You are on page 1of 11

Science Watch

I li i ~

Health Psychology
The Science and the FieM

Shelley E. Taylor University of California, Los Angeles

Ill llllillll H Ill I I Ill¸ i 3illlllll?/i Illl Ill II Ill fill IIlll Illlll /I Ill Ill il . . . .

ABSTRACT: Reviews scientific and professional trends justment to stress or illness. As such, these interventions
in the field of health psychology. I discuss recent research constitute both tests and applications of the theories.
on health promotion, psychological factors in the devel- Research in behavioral medicine and, correspond-
opment of illness, cognitive representations of health and ingly, in health psychology has taken the position that
illness, stress and coping, social support, interventions to biological, psychological, and social factors are implicated
promote coping, and trends that will affect progress in the in all stages of health and illness, ranging from those be-
field, such as the needfor cost containment and the aging haviors and states that keep people healthy to those that
of the population. produce severe, long-term, and debilitating disease. This
position is termed the biopsychosocial model and is a
guiding framework for both research and practice (Engel,
Health psychology is defined as the "educational, scien- 1977; Schwartz, 1982). This article, will emphasize its
tific, and professional contributions of the discipline of guiding role for basic research. Define biopsychosocial model
psychology to the promotion and maintenance of health,
the prevention and treatment of illness, the identification Recent Scientific Developments in
of etiologic and diagnostic correlates of health, illness, Health Psychology
and related dysfunction, and the improvement of the
health care system and health policy formation" (Matar- If one asks where the field of health psychology is going,
azzo, 1980, p. 815). As such, its mission is broad, in- there would he no simple answer. As a microcosm of
volving all branches of psychology in virtually every aspect both psychology and the interdisciplinary endeavor of
of the health enterprise. behavioral medicine, the field is pulled and pushed in
As a field, health psychology has made substantial many directions simultaneously. In this article, I first at-
contributions to the understanding of healthy behaviors tempt to characterize some of the recent scientific devel-
and to the comprehension of the myriad factors that un- opments that illustrate the trends in the field generally. I
dermine health and often lead to illness. Much of the then focus on some of the major directions and forces
strongest work has involved providing theoretical and affecting the future of the field as a whole. Although an
conceptual frameworks that elucidate the (non)practice exhaustive analysis of health psychology's contributions
of health behaviors, the role of stress in affecting illness is precluded here, a focus on some of the more recent
and illness behavior, the representations that people hold and exciting research developments is instructive in il-
regarding their health and illness, and the ways in which lustrating the progress of the field. For more comprehen-
people cope with illness and the determinants of their sive reviews, the reader is referred to overviews of the
adjustment to it. field (e.g., H. S. Friedman & DiMatteo, 1989; Taylor,
Define,theorical 1986) and to the Annual Review of Psychology chapter
These theoretical conceptualizations constitute ma-
jor contributions inasmuch as they are often lacking in by Rodin and Salovey (1989).
traditional medicine and medical practice. They provide
a basis for making sense of otherwise isolated and con- Health Promotion and Health Habit Modification
fusing bits of data. For example, it is difficult for physi- Health promotion and primary prevention have been of
cians to understand why 93% of patients fail to adhere increasing concern to researchers and practitioners be-
to certain aspects of their treatment regimens; social psy- cause of changing patterns of illness. In the past 80 years
chological models not only make sense of these data but in the United States, the prevalence of acute infectious
suggest ways of ameliorating them. Theoretical models disorders such as influenza, tuberculosis, measles, and
also suggest new directions for research that might oth- polio has declined while what have been termed the "pre-
erwise remain elusive and left as isolated observations. ventable" disorders have increased, including lung cancer,
Finally, such models often point directly to interventions cardiovascular disease, drug and alcohol abuse, and ve-
that can improve the practice of health behavior and ad- hicular accidents (Matarazzo, 1982).

40 January 1990 • American Psychologist


Copyright 1990 by the America PaychololgicalAs~aciation, Inc. 0003-066X/90/$00.75
Vol. 45, No. 1, 40-50
The role of behavioral factors in the development of smoking). For example, although peer group influence
these diseases and disorders is increasingly clear. For ex- and issues of personal identity may be bound up in the
ample, 25% of all cancer deaths and approximately practice of faulty health habits in adolescence, these fac-
350,000 premature deaths from heart attack could be tors are less important in considering health habits in
avoided each year by modifying just one risk factor: adulthood (Botvin & Eng, 1982; Evans et al., 1978). Con-
smoking (American Heart Association, 1988). A 10% sequently, it is often difficult to develop intervention pro-
weight reduction in men aged 35 to 55 through dietary grams that will appeal to a broad segment of the popu-
modifications and exercise would produce an estimated lation to change some targeted health habit. Whereas in-
20% decrease in coronary artery disease (American Heart dividualized appeals often have the greatest impact on
Association, 1984a, 1984b; Ashley & Kannell, 1974); it behavior, such methods are intensive and expensive and
would also lower the degree of degenerative arthritis, gas- affect only a limited portion of the population at a time.
trointestinal cancer, diabetes, stroke, and heart attack. Mass-media appeals, however, sometimes change attitudes
The percentage of the gross national product that goes about health problems but may produce only modest be-
for health care has been climbing steadily, in part because havior change (Lau, Kane, Berry, Ware, & Roy, 1980;
the diseases that are currently most prevalent are chronic Leventhal, Meyer, & Nerenz, 1980). How best to combine
in nature and thus require continual treatment and mon- the advantages of the two methods is a problem on which
itoring. Successful modification of health behaviors may much recent attention has been focused, and efforts to
help to reduce both the numbers of deaths and the inci- tailor interventions to designated communities have been
dence of preventable disease as well as make a dent in one method used (Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial
the more than $400 billion spent yearly on health and Research Group, 1982; see Matarazzo, Weiss, Herd,
illness (cf. Matarazzo, 1982). Miller, & Weiss, 1984, for a review).
The desire to keep people healthy rather than wait Perhaps the most important problem for future re-
to treat them after they become ill has been the impetus search is that of preventing relapse (Marlatt & Gordon,
for much work on the development of the healthy life- 1985), the often-observed phenomenon that, after suc-
style and the modification of faulty health habits. Al- cessfully altering a health practice on their own for weeks
though a number of conceptual models have been devel- or even months, many individuals revert to their former
oped both to explain existing health practices and as im- behaviors (Brownell, Marlatt, Lichtenstein, & Wilson,
peti for changing faulty ones, there is now considerable 1986). In this context, it becomes essential to consider
convergence on the beliefs that contribute to a given health not only the short-term effects of interventions designed
practice. Specifically, we now know that people are most to modify health habits but also their long-range effec-
likely to practice a good health measure or to change a tiveness, focusing especially on factors that undermine
faulty one when (a) the threat to health is severe; (b) the long-term maintenance of short-term change.
Self perceived personal vulnerability and/or the likelihood of
efficacy( developing the disorder is high; (c) the person believes Do Psyclmlogleal States Cause Illness?
bandura’ that he or she is able to perform the response that will For centuries, philosophers and scientists have speculated
s) reduce the threat (self-efficacy); and (d) the response is about the role that personality factors and coping styles
elements effective in overcoming the threat (response efficacy) may play in the development of illness. In the 1930s and
(Bandura, 1986; Janz & Becker, 1984; Rogers, 1984). the 1940S, Flanders Dunbar (1943), Franz Alexander
These four elements borrow heavily from distinct (1950), and their associates developed specific personality
theoretical models---specifically, Bandura's self-efficacy profiles of those prone to hypertension, coronary artery
framework, Rogers's protection/motivation theory, and disease, cancer, ulcers, rheumatoid arthritis, and other
the health belief model--but in practice, most researchers specific disease states. Until recently, however, theory
now make efforts to conceptualize and measure all four outstripped methodology, so that convincing evidence re-
components. garding such relationships was lacking (e.g., Fox, 1978).
Conceptual convergence has helped to clarify the Several recent research developments have improved this
difficulty and complexity of actually modifying health situation.
behaviors. Health habits predict each other only modestly, Early research relating personality factors to disease
and their interrelations may decline with age (see Me- states focused on whether personality states were related
chanic, 1979). One of the reasons for this is the fact that to a host of diseases (the general model) or whether par-
each health habit has a complex pattern of etiology, ticular personality traits could be related to specific dis-
maintenance, change, and relapse (see Jessor & Jessor, eases (the specificity model). Research continues to in-
1982, and Leventhal & Cleary, 1980, for discussions of vestigate both kinds of links. Recent research using meta-
analysis suggests that a general negative affective style
Preparation ofthis articlewassupportedby twograntsfromthe National marked by depression, anxiety, and, to a lesser extent,
InstituteofMentalHealth(MH42152and MH 42258)and bya Research hostility may be associated with the development of a
Scientist DevelopmentAward from the National Institute of Mental broad range of diseases, including coronary artery disease,
Health (MH 03311).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to asthma, headache, ulcers, and arthritis (H. S. Friedman
ShelleyE. Taylor,Departmentof Psychology,405 HilgardAvenue,Uni- & Booth-Kewley, 1987). Repression, as a coping style,
versityof California at Los Anseles,Los Angeles,CA 90024. may also be implicated in this cluster (Weinberger, in

January 1990 • American Psychologist 41


press). These findings suggest the possibility of a general in certain indicators of immune activity, such as natural
disease-prone personality, although at present the exact killer cell activity and lymphocyte proliferation in re-
causal nature of the relationship is uncertain. Although sponse to mitogenic stimulation (see Kiecolt-Glaser &
negative emotional states certainly result from illness, Glaser, in press, and Stein, Keller, & Schleifer, 1985, for
longitudinal studies also suggest the validity of the reverse reviews). Although direct links to disease have usually
direction of causality. The links through which such re- not been established, such results are useful in identifying
lationships occur have drawn considerable attention potential biobehavioral mechanisms whereby psychologic
(Matthews, 1988). A negative emotional state may pro- states may exert adverse effects on health. The psychologic
duce pathogenic physiological changes; it may lead people factors implicated to date in immunocompromise are the
to practice faulty health behaviors; it may produce illness stressful event of bereavement, the state of depression,
behavior (such as visiting a physician) but no underlying and stressful events involving the lack or loss of perceived
pathology; or it may be associated with illness via other control (see Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser, in press; Stein et
factors in some as-yet-undetermined manner. al., 1985). For example, animal studies have suggested a
These developments have been paralleled by in- causal chain linking uncontrollable stress to cancer in
creasing attention to specific models of personality-dis- susceptibleanimals (Laudenslager, Ryan, Drugan, Hyson,
ease relationships. Chief among these have been the ex- & Maier, 1983), although the relationship appears to be
ploration of the Type A behavior syndrome, characterized moderated by the temporal course of the stressor in as
by competitive drive, impatience, hostility, and rapid yet unpredictable ways. This knowledge is now being ap-
speecff and motor movements, in the development of cor- plied to the effort to understand the role of psychosocial
onary heart disease (CHD). Compared to earlier work, factors in the development of the acquired immunode-
recent research has reported weaker links between Type ficiency syndrome (AIDS) following exposure to the
A behavior and CHD (Matthews, 1988), although re- human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) (Kemeny et al.,
searchers concur that the structured interview assessment 1989).
technique shows a stronger relationship to CHD than Increasingly, researchers are focusing on the poten-
other measures (H. S. Friedman & Booth-Kewley, 1988; tially protective role of positive emotional states and cop-
Matthews, 1988). The emphasis on negative emotional ing styles in the development of illness. Chief among these
states has prompted researchers investigating the role of are optimism (e.g., Scheier & Carver, 1985)and perceived
Type A behavior in the etiology of CHD to focus most control. Optimists appear to experience fewer physical
closely on hostility as the potential culprit (Dembroski symptoms (Scheier & Carver, 1985), and they may show
& Costa, 1987; H. S. Friedman & Booth-Kewley, 1987). faster or better recoveries from certain illnesses (Scheier
Other components of the Type A behavior syndrome, such et al., in press; see also Peterson, Seligman, & Vaillant,
as competitiveness and time urgency, do not seem to be 1988). The importance of self-efficacy beliefs in the prac-
as lethal. Such discoveries lay the groundwork for more tice of health behaviors (Bandura, 1986) and the health
sophisticated interventions to modify the Type A behavior benefits of control (e.g., I.anger & Rodin, 1976; Rodin,
syndrome in the hope that those susceptible to it may 1986) are well established.
avoid the development of disease (e.g., Suinn, 1982; Tho- Of the many issues meriting further exploration, a
resen, Friedman, Gill, & Ulmer, 1982). Research to date chief concern is whether these psychological variables
suggests that interventions to reduce Type A behavior represent predisposing (or protective) personality states
can be successful (M. Friedman et al., 1986). or whether they exert their impacts on health in inter-
For many years, researchers have suspected links be- action with situational variables such as stress. For ex-
tween a passive, acquiescent, or repressed personality style ample, whereas some researchers have regarded the Type
and the development or progression of cancer. Although A constellation as a predisposing personality factor for
research relating personality variables to the development CHD, others have considered it to be a behavioral syn-
of cancer is lacking, in large part because such studies drome elicited by certain circumstances and not others
are difficult to design (Fox, 1978), some evidence for the (see Matthews, 1982). Good cases for particular causal
role of an acquiescent, repressed personality style in the paths have not yet been made. This issue is part of the
rapid progression of cancer has accumulated (e.g., Der- larger question regarding the pathways by which psycho-
ogatis, Abeloff, & Melasaratos, 1979; Levy, Herberman, logic factors are involved in the etiology of health and
Lippman, & d'Angelo, 1987; Levy, Herberman, Maluish, illness, a concern that will guide research in the coming
Schlien, & Lippman, 1985). The increasingly sophisti- years.
cated efforts to relate personality variables to the onset The additional question arises as to whether the lit-
or progression of cancer have been plagued by method- erature linking psychosocial factors to the etiology of ill-
ological difficulties: Inasmuch as cancers grow over a long ness provides any basis for intervention studies. Should
period, it is difficult to establish causality unambiguously we be intervening with people who show pronounced
(Fox, 1978). negative affect (H. S. Friedman & Booth-Kewley, 1988)
Research on the health significance of negative emo- or a pessimistic explanatory style (Peterson et al., 1988)
tional states is fueled by related discoveries in other fields. with the goal of improving their health farther down the
Research in psychoimmunology using quasi-experimental line? The effect sizes in these relationships are typically
studies of samples exposed to stress documents declines small, and it is not yet clear whether such interventions

42 January 1990 • American Psychologist


would have an identifiable impact on health. The possi- much of it was conducted with animals. Human research
bility of using such relationships to develop interventions, spearheaded by Lazarus and his associates (e.g., Lazarus,
however, remains a prospect for future research. 1966; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), however, identified
psychological appraisal as a crucial mediating process in
Cognitive Factors in Health and Illness
the experience of stress. Events are judged to be positive,
Just as emotional factors are involved in the experience negative, or neutral in their implications, and if judged
of health and illness, cognitive factors influence how peo- negative, they are further evaluated as to whether they
ple appraise their health and cope with the threat of illness. are harmful, threatening, or challenging.
Several researchers (e.g., Jemmott, Croyle, & Ditto, 1988; Early efforts to identify stressful events in humans
Lau, Bernard, & Hartman, 1989; Leventhal et al., focused on the amount of change that was required to
1980, Millstein & Irwin, 1987; Turk, Rudy, & Salovey, deal with these events, that is, the life events approach.
1985) have argued that people hold articulated general The measurement of stress involved checking offwhether
conceptions of illness against which particular symptoms particular stressful events (e.g., death of a spouse, argu-
or disorders may be evaluated. These so-called common- ments with family members) had occurred within a given
sense representations of illness include several dimensions: time period (e.g., 6 months) and then relating them to
identity (the label of the illness and its symptoms), cause, disease at later points in time. Such studies established
consequences, time frame, and cure (e.g., Leventhal et modest but reliable relationships to illness.
al., 1980). When people make appropriate matches of More recent and sophisticated work has enabled re-
symptoms to their preexisting representations of illness, searchers to identify the dimensions of events that are
they may show appropriate illness behavior such as seek- most likely to produce stress. Events appraised as negative,
ing treatment promptly and demonstrating effective fol- uncontrollable, unpredictable, or ambiguous are typically
low-through; however, the improper match of symptoms experienced as more stressful than those not so appraised.
to illness conceptions may account for delay behavior, the Research identifying the significance of these dimensions
faulty practice of certain health behaviors, poor adherence has come both from well-controlled laboratory studies
to health recommendations, and other adverse effects on (e.g., Glass, 1977) and from opportunistic studies of peo-
health (Baumann & Leventhal, 1985; Turk, Rudy, & Sa- ple undergoing major stressful events such as unemploy-
lovey, 1984). ment (I. Fleming, Baum, Davidson, Rectanus, & Mc-
Cognitions have also been examined in the context Ardle, 1987) or crises such as the Three Mile Island ca-
of coping with chronic disease and disability. In particular, tastrophe (R. Fleming, Baum, Gisriel, & Gatchel, 1982).
researchers have focused on the causal attributions that Of the factors implicated in stress, controllability may be
people make for their chronic conditions and on the per- especially important. In studies involving stressful events,
ceptions of control that they generate regarding the cur- when those events were under the control of the organisms
rent course of their disorder and/or its daily symptoms, studied, those organisms showed physiological profiles
treatments, and side effects. Attributions for the .cause of similar to those of organisms undergoing no stress at all,
a chronic illness appear to be commonly made (e.g., Tay- whereas organisms experiencing the event without the
lor, Lichtman, & Wood, 1984a), and a relatively high per- experience of control showed physiological reactions in-
centage are made to the self. Some have regarded self- dicative of anxiety and arousal (Hanson, Larson, &
attribution as potentially destructive, whereas others have Snowden, 1976; Laudenslager et al., 1983).
considered it indicative of efforts to assert control over In this context, it is useful to distinguish explicitly
the illness (see Bulman & Wortman, 1977). Results con- between illness behavior and illness inasmuch as the two
cerning self-blame or self-responsibility for illness have only partially overlap. Illness behavior refers to the steps
been inconclusive (Miller & Porter, 1983), perhaps be- people take when they believe they are experiencing
cause causality, blame, and responsibility have been used symptoms of illness (such as going to the doctor, taking
somewhat interchangeably in this literature (Shaver & days off from work). Illness itself involves documented
Drown, 1986). Attributions for an illness made to another pathology. The distinction is important because illness
person (such as an ex-spouse for causing stress), however, behaviors do not necessarily implicate underlying pa-
have been uniformly associated with poor psychological thology, and the psychologic and biologic pathways re-
adjustment to chronic conditions (e.g., Bulman & Wort- sponsible for the two types of outcomes are often totally
man, 1977; Taylor et al., 1984a). A factor that appears different (cf. Cohen, 1988).Much of the research impli-
to promote positive adjustment is perceived control. cating psychosocial factors in illness looks primarily at
Those who believe they can control either the course of illness behaviors as the outcome points, rather than at
their illness or their day-to-day symptoms appear to be pathology verified through such sources as physician rec-
better adjusted to their disorders (Attleck, Tennen, Pfeiffer, ords (see Karl, 1983). Extensions of these relationships
& Fifield, 1987; Taylor et al., 1984a). to documented illness are needed.
Stress and Illness Until relatively recently, research examining the role
of stress in illness behavior and the development of illness
Conceptual work on stress began with the fight-flight re- focused on major stressful events and the impact these
action described by Cannon (1932). Early work on stress often extreme and dramatic conditions can have on
largely ignored psychological factors, perhaps because health. More recently, research has investigated the day-

January 1990 • American Psychologist 43


to-day process of coping with minor stressful events such ment, which is based on the Lazarus group's work and
as daily hassles (Kanner, Coyne, Schaeffer, & lazarus, which identifies the specific actions, thoughts, and reac-
1981). Unfortunately, the measurement of day-to-day tions that people have to stressful events; Stone and
stress sometimes confounds stress with psychological and Neale's (1984) measure of daily coping, specifically de-
physical symptoms, and it has been difficult to disentangle signed for use in longitudinal studies; and the COPE
cause and effect (Dohrenwend, Dohrenwend, Dodson, & measure (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989). Increas-
Shrout, 1984). Whether minor stressors will ultimately ingiy, too, researchers have found that coping measures
prove to be important predictors of psychological distress targeted to particular populations experiencing particular
and illness remains to be seen. stressors may be more useful than more general coping
Just as recent efforts to relate personality variables measures. For example, in this context, Wills (1986) has
to illness have focused on potential pathways, so research designed a coping measure for use with adolescents.
relating stress to illness has focused on the pathways by There are many ways in which coping responses can
which such developments may occur. Of particular con- be grouped (Billings & Moos, 1982). Two general cate-
cern have been the patterns of physiological reactivity gories of coping strategies are problem-solving efforts and
produced by stressful events. Initially, work was guided strategies aimed at the regulation of emotions (Lazarus
by Selye's (1956) general adaptation syndrome, which & Folkman, 1984). Although both sets of strategies are
maintained that people develop patterned physiological brought to bear on most stressful events, problem-solving
reactions to stress which they then exhibit across a wide efforts are especially useful for managing controllable
variety of stressful situations. More recent evidence, stressors, and emotional-regnlation efforts are well-suited
however, suggests that there is some physiological speci- to managing the impact of uncontrollable stressors. A
ficity in response to particular kinds of stressful events distinction among coping strategies that overlaps with but
(e.g., Mason, 1974) and emotional reactions (Smith, does not perfectly correspond to the problem-solving/ Types of
1989). Other pathways explored include the likelihood emotional-regulation distinction is that between active coing
that stressful events erode health habits, such as smoking, coping (behavioral or cognitive efforts to manage a stress- (active &
drinking, and appropriate eating and sleeping patterns ful event directly) and avoidance (attempts to avoid deal-
(see Krantz, Grunberg & Baum, 1985), or change illness ing with the problem or reduce tension through escapist avoidance
behaviors without necessarily affecting health. behaviors). Although most negative life events appear to )
elicit both types of coping strategies, people with more
personal and environmental resources may rely more on
Understanding Coping define Coping active coping and less on avoidance coping (Holahan &
Coping has been defined as the process of managing ex- Moos, 1987). A long-standing issue in the individual-dif-
ternal or internal demands that are perceived as taxing ferences perspective is whether avoidant/repressive re-
or exceeding a person's resources (Lazarus & Folkman, sponses to stressful events are more adaptive or whether
1984). Coping may consist of behaviors and intrapsychic more vigilant/confrontational coping methods are su-
responses designed to overcome, reduce, or tolerate these perior. Avoidant responses may be more effective for
demands (Lazarus & Launier, 1978). Until recently, re- managing short-term threats, but for long-term threats
search on coping was in disarray, characterized in one vigilant coping may manage stress more effectively (Suls
report as a "three-car garage filled to the rafters with junk" & Fletcher, 1985). These findings, too, will no doubt prove
(Taylor, 1984, p. 2313). The reason was that researchers to be contingent on the nature of the stressful event (see
studied the same phenomena in different ways using idio- Taylor & Clark, 1986, for a review).
syncratic concepts, measures, and methods. The rise of This emphasis on individual differences has shifted
health psychology as a field has ameliorated this situation somewhat with the findings that most people appear to
in one important way by providing avenues and forums use a variety of coping strategies to deal with any given
for communication. This has moved researchers toward stressor (e.g., Folkman et al., 1986). Successful coping
greater awareness of the need for commonality in the may depend more on a match of coping strategies to the
definition of concepts and issues in the study of coping. features of the stressful event than on the relative efficacy
Other important developments include advances in of one strategy over another (Folkman et al., 1986). This
the conceptualization and measurement of coping (e.g., may explain the observation that the use of multiple cop-
Folkman, laTarus, Dunkel-Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, ing styles may be most adaptive in managing at least some
1986; Holahan & Moos, 1987; Stone & Neale, 1984). For stressful events (e.g., Collins, Taylor, & Skokan, 1989).
example, as conceptualized by Lazarus and his associates, Many issues in coping remain to be investigated. In
coping is initiated by an appraisal process secondary to particular, the relationship between ongoing self-regula-
the assessment of circumstances as harmful, threatening, tory activities (such as mentally simulating potential
or challenging. In this view, a person judges his or her stressful events; Taylor & Schneider, 1989) and the ini-
resources, such as time or money, assesses his or her cop- tiation of specific coping activities to deal with specific
ing skills and abilities, and then determines whether or stressors has received relatively little attention. Coping
not they will be sufficient to overcome the threat or chal- researchers are also investigating some of the costs of cop-
lenge posed by a stressful event. Several measures of cop- ing, such as the energy expenditure and physiologic
ing now exist. They include the Ways of Coping instru- arousal that may be required when people must be vigilant

44 January 1990 • American Psychologist


in response to threatening events (Cohen, Evans, Stokols, involved in clinical practice, 55% in research activities,
& Krantz, 1986). and 50% in teaching and supervision (Houston, 1988).
Reflecting the composition of the division, interven-
Social Support tion has been a major concern of health psychology since
Coping research has also focused on coping resources, the inception of the field. Indeed, successful interventions
most particularly on social support. A substantial amount with patients undergoing noxious medical procedures
of research documents the psychological and physical such as surgery actually predated sophisticated models of
benefits of social support and shows how those with social stress and coping (see Janis, 1958). Psychological control
support adjust better psychologically to stressful events, has been the conceptual focus of much of this intervention
recover more quickly from already-diagnosed illness, and work. Those who believe that they can exert some con-
reduce their risk of mortality from specific diseases trolling behavior in response to a stressful event, whether
(House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988). Findings concerning behavioral or cognitive, appear to adjust better to those
the impact of social support on the likelihood of devel- stressful events than those without such feelings of control.
oping illness have been mixed (Holahan & Moos, 1986; Research documents the coping benefits of being told in
Sarason & Sarason, 1984; Wallston, Alagna, DeVellis, & advance what sensations to expect and why, being alerted
DeVellis, 1983). Certain of the positive effects of social to the specific procedures to be undertaken, and being
support appear to occur whether an organism is under given cognitive or behavioral coping strategies to use dur-
stress or not (direct effects), whereas other salutary effects ing the noxious procedure (Taylor & Clark, 1986;
of social support appear largely to exert a buffering effect Thompson, 1981). More recently, similar interventions
such that they are protective primarily when people are have been undertaken successfully with children awaiting
under high degrees of stress (Cohen & McKay, 1984). In noxious medical procedures (e.g., Melamed, 1986).
particular, studies that have measured social support in This is not to suggest that control is a panacea for
terms of social integration or social networks have tended stressful events, whether naturally occurring or induced
to report direct effects, whereas studies that have focused during treatment. Manipulations designed to enhance
on aid, resources, and emotional support from specific feelings of psychological control may produce feelings of
network members have tended to uncover buffering effects responsibility or blame instead (Kxantz & Schulz, 1980;
(Cohen & Wills, 1985). Rodin, 1986), and in many circumstances, they may ag-
Time and additional data have produced a more gravate negative consequences and lead to increased stress
differentiated view of support. For example, research now and worry (Burger, 1989; Rodin, 1986). Thus the potential
examines different kinds of social support, such as emo- benefits of the concept of psychological control and in-
tional support, information and advice, tangible assis- terventions based on it must be tempered by knowledge
tance, and appraisal support, identifying which types of of its potential psychological costs as well.
support are perceived to be helpful for which types of Some interventions have focused on the regulation
events (Cohen, 1988; Dunkel-Schetter, Folkman, & Laz- of emotional and physiologic states through relaxation
arus, 1987). A question that arises in this context is and guided imagery (Burish & Lyles, 1979), whereas oth-
whether social support may actually be an individual- ers have encouraged more confrontational coping meth-
difference resource such that some people have better ods, such as training in swallowing to ease the passage of
skills for making effective use of potential social support a tube in an endoscopic examination (Johnson & Lev-
than others (Dunkel-Schetter et al., 1987). enthal, 1974). Some intervention procedures use avoidant
Research also addresses the fact that social support techniques such as cognitive distraction, whereas others
is sometimes not forthcoming to those under stress enlist vigilant coping, as in inducing a person to reinter-
(Wortman & Dunkel-Schetter, 1979) and that some efforts pret a stressful experience as a positive one. Although all
to provide social support misfire and aggravate stressful of these types of interventions appear to be successful in
circumstances (Coyne, Wortman, & Lehman, 1988). So- reducing stress, those involving relaxation, avoidance, and
cial support researchers are also increasingly turning their cognitive restructuring have been the most commonly
attention to the problems that providers experience in used, perhaps because many noxious medical procedures
their attempts to provide social support, as in trying to are typically only minimally conducive to direct patient
care for an ill family member (see, e.g., Coyne et al., intervention. Indeed, the psychological technology of re-
1988; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 1987; Schulz, Tompkins, laxation has proven to have wide applicability to a variety
Wood, & Decker, 1987). Other outstanding issues in the of issues relating to health and illness, including its use
field include how best to measure social support (e.g., as an accompanying intervention in efforts to modify
Heitzmann & Kaplan, 1988) and the need to identify the health habits such as obesity or smoking (see Rodin &
psychologic and biologic pathways by which different as- Salovey, 1989); its application to stress management
pects of social support affect health (Cohen, 1988). (Suinn, 1982); its widespread use in the management of
pain (Turner & Chapman, 1982a); and its impact on cop-
Interventions to Improve Coping Witk Stressful Events ing with chronic disease and its treatments (Taylor, Licht-
Health Psychology became Division 38 of the American man, & Wood, 1984b).
Psychological Association in 1978; the division has ap- Aided self-help is another low-cost effective inter-
proximately 3,000 members, roughly 65% of whom are vention technique that psychologists and health practi-

January 1990 • American Psychologist 45


tioners have implemented across a broad array of health to cut across specific diseases and specific issues of health
problems. Programs designed to help smokers quit on and illness. For example, the importance of feelings of
their own using manuals provided by the American Can- personal control emerges in whether people practice par-
cer Society and other organizations are a reasonably ef- ticular health behaviors, whether they experience stress,
fective and cost-effective way of inducing people to quit whether their pain control efforts are successful, and how
(e.g., Cummings, Emont, Jaen, & Seiandra, 1988). Self- they adapt to chronic disease and disability. Relaxation
help telephone lines (Ossip-Klein, Shapiro, & Stiggins, training, a psychological intervention that requires little
1984) also provide support for those who are attempting training and expense, can be applied in a wealth of set-
with difficulty to maintain behavior change over time, tings, as noted earlier. Thus, unlike medicine, which is
such as not smoking. highly specialized and organized around particular dis-
eases, health psychology affords the opportunity to look
Conclusion beyond particular disorders to the broad principles of
This brief overview of the health psychology field may be thought and behavior that cut across specializations of
significant less for what it has included than for what it diseases or problems studied to elucidate more funda-
has left out. The field is now so diverse and productive mental psychosocial mechanisms.
that no one article can easily cover all of the significant
trends. For example, the review touched only briefly on Trends Affecting Health Psychology
the enormous and important areas of health promotion In the past 10 years, I have had the opportunity to com-
and health behavior modification, areas in which health ment on the developing field of health psychology on four
psychologists have made consistent and exceptional con- prior occasions. What seems to distinguish the present
tributions. Research on substance abuse is not addressed occasion from prior ones is an increasing sense that health
at all, nor the complex question of ethnic, socioeconomic, psychologists are now better integrated into the health
gender, and racial patterns in health and illness (see Rodin enterprise. No longer acting primarily as consultants, sta-
& Salovey, 1989, for a discussion of these issues). Only a tistical advisors, or peripheral members of research teams,
passing nod was given to the huge problem of nonadher- health psychologists now number heavily among the chief
ence to medical treatment regimens (Meichenbaum & architects framing the research questions, providing the
Turk, 1987), which can be as high as 95% for some health conceptual structure, developing the research designs, and
care recommendations. Pain mechanisms and pain man- carrying the projects to fruition. In the early days of re-
agement received little mention, There is also little cov- search in this field, we often worked alone or with a few
erage of the problems and issues associated with particular students, seeking each other's counsel concerning how
chronic diseases, including the most common ones of best to reach practitioners, obtain samples, and convince
coronary artery disease, cancer, stroke, and diabetes medical establishments of the value of our enterprise.
(Burish & Bradley, 1983). And finally, research investi- That has changed. We seem no longer to need each other
gations on biobehavioral pathways to disease was alluded so much, as many of us are well integrated into the col-
to only briefly (for examples of this work in the areas of laborative arrangements we have engineered with other
CHD and hypertension, see Cinciripini, 1986a, 1986b; health professionals. In so doing, we have come face to
Krantz & Manuek, 1984). face with some of the major issues facing the health en-
Despite the gaps in this review, certain commonal- terprise.
ities in the field may be highlighted. Research that ex- One of the major forces facing health psychology, as
amines whether or not psychological and social factors well as every other disciplinary contributor to behavioral
are involved in health and illness has largely made its medicine, is the growing cost of health care services and
point. More recent investigations have gone beyond the the accompanying mounting pressures to contain costs.
demonstration of such simple relationships to an attempt Fueled by the spiraling expense of high-tech medicine
to specify the models and pathways whereby psychological and the increasing costs of malpractice cases and insur-
and social factors can be integrated into the biology of ance, health care now costs roughly $400 billion a year
health and illness in multifactorial causal chains. This in the United States alone. The growing spectre of the
trend is evidenced in research on health promotion, stress AIDS crisis threatens to push costs even further, simul-
and illness, personality and disease, coping, social support, taneously putting affordable health care and insurance
and the factors affecting recovery. These investigations out of reach of increasing numbers of people.
have addressed the direct impact of stress and other psy- This unhappy reality is relevant to health psychol-
chological states on physiological processes, the impact ogists in several respects. It nudges us to keep an eye on
of psychological and social factors on risky health prac- the bottom line in research and intervention. Although
tices, and the impact of psychological and social factors effective health care interventions are an important goal
on how people respond to potential illness states, such as of the field, the likelihood of their being integrated into
whether or not they engage in appropriate illness behavior. medical practice will be influenced by their cost-effec-
As such, the field has advanced to an unprecedented level tiveness. Subtly, the pressures of cost containment push
of complexity in research investigations. us in the direction of research questions designed to keep
Yet simultaneously the field has also succeeded in people out of the health care system altogether. On the
identifying certain broad principles of behavior that seem clinical practice side, interventions increasingly examine

46 January 1990 • American Psychologist


the benefits and liabilities of self-help groups, peer coun- behaviors as well as in how to cope psychologically with
seling, self-management programs such as those for he- their risk status.
modialysis (Kirschenbaum, Sherman, & Penrod, 1987), Health psychologists may serve the effort toward
and other inexpensive ways to provide service delivery to primary prevention even further by refocusing health ef-
those who might otherwise not receive care. Research forts on ways to keep people from developing problematic
suggesting that the stress-reduction and pain-amelioration health habits initially. This trend represents part of an
benefits of expensive biofeedback equipment and training increasing emphasis within health psychology on opti-
can be achieved by simple, less expensive techniques of mizing health rather than on preventing illness (Evans,
relaxation (e.g., Blanchard et al., 1988; Turner & Chap- 1988). At present, the concept of a healthy life-style has
man, 1982a, 1982b) is consistent with this viewpoint. On clear m~dia appeal but less applicability to the general
the research side, the emphasis on cost containment draws population. Because health habits are only modestly in-
researchers heavily into primary prevention activities de- tercorrelated, bringing about integrated life-style change
signed to keep people healthy with the goal of reducing essential in the prevention of certain disorders, such as
the use of health care services. By identifying the risks of coronary artery disease, is difficult. At least some of the
smoking and drinking and the health benefits of exercise, emphasis on health habit change should go to developing
stress management, and a proper diet and by developing methods designed to modify more than one health habit
programs that best help people to achieve a healthy life- simultaneously, such as smoking, diet control, and ex-
style, psychology contributes to the larger endeavor that ercise among individuals at risk for CHD.
attempts to keep people healthy with the ultimate goal Because there is an applied component to the field,
of containing health care costs. Whether this implicit goal health psychology is necessarily responsive to social
is a pipe dream remains to be seen. Research examining problems and issues, including those within medicine.
the efficacy of health behavior interventions in reducing One need not be clairvoyant to appreciate certain de-
disease and lowering health costs has so far not been very velopments that can be expected as a result. Increasingly,
encouraging (Kaplan, 1984). To date, however, the evi- the field will be called on to address concerns of aging,
dence has only slightly tempered the idealistic goal. including the problems of living with chronic disability
There are benefits and risks attached to the formi- and disease, the problems of adjustment to bereavement
dable role that economic exigencies play in the field. On and geographic relocation (Rowe & Kahn, 1987), and
the one hand, health psychology cannot afford to pursue psychological changes associated with aging.
its scientific and clinical mission without at least some This point illustrates a larger concern of health psy-
regard to cost. To do so would produce an ivory-tower chologymnamely, the need to monitor changing patterns
isolation that would render its results of limited use. On of illness and disease and their implications. To the extent
the other hand, cost-containment issues can compromise that we can successfully anticipate health- and illness-
the scientific and practical mission of the field by choking related problems of the future, we can begin to anticipate
offprematurely areas of inquiry that do not immediately now how we must prepare for them. With respect to the
appear to be cost-effective. The relative lack of attention elderly, for example, we need to identify the kinds of living
to issues of rehabilitation, in contrast to the heavy pre- situations these increasing numbers of people will have
ponderance of research in primary prevention activities, and what kinds of economic resources will be available
can be regarded as one casualty of these pressures. Just to them. These factors in turn will influence their health
as we must keep an intermittent eye on the bottom line habits, their levels of health, and their need to seek treat-
to avoid putting our science and practice out of financial ment, all of which will require advance planning.
reach, we must also watch the bottom line to be sure that Foretelling the future is never an easy task. Some
it does not come to dictate the nature of the field as a trends are obvious and have relatively clear implications
basic scientific enterprise. for the field; others are not so easily anticipated, and thus
The emphasis on primary prevention, both in med- their implications for health psychology are still elusive.
icine and in health psychology, will likely increase, es- The foregoing set of issues represents a mere fraction of
pecially as medicine itselfbecomes more oriented toward the ways in which health psychology will be shaped and
preventive health activities. Although the incidence of molded by the changing dimensions of medicine and
heart disease, stroke, and infectious disease is decreasing, medical practice.
the incidence of cirrhosis, lung cancer, and automobile Only one prediction regarding the future of health
deaths is still increasing. Our high-frequency illnesses and psychology can be generated with confidence--namely,
growing health problems continue to be tied directly to that articles like this one will gradually disappear from
behavioral pathogens or life-style factors (Matarazzo, the literature. The diversity of issues studied and the
1983). Consequently, there will be a continuing role for complexity and sophistication of the models and designs
health psychologists in this endeavor. As medicine and used to explore them will preclude simple statements
health psychology pay increasing attention to risky health about the major empirical directions and developments
behaviors, the at-risk role may become a more important of the field. Asking "What's new in health psychology?"
construct (e.g., Allen et al., 1987). Individuals who are will be like asking "What's new in psychology or in med-
identified early as at risk for particular disorders need to icine?", queries that can be answered in only the most
be trained in how to change any modifiable risk-relevant general and superficial ways. Those of us who have reg-

January 1990 • American Psychologist 47


ularly taken the temperature and pulse of the field and hypothes~: A theoretical analysis. In A. Baum, J. E. Singe~ & S. E.
Taylor (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and health (Vol. 4, pp. 253-
confidently offered diagnoses and prognoses will be out 267). Hillsdale, NJ: Eflbaum.
of business, for whatever trends could be culled from the Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering
myriad and diverse directions in the field will be dwarfed hypothe~. PsychologicalBulletin, 98, 310-357.
in significance by the divergences. Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Skokan, L. A. (1989), A better worldor
a shattered vision?Positive and negative assumptions about the world
following victimization. Manuscript submitted for publication.
REFERENCES Coyne, J. C., Wortman, C. B., & Lehman, D. R. (1988). The other side
of support: Emotional overinvolvement and miscarried helping. In
Ameck, G., Tennen, H., Pfeiffer, C., & Fifield, J. (1987). Appraisals of B. Gottlieb (Ed.), Marshalling social support (pp. 305-330). Newbury
control and predictability in adapting to a chronic disease. Journal Park, CA: Sage Pubfications.
of Personality and Social Psychology,, 53, 273-279. Cummings, IC M., Emont, S. L., Jaen, C., & Sciandra, R. (1988). Format
Alexander, E (1950). Psychosomatic medicine. New York: Norton. and quitting instructions as factors influencing the impact of a self-
Allen, M. T., Lawler, K. A., Mitchell, V. P., Matthews, K. A., Rakaczky, administered quit smoking program. Health Education Quarterly, 15,
C. J., & Jamison, W. (1987). Type A behavior pattern, parental history 199-216.
of hypertension, and cardiovascular reactivity in college males. Health Dembroski, T. M., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Coronary prone behavior:
Psychology,, 6, 113-130. Components of the Type A pattern and hostility. Journal of Personality,
American Heart Association. (1984a). Exercise and your heart. Dallas, 55, 211-235.
TX: American Heart Assocmtion. Derngatis, L. R., Abeloff, M., & Melasaratos, N. (1979). Psychological
American Heart Association. (1984b). Nutritional counseling for car- coping mechanisms and survival time in metastatic breast cancer.
diovascular health. Dallas, TX: American Heart Association. Journal of the American Medical Association, 242, 1504-1508.
American Heart Association. (1988). Cigarette smoking and cardiovas- Dohrenwend, B. S., Dohrenwend, B. P., Dodson, M., & Shrout, P. E.
cular disease: Special report for the public. Dallas, TX: American (1984). Symptoms, hassles, social supports, and life events: Problem
Heart Association. of confounded measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 222-
Ashley, E, Jr., & Kannel, W. (1974). Relation of weight change to changes 230.
in atherogenic traits: The Framingham Study. Journal of Chronic Dis- Dunbar, E (1943). Psychosomatic diagnosis. New York: Hoeber.
eases, 27, 103-114. Dunkel-Schetter, C., Folkman, S., & laTarus, R. S. (1987). Correlates
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social of social support receipt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,,
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hail. 53, 71-80.
Baurnann, L. J., & Leventhal, H. (1985). I can tell when my blood Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for
pressure is up, can't I? Health Psychology, 4, 203-218. biomedicine. Science, 196, 126-129.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1982). Family environments and ad- Evans, R. I. (1988). Health promotion--science or ideology? Health
aptation: A clinically applicable typology.American Journal of Family Psychology,, 7, 203-219.
Therapy, 10, 26-38. Evans, R. I., Rozelle, R. M., Mittelmark, M. B., Hansen, W. B., Bane,
Blanchard, E. B., McCoy, G. C., Wittrock, D., Musso, A., Gerardi, A. L., & Havis, J. (1978). Deterring the onset of smoking in children:
R. J., & Pangburn, L. (1988). A controlled comparison of thermal Knowledge of immediate physiological effects and coping with peer
biofeedback and relaxation training in the treatment of essential hy- pressure, media pressure, and parent modeling. Journal of Applied
pertension: II. Effects on cardiovascular reactivity. Health Psychology, Social Psychology, 8, 126-135.
7, 19-33. Fleming, I., Baum, A., Davidson, L. M., Rectanus, E., & McArdle, S.
Botvin, G. J., & Eng, A. (1982). The efficacy of a multicomponent ap- (1987). Chronic stress as a factor in physiologic reactivity to challenge.
proach to the prevention of cigarette smoking. Preventive Medicine. Health Psychology, 6, 221-237.
11, 199-211. Fleming, R., Baum, A., Gisriel, M. M., & Gatehel, R. J. (1982, Septem-
Brownell, IC D., Marlatt, G. A., Lichtenstein, E., & Wilson, G. T. (1986). ber). Mediating influences of social support on stress at Three Mile
Understanding and preventing relapse. American Psychologist, 41, Island. Journal of Human Stress, 14-22.
765-782. Folkman, S., lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schette~ C., DcLongis, A., & Gruen,
Bulman, J. R., & Wortman, C. B. (1977). Attributions of blame and R. J. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter. Cognitive appraisal,
coping in the "real world": Severe accident victims react to their lot. coping, and encounter outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 35, 351-363. Psychology, 50, 992-1003.
Bur$~ J. M. (1989). Negative reactions to increases in perceived personal Fox, B. H. (1978). Premorbid psychological factors as related to cancer
control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 246-256. incidence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 1, 45-134.
Burish, T. G., & Bradley, L. A. (1983). Coping with chronic disease: Friedman, H. S., & Booth-Kewley, S. (1987). The "disease-prone per-
Research and applications. New York: Academic Press. sonality": A meta-analytic view of the construct. American Psychol-
Burish, T. G., & Lyles, J. N. (1979). Effectiveness of relaxation training ogist, 42, 539-555.
in reducing the ~ e s s of chemotherapy in the treatment of cancer. Friedman, H. S., & Booth.Kewley, S. (1988). Validity of the Type A
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychology, 1O, 357- construct: A reprise. PsychologicalBulletin, 104, 381-384.
361. Friedman, H. S., & DiMatten, M. R. (1989). Health psychology. Engle-
Cannon, W. B. (1932). The wisdom of the body, New York: Norton. wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Carve~ C. S., Scheie~ M. E, & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping Friedman, M., Thoresen, C. E., Gill, J. J., Ulmer, D., Powell, L. H.,
strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Price, V. A., Brown, B., Thompson, L., Rabin, D. D., Breall, W. S.,
Social Psychology, 56, 267-283. Bour~ E., Levy, R., & Dixon, T. (1986). Alteration of Type A behavior
Cincirip~ni, P. M. (1986a). Cognitive stress and cardiovascolar reactivity. and its effect on cardiac recurrences in post myocardial infarction
I. Relationship to hypertension.American Heart Journal 112, 1044- patients: Summary results of the recurrent coronary prevention proj-
1050. ect. American Heart Journal, 112, 653-665.
Cinciripiui, P. M. (1986b). Cognitive stress and cardiovascular reactivity. Glass, D. C. (1977). Behavior patterns, stress, and coronary disease.
II. Relationship to atherosclero~, arrhythmias, and cognitive control. Hillsdale, NJ: Edhaum.
American Heart Journal, 112, 1051-1065. Hanson, J. D., Larson, M. E., & Snowden, C. T. (1976). The effects of
Cohen, S. (1988). Psychosocial models of the role of social support in control over high intensity noise on plasma cortisol levels in rhesus
the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology,, 7. 269-297. monkeys. Behavioral Biology, 16, 333-334.
Cohen, S., Evans, G. W., Stokols, D., & Krantz, D. S. (1986). Behavior, Heitzmann, C. A., & Kaplan, R. M. (1988). Assessment of methods for
health, and environmental stress. New York: Plenum. measuring social support. Health Psychology, 7, 75-109.
Cohen, S., & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress and the buffering Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1986). Personality, copin~ and family

48 J a n u a r y 1990 • A m e r i c a n Psychologist
resources in stress resistance: A longitudinalanalysis. Journal of Per- Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychologicalstress and the coping process. New
sonality and Social Psychology, 51, 389-395. York: McGraw-Hill.
Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1987). Personal and contextual deter- Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.
minants of coping strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- New York: Springer.
chology, 52, 946-955. R. S., & Launie~ R. (1978). Stress-related transactions between
House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships person and environment. In L. A. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds.), Internal
and health. Science, 241, 540-545. and external determinants of behavior (pp. 287-327). New York:
Houston, B. K. (1988). Division 38 survey: Synopsis of results. The Plenum.
Health Psychologist, 10, 2-3. Leventhal, H., & Cleary, P. D. (1980). The smoking problem: A review
Janis, I. L. (1958). Psychological stress. New York: WHey. of the research and theory in behavioral risk modification. Psycho-
Janz, N. K., & Becke~ M. H. (1984). The health belief model: A decade logical Bulletin, 88, 370-405.
later. Health Eduction Quarterly, 11, 1-47. Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The commonsense rep-
Jemmott, J. B. III, Croyle, R. T., & Ditto, P. H. (1988). Commonsense resentation of illness danger. In S. Rachman (Ed.), Contributions to
epidemiology: Self-hased judgments from laypersous and physicians. medical psychology (pp. 7-30). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.
Health Psychology,, 7, 55-73. Levy, S., Herberman, R., Lippman, M., & d'Angelo, T. (1987). Corre-
Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1982). Adolescence to young adulthood: A lation of stress factors with sustained depression of natural killercell
twelve year prospective study of problem behavior and psychosocial activity and predicted prognosis in patients with breast cancer. Jourrud
development. In A. A. Mednick & M. Harway (Eds.), Longitudinal of Clinical Oncology, 5, 348-353.
research in the United States (pp. 34-61). Boston: Martinns Nijhoff. Levy, S., Herberman, R., Maluish, A., Sehlien, B., & Lippman, M. (1985).
Johnson, J. E., & Leventhal, H. (1974). Effects of accurate expectations Prognostic risk assessment in primary breast cancer by behavioral
and behavioral instructions on reactions during a noxious medical and immunological parameters. Health Psychology, 4, 99-113.
examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 710- Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (Eds.). (1985). Relapse prevention:
718. Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. New
Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaeffer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). York: Guilford.
Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and Mason, J. W. (1974). Specificity in the organization of neuro-endoerine
uplifts versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 1- response profiles. In P. Seeman & G. M. Brown (Eds.), Frontiers in
39. neurology and neuroscience research. First International Symposium
Kaplan, R. M. (1984). The connection between clinical health promotion of the Neuroscience Institute. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University
and health status: A critical ~ e w . American Psychologist, 39, 755- of Toronto.
765. Matarazzo, J. D. (1980). Behavioral health and behavioral medicine:
Kasl, S. V. (1983). Pursuing the link between stressful life experiences Frontiers for a new health psychology. American Psychologist, 35,
and disease: A time for reappraisal. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Stress 807-817.
research (pp. 79-102). New York: Wiley. Matarazzo, J. D. (1982). Behavioral health's challenge to academic, sci-
Kemeny, M. E., Weiner, H., Taylor, S. E., Schneider, S., Visscher, B., & entitle, and professional psychology. American Psychologist, 37, 1-
Fahey, J. L. (1989). Repeated bereavement, depressed mood, and im- 14.
mune parameters in HIV seropositive and seronegative homosexual Matarazzo, J. D. (1983). Behavioral health: A 1990 challenge for the
men. Manuscript submitted for publication. health sciences professions. In J. D. Matarazzo, N. E. Millet; S. M.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (in press). Behavioral influences on Weiss, J. A. Herd, & S. M. Weiss(Eds.), Behavioral health:A handbook
immune function: Evidence for the interplay between stress and health. of health enhancement and disease prevention (pp. 3--40). New York:
In T. Field, P. McCabe, & N. Schneiderman (Eds.), Stress and coping Wiley.
(Vol. 2)- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Matarazzo, J. D., Weiss, S. M., Herd, J. A., Millet; N. E., & Weiss,
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glas~ R., Shuttleworth, E. C., Dye~ C. S., Ogrocki, S. M. (Eds.). (1984). Behavioral health: A handbook of health en-
P., & Speicher, C. E. (1987). Chronic stress and immunity in family hancement and disease prevention. New York: Wiley.
earegivers of Alzheimer's disease victims. Psychosomatic Medicine, Matthews, K. A. (1982). Psychological perspectives on the Type A be-
49, 523-535. havior pattern. PsychologicalBulletin, 91, 293-323.
Kirschenbaum, D. S., Sherman, J., & Penrod, J. D. (1987). Promoting Matthews, K. A. (1988). Coronary heart disease and Type A behavior:.
self-directed hemodialysis: Measurement and cognitive-behavioral Update on and alternative to the Booth-Kewley and Friedman (1987)
intervention. Health Psychology,, 6, 373-385. quantitative review. PsychologicalBulletin, 104, 373-380.
Krantz, D. S., Grunherg, N. E., & Baum, A. (1985). Health psychology. Mechanic, D. (1979). The stability of health and illness behavior. Results
Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 349-383. from a 16-year followup.American Journal of Public Health, 69, 1142-
Krantz, D. S., & Manuck, S. B. (1984). Acute psychephysiologlereactivity 1145.
and risk of cardiowasculardisease: A review and mcthodologle critique. Meichenbaum, D., & Turk, D. C. (1987). Facilitating treatment adher-
Psychological Bulletin, 96, 435-464. ence. New York: Plenum.
Krantz, D. S., & Sehulz, R. (1980). A model of life crisis, control, and
health outcomes: Cardiac rehabilitation and relocation of the elderly. Melamed, B. G. (1986). Special issue on child health psychology. Health
In A. Bantu & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psy- Psychology, 5(3).
chology Volume2."Applications ofpersonal control (pp. 25-60). Hills- Miller, D. T., & Porter, C. A. (1983). Self-blame in victims of violence.
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 139-152.
Langer, E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and enhanced Millstein, S. G., & Irwin, C. E., Jr. (1987). Concepts •fhea•th and illness:
personal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an insti- Different constructs of variations on a theme? Health Psychology, 6,
tutional setfin&Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,,34, 191- 515-524.
198. Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial Research Group (MRFIT).
Lau, R. R., Bernard, T. M., & Hartman, K. A. (1989). Further explo- (1982). Multiple risk factor intervention trial: Risk factor changes
rations of common sense representations of common illnesses. Health and mortality results. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Psychology, 8, 195-219. 248, 1465-1477.
Lau, R. R., Kane, R., Berry, S., Ware, J., & Roy, D. (1980). Channeling Ossip-Klein, D. J., Shapiro, R. M., & Stiggins, J. (1984). Brief report:
health: A review of televised health campaigns. Health Education Freedom line: Increasing utilization of a telephone support service
Quarterly, 7, 56-89. for ex-smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 9, 227-230.
Laudenslage~ M. L., Ryan, S. M., Drugan, R. C., Hyson, R. L., & Maier, Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Pessimistic
S. E (1983). Coping and immunosuppression: Inescapable but not explanatory style is a risk factor for physical illness: A thirty-five-year
escapable shock suppresses lymphocyte proliferation. Science, 231, longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 55,
568-570. 23-27.

J a n u a r y 1990 ° A m e r i c a n Psychologist 49
Rodin, J. (1986). Aging and health: Effects of the sense of control. Science, nonavoidant coping strategies: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 4,
233, 1271-1276. 249-288.
Rodin, J., & Salovey, P. (1989). Health psyeholngy. Annual Review of Taylor, S. E. (1984). Issues in the study of coping: A commentary. Cancer,
Psychology,, 40, 533-579. 53, 2313-2315.
Rogers, R. W. (1984). Changing health-related attitudes and behavior: Taylor, S. E. (1986). Health psychology. New York: Random House.
The role of preventive health psychology. In J. H. Harvey, J. E. Mad- Taylor, S. E., & Clark, L. E (1986). Does information improve adjustment
dux, R. P. MeGlynn, & C. D. Stoltenberg (Eds.), Socialperception in to noxious events? In M. J. Saks & L. Saxe (Eds.), Advances in applied
clinical and consultingpsychology (Vol. 2, pp. 91-112). Lubbock: Texas social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1-28). Hillsdale, NJ: Edbaum.
Tech University Press. Taylor, S. E., Liehtman, R. R., & Wood, J. V. (1984a). Attributions,
Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (! 987). Human aging: Usual and successful. beliefs about control, and adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of
Science, 237, 143-149. Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 489-502.
Sarason, 1. G., & Sarason, B. R. (Eds.). (1984). Socialsupport: Theory, Taylor, S. E., Lichtman, R. R., &Wood, J. V. (19~4b). Compliance with
research and applications. The Hague, The Netherlands: Martinus chemotherapy among breast cancer patients. Health Psychology,, 3,
Nijhof. 553-562.
Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Taylor, S. E., & Sehneide~ S. K. (1989). Coping and the simulation of
Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. events. Social Cognition, 7, 176-196.
Health Psychology, 4, 219-247. Thompson, S. C. (1981). Will it hurt less ifI can control it? A complex
Scbeier, M. E, Matthews, K. A., Owens, J., Magovern, G. J., ST., Lefebvre, answer to a simple question. PsychologicalBulletin, 90, 89-101.
R. C., Abbott, R. A., & Carve, C. S. (in press). Dispositional optimism Thoresen, C. E., Friedman, M., Gill, J. K., & Ulmer, D. (1982). Recurrent
and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The beneficial coronary prevention project: Some preliminary findings. Acta Medica
effects on physical and psychological weB-being.Journal of Personality Scandinavica Supplement, 660, 172-192.
and Social Psychology, Turk, D. C., Rudy, T. E., & Salovey,P. (1984). Health protection: Attitudes
Sehulz, R., Tompkius, C., Wood, D., & Decker, S. (1987). The social and behaviors of LPNs, teachers, and college students. Health Psy-
psychology of caregiving: The physical and psychological costs of pro- chology, 3, 189-210.
viding support to the disabled. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Turk, D. C., Rudy, T. E., & Salovey, P. (1985). Implicit models of illness.
17, 401--428. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9, 453--474.
Schwartz, G. E. (1982). Testing the biopsychosocial model: The ultimate Turner, J. A., & Chapman, C. R. (1982a). Psychological interventions
challenge facing behavioral medicine? Journal of Consulting and for chronic pain: A critical review. 1. Relaxation training and bio-
Clinical Psychology, 50, 1040-1052. feedback. Pain, 12, 1-21.
Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill. Turner, J. A., & Chapman, C. R. (1982b). Psychological interventions
Shaver, K. G., & Drown, D. (1986). On causality, responsibility, and for chronic pain: A critical review. II. Operant conditioning, hypnosis,
self-blame: A theoretical note. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- and cognitive-behavioral therapy. Pain, 12, 23-46.
chology, 50, 697-702. Wallston, B. S., Alagna, S. W., DcVellis, B. McE., & DcVellis, R. E
Smith, C. A. (1989). Dimensions of appraisal and physiological response (1983). Social support and physical health. Health Psychology, 2, 367-
in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 56, 339- 391.
353. Weinberger, D. A. (in press). The construct validity of the repressive
Stein, M., Keller, S. E., & Schleifer, S. J. (1985). Stress and immunom- coping style. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and association: Defense
odulation: The role of depression and neuroendocrine function. Jour- mechanisms and personality style. Chicago: University of Chicago
nal of Immunology, 135, 827s-833s. Press.
Stone, A. A., & Neale, J. M. (1984). New measure of daily coping: Wills, T. A. (! 986). Stress and coping in early adolescence: Relationships
Development and preliminary results. Journal of Personality and So- to substance use in urban school samples. Health Psychology, 5, 503-
cial Psychology, 46, 892-906. 529.
Suinn, R. M. (1982). Intervention with Type A behaviors. Journal of Wortman, C. B., & Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1979). Interpersonal relation-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 797-803. ships and cancer: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Social Issues, 35,
Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy of avoidant and 120-155.

50 J a n u a r y 1990 • A m e r i c a n P s y c h o l o g i s t

You might also like