibly followed from
expectancy-value theory. Subjects were asked to
More obvious problems were encountered indicate the extent to which the time they spent on
in the remaining five investigations. Newton academic activities led to good grades. Evaluation of
and Newton (19SO) and Brannon, Cyphers, good grades was measured in terms of the expected
Hesse, Hesselbart, Keane, Schuman, Viccaro, consequences of obtaining good grades and the
subjective values of these consequences. The belief
and Wright (1973) measured intentions in- that time spent on academic activities led to good
stead of attitudes. We noted earlier that in- grades was multiplied with this evaluation of good
tentions are expected to predict correspond- grades. The product may be viewed as an incom-
plete measure of attitude toward spending time on
ing behaviors, and in fact, Brannon et al. academic activities. Expectancy theory would sug-
found a strong relation between intentions to gest that consequences other than obtaining good
vote for an open housing law and signing a grades (e.g., learning about interesting issues) need
to be taken into consideration when attitude toward
petition in support of such a law. Although spending time on academic activities is measured in
no coefficient of association was reported, this fashion.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 903

tudes by means of persuasive communica- of attitude that varied in the extent to which
tion, yet the correlations with subsequent they corresponded to the behavioral criterion.
behavior that were reported involved the pre- One or more attitude measures had no corre-
communication attitudes rather than the post- spondence with the criterion, whereas the re-
communication attitudes.13 As might be ex- maining attitudes corresponded in either the
pected, the correlations were relatively low, action element or the target element, but not
although they did reach statistical signifi- in both.
cance in some conditions. For example, in a series of three experi-
ments, Norman (197S) measured under-
Conclusion graduates' attitudes toward "volunteering as
a subject" and toward "acting as a subject in
The findings of studies with high cor- psychological research." The behavioral cri-
respondence between attitudinal and behav- terion was a measure of volunteering to par-
ioral entities must be judged in comparison ticipate in a given psychological experiment.
with the rather low and mostly nonsignificant It can be seen that the first measure of atti-
attitude-behavior relations obtained under tude corresponded to the criterion in its ac-
conditions of partial or complete lack of tion element (volunteering) but not in its
correspondence. When the target and action target element (psychological research in
elements of the attitudinal entity corre- general vs. the specific experiment-experi-
sponded to the target and action elements of menter combination). The second attitude
the behavioral entity, attitude-behavior cor- measure failed to correspond to the criterion,
relations were found to be quite high and sig- not only in its target element but also in its
nificant. This was often true, even when mea- action element, since acting as a subject is
sures of doubtful validity were employed. very different from volunteering to be a sub-
Although these conclusions are clearly con- ject.
sistent with our analysis of the relations be- Our analysis suggests that the first atti-
tween attitudes and behaviors, they were not tude measure, which partly corresponded to
based on direct comparisons of the effects the criterion, should have yielded higher atti-
produced by variations in degree of corre- tude-behavior correlations than the second
spondence. The studies reviewed in the fol- attitude measure. The results in all three
lowing section employed multiple measures of studies supported this analysis. The average
attitudes or behaviors, and they did, inten- correlation across studies was .40 under par-
tionally or unintentionally, provide the data tial correspondence and .21 under lack of
needed to make the appropriate comparisons. correspondence. The same pattern of results
was obtained in another study dealing with
Comparisons: Varying Degrees of volunteering to participate in a psychological
Correspondence experiment (Wicker & Pomazal, 1971), al-
though the correlations obtained were of
The advantage of comparing attitude- lower magnitude.
behavior relations under varying degrees of Fischer (1971) asked undergraduates to
correspondence within a given study is that indicate on a 4-point scale their interest in
other potentially relevant factors are held joining the "College Student Companion
constant. Differences in the magnitude of the Program" at Connecticut Valley Hospital.
relations between attitude and behavior can Since it was made clear to the subjects that
then be attributed with confidence to the they would be contacted later if they ex-
differences in degree of correspondence. pressed some interest, this scale may be
viewed as a behavioroid measure involving a
Partial Correspondence Versus Lack of
Correspondence is Mazen and Leventhal also reported correla-
tions between attitude change and the behavioral
In the eight studies reviewed in this sec- ciiteria. This analysis again provides little informa-
tion, investigators obtained several measures tion about the attitude-behavior relation.
904 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

relatively low degree of commitment. Atti- the predictor whose action element corre-
tudes had been measured several weeks prior sponded to that of the criterion was not a
to the recruitment by means of a 68-item measure of attitude. Rokeach and Kliejunas
Likert-type scale. A factor analysis of the 68 measured students' attitudes toward specific
items resulted in four subscales. Two of these instructors in five psychology courses and
scales were related to helping (one was la- toward psychology instructors in general (by
beled "helping," the other "social responsibil- averaging the specific ratings). In addition,
ity"), and the remaining two scales mea- they assessed the importance of attending
sured "sex progressivism" and "attitude class in general and treated these ratings as
toward criminals." another measure of attitude. From our point
It can be seen that the last two attitude of view, however, this was not an appropriate
measures lacked correspondence with the be- measure of attitude, since ratings of impor-
havioral criterion, both in terms of the tar- tance are not expressions of evaluation.
get and the action elements. The first two If we disregard this problem, we can turn
attitude measures involved the same action as to an examination of the correspondence be-
the behavior (volunteering to help), but they tween the predictors and the behavioral cri-
differed in their target elements (the hos- teria. Subjects were asked to report, for each
pital's companion program for the behavior course, the percentage of classes they had cut
vs. no specific target for the attitudes). for reasons other than illness or accident, in
The two measures of attitude toward help- each of the psychology courses they had
ing each had a correlation of .36 (p < .01) taken. A more general measure of cutting
with the partially corresponding behavioral psychology classes was obtained by comput-
criterion, and as would be expected, these ing the average percentage. The behavioral
correlations were significantly higher than criteria thus involved cutting either specific
those obtained for the remaining two atti- psychology classes or psychology classes in
tude measures (rs — .15 and .21). Much the general. Clearly, the attitudes toward specific
same results were reported by Hornstein, instructors and toward instructors in general
Masor, Sole, and Heilman (1971), in another had little or no correspondence with these
study of helping behavior, and by B. Rosen criteria. The importance of attending class in
and Komorita (1971), who attempted to pre- general corresponded to the criteria in terms
dict a behavioroid measure of commitment to of its action element (attending class) but
attend a discussion on the "current war on not its target element (no reference to psy-
poverty." u chology classes).
Carr and Roberts (196S) obtained 29 mea- Consistent with this analysis, the correla-
sures of race-related attitudes and two self- tion between attitude toward psychology in-
reports of participation in civil rights activi- structors and the general measure of cutting
ties. Only two of the 29 attitude measures psychology classes (lack of correspondence)
corresponded at least in part to the behav- was .20 and not significant, whereas the mea-
ioral criterion—one in its target element, the sure of importance (partial correspondence)
other in its action element. Of the 58 corre- had a significant correlation of .46 with this
lations between attitude and behavior, none criterion. Considering each of the five courses
were higher than .30 and about 80% of the separately, neither the attitude toward the
correlations were below .15. Interestingly, instructor of a given class nor the measure of
however, most of the significant predictions the importance of attending class in general
were made by the two attitude measures
whose entities corresponded in part to the
"The B. Rosen and Komorita study entailed
entities of the two behavioral criteria. some measurement problems, since of the five pre-
A study by Rokeach and Kliejunas (1972) dictors, only one was a clear measure of attitude.
also permits comparisons between results ob- Despite this problem, the correlations between the
predictors and the criterion were .35, .39, and .59
tained under partial correspondence and un- under partial correspondence and .03 and .08 under
der lack of correspondence. Unfortunately, lack of correspondence.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 905

was systematically related to class attendance. their magnitude under partial or low corre-
However, with respect to four of the five com- spondence. High correspondence between
parisons, the partially corresponding impor- attitudinal and behavioral entities should, of
tance measure was the better predictor of course, produce stronger relations than should
behavior. partial or low correspondence. As usual, some
One final study (Brislin & Olmsted, Note of the studies designed to test the relation
1) that permits a comparison of partial cor- between attitude and behavior are difficult to
respondence with lack of correspondence in- interpret, due to problems of measurement or
volved a behavioral criterion of questionable general methodology. A review of these stud-
validity. The criterion was the percentage of ies is followed by a discussion of studies with
phosphate contained in the laundry detergent appropriate measures and procedures.
used by 132 persons in a public launderette. Questionable measures. Among the studies
The phosphate content was obtained from a that permit comparisons between high and
government publication. Since it did not ap- partial or low correspondence, five employed
pear on the boxes, it is likely that most sub- measures of questionable validity. In two
jects were unaware of the percentage of phos- studies (Heberlein & Black, 1976; Silverman
phate contained in their detergents. Conse- & Cochrane, 1971) the problem relates to the
quently, the semantic-differential measure of attitudinal predictor; in the remaining three
attitude toward low-phosphate laundry de- studies (Bruvold, 1972b; Liska, 1974a;
tergents obtained by the investigators must Mann, 1959) problematic criteria were em-
be viewed as lacking correspondence with the ployed.
behavioral criterion in target as well as action Heberlein and Black (1976) designed a
elements. Four other attitude measures dealt study that varied degree of correspondence,
with using low phosphate detergents. These but some of their predictors were beliefs
measures corresponded to the behavior in rather than attitudes. The behavioral criterion
their action elements but again not in their was essentially an index of whether a person
target elements. Attitude-behavior correla- did or did not purchase lead-free gasoline.
tions were significant, but there were no Various predictors were used, ranging from
systematic differences under partial corre- a general attitude toward the environment,
spondence and lack of correspondence. The through attitudes toward air pollution and
correlation of the semantic differential mea- toward lead-free gasoline, to a general com-
sure with behavior was .34; the remaining mitment to use lead-free gasoline. Finally, a
attitude-behavior correlations ranged from single item was used to measure the respon-
.18 to .38. dent's felt obligation to purchase lead-free
To summarize briefly, relations between gasoline.
attitudes and behaviors under partial corre- It can be seen that the attitudes toward
spondence tend to be somewhat stronger than the environment and toward air pollution
relations under low correspondence between lacked any correspondence with the criterion;
attitudinal and behavioral entities. Even the measures referring to lead-free gasoline
under partial correspondence, however, the corresponded to the criterion in their target
relations tend to be of low magnitude. elements but not in their action elements;
and the remaining predictors, although not
measures of attitude, corresponded highly
High Versus Partial or Low Correspondence with the criterion. As might be expected, the
prediction of behavior became more accurate
Of all studies concerning the attitude-be-
as degree of correspondence increased. The
havior relation, the investigations reviewed
in this final section are most directly rele- correlations ranged from .12 to .21 under
vant to our conceptual analysis. In this sec- lack of correspondence, from .36 to .39 under
tion we review studies that permit a com- partial correspondence, and from .SO to .59
parison of the magnitude of attitude-behavior for high correspondence.
relations under high correspondence with Stronger relations under high correspon-
906 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

dence (rs — .63 and .58) than under partial correlated .47 with the multiple-act criterion.
correspondence (rs = .38 and .26) were also In contrast, under partial or low correspon-
reported by Silverman and Cochrane (1971), dence, correlations were very low (r < .20)
who used measures of intention (rather than and mostly nonsignificant.
attitude) to predict the signing of two peti- Bruvold (1972b) interviewed 99 residents
tions dealing with open housing. in an area near three recreational facilities
In the remaining studies, problems were (a golf course, a park, and a swimming pool)
related to the behavioral criterion rather than supplied with reclaimed water. The criterion
to the measure of attitude. For example, was whether a given facility had been used
Liska (1974a) obtained self-reports of eight by the respondent or members of his family
cheating behaviors that involved receiving with knowledge that the facility was supplied
help from others during the preceding se- with reclaimed water. In addition to these
mester. The 183 college students who served three single-act criteria—one for each facil-
as subjects responded to a S-point scale (don't ity—Bruvold also obtained a multiple-act cri-
remember, never, once, twice, three or more terion by computing the sum over the three
times) with respect to such behaviors as "got scores. Attitudes toward "reclaimed water for
help on a term paper from another student" noningestive, close-contact use" were mea-
and "secured a copy of an old examination." sured by means of a Thurstone scale.
In addition to these eight single-act criteria, Two problems are associated with the be-
a multiple-act criterion was obtained by havioral criteria in this study. First, a person
summing over the eight behavioral self- was classified as performing the behavior in
reports. question when a member of his or her family
Two problems concerning these measures made use of a facility, even though the per-
of behavior must be noted. First, it may be son did not. Second, use of a facility without
inappropriate to assign the lowest behavioral knowledge of its water supply resulted in a
score to the "don't remember" category. Sec- negative behavioral classification, even though
ond, the use of self-reports in this situation the person might have used it in any case.
must again be viewed with caution, since the Users and nonusers of a given facility were
behaviors in question had low social desirabil- not found to differ significantly in their atti-
ity. tudes. These negative results would be ex-
Attitudes toward each of the eight behav- pected, since the attitudinal target (reclaimed
iors were measured by means of a S-point water) differed greatly from the behavioral
approve-disapprove scale.15 Attitudes toward targets (the specific facilities) for the three
the target of "using others to prepare for and single-act criteria (see also Bruvold, 1972a).
take examinations" were obtained by sum- The multiple-act criterion, however, corre-
ming over the eight single scales. In addition, sponded to the attitude in its target element
S-item scales were used to measure attitudes (nonspecific) and its action element (use of
toward cheating in college, intellectualism, reclaimed water). A significant linear trend
and honesty. was found, indicating a significant relation
The eight attitudes toward a behavior cor- between attitude and the corresponding be-
responded in both target and action to the havioral criterion. According to Friedman's
eight single-act criteria. High correspondence (1968) table, the magnitude of this relation
also existed between the attitude toward "us- is about .35. Using similar measures of atti-
ing others" and the multiple-act criterion. All tude and behavior, Bruvold (1973) again re-
other combinations of attitudinal and behav-
ioral measures corresponded only in part or 16
This suggests a methodological problem, since
not at all. This analysis is supported by the many subjects may have had little opportunity to
data. Under high correspondence, all atti- perform a given behavior during the preceding
tude-behavior correlations were significant. semester, even if they had favorable attitudes toward
that behavior. Failure to take into account lack of
For single behaviors, the correlations ranged opportunity will tend to reduce the attitude-be-
from .21 to .43; the attitude toward a target havior correlations.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 907

ported a significant but low correlation (r — by Weinstein's (1972) attempt to predict


.30). signing a petition from three measures of
Finally, Mann (1959) failed to obtain any attitude. The petition read, "I ... am op-
clear measure of behavior. Black and white posed to the quarter system and recommend
subjects interacted in six-person groups. an alternative system" (p. 3S7). Attitudes
Afterward, each subject ranked the other were assessed by means of a 7-item evalua-
group members in terms of their degree of tive semantic differential. The concepts rated
racial prejudice. The subject's average rank were "signing a petition protesting the quar-
served as the criterion. Clearly, this measure ter system," "the quarter system," and "sign-
is best viewed as an assessment of racial atti- ing a protest petition." Ninety-four under-
tude based on peer ratings, not as a behavior. graduate college students served as subjects.
The patriotism subscale of the Ethnocentrism The target and action elements of the first
Scale was used as a general measure of preju- attitude corresponded to those of the behavior.
dice. In addition, attitudes toward members The second attitude measure may also be
of the other race were assessed by asking viewed as corresponding highly to the cri-
subjects to rank the group member in terms terion. Although it did not make explicit
of friendship choice. The average rank of reference to signing a petition, it did represent
own-race members was divided by the average an expression of a position concerning the
rank of the other-race members. It can be quarter system, and as noted earlier, signing
seen that only the general prejudice measure a petition may also be considered as an ex-
corresponded to the criterion in both its tar- pression of a position on an issue. Neverthe-
get and action elements.10 Its correlation with less, the person's attitude toward "signing a
the criterion was relatively high ( r = . S l ) petition opposing the quarter system" is a
and significant. The second measure of atti- more appropriate attitudinal predictor. The
tude had a lower, though still significant, re- third attitude measure corresponded to the
lation to the criterion (r = .22). criterion only in terms of its action element;
In sum, although the five studies reviewed its target element (protest petition) differed
above employed measures of attitude or be- considerably from that of the behavior (a
havior that do not meet the requirements set petition opposing the quarter system).
forth in this article, the reported attitude- Consistent with these considerations, the
behavior relations were in line with expecta- attitude toward "signing a petition protesting
tions. To be sure, the methodological diffi- the quarter system" yielded the best predic-
culties resulted in relatively low relations, tion of behavior (r — .69), although the atti-
even under conditions of high correspon- tude toward "the quarter system" also re-
dence. However, these relations were signifi- sulted in a significant correlation (r = .52).
cant and consistently higher than the rela- The third attitude did not correlate signifi-
tions obtained under low or partial corre- cantly with the behavior (r = .22).
spondence. Fishbein, Thomas, and Jaccard (Note 2)
Appropriate measures. The remaining sec- compared the predictive validity of an atti-
tion is devoted to comparisons of attitude- tude toward a target with an attitude toward
behavior relations under high correspondence voting for the target. Although, as we saw
with relations under partial or low correspon- earlier, high correlations between attitudes
dence, in studies that have obtained adequate toward candidates and voting behavior are
measures of attitudes and behaviors. The 10 obtained under the electoral system in the
studies reviewed in this section all dealt ex- United States (where voting for the most
plicitly with the attitude-behavior relation, preferred candidate is a reasonable voting
and they were all designed to compare cor- strategy), this is not necessarily the case in
relations between behavior and different mea-
sures of attitude or between attitude and 16
Note that the Ethnocentrism Scale is actually
different behavioral criteria. a measure of personality, although it is usually found
The first type of comparison is exemplified to correlate with racial prejudice.
908 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

other democratic elections. For example, in ligion did not correlate significantly with the
Great Britain, voting for the most preferred criterion (r — .18).
candidate may contribute to the formation of Finally, 270 college students expressed
a government by the least preferred party their attitudes toward "blood donation" and
under the leadership of a disliked prime toward "donating blood at the upcoming
minister. Voting for a given candidate in blood drive." The correlations of these atti-
Great Britain may thus be an indication of tudes with whether or not the respondents
the person's attitude toward the party rather actually donated blood at the blood drive in
than toward the candidate. This implies that question were .30 and .43, respectively.17 All
in Great Britain, the attitude toward voting three studies thus support the claim that the
for a given candidate would correlate more magnitude of an attitude-behavior correla-
highly with actual voting for that candidate tion increases with correspondence between
than would the attitude toward the candidate. attitudinal and behavioral entities.
Fishbein, Thomas, and Jaccard obtained Weigel, Vernon, and Tognacci (1974) also
measures of attitudes toward 12 candidates varied the specificity of the attitudinal pre-
(3 in each of 4 constituencies) in the October dictors and observed the effects on their
1974 general election in Great Britain. In relations to behavior. The subjects were 113
addition, measures of attitude toward voting residents of a medium-sized western city who
for each of the 12 candidates were obtained. were included in a survey of attitudes toward
Consistent with expectations, the average cor- a variety of environmental issues. Four atti-
relation between attitude toward a candidate tude scales were of interest to the study. The
and voting behavior was .51 ( p < . Q l ) , first (^i) was a measure of attitude toward
whereas the average correlation between atti- the Sierra Club (an organization concerned
tude toward voting for the candidate and with such issues as conservation of natural
voting behavior was .85 (p < .01). resources and pollution control). The second
Jaccard, King, and Pomazal (Note 3) re- attitude measure (^2) was a 15-item Likert
ported comparable findings for the prediction scale concerning conservation of natural re-
of three different behavioral criteria. In one sources. Attitudes toward pollution control
study, 270 women indicated their attitudes (A3) were also measured by a 15-item Likert
toward "birth control," toward "birth con- scale. Finally, attitudes toward the attain-
trol pills," and toward "using birth control ment of ecological goals (A*) were assessed
pills." The behavioral criterion was the self- by means of eight 5-point scales, each of
report of current use or nonuse of birth con- which evaluated the satisfaction derived from
trol pills. Consistent with the degree of cor- attaining a specific goal (e.g., freedom from
respondence, the criterion was predicted best overcrowding, being able to live in harmony
(r — .65) from the attitude toward the act of with nature).
using birth control pills, and with least ac- The criterion measure was a 4-step be-
curacy (r = .16) from the general attitude havioral scale concerning the Sierra Club.
toward birth control. The attitude toward
birth control pills had an intermediate corre-
17
lation with the criterion (r = .34). The relatively low correlation under high cor-
In a second study, 49 students in an intro- respondence is explained in part by the fact that
ductory speech class reported whether or not many subjects who appeared to donate blood were
rejected for medical reasons or because of over-
they had attended church on a given Sunday. crowding (Pomazal & Jaccard, 1976). If showing up
Attitudes were assessed toward "religion," to donate blood had been used as the behavioral
toward the "church," and toward "attending criterion, the correlation would have increased con-
siderably. In fact, intentions to donate blood at the
church this Sunday." Again, attitude toward upcoming blood drive were found to have a corre-
the action was the best predictor of actual lation of .46 with actual blood donations, and a
church attendance (r = .65), attitude toward correlation of .59 with showing up to donate blood
(Pomazal & Jaccard, 1976). Although not reported,
the church yielded an intermediate correlation we would expect a similar increment for the attitude-
of .47, and the general attitude toward re- behavior correlation.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 909

This measure was obtained 5 months after The attitude toward the other player corre-
the assessment of attitudes. The lowest level sponded to the behavior in its target element,
on this scale consisted of refusing to have any but it made no reference to a specific action.
further contact with the Sierra Club. The re- As would be expected, the attitude-behavior
maining 3 levels ranged from agreeing to be correlations strongly reflected this difference
on the club's mailing list to becoming a club in correspondence. Looking at the three
member. It can be seen that this behavioral games played in the two experiments, actual
criterion had the Sierra Club as its target and choice of cooperative moves correlated .63,
that it represented a generalization across .70, and .65 (p < .01 in each case) with
different behaviors. As such, it corresponded attitude toward choosing the cooperative
highly only to A\, the attitude toward the strategy. In contrast, the correlations between
Sierra Club. A2, Aa, and At corresponded to attitude toward the other player and coopera-
the behavior in their action elements (unspec- tive game behavior were very low and incon-
ified) but not in their target elements. The sistent (r = .26, p < .05; r = .09, ns; r =
target elements of A-2 and AH, however, seem .27, p < .05, respectively).
more relevant to the Sierra Club (the target Similar results were reported by Ajzen and
of the behavior) than does the target element Fishbein (1974), whose student subjects, in
of At. groups of three, were required to coordinate
Consistent with this analysis, Weigel, Ver- their behaviors on an experimental task.
non, and Tognacci (1974) reported a corre- Attitudes toward sending instructions to each
lation of .60 between Al and the behavioral of the two co-workers correlated .60 (p <
criterion. The correlations of A2 and As with .01) with actual communicative behavior, and
behavior, although significant, were much attitudes toward complying with received
lower (rs = .37 and .38, respectively). A instructions correlated .57 (p < .01) with
nonsignificant coefficient of .16 was obtained actual compliance. In marked contrast, atti-
for the relation between A\ and behavior. tudes toward the co-workers (partial corre-
In a series of investigations, Ajzen (1971) spondence) predicted the two criteria .02 (not
and Ajzen and Fishbein (1970, 1974) com- significant) and .19 (p < .05), respectively.18
pared the predictive power of different atti- The studies reviewed thus far attempted
tude measures. Ajzen and Fishbein (1970) to predict a given behavioral criterion from
and Ajzen (1971) attempted to predict co- different kinds of attitudinal predictors. The
operative behavior in Prisoner's Dilemma results clearly show that high relations are
games. The subjects in the two studies were obtained only when the attitudinal entity
pairs of same-sex college students who played corresponds to the behavioral entity both in
three Prisoner's Dilemma games that varied terms of its target element and in terms of
in their payoff matrices. Following a few its action element. Three other studies held
practice trials, the players were asked to constant the nature of the attitudinal predic-
complete a questionnaire that included two tor and varied the behavioral criterion. The
semantic-differential measures of attitude results again reflect the different degrees of
comprised of four or five bipolar evaluative correspondence created in this fashion.
scales. These scales were used to obtain mea- Tittle and Hill (1967) reported a study of
sures of attitude toward choosing the co- political activity among a sample of 301
operative strategy and of attitude toward the college students. Attitudes toward involve-
other player. The proportion of cooperative ment in student political activities were mea-
strategy choices following completion of the sured by means of six different, but inter-
questionnaire served as the behavioral correlated, instruments: A 15-item Thurstone
criterion. scale; a Likert scale based on the same 15
The target and action elements of the
behavioral entity corresponded to the target "Differences were computed between the behav-
iors with respect to the two co-workers, and the
and action elements of the first attitude same procedure was followed for the measures of
(toward choosing the cooperative strategy). attitude.
910 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

items; a 10-item Likert scale; the sum over obtained under partial correspondence, the
the evaluations of five political activities on difference was not very impressive. The high
semantic differentials; a 10-item Guttman correlations between the two types of criteria
scale; and a self-report of attitude toward may be at least in part responsible for this
student politics. finding.
Five behavioral criteria were constructed. Much more convincing results were re-
By inspecting the voting records, it was pos- ported by Fishbein and Ajzen (1974) in an
sible to determine whether each subject had investigation of religious behavior. Sixty-two
voted in the last student election ( B i ) . The undergraduate college students were given a
second criterion (Bz) was the self-reported list of 100 behaviors dealing with matters of
frequency of voting in the last four elections. religion and were asked to check the be-
A Guttman scale based on the self-reported haviors they had performed. These behaviors
frequency of eight political activities served included praying before and after meals,
as the third behavioral criterion ( B s ) , Self- taking a religious course for credit, and
reported frequencies of 10 political activities dating a person against parents' wishes. Each
were scored in a Likert fashion (Bi). The response constituted a single-act criterion. In
final criterion (B5) was an index based on addition, four multiple-act criteria were con-
the self-reported frequency of five activities, structed on the basis of multiple behaviors.
scored as suggested by Woodward and Roper The first was simply the sum of the total set
(1950). of 100 behaviors, taking into account the
Tittle and Hill (1967) intended the first evaluation of religiosity implied by each be-
two criteria to reflect specific behaviors havior. A second index was a Guttman scale
(single-act criteria), whereas the remaining of a subset of eight behaviors. The third and
three indices were designed to represent gen- fourth indices were Likert and Thurstone
eral behavioral patterns (multiple-act cri- scales based on 20 and 13 behaviors,
teria). From our point of view, B3> Bt, and respectively.
B5 corresponded to the attitude measures in Attitudes toward religion were assessed by
both their target and action elements. BI and means of five scales that were highly inter-
B2 corresponded to the target element of the correlated. The measures were a self-report
attitudes, but not to their action element, of religiosity, a semantic-differential evalu-
since the attitude measures were not action ation of "being religious," and three standard
specific. Nevertheless, voting in student elec- scales measuring religiosity: a Likert scale,
tions may be one of the major expressions of a Guttman scale, and a Thurstone scale.
political activism for many students. If so, It can be seen that all multiple-act criteria
.Bi and J52 might correlate highly with B3, corresponded both in their targets (religion)
B4, and J35, and great differences in attitude- and in their action elements (unspecified), to
behavior correlations could hardly be the general measures of attitudes toward
expected. religion. In contrast, the 100 single-act criteria
Inspection of the data reveals that this corresponded to the attitudinal entities in
was indeed the case. The correlations between terms of their targets but not in terms of
fii and B2 on the one hand and Ba, B±, and their action elements. We would thus expect
-BB on the other ranged from .56 to .79 (all the behavioral indices to be highly related to
highly significant). The relations between the the different attitude scales, and we should
six attitudinal predictors and the first two find relatively low and inconsistent relations
(partially corresponding) criteria were of between attitudes and the 100 single
moderate size; the average gamma coefficient behaviors.
was .37, The average gamma for the predic- The results of the study strongly supported
tion of Bs, £4, and Bs from the six attitude these expectations. Correlations between
scales was .48. Although the average coefficient single behaviors and attitudes were quite low;
obtained under high correspondence was the average correlations for the five attitude
indeed greater than the average coefficient scales ranged from .12 to .15. In contrast, the
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 911

correlations with the behavioral indices were behavior relations tend to be attenuated
high and significant. The range of attitude- whenever inappropriate measures of attitude
behavior correlations under these conditions or behavior are obtained. Such problems can
of high correspondence was from .45 to .75, be avoided by using standard scaling proce-
and the average correlation was .63. dures, both with respect to measures of atti-
It could be argued that one weakness of tude and with respect to behavioral criteria
this investigation was its reliance on self- consisting of more than a single behavior.
reports (cf. Schuman & Johnson, 1976). A Whenever adequate precautions have been
recent study by Weigel and Newman (1976), taken, high correspondence has been found
however, showed the same pattern of results to produce relatively strong relations between
for observations of actual behavior. These attitude and behavior.
investigators used a 16-item Likert scale to
measure attitudes toward protecting environ- Ambiguity in Correspondence
mental quality. A total of 14 behavioral
observations were obtained 3 to 8 months We should note at this point that it may
later. The behaviors involved signing and cir- sometimes be quite difficult to determine
culating three different petitions concerning correspondence in a given instance. What, for
environmental issues, participating in a litter example, is the target of donating money to
pick-up program, and participating in a re- the Heart Fund if the money is collected by
cycling program on eight separate occasions. a neighbor? Is it the Heart Fund, the neigh-
In addition to these 14 single-act criteria, bor, or both? Alternatively, the Heart Fund
Weigel and Newman constructed four might be viewed as the target, and the neigh-
multiple-act indices: one based on petition bor as part of the context. In fact, the reader
signing behaviors, one on litter pick-ups, one may occasionally have disagreed with our
on recycling, and one overall index based on interpretation of correspondence in the stud-
all 14 single behaviors. ies reviewed. With respect to a number of
Consistent with our analysis, attitude- studies, we found it all but impossible to
behavior correlations increased as the action make a decision and we therefore did not
element of the criterion increased in its degree include them in our review.
of correspondence with the unspecified action Consider, for example, the study by Hoi-
element of the attitude. The average correla- man (1956), which examined attendance at
tion between attitude and the 14 single be- football games among 256 students at the
haviors was not significant (r = .29), the University of Southern California. The sub-
average correlation with the three behavioral jects were given a 12-item questionnaire
indices of intermediate generality was .42 designed to measure "attitude toward football
(p < .01), and under the highest degree games." However, one of the items Holman
of correspondence the correlation with the provided as an example was, "How would you
overall index was .62 (p < .01). describe attendance at football games? (check
one): very worthwhile, worthwhile, not very
Conclusion worthwhile, worthless." Clearly, a subject may
have a favorable attitude toward football
Studies permitting comparisons between games but an unfavorable attitude toward
different levels of correspondence have gen- attending football games. It is not clear
erally been supportive of our analysis of the whether the questionnaire measured only the
attitude-behavior relation. The relations be- latter attitude or a combinaion of both. The
tween attitude and behavior tend to increase behavioral criteria were self-reports of attend-
in magnitude as the attitudinal and be- ance following each of seven football games,
havioral entities come to correspond more as well as the sum of these seven scores.
closely in terms of their target and action Depending on our interpretation of the atti-
elements. This is especially true when we tude measure, the attitudinal and behavioral
compare partial or low correspondence with entities may or may not have corresponded
high correspondence. Of course, attitude- in their action elements (attending football
912 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

games). There was clearly little correspond- A woman who is generally opposed to the use
ence of target elements, since attitudes were of birth control methods may favor their use
measured with respect to football games in in her own case for health-related reasons.
general, whereas the behavior was directed at Conversely, a woman who holds a favorable
the local home games. The attitude-behavior attitude toward using birth control methods
correlations were of low to moderate magni- in general may have a negative attitude
tude, ranging from .21 to .59. toward using such methods herself if, for
Relatively low and inconclusive findings example, she wants to have a child. Consistent
were also reported by Izzett (1971), O'Keefe with this argument, Insko et al. reported sig-
(1971), Wicker (1971), Hackman and Porter nificant but relatively low correlations be-
(1968), and Bostrom (1970). As in Holman's tween their two attitude scales and their
study, the correspondence between attitudinal behavioral criterion (rs = .29 and .25).
and behavioral entities in these investigations Similar considerations apply when measur-
was ambiguous. For example, Wicker (1971) ing attitudes toward smoking or drinking, as
reported correlations between 13 different opposed to attitudes toward my smoking or
predictors and 4 behavioral criteria. Many of my drinking. To ensure correspondence, it
these measures were indices based on a num- may be necessary to make the personal
ber of responses combined in complicated reference of the attitude measure as explicit
fashions. As a result, it is difficult to identify as possible.
their target and action elements. The attitude-
behavior correlations were low, ranging from General Discussion
—.01 to .45. However, the measures that
appeared to correspond most closely to the The view that attitude is the most distinc-
criterion were found to provide the most tive and indispensible concept in social psy-
accurate predictions (for a detailed analysis, chology (Allport, 1935) has recently given
see Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975, chapter 8). way to a more skeptical view of the concept's
One other issue deserves comment in this utility. The skepticism is in large part due
context. Attitudes toward actions, even when to the disappointing results of studies at-
they correspond to the criterion in their tar- tempting to make use of attitudes to predict
get and action elements, rarely make explicit overt behavior. Although these attempts have
reference to the person's own performance of occasionally been successful, most have pro-
the behavior under consideration. For ex- duced rather low and nonsignificant attitude-
ample, like Kothandapani (1971), Insko, behavior relations. It is thus hardly surpris-
Blake, Cialdini, and Mulaik (1970) at- ing that the attitude concept has come under
tempted to predict use of birth control increasingly strong criticism.
methods among black and white women The present article has attempted to show
between 15 and 44 years of age. Two mea- that the disenchantment with the attitude
sures of attitudes toward "using birth control concept may be unwarranted. In fact, we have
methods" were obained. The behavioral cri- seen that the findings concerning the relation
terion was the self-reported frequency of use between attitude and behavior only appear to
of birth control methods. be inconsistent. A person's attitude has a con-
Whereas the attitudinal entity in this study sistently strong relation with his or her be-
dealt with the general use of birth control havior when it is directed at the same target
methods, the behavioral entity referred to and when it involves the same action. Gen-
the respondent's use of such methods.19 erally low and inconsistent relations are ob-
Although the distinction between attitude served when the attitudinal and behavioral
toward performing a behavior in general and entities fail to correspond in one or both of
attitude toward my performing the same
behavior may often be of little importance, 19
Recall that Kothandapani (1971) did in fact
there is reason to believe that this distinction measure attitudes toward personal use of birth con-
cannot be neglected in the present instance. trol methods.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 913

these elements. Table 1 shows the effect of Table 1


correspondence on the magnitude of attitude- Effect of Correspondence on the
behavior relations. Attitude-Behavior Relation
Of the 109 investigations reviewed in this
article, 84 displayed either low, partial, or Attitude-behavior
relation
high correspondence between attitudinal and
behavioral entities. The remaining 25 studies Low"
reported relations under two or three differ- Not or
ent degrees of correspondence. Consequently, signifi- incon-
Correspondence cant sistent High b
a total of 142 attitude-behavior relations are
reported in Table 1. It can be seen that low Low 26 1 0
correspondence produces mostly nonsignifi- Partial 20 47 4
cant attitude-behavior relations. Partial cor- High
respondence tends to result in highly incon- Questionable measures 0 9 9
Appropriate measures 0 0 26
sistent findings. The majority of studies in
this category obtained either low but signifi- Note, The cell entries represent the number of atti-
cant relations, or inconsistent results across tude-behavior relations.
0
different conditions and measures. The re- r < .40.
b
r > .40.
maining studies reported mainly nonsignifi-
cant relations. In contrast, under high corre-
Although this discussion may appear to
spondence, significant attitude-behavior rela-
imply that attitude measures have to be
tions of considerable magnitude were found.
specific with respect to target, action, and
Inappropriate measures tend to reduce the
context, we have argued that the problem is
obtained relations between attitude and be-
one of correspondence between predictor and
havior, but appropriate measures invariably
criterion, not a problem of specificity. Of
produce strong relationships.20
course, if the investigator chooses to observe
Our review of research on the attitude-
a single action with respect to a given target
behavior relation has focused on correspond-
in a given context in order to obtain cor-
ence in terms of target and action elements.
Little attention has been given to correspond- respondence, the attitude also has to be very
specific. On the other hand, if she is really
ence in contextual and time elements, pri-
interested in a general behavioral pattern,
marily because few studies have provided
such as discrimination toward blacks, the
direct evidence concerning the effects of
behavioral criterion should involve obser-
correspondence in these elements. Lack of
vation of different discriminatory behaviors
correspondence in the contextual element may
toward various black individuals in a variety
well have contributed to some of the low
of contexts. A general measure of attitude
correlations mentioned in this review. Given
toward blacks will correspond to such a
the increased interest in the attitude-behavior
criterion.
relation, and the recent concern with the
In conclusion, our review and theoretical
effects of private versus public conditions on
analysis suggest that low and inconsistent
this relation (e.g., Acock & DeFleur, 1972;
attitude-behavior relations are attributable
Albrecht, DeFleur, & Warner, 1972; Frideres,
to low or partial correspondence between
Warner, & Albrecht, 1971; Liska, 1974b;
attitudinal and behavioral entities. To pre-
Warner & DeFleur, 1969; Schofield, 197S),
dict behavior from attitude, the investigator
more attention will have to be paid to cor- has to ensure high correspondence between
respondence in context. To obtain high atti- at least the target and action elements of the
tude-behavior correlations in studies in which measures he employs.
the context is systematically manipulated, it
is essential to ensure not only correspondence 20
The rank-order correlation between correspon-
in target and action elements but also corre- dence and the magnitude of the attitude-behavior
spondence in the contextual element. relation in Table 1 was found to be .83.
914 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

Lest attitude researchers conclude that all 2. Fishbein, M., Thomas, K., & Jaccard, J. J. An
is well and resume their complacency, we attitudinal analysis of British voting behavior.
Unpublished manuscript, Social Science Research
hasten to add a few words of caution. First, Council, Survey Unit, London, 197S.
attitude measurement, even by means of 3. Jaccard, J. J., King, G. W., & Pomazal, R. Atti-
sophisticated instruments, may add little to tudes and behavior: An analysis of specificity of
our understanding of social phenomena. An attitudinal predictors. Unpublished manuscript,
investigator attempting to explain a certain University of Illinois, 1975.
phenomenon in terms of an attitudinal
analysis must first define the behaviors of References
interest, the targets at which they are directed,
and the context and time of their occurrence. Acock, A. C., & DeFleur, M. L. A configurational
Measures of attitude will serve to explain the approach to contingent consistency in the attitude-
behavior relationship. American Sociological Re-
behaviors to the extent that they involve view, 1972, 37, 714-726.
identical target, action, context, and time Ajzen, I. Attitudinal vs. normative messages: An
elements. investigation of the differential effects of persua-
Second, attempts to influence behavior by sive communications on behavior. Sociometry,
means of attitude change must also consider 1971, 34, 263-280.
Ajzen, I., Darroch, R. K., Fishbein, M., & Hornik,
the degree of correspondence between the J. A. Looking backward revisited: A reply to
behavior that is to be changed and the atti- Deutscher. The American Sociologist, 1970, 5, 267-
tude at which the influence attempt is 273.
directed. Demonstration of attitude change is Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. The prediction of behavior
from attitudinal and normative variables. Journal
insufficient evidence for one's ability to change of Experimental Social Psychology, 1970, 6, 466-
behavior; only behaviors that correspond to 487.
the attitude are likely to change as a result Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. Attitudinal and normative
of revisions in attitude. variables as predictors of specific behaviors.
Finally, high correspondence between pre- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
1973, 27, 41-57.
dictors and criteria will ensure strong atti- Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. Factors influencing inten-
tude-behavior relations only to the extent tions .and the intention-behavior relation. Human
that appropriate measurement procedures are Relations, 1974, 27, 1-15.
employed. For too long it has been at the Albrecht, S. L., DeFleur, M. L., & Warner, L. G.
Attitude-behavior relationships: A reexamination
investigator's discretion to select any mea- of the postulate of contingent consistency. Pacific
sures deemed useful and to assign the labels Sociological Review, 1972, 15, 149-168.
attitude and behavior to them. Attitudinal Allport, G. W. Attitudes. In C. Murchinson (Ed.),
and behavioral measures are often selected in A handbook of social psychology. Worcester,
an arbitrary manner, leading to apparently Mass.: Clark University Press, 1935.
Allport, G. W. The nature of prejudice. Reading,
inconsistent research findings. Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1954.
In sum, only when standard procedures are Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. Experimentation in
employed to scale attitudes and to select be- social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson
haviors and only when attention is paid to (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2).
Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1968.
the correspondence between attitudinal and Asch, S. Effects of group pressure upon the modifi-
behavioral entities will the concept of attitude cation and distortion of judgment. In H. Guetz-
be able to resume the place it was accorded kow (Ed.), Groups, leadership and men. Pitts-
by Allport (1954) as the cornerstone in the burgh, Pa.: Carnegie Press, 1951.
edifice of social psychology. Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. Relative
efficacy of desensitization and modeling approaches
for inducing behavioral, affective, and attitudinal
changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Reference Notes chology, 1969, 13, 173-199.
Baron, R. A. Attraction toward the model and
1. Brislin, R. W., & Olmsted, K. An examination of model's competence as determinants of adult imi-
two models designed to predict behavior from tative behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
attitude and other verbal measures. Unpublished Psychology, 1970, 14, 345-351.
manuscript, East-West Center, Honolulu, Hawaii, Baron, R. A. Behavioral effects of interpersonal
1973. attraction: Compliance with requests from liked
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 915

and disliked others. Psychonomic Science, 1971, Calder, B. J., & Ross, M. Attitudes and behavior.
25, 32S-326. Morristown, N. J.: General Learning Press, 1973.
Bellin, S. S., & Kriesberg, L. Relationship among Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., &
attitudes, circumstances, and behavior: The case Stokes, D. E. The American voter. New York:
of applying for public housing. Sociology and Wiley, 1960.
Social Research, 1967, 51, 453-469. Campbell, A., & Stokes, D. E. Partisan attitudes and
Berg, K. E. Ethnic attitudes and agreement with a the presidential vote. In E. Burdick & A. J. Brod-
Negro person. Journal of Personality and Social beck (Eds.), American voting behavior. Glencoe,
Psychology, 1966, 4, 215-220. 111.: Free Press, 1959.
Bernberg, R. E. Socio-psychological factors in indus- Campbell, D. T. Social attitudes and other acquired
trial morale: I. The prediction of specific indica- behavioral dispositions. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psy-
tors. Journal oj Social Psychology, 1952, 36, 73- chology: A study of a science (Vol. 6). New York:
82. McGraw-Hill, 1963.
Bickman, L. Environmental attitudes and actions. Carr, L., & Roberts, S. O. Correlates of civil-rights
Journal oj Social Psychology, 1972, 87, 323-324. participation. Journal of Social Psychology, 1965,
Bostrom, R. N. Affective, cognitive, and behavioral 67, 259-267.
dimensions of communicative attitudes. Journal Cherrington, D. J., Reitz, H. J., & Scott, W. E., Jr.
of Communication, 1970, 20, 359-369. Effects of contingent and noncontingent reward
Bowers, W. J. Normative constraints on deviant on the relationship between satisfaction and job
behavior in the college context. Sociometry, 1968, performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1971,
31, 370-385, 55, 531-536.
Boyanowski, E. O., & Allen, V. L. Ingroup norms Corey, S. M. Professed attitudes and actual behavior.
and self-identity as determinants of discriminatory Journal of Educational Psychology, 1937, 28,
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- 271-280.
chology, 1973, 25, 408-418. Dean, L. R. Interaction, reported and observed: The
Brannon, R. Attitudes and the prediction of be- case of one local union. Human Organization,
havior. In B. Seidenberg & A. Snadowsky (Eds.), 1958, 17, 36-44.
Social psychology: An introduction. New York: DeFleur, M. L., & Westie, F. R. Verbal attitudes and
Free Press, 1976. overt acts: An experiment on the salience of atti-
Brannon, R., Cyphers, G., Hesse, S., Hesselbart, S., tudes. American Sociological Review, 1958, 23,
Keane, R., Schuman, H., Viccaro, T., & Wright, D. 667-673.
Attitude and action: A field experiment joined to DeFleur, M. L., & Westie, F. R. Attitude as a sci-
a general population survey. American Sociological entific concept. Social Forces, 1963, 42, 17-31.
Review, 1973, 38, 625-636. DeFriese, G. H., & Ford, W. S., Jr. Open occupancy
Bray, D. W. The prediction of behavior from two —-What whites say, what they do. Trans-action-
attitude scales. Journal of Abnormal and Social Social Science and Modern Society, 1968, 5, 53-56.
Psychology, 1950, 45, 64-84. DeFriese, G. H., & Ford, W. S., Jr. Verbal attitudes,
Brayfield, A. H., & Crockett, W. H. Employee atti- overt acts, and the influence of social constraint
tudes and employee performance. Psychological in interracial behavior. Social Problems, 1969, 16,
Bulletin, 1955, 52, 396-424. 493-505.
Bruvold, W. H. Consistency among attitudes, beliefs, Deutscher, I. Words and deeds. Social Problems,
and behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 1972, 1966, 13, 235-254.
86, 127-134. (a) Deutscher, I. Looking backward: Case studies on the
Bruvold, W. H. Attitude-belief and attitude-behavior progress of methodology in sociological research.
consistency. Journal oj Social Psychology, 1972, American Sociologist, 1969, 4, 35-41.
88, 241-246. (b) Deutscher, I. What we say/what we do: Sentiments
Bruvold, W. H. Belief and behavior as determinants and acts. Glenview, 111.: Scott, Foresman, 1973.
of attitude. Journal of Social Psychology, 1973, Dillehay, R. C. On the irrelevance of the classical
90, 285-289. negative evidence concerning the effect of attitudes
Burnstein, E., & McRae, A. V. Some effects of shared on behavior. American Psychologist, 1973, 28,
threat and prejudice in racially mixed groups. 887-891.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, Ehrlich, H. J. Attitudes, behavior, and the interven-
64, 257-263. ing variables. American Sociologist, 1969, 4, 29-34.
Byrne, D. Interpersonal attraction and attitude Ehrlich, H. J., & Graeven, D. B. Reciprocal self-
similarity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- disclosure in a dyad. Journal of Experimental
chology, 1961, 62, 713-715. Social Psychology, 1971, 7, 389-400.
Byrne, D., Baskett, G. D., & Hodges, L. Behavioral Epstein, Y. M., & Hornstein, H. A. Penalty and
indicators of interpersonal attraction. Journal of interpersonal attraction as factors influencing the
Applied Social Psychology, 1971, 1, 137-149. decision to help another person. Journal of Experi-
Byrne, D., Ervin, C. R., & Lamberth, J. Continuity mental Social Psychology, 1969, 5, 272-282.
between the experimental study of attraction and Fendrich, J. M. Perceived reference group support:
real-life computer dating. Journal of Personality Racial attitudes and overt behavior. American
and Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 157-165. Sociological Review, 1967, 32, 960-970. (a)
916 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

Fendrich, J. M. A study of the association among study of attitude toward organ transplantation.
verbal attitudes, commitment, and overt behavior Journal of Social Issues, 1971, 27, 171-183.
in different experimental situations. Social Forces, Goodstadt, M. S. Helping and refusal to help: A
1967, 45, 347-355. (b) test of balance and reactance theories. Journal of
Fischer, E. H. Who volunteers for companionship Experimental Social Psychology, 1971, 7, 610-622.
with mental patients? A study of attitude-belief- Graen, G. Instrumentality theory of work motiva-
intention relationships. Journal of Personality, tion: Some experimental results and suggested
1971, 39, 552-563. modifications. Journal of Applied Psychology
Fishbein, M. An investigation of the relationships Monograph, 1969, 53(2, Pt. 2 ) .
between beliefs about an object and the attitude Green, J. A. Attitudinal and situational deter-
toward that object. Human Relations, 1963, 16, minants of intended behavior toward blacks.
233-240. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Fishbein, M. Attitude and the prediction of behavior. 1972, 22, 13-17.
In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Readings in attitude theory Greenberg, M. S., & Frisch, D. M. Effect of inten-
and measurement. New York: Wiley, 1967. tionality on willingness to reciprocate a favor.
Fishbein, M. The prediction of behavior from atti- Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1972,
tudinal variables. In C. D. Mortensen & K. K. 8, 99-111.
Sereno (Eds.), Advances in communication re- Hackman, J. R., & Porter, L. W. Expectancy theory
search. New York: Harper & Row, 1973. predictions of work effectiveness. Organizational
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. Attitudes and opinions. Behavior and Human Performance, 1968, 3, 417-
Annual Review of Psychology, 1972, 23, 487-544. 426.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. Attitudes toward objects as Heberlein, T. A., & Black, J. S. Journal of Person-
predictors of single and multiple behavioral cri- ality and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 474-479.
teria. Psychological Review, 1974, 81, 59-74. Hendrick, C., & Taylor, S. P. Effects of belief sim-
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. Belief, attitude, intention, ilarity and aggression on attraction and counter-
and behavior: An introduction to theory and re- aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
search. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1975. chology, 1971, 17, 342-349.
Fishbein, M., & Coombs, F. S. Basis for decision: Himelstein, P., & Moore, J. C. Racial attitudes and
An attitudinal analysis of voting behavior. the action of Negro- and white-background fig-
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1974, 4, ures as factors in petition signing. Journal of
95-124. Social Psychology, 1963, 61, 267-272.
Freeman, L. C., & Ataov, T. Validity of indirect and Holman, P. A. Validation of an attitude scale as a
direct measures of attitude toward cheating. device for predicting behavior. Journal of Applied
Journal of Personality, 1960, 28, 444-447. Psychology, 1956, 40, 347-349.
Frideres, J. S., Warner, L. G., & Albrecht, S. L. The Hornstein, H. A., Masor, H. N., Sole, K., & Heilman,
impact of social constraints on the relationship M. Effects of sentiment and completion of a help-
between attitudes and behavior. Social Forces, ing act on observer helping: A case for socially
1971, SO, 102-112. mediated Zeigarnik effects. Journal of Personality
Friedlander, F., & Greenberg, S. Effect of job atti- and Social Psychology, 1971, 17, 107-112.
tudes, training, and organization climate on per- Insko, C. A., Blake, R. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Mulaik,
formance of the hard-core unemployed. Journal S. A. Attitude toward birth control and cognitive
of Applied Psychology, 1971, 55, 287-295. consistency: Theoretical and practical implica-
Friedman, H. Magnitude of experimental effect and tions of survey data. Journal of Personality and
a table for its rapid estimation. Psychological Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 228-237.
Bulletin, 1968, 70, 245-251. Izzett, R. R. Authoritarianism and attitudes toward
Gardin, H., Kaplan, K. J., Firestone, I. J., & Cowan, the Vietnam war as reflected in behavioral and
G. A. Proxemic effects on cooperation, attitude, self-report measures. Journal of Personality and
and approach-avoidance in a Prisoner's Dilemma Social Psychology, 1971, 17, 145-148.
game. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Jaccard, J. J. Predicting social behavior from per-
ogy, 1973, 27, 13-18. sonality traits. Journal of Research in Personality,
Genthner, R. W., & Taylor, S. P. Physical aggression 1974, 7, 358-367.
as a function of racial prejudice and the race of Janis, I. L., & Hoffman, D. Facilitating effects of
the target. Journal of Personality and Social daily contact between partners who make a deci-
Psychology, 1973, 27, 207-210. sion to cut down on smoking. Journal of Person-
Goldberg, G. N., Kiesler, C. A., & Collins, B. E. ality and Social Psychology, 1970, 17, 25-35.
Visual behavior and face-to-face distance during Kamenetsky, J., Burgess, G. G., & Rowan, T. The
interaction. Sociometry, 1969, 32, 43-53. relative effectiveness of four attitude assessment
Goldberg, G., & Mettee, D. R. Liking and perceived techniques in predicting a criterion. Educational
communication potential as determinants of look- and Psychological Measurement, 1956, 16, 187-194.
ing at another. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 16, Katz, I., & Benjamin, L. Effects of white author-
277-278. itarianism in biracial work groups. Journal of
Goodmonson, C., & Glaudin, V. The relationship of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 61, 448-
commitment-free and commitment behavior: A 456.
ATTITUDE-BEHAVIOR RELATIONS 917

Kelman, H. C. Attitudes are alive and well and study of television's impact on behavior. Public
gainfully employed in the sphere of action. Opinion Quarterly, 1971, 35, 242-248.
American Psychologist, 1974, 29, 310-324. Oskamp, S., & Perlman, D. Effects of friendship and
Kothandapani, V. Validation of feeling, belief, and disliking on cooperation in a mixed-motive game.
intention to act as three components of attitude Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1966, 10, 221-226.
and their contribution to prediction of contra- Ostrom, T. M. The relationship between the af-
ceptive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social fective, behavioral, and cognitive components of
Psychology, 1971, 19, 321-333. attitude. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Krauss, R. M. Structural and attitudinal factors in ogy, 1969, 5, 12-30.
interpersonal bargaining. Journal of Experimental Peabody, 0. Trait inferences: Evaluative and de-
Social Psychology, 1966, 2, 42-45. scriptive aspects. Journal of Personality and Social
Kutner, B., Wilkins, C., & Yarrow, P. R. Verbal Psychology Monograph, 1967, 7(2, Pt. 2, Whole
attitudes and overt behavior involving racial No. 642).
prejudice. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- Pomazal, R. J., & Jaccard, J. J. An informational
chology, 1952, 47, 649-652. approach to altruistic behavior. Journal of Per-
LaPiere, R. T. Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, sonality and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 317-326.
1934, 13, 230-237. Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. Organizational, work,
Linn, L. S. Verbal attitudes and overt behavior: A and personal factors in employee turnover and
study of racial discrimination. Social Forces, 1965, absenteeism. Psychological Bulletin, 1973, SO, 151-
44, 353-364. 176.
Liska, A. E. Attitude-behavior consistency as a Potter, H. W., & Klein, H. R. On nursing behavior.
function of generality equivalence between atti- Psychiatry, 1957, 20, 39-46.
tude and behavior objects. Journal of Psychology, Regan, D. T. Effects of a favor and liking on com-
1974, 86, 217-228. (a) pliance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Liska, A. E. The impact of attitude on behavior: ogy, 1971, 7, 627-639.
Attitude-social support interaction. Pacific Socio- Repass, D. E. Issue salience and party choice.
logical Review, 1974, 17, 83-97. (b) American Political Science Review, 1971, 65, 389-
Liska, A. E. (Ed.). The consistency controversy: 400.
Readings on the impact of attitude on behavior. Rokeach, M. Attitude change and behavior change.
New York: Wiley, 1975. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1967, 30, 529-550.
Malof, M., & Lott, A. J. Ethnocentrism and the Rokeach, M., & Kliejunas, P. Behavior as a func-
acceptance of Negro support in a group pressure tion of attitude-toward-object and attitude-
situation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- toward-situation. Journal of Personality and
chology, 1962, 65, 254-258. Social Psychology, 1972, 22, 194-201.
Mann, J. H. The relationship between cognitive, Rokeach, M., & Mezei, L. Race and shared belief
affective, and behavioral aspects of racial prejudice. as factors in social choice. Science, 1966, 151, 167-
Journal of Social Psychology, 1959, 49, 223-228. 172.
Mazen, R., & Leventhal, H. The influence of com- Rosen, B., & Komorita, S. S. Attitudes and action:
municator-recipient similarity upon the beliefs The effects of behavioral intent and perceived
and behavior of pregnant women. Journal of effectiveness of acts. Journal of Personality, 1971,
Experimental Social Psychology, 1972, 8, 289-302. 39, 189-203.
McGrew, J. M. How "open" are multiple-dwelling Rosen, S., Johnson, R. D., Johnson, M. J., & Tesser,
units? Journal of Social Psychology, 1967, 72, A. Interactive effects of news valence and attrac-
223-226. tion on communicator behavior. Journal of Per-
Mischel, W. Personality and assessment. New York: sonality and Social Psychology, 1973, 28, 298-300.
Wiley, 1968. Sagi, P. C., Olmsted, D. W., & Atelsek, F. Predicting
Mitchell, T. R., & Nebeker, D. M. Expectancy theory maintenance of membership in small groups.
predictions of academic effort and performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955,
Journal of Applied Psychology, 1973, 57, 61-67. 51, 308-311.
Nemeth, C. Effects of free versus constrained be- Sample, J., & Warland, R. Attitude and prediction
havior on attraction between people. Journal of of behavior. Social Forces, 1973, 51, 292-303.
Personality ind Social Psychology, 1970, 15, 302- Sampson, E. E., & Insko, C. A. Cognitive consistency
311. and performance in the autokinetic situation.
Newton, N., & Newton, M. Relationship of ability Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964,
to breast-feed and maternal attitudes toward 68, 184-192.
breast-feeding. Pediatrics, 1950, 5, 869-875. Sapolsky, A. Effect of interpersonal relationships
Nisbett, R. E. Taste, deprivation, and weight deter- upon verbal conditioning. Journal of Abnormal
minants of eating behavior. Journal of Person- and Social Psychology, 1960, 60, 241-246.
ality and Social Psychology, 1968, 10, 107-116. Schneider, D. J. Social psychology. Reading, Mass.:
Norman, R. Affective-cognitive consistency: Atti- Addison-Wesley, 1976.
tudes, conformity, and behavior. Journal of Per- Schneider, F. W. Conforming behavior of black and
sonality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 83-91. white children. Journal of Personality and Social
O'Keefe, M. T. The anti-smoking commercials: A Psychology, 1970, 16, 466-471.
918 ICEK AJZEN AND MARTIN FISHBEIN

Schofield, J. W. The effect of norms, public dis- distance as intervening variables between attitudes
closure, and need for approval on volunteering and action. American Sociological Review, 1969,
behavior consistent with attitudes. Journal of 34, 153-169.
Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 1126- Warner, L. G., & Dennis, R. M. Prejudice versus
1133. discrimination: An empirical example and theo-
Schopler, J., & Thompson, V. D. Role of attribution retical extension. Social Forces, 1970, 48, 473-484.
processes in mediating amount of reciprocity for Waters, L. K., & Roach, D. Relationship between
a favor. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- job attitudes and two forms of withdrawal from
chology, 1968, 10, 243-2SO. the work situation. Journal of Applied Psychol-
Schuman, H., & Johnson, M. P. Attitudes and be- ogy, 1971, 55, 92-94.
havior. Annual Review of Sociology, 1976, 2, 161- Webb, W. B., & Hollander, E. P. Comparison of
207. three morale measures: A survey, pooled group
Schwab, D. P., & Cummings, L. L. Theories of per- judgments, and self-evaluations. Journal of
formance and satisfaction: A review. Industrial Applied Psychology, 1956, 40, 17-20.
Relations, 1970, 9, 408-430. Weigel, R. H., & Newman, L. S. Increasing attitude-
Schwartz, S. H., & Tessler, R. C. A test of a model behavior correspondence by broadening the scope
for reducing measured attitude-behavior discrep- of the behavioral measure. Journal of Personality
ancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 793-802.
ogy, 1972, 24, 225-236. Weigel, R. H., Vernon, D. T. A., & Tognacci, L. N.
Sherif, C. W., Kelly, M., Rodgers, H. L., Jr., Sarup, The specificity of the attitude as a determinant of
G., & Tittler, B. I. Personal involvement, social attitude-behavior congruence. Journal of Person-
judgment, and action. Journal of Personality and ality and Social Psychology, 1974, 30, 724-728.
Social Psychology, 1973, 27, 311-328. Weinstein, A. G. Predicting behavior from attitudes.
Sherif, M. A study of some social factors in per- Public Opinion Quarterly, 1972, 36, 355-360.
ception. Archives of Psychology, 1935, 27, No. 187. Weitz, J., & Nuckols, R. C. The validity of direct
Silverman, B. I., & Cochrane, R. The relationship and indirect questions in measuring job satisfac-
between verbal expressions of behavioral intention tion. Personnel Psychology, 1953, 6, 487-494.
and overt behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, Wicker, A. W. Attitudes versus actions: The rela-
1971, 84, 51-56. tionship of verbal and overt behavioral responses
Smith, E. W. L., & Dixon, T. R. Verbal conditioning to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 1969,
as a function of race of the experimenter and 25, 41-78.
prejudice of the subject. Journal of Experimental Wicker, A. W. An examination of the "other vari-
Social Psychology, 1968, 4, 285-301. ables" explanation of attitude-behavior inconsist-
Stapleton, R. E., Nacci, P., & Tedeschi, J. T. Inter- ency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
personal attraction and the reciprocation of bene- ogy, 1971, 19, 18-30.
fits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Wicker, A. W., & Pomazal, R. J. The relationship
1973, 28, 199-205. between attitudes and behavior as a function of
Taffel, C. Anxiety and the conditioning of verbal specificity of attitude object and presence of a
behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Be- significant person during assessment conditions.
havior, 1955, S I , 496-501. Representative Research in Social Psychology,
Tarter, D. E. Toward prediction of attitude-action 1971, 2, 26-31.
discrepancy. Social Forces, 1969, 47, 398-405. Wiggins, J. W. Personality and prediction: Princi-
Tittle, C. R., & Hill, R. J. Attitude measurement and ples of personality assessment. Reading, Mass.:
prediction of behavior: An evaluation of condi- Addison-Wesley, 1973.
tions and measurement techniques. Sociometry, Winters, L. C. Measuring attitudes and behavior
1967, 30, 199-213. toward ecologically significant products. Psycho-
Tornatzky, L., & Geiwitz, P. J. The effects of threat logical Reports, 1971, 29, 893-894.
and attraction on interpersonal bargaining. Psy- Woodward, J. L., & Roper, E. Political activity of
chonomic Science, 1968, 13, 125-126. American citizens. American Political Science
Veevers, J. E. Drinking attitudes and drinking be- Review, 1950, 44, 872-885.
havior: An exploratory study. Journal of Social Worthy, M., Gary, A. L., & Kahn, G. M. Self-dis-
Psychology, 1971, 85, 103-109. closure as an exchange process. Journal of Per-
Vroom, V. H. Work and motivation. New York: sonality and Social Psychology, 1969, 13, 59-63.
Wiley, 1964. Zunich, M. A study of relationships between child-
Warland, R. H., & Sample, J. Response certainty as
a moderator variable in attitude measurement. rearing attitudes and material behavior. Journal
Rural Sociology, 1973, 38, 174-186. of Experimental Education, 1961, 30, 231-241.
Warner, L. G., & DeFleur, M. L. Attitude as an
interactional concept: Social constraint and social Received May 27, 1976 •

You might also like