Attachment Insecurity Moderates The Rela

You might also like

You are on page 1of 8

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Attachment Insecurity Moderates the Relationship


Between Disease Activity and Depressive
Symptoms in Ulcerative Colitis
Robert G. Maunder, MD,* William J. Lancee, PhD,* Jonathan J. Hunter, MD,*
Gordon R. Greenberg, MD,† and A. Hillary Steinhart, MD†

active.5,6 Although in principle, the relationship between dis-


Background: Among people with ulcerative colitis, depression ease activity and depression could be bidirectional, there is
occurs more frequently when inflammation is active. We hypothe-
very little evidence that depressive symptoms precede UC in-
sized that individual differences in interpersonal style affect the risk
flammatory activity.7,8 Therefore, it is more likely that the rela-
that active disease will be accompanied by depressive symptoms.
tionship between disease activity and depression is caused by
Methods: In this study, disease activity, depressive symptoms, and the causal influence of active disease, whether UC acts through
2 dimensions of interpersonal style, attachment anxiety and attach- the psychologic experience of being ill, through pathophysio-
ment avoidance, were measured in 146 ulcerative colitis outpatients logical changes associated with active inflammation, or through
at time 1 and in 99 of these patients at a second time-point, 7 to some other mechanism.
37 months later. Test–retest correlations of attachment anxiety (r = 0.83,
The presence of depressive symptoms or the full syn-
P , 0.001) and attachment avoidance (r = 0.76, P , 0.001) con-
drome of major depression during the course of UC is im-
firmed that these dimensions are stable.
portant for several reasons. The presence of depression in
Results: There was a stepwise increase in the correlation between medical illness is associated with amplification of symptom
time 2 disease activity and depression from the lowest tercile of at- severity, increased reporting of unexplained physical symp-
tachment anxiety (r = 0.00, P = 0.99), through the middle tercile toms, increased use of health care resources, and compromised
(r = 0.36, P = 0.05), to the highest tercile (r = 0.52, P = 0.002). For treatment adherence.9 The negative effects of depression and
attachment avoidance, disease activity and depression were only
active disease on health-related quality of life and function are
significantly correlated in the highest tercile (r = 0.49, P = 0.005).
additive.10 Thus, it is important to identify depression when it
Conclusions: Attachment anxiety meets all tested criteria as a mod- occurs in the course of UC.
erator of the relationship between disease activity and depressive It would be valuable to identify moderators of the rela-
symptoms. Further attention to interpersonal style as a moderator of tionship between disease activity and depression in UC, par-
depressive risk in ulcerative colitis is warranted. ticularly if identifying moderating factors allows for more
Key Words: attachment anxiety, depression, relationship style, effective or earlier identification of depression through closer
ulcerative colitis scrutiny of high-risk populations. Identification of moderators
may also help to identify targets for therapeutic intervention.
(Inflamm Bowel Dis 2005;11:919–926)
Interpersonal variables, especially those related to social
support, attachment relationships, isolation, and bereavement,
have long been considered to add to the risk of developing
T he prevalence of depression in individuals with ulcerative
colitis (UC) is similar or modestly higher than the prev-
alence of depression in healthy individuals,1–4 and depressive
depression,11 and, thus, individual differences in patterns of
interpersonal interaction may help to explain why some people
symptoms are more common and more severe when disease is become depressed when they are ill and others do not. At-
tachment theory is a particularly well-developed interpersonal
Received for publication June 9, 2005; accepted July 14, 2005. theory that describes the ways that people use close inter-
From the *Integrated Medicine Project, Department of Psychiatry, and personal relationships to achieve a sense of security.12 Feeling
†Division of Gastroenterology, Mount Sinai Hospital and University of secure refers to a subjective sense of safety and comfort. Sev-
Toronto, Toronto, Canada. eral lines of evidence suggest that attachment theory may be
Supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grant MOP-43985.
Reprints: R. Maunder, MD, Mount Sinai Hospital, 600 University Avenue,
valuable in determining why there are individual differences in
Toronto, Canada M5G 1X5 (e-mail: rmaunder@mtsinai.on.ca) the relationship between disease activity and depression and in
Copyright Ó 2005 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins identifying the people who are at greatest risk.

Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005 919
Maunder et al Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005

In adult attachment theory, individuals are considered to secure or insecure, where insecure attachment designates a
have relatively stable preferences and patterns of dealing with style high in attachment anxiety, high in attachment avoidance,
others in close relationships. A person’s typical pattern of or both.
relating to others in close relationships is called an attachment The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that
style. Although attachment style is a construct that is unfa- insecure attachment moderates the risk that active inflamma-
miliar to many internists, it is a sensible candidate moderator tion in UC will be associated with depression. That is, we
of the relationship between disease activity and depression for hypothesize that disease activity is more likely to be associated
3 reasons. First, relationships between attachment style and with depression in people with high attachment anxiety or high
medical outcomes have been confirmed in several settings, in- attachment avoidance. If this hypothesis is confirmed then it
cluding associations with health care use,13 medically un- may be possible to identify patients who are at high risk more
explained symptoms,14 and depression.15,16 There is growing effectively, and even to identify them before the onset of
attention to the impact of attachment relationships on the ex- depression. It may also provide clues about how to intervene to
perience of life stress,17–19 on the ability of attachment relation- reduce the risk of depression.
ships to buffer stress,20 and on the influence of attachment
style on relationship between life stress and a variety of medi-
cal outcomes in people with serious illness.21 Second, our expe- MATERIALS AND METHODS
rience is that attachment style can be easily recognized by Three hundred sixty-one patients with UC confirmed by
clinicians without structured assessment tools or special psy- endoscopy or biopsy, of at least 18 years of age, were identified
chologic training once clinicians are familiar with basic aspects by chart review and contacted by telephone. Exclusion criteria
of the construct. Third, modifications in physicians’ manage- were colectomy and indications of cardiovascular illness
ment of patients based on recognition of patterns of inter- (exclusion criteria for an aspect of the study not reported here).
personal style may reduce patient–provider difficulties.22–24 Forty percent (146) consented to participate and were assessed
Attachment theory describes the ways that a child and by a gastroenterologist, including physical examination and
his or her primary caregiver interact during early development,12 endoscopy, and completed self-reports of UC symptoms,
resulting in reliably identifiable patterns of attachment style attachment style, and depressive symptoms at time 1 (T1).
during the second year of development.25 For example, each Subjects were recontacted to provide information at a second
person develops preferred strategies for approach and with- time-point through a mailed survey. Ninety-nine subjects (68%)
drawal to other people who can provide security at the time of completed the measures of attachment style and depression
stress. Patterns of approach and withdrawal are determined in and the self-report items of the St. Mark’s index of UC
part by a person’s experience of the reliability, responsiveness, severity35 at time 2 (T2), which was 7 to 37 months after T1
and availability of others at times of need. Longitudinal studies (median = 686 d).
investigating the developmental continuity of patterns of at- The St. Mark’s index35 grades general health (scored
tachment support the general stability of attachment style over 0–3), abdominal pain (0–2), bowel frequency (0–2), stool
time 26,27 and identify conditions under which it can be consistency (0–2), blood in stool (0–2), anorexia (0–1), nausea
modified.26,28–30 Adult attachments are not primarily with and vomiting (0–1), abdominal tenderness (0–3), extraintes-
parents. Whereas romantic partnerships are prototypic attach- tinal manifestations (eye inflammation, arthralgia, oral ulcera-
ment relationships in adulthood, any relationship that affects tion, related skin lesions; 0–2), fever (0–2), and sigmoido-
one’s sense of security may be considered an attachment scopic appearance (0–2), generating a summary index of
relationship,31 and adults may have several people in their severity with scores ranging from 0 to 22. At T2, disease
‘‘inner circle’’ who serve this function.32 activity was based on the summed score of self-report items of
Self-report measures of adult attachment characterize an the St. Mark’s index: general health, abdominal pain, bowel
individual’s attachment style along 2 dimensions.33,34 The first frequency, stool consistency, blood in stool, anorexia, nausea,
is a dimension of dependency and concern about the avail- and vomiting. A high correlation between full St. Mark’s index
ability and responsiveness of others, called attachment anxiety. and an index composed only of the self-report items (R = 0.98,
This dimension describes the degree to which an individual P , 0.001) has previously been reported in this same subject
prefers close contact with another adult (a partner, a confidante, sample.36 We, therefore, expected the self-report index to
or a health care worker), is vigilant for the presence and re- provide a valid measure of disease activity in this group.
sponsiveness of the other, and depends on that contact for Attachment insecurity was measured with the Experi-
reassurance and security. The second is a dimension of auton- ence in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) questionnaire,
omy, called attachment avoidance. This dimension describes a 36-item questionnaire that probes attitudes toward close
the degree to which the individual requires independence relationships with intimate partners. Each item is a statement
and interpersonal distance to negotiate situations that threaten scored on a 7-point scale ranging from strongly disagree
personal security. Attachment style is often categorized as to strongly agree. The attachment anxiety scale and the

920 q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005 Disease Activity and Depressive Symptoms in UC

attachment avoidance scale (18 items each) of the ECR-R have 3. Attachment insecurity is not correlated with disease activ-
been derived through the application of item-response theory ity. Pearson correlations of each dimension of attachment
to choose the 36 best items from a pool of 323 attachment insecurity and disease activity were calculated.
items drawn from the commonly used attachment instruments, 4. The strength of the relationship between disease activity
all completed by 1086 undergraduate students.34,37 Although and depression is different at different levels of attachment
there is no gold standard measurement of adult attachment, test anxiety. Subjects were divided into 3 groups based on a
information (a measure of the extent to which the scale is tercile split of attachment anxiety at T1. In each group, the
informative across the full range of the underlying trait) is relationship between disease activity and depression at T2
greater in the ECR-R than in any other commonly used at- was expressed as a Pearson correlation coefficient and as an
tachment self-report instrument.37 estimated effect size (h2). Effect sizes were estimated using
Depressive symptoms were measured with the Center analysis of variance with CESD score as the dependent
for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression Scale (CES-D), a variable and disease severity score as the sole independent
20-item self-report screening instrument.38 In large commu- variable in each tercile of attachment anxiety. The analysis
nity samples, a cut-off of 16 has identified 84% to 94% of was repeated for attachment avoidance.
subjects classified as depressed by the Schedule of Affective To test for an interaction of attachment anxiety and
Disorder and Schizophrenia (SADS) structured interview, and attachment avoidance, this analysis was repeated, splitting the
a score of less than 16 has correctly identified 60% to 64% of sample into 4 groups based on a median split of attachment
subjects not classified as depressed on the SADS.39 anxiety (high/low) and a median split of attachment avoidance
Analyses were conducted in the following sequence to (high/low). Sample size does not permit examination of the
test attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance as moder- 9-cell table that would result from using a tercile split of each
ators of the relationship between UC disease activity and dimension of attachment insecurity in this analysis.
depression symptoms. For A to be a moderator of a relationship It is possible that this analysis could be biased by a
between B and O, the following criteria must be met according common underlying factor that influences self-reporting and
to the MacArthur model of mediators and moderators40: (1) B therefore is correlated with each of self-reported attachment
precedes O, (2) A precedes B, (3) A is not associated with B, style, depressive symptoms, and UC symptoms. As a check
and (4) different values of A result in different potency of on this potential source of error, depressive scores were com-
the relationship between B and O. Each of these criteria was pared in groups defined by physician-assessed items of the
examined separately in our UC sample. St. Mark’s index only. Subjects were divided into those with
1. Disease activity precedes depression. The correlation be- or without objective indications of inflammation on endoscopy
tween disease activity and depression is tested. The tem- or physical examination. Depressive scores were compared be-
poral precedence of disease activity is not tested but is tween the active UC and inactive UC groups using a t test. This
assumed, based on previous longitudinal studies.7,8 comparison was repeated for subjects with high attachment
2. Attachment insecurity precedes UC disease activity. In anxiety and subjects with low attachment anxiety.
theory, attachment style refers to a stable trait-like char- This study was approved by the Research Ethics Boards
acteristic with developmental roots that, therefore, precedes of Mount Sinai Hospital and the University Health Network.
current UC activity. Nonetheless, it is an empirical question All subjects provided informed consent.
whether the self-report methods of measuring attachment
anxiety and attachment avoidance employed in this study
yield a measure that is or is not sensitive to change in the RESULTS
context of changing disease activity. First, simple correla- Characteristics of the participants with respect to psy-
tions of attachment anxiety score at T1and at T2, several chologic and UC status are found in Table 1. Of 146 subjects,
months later, were calculated. Second, for each subject, T1 81 (55.1%) had inactive disease (St. Mark’s score of 0 or 1) at
and T2 disease activity scores were coded as 1 maximum T1. Most of those with active disease were experiencing mild
score and 1 minimum score. The 95% confidence intervals to moderate symptoms; of the maximum possible St. Mark’s
(CIs) of mean attachment anxiety at the time of maximum score of 22, subjects’ scores ranged from 0 to 16; 90% of
disease activity and mean attachment anxiety at the time of scores were less than or equal to 5. In comparing participants
minimum disease activity were compared. These 2 tests and nonparticipants at T2, nonparticipants were younger (mean,
serve to show if anxiety attachment is stable and if it varies 38 versus 45 yr; P = 0.001), earlier in the course of their dis-
with disease activity. Finally, as a methodological control ease (mean, 11 versus 16 yr; P = 0.001), and experiencing
for precedence in time, attachment anxiety at T1 was tested more depressive symptoms (mean CESD score, 13.6 versus
as a moderator of the effect size of the relationship between 9.3; P = 0.009).
disease activity and depression at T2, several months later. Table 2 shows a moderate concurrent association of
This analysis was repeated for attachment avoidance. disease activity and depressive symptoms (r = 0. 30–0.28),

q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 921


Maunder et al Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005

TABLE 1. Comparison of T1 Characteristics of Patients with UC Who Participated at T2 and Those Who Did Not
Statistical Significance of Difference
Full Group Nonparticipant at Participant at Between Nonparticipant and
(n = 146) T2 (n = 47) T2 (n = 99) Participant Groups (P)*

Female: number (%) 65 (44.5%) 20 (42.5%) 45 (45.4%) 0.86


Age: mean (SD) 42.7 (12.6) 37.6 (12.3) 45.2 (12.0) 0.001
Duration of UC (years): mean (SD) 14.6 (10.3) 10.8 (9.1) 16.4 (10.4) 0.001
UC disease activity: mean (SD) 2.2 (2.7) 2.5 (2.7) 2.1 (2.7) 0.37
Attachment anxiety: mean (SD) 2.5 (1.4) 2.7 (1.3) 2.3 (1.4) 0.08
Attachment avoidance: mean (SD) 2.7 (1.2) 3.0 (1.2) 2.6 (1.2) 0.06
Depressive symptoms: mean (SD) 10.7 (8.9) 13.6 (9.2) 9.3 (8.6) 0.009
*Significance of t test for difference of means, except sex, which was significance of x2.

a moderate concurrent association of attachment avoidance Table 3 shows that, whereas depression scores differed
and depression (r = 0.37–0.43), and a strong concurrent corre- significantly between the highest and lowest terciles of attach-
lation of attachment anxiety and depression (r = 0.56–0.70) at ment anxiety and that the same was true for attachment avoid-
each time-point. The correlation between disease activity and ance, there was no relationship between UC disease activity
depressive symptoms measured at different time-points was and either dimension of attachment insecurity. It is also note-
weak and nonsignificant (r = 0.19). Disease severity had a worthy that significant depressive symptoms were present even
negligible and nonsignificant association to each dimension of in the lowest tercile of attachment anxiety, as indicated by
attachment insecurity at both time-points (r = 0.01–0.16). 4 subjects (12.1%) with a clinically significant depression
With respect to the stability of attachment scores be- score (CESD $ 16).
tween time-points, the test–retest correlation of attachment Figure 1A shows a stepwise increase in the strength of
anxiety was 0.83 (P , 0.001) and of attachment avoidance was association between UC disease activity and depression as at-
0.76 (P , 0.001). At the time-point in which disease activity tachment anxiety increases. In the lowest tercile of attachment
was maximal for each individual, the mean attachment anxiety anxiety, the correlation between disease activity and depres-
score was 2.43 (95% CI, 2.16–2.70) and the mean attachment sion did not differ from 0 (r = 0.002, P = 0.99), and the es-
avoidance score was 2.57 (95% CI, 2.27–2.87). At the time- timated effect size was 0. In the highest tercile of attachment
point at which disease activity was minimal, attachment inse- anxiety, the correlation was strong (r = 0.52, P = 0.002), and
curity scores were indistinguishable from the former (mean the estimated effect size was 0.27.
attachment anxiety score, 2.39; 95% CI, 2.13–2.66; mean Figure 1B shows a strong association between UC dis-
attachment avoidance score, 2.74; 95% CI, 2.41–3.07). ease activity and depression in the highest tercile of attachment

TABLE 2. Intercorrelations Between Study Variables at T1 and T2 in 99 People with UC


T1 T2

Attachment Attachment Attachment


Avoidance Depression UC Activity Anxiety Avoidance Depression UC Activity

T1
Attachment anxiety 0.53† 0.56† 0.09‡ 0.83† 0.52† 0.61† 0.02‡
Attachment avoidance – 0.37† 0.11‡ 0.50† 0.76† 0.42† 0.06‡
Depression – – 0.30* 0.57† 0.32* 0.71† 0.19‡
Disease activity – – – 0.08‡ 0.16‡ 0.19 ‡ 0.40†
T2
Attachment anxiety – – – – 0.57† 0.70† 0.01‡
Attachment avoidance – – – – – 0.43† 0.01‡
Depression – – – – – – 0.28*
*P # 0.01; †P # 0.001; ‡P . 0.05.

922 q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005 Disease Activity and Depressive Symptoms in UC

TABLE 3. T1 Depression and Disease Activity Scores by Tercile Split of Attachment Scores
Depression (CESD Score) Disease Activity (St. Mark’s Index)

Mean (95% CI) CESD $16: Number (%) Mean (95% CI)

Attachment anxiety
Lowest tercile (n = 33) 5.52 (3.08–7.95) 4 (12.1%) 1.97 (0.91–3.03)
Middle tercile (n = 33) 7.72 (5.62–9.83) 6 (18.2%) 1.88 (1.23–2.52)
Highest tercile (n = 32) 15.13 (11.64–18.61) 14 (43.8%) 2.41 (1.27–3.55)
Attachment avoidance
Lowest tercile (n = 35) 3.43 (2.09–4.77) 1 (2.9%) 1.80 (0.82–2.77)
Middle tercile (n = 31) 11.77 (8.89–14.64) 12 (38.7%) 1.55 (0.76–2.33)
Highest tercile (n = 32) 13.63 (10.19–17.06) 11 (34.4%) 2.91 (1.86–3.96)

avoidance (r = 0.49, P = 0.005, estimated effect size = 0.24) Finally, the data were reanalyzed using physician-
and no correlation between disease activity and depression in determined inflammation from the endoscopic examination
the low and middle tertiles of attachment avoidance. instead of the St. Mark’s index to remove potential bias re-
Regarding possible interactive effects of attachment anx- sulting from self-reported symptoms of UC. This required ana-
iety and attachment avoidance, Figure 2 shows that, in a me- lyzing data from T1 only, because endoscopy was performed at
dian split (the highest degree of resolution available in this T1. As shown in Figure 3, the overall pattern of interaction
dataset), the correlation between disease activity and depres- between attachment and disease activity appears similar for
sion was negligible in all subjects with low attachment anxiety. attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. However,
A correlation of 0.30 in the high anxiety–low avoidance group
was not statistically significant (n = 11), whereas a correlation
of 0.45 in the larger high anxiety–high avoidance group (n =
32) was significant (P = 0.009). It is noteworthy that subjects
in the high attachment anxiety group tended to also have high
avoidance (x2 = 18.27, P , 0.001).
Three further analyses were performed to test for pos-
sible sources of error. First, the table of correlations (Table 2)
was used to test for multicollinearity between the 3 variables
of the moderation model (T1 attachment anxiety or T1 at-
tachment avoidance, T2 depression, T2 disease activity). The
highest correlation between these variables was a correlation
of r = 0.61 between T1 attachment anxiety and T2 depression.
Thus, attachment anxiety accounted for 37% (r2) of the vari-
ance in depression and multicollinearity did not interfere with
the interpretation of these data.41
Second, to test if the results were biased by differences
between subjects who agreed or did not agree to participate at
T2, the analysis was repeated for all subjects who participated
at T1 (n = 146) using attachment, disease activity, and
depression scores collected at T1. The results were similar to
those reported for the T2 participants alone. In particular, the
correlation between disease activity and depression in each
tercile of attachment anxiety was as follows: low attachment
anxiety, r = 20.03, P = 0.84; moderate, r = 0.40, P = 0.004;
high, r = 0.46, P , 0.001. The correlation between disease
activity and depression in each tercile of attachment avoidance FIGURE 1. The correlation between UC disease activity and
was as follows: low attachment avoidance, r = 0.08, P = 0.60; depressive symptoms in groups defined by tercile split of
moderate, r = 0.19, P = 0.20; high, r = 0.42, P = 0.002. attachment anxiety (A) or attachment avoidance (B).

q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 923


Maunder et al Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005

statistical testing showed that a pattern consistent with a disease group than in the inactive disease group when
moderating effect of attachment insecurity was present for attachment anxiety was high (P = 0.02) but not when attach-
attachment anxiety and not for attachment avoidance. Specif- ment anxiety was low (P = 0.47). For attachment avoidance, the
ically, mean depressive symptom score was higher in the active comparison of depressive symptoms between disease activity
groups was nonsignificant in both high and low avoidance
groups (high attachment avoidance, P = 0.11; low attachment
avoidance, P = 0.08).

DISCUSSION
This study shows that, in a population with mild to
moderately severe UC, there is a relationship between disease
activity and depressive symptoms and that this relationship is
moderated by the intensity of attachment insecurity. Among
those with insecure attachment, more severe disease is
associated with more severe depressive symptoms. Whereas
there are clinically significant depressive symptoms in some
people with secure attachment, these symptoms bear little or
no relationship to UC disease activity.
The moderating effect of attachment insecurity seems to
FIGURE 2. The correlation between disease activity and be primarily caused by attachment anxiety. The 2 dimensions
depression in groups defined by a median split of attachment of attachment insecurity are correlated, so that individuals high
anxiety and attachment avoidance. in attachment anxiety are also likely to be high in attachment
avoidance; however, attachment avoidance in itself does not
seem to increase the risk of concurrent elevation of disease
activity and depressive symptoms (Figs. 2 and 3).
This study has shown that attachment anxiety meets
all of the conditions of a moderating relationship except for
1 condition that was not tested. First, attachment anxiety has
been shown to be stable enough over time that it can be reliably
assumed to have been present before the occurrence of active
disease, because attachment anxiety has test–test stability of
r = 0.83, with tests separated by 7 to 37 months and because
there is no evidence that attachment anxiety changes when UC
disease activity changes. It bears emphasis that this analysis is
concerned with whether or not attachment anxiety is present
before a current episode of active disease and does not speak to
the longer-term question of a patient’s attachment style before
the onset of disease. Second, it is shown that there is no cor-
relation between attachment anxiety and disease activity. Third,
it is shown that the relationship between disease activity and
depression is stronger at higher levels of attachment anxiety.
The untested condition is that disease activity precedes
depression. If attachment anxiety moderates a causal relation-
ship between disease activity and depression, disease activity
must necessarily precede depression. This temporal order
would be consistent with previous studies.6–8 We consider the
alternative causal interpretation—that in the context of attach-
ment anxiety, depression provokes inflammatory activity—to
be less likely, but it is not ruled out by this study. Similarly,
FIGURE 3. Comparison of depression symptom score in sub-
jects with or without physician-observed signs of gut in- the methodology of this study does not rule out alternative
flammation at high and low levels of attachment anxiety (A) or explanations of the disease–depression relationship (such as
attachment avoidance (B). that each is caused by an unmeasured third factor).

924 q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005 Disease Activity and Depressive Symptoms in UC

An important implication of this result is that physicians experiencing mild to moderate severity UC. It is not known
can identify the patients who are at greatest risk of depression if the same results would hold in a more severely ill popula-
during active disease by identifying the patients who are high tion. Second, subjects who were available and agreed to par-
in attachment anxiety. The behavioral patterns that are typi- ticipate at T2 were different than nonparticipants with respect
cally observed in attachment anxiety are described else- to age, duration of disease, and depressive symptoms. T2
where12,23,31,42,43 but can be briefly summarized. The typical participants were older, later in the course of their UC, and less
features of high attachment anxiety in adults include (1) a depressed. However, the evidence for a moderating effect is
sense of personal fragility and fear, (2) an expectation of aban- essentially identical whether using T1 or T2 data and so this
donment, (3) dependency—a preference for remaining close possible bias does not seem to alter the evidence for modera-
to others who could provide security, (4) anxious vigilance tion by attachment anxiety. A further limitation is that this
regarding the proximity, responsiveness, and competence of study examines the risk that depressive symptoms will occur
others, (5) persistent, explicit expressions of distress and re- when UC is active. Further research using diagnostic evalua-
quests for help, which may take the form of symptom reports, tion is required to determine if this risk extends to a higher risk
and (6) a vague and dramatic style of speaking that com- of major depression.
municates emotional distress more effectively than it conveys It cannot be assumed that the relationship that has been
useful diagnostic information. shown in this study is causal. However, because attachment
Previous research shows that patients with high attach- anxiety is theoretically modifiable through psychotherapy or
ment anxiety tend to report physical symptoms that are not relationship experiences, a causal relationship is testable. If an
explained by their underlying illness.14 Although the reasons interpersonal factor moderates the disease–depression rela-
for this are not fully understood, it is thought that overreporting tionship, a trial of interpersonally focused psychotherapy to treat
of symptoms is the result of anxious vigilance toward one’s depression in UC may be promising. Interpersonal psycho-
inner sensations and a style of keeping other people close by therapy for depression is a brief modality of therapy with strong
frequently emitting signals of distress.23 In this case, the dis- empirical support for efficacy in mild to moderate depres-
tress signal is the report of a physical symptom, and the sig- sion,45,46 which can be modified for use in medical popula-
nificant ‘‘other’’ is the health care provider. This interpretation tions,47 but which has not been evaluated in depression in UC.
is consistent with research that shows higher rates of health In summary, a moderating effect of attachment anxiety
care use among patients high in attachment anxiety.13 on a statistical relationship between disease activity and
When high attachment anxiety is combined with high depression was found, which may have clinical value and
attachment avoidance, a person experiences conflict between implications for the treatment of depression in illness. Because
the preference for proximity to others and apprehension about there is no reason to assume that the moderating effect of
the interpersonal risks associated with closeness. This com- attachment anxiety is specific to UC, efforts at replication in
bination is designated the fearful attachment style.33,43,44 Pro- other medical conditions are encouraged to determine the
totypically, fearful individuals display an attitude of anxious extent to which this relationship is generalizable.
interpersonal avoidance. They may appear shy or overly
cautious.33,43 Compared with people with high attachment ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
anxiety who are not also high in avoidance, fearful individuals The authors thank Drs. Mark Silverberg, Alvin Newman,
may have a less dramatic communication style and a marked and Maria Cino for recruitment of subjects.
lack of assertiveness in making any requests of help.
This study may lead to further work directed toward
understanding the role of interpersonal factors in the course of REFERENCES
inflammatory bowel disease and other chronic diseases. First, 1. Helzer JE, Stillings WA, Chammas S, et al. A controlled study of the
there is no reason to believe that the moderating relationship association between ulcerative colitis and psychiatric diagnosis. Dig Dis
Sci. 1982;27:513–518.
studied here is specific to UC. Further study in Crohn’s disease 2. Andrews H, Barczak P, Allan RN. Psychiatric illness in patients with
and other chronic illnesses is warranted. Second, the role of inflammatory bowel disease. Gut. 1987;28:1600–1604.
interpersonal factors in the course of UC deserves further 3. Tarter RE, Switala J, Carra J, et al. Inflammatory bowel disease:
psychiatric status of patients before and after disease onset. Int J
study to determine if interpersonal factors have a causal effect Psychiatry Med. 1987;17:173–181.
on the course of illness or are simply markers of a differential 4. Schwarz SP, Blanchard EB, Berreman CF, et al. Psychological aspects
risk of depression in the course of disease. If interpersonal fac- of irritable bowel syndrome: comparisons with inflammatory bowel dis-
ease and nonpatient controls. Behav Res Ther. 1993;31:297–304.
tors affect disease course, interpersonal interventions, designed 5. North CS, Alpers DH, Helzer JE, et al. Do life events or depression
to increase social support or resilience, may be indicated for exacerbate inflammatory bowel disease? Ann Intern Med. 1991;114:
some individuals. 381–386.
6. Porcelli P, Zaka S, Centonze S, et al. Psychological distress and levels
The methods of this study impose limitations on the of disease activity in inflammatory bowel disease. Ital J Gastroenterol.
conclusions that can be made. First, the subjects studied were 1994;26:111–115.

q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 925


Maunder et al Inflamm Bowel Dis  Volume 11, Number 10, October 2005

7. Bitton A, Sewitch MJ, Peppercorn MA, et al. Psychosocial determinants 27. Waters E, Merrick S, Treboux D, et al. Attachment security in infancy and
of relapse in ulcerative colitis: a longitudinal study. Am J Gastroenterol. early adulthood: a twenty year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2000;71:
2003;98:2203–2208. 684–689.
8. Levenstein S, Prantera C, Varvo V, et al. Stress and exacerbation in 28. Klohnen EC, Bera S. Behavioral and experiential patterns of avoidantly
ulcerative colitis: a prospective study of patients enrolled in remission. and securely attached women across adulthood: a 31-year longitudinal
Am J Gastroenterol. 2000;95:1213–1220. perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:211–223.
9. Katon W. The impact of major depression on chronic medical illness. 29. Weinfield NS, Sroufe LA, Egeland B. Attachment from infancy to early
Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1996;18:215–219. adulthood in a high-risk sample: continuity, discontinuity, and their
10. Wells KB, Stewart A, Hays RD, et al. The functioning and well-being of correlates. Child Dev. 2000;71:695–702.
depressed patients. Results from the Medical Outcomes Study. JAMA. 30. Davila J, Cobb RJ. Predicting change in self-reported and interviewer-
1989;262:914–919. assessed adult attachment: tests of the individual difference and life stress
11. Klerman GK, Weissman MM, Rounsaville BJ, et al. Interpersonal models of attachment change. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29:859–870.
Psychotherapy of Depression. New York: Basic Books; 1984. 31. West ML, Sheldon-Kellor AE. Patterns of Relating: An Adult Attachment
12. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Volume 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Perspective. New York: Guilford Press; 1994.
Books; 1969. 32. Trinke SJ, Bartholomew K. Hierarchies of attachment relationships in
13. Ciechanowski P, Walker EA, Katon WJ, et al. Attachment theory: young adulthood. J Soc Pers Relat. 1997;14:603–625.
a model for health care utilization and somatization. Psychosom Med. 33. Bartholomew K, Horowitz LM. Attachment styles among young adults:
2002;64:660–667. a test of a four-category model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61:226–244.
14. Ciechanowski P, Katon WJ, Russo JE, et al. Association of attachment 34. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR. Self-report measurement of adult
style to medically unexplained symptoms in patients with hepatitis C. attachment: an integrative overview. In: Simpson JA, Rholes WS, eds.
Psychosomatics. 2002;43:206–212. Attachment Theory and Close Relationships. New York: Guilford; 1998:
15. Bifulco A, Moran PM, Ball C, et al. Adult attachment style. I: Its 46–76.
relationship to clinical depression. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 35. Powell-Tuck J, Brown RL, Lennard-Jones JE. A comparison of oral
2002;37:50–59. prednisone given as single or multiple daily doses for active proctocolitis.
16. Bifulco A, Moran PM, Ball C, et al. Adult attachment style. II: Its Scand J Gastroenterol. 1978;13:833–837.
relationship to psychosocial depressive-vulnerability. Soc Psychiatry 36. Maunder RG, Greenberg GR. Comparison of a disease activity index and
Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2002;37:60–67. patients’ self-reported symptom severity in ulcerative colitis. Inflamm
17. Mikulincer M, Florian V. The relationship between adult attachment styles Bowel Dis. 2004;10:632–636.
and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful events. In: Simpson JA, 37. Fraley RC, Waller NG, Brennan KA. An item response theory analysis of
Rholes WS, eds. Attachment Theory and Close Relationships. New York: self-report measures of adult attachment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;78:
Guilford; 1998. 350–365.
18. Mikulincer M, Florian V, Weller A. Attachment styles, coping strategies, 38. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self report depression scale for research
and posttraumatic psychological distress: the impact of the Gulf War in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401.
in Israel. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:817–826. 39. Schutte NS, Malouff JM. Sourcebook of Adult Assessment Strategies.
19. Mikulincer M, Horesh N, Eilati I, et al. The relationship between adult New York: Plenum; 1995.
attachment style and mental health in extreme life-endangering con- 40. Kraemer HC, Stice E, Kazdin A, et al. How do risk factors work together?
ditions. Pers Individ Dif. 1999;27:831–842. Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk
20. West M, Livesley WJ, Reiffer L, et al. The place of attachment in the life factors. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:848–856.
events model of stress and illness. Can J Psychiatry. 1986;31:202–207. 41. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, et al. Applied Multiple Regression/Cor-
21. Maunder RG, Hunter JJ. Attachment and psychosomatic medicine: relation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ:
developmental contributions to stress and disease. Psychosom Med. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2003.
2001;63:556–567. 42. Hazan C, Shaver PR. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
22. Ciechanowski PS, Katon WJ, Russo JE, et al. The patient-provider process. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52:511–524.
relationship: attachment theory and adherence to treatment in diabetes. 43. Klohnen EC, John OP. Working models of attachment: a theory-based
Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:29–35. prototype approach. In: Simpson JA, Rholes WS, eds. Attachment Theory
23. Hunter JJ, Maunder RG. Using attachment theory to understand illness and Close Relationships. New York: Guilford; 1998:115–140.
behavior. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2001;23:177–182. 44. Bartholomew K. Avoidance of intimacy: an attachment perspective. J Soc
24. Panzer A, Viljoen M, Maunder RG, et al. The difficult patient: an attach- Pers Relat. 1990;7:147–178.
ment perspective. SA Fam Pract. 2003;45:11–13. 45. Elkin I, Shea MT, Watkins JT, et al. NIMH treatment of depression
25. Ainsworth M, Blehar M, Waters E, et al. Patterns of Attachment: A collaborative research program. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46:971–982.
Psychological Study Of The Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 46. Weissman MM, Markowitz JC. Interpersonal psychotherapy. Current
1978. status. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:599–606.
26. Fraley RC. Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: meta-analysis 47. Markowitz JC, Klerman GL, Perry SW. Interpersonal psychotherapy of
and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Pers Soc Psychol depressed HIV-positive outpatients. Hosp Comm Psychiatry. 1992;43:
Rev. 2002;6:123–151. 885–890.

926 q 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

You might also like